Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 04 June 2021
Sec. Neuropsychology

Neuropsychological Tests in Post-operative Cognitive Dysfunction: Methods and Applications

\nJun LiuJun LiuKequn HuangKequn HuangBinbin ZhuBinbin ZhuBin ZhouBin ZhouAhmad Khaled Ahmad HarbAhmad Khaled Ahmad HarbLin Liu
Lin Liu*Xiang Wu
Xiang Wu*
  • Department of Anesthesiology, The Affiliated Hospital of Medical School, Ningbo University, Ningbo, China

Post-operative cognitive dysfunction (POCD) is a neurological complication that relatively frequently occurs in older people after anesthesia/surgery, with varying durations and significant differences in the severity of cognitive impairment. POCD is mainly characterized by memory loss mostly without consciousness disorders, accompanied by abnormal emotions, behaviors, and language, mostly without consciousness disorder. The clinical performance of POCD lacks specificity but can reflect the severity of cognitive impairment in patients. The diagnosis of POCD cannot be separated from the evaluation of perioperative cognitive function of patients, and the more popular and accepted method is neuropsychological tests (NPTs).

Post-operative cognitive dysfunction (POCD) is the functional impairment of the nervous system activities, such as memory, executive function, language, direction, emotion, visual space structural ability, in patients after surgery/anesthesia, which is a kind of reversible mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Most of them last for a short period (Abildstrom et al., 2000). POCD can hinder patients from recovery, prolong the length of stay, as well as increase mortality (Monk et al., 2008), and the risk of leaving the labor market prematurely (Steinmetz et al., 2009). In 2018, several studies (Evered et al., 2018) recommended perioperative neurocognitive disorders (PNDs) to replace POCD. PND facilitates diagnosing cognitive changes after anesthesia/surgery with the diagnostic criteria of neurocognitive disorder in the general population. Neuropsychological tests (NPTs) are often used to assess cognitive function in perioperative patients. The timing of neuropsychological evaluation of patients and the specificity and sensitivity of NPTs may all have an impact on the diagnosis of POCD. There are many kinds of NPTs for evaluation, but there is no uniform and complete set of NPT batteries as the diagnostic criteria for POCD. In this review, we highlight the common NPTs of POCD and their development.

Post-operative Cognitive Dysfunction and Post-operative Delirium

Post-operative delirium (POD) and POCD are common complications after surgery. They are two different pathological states but with some correlation. POD is an acute and severe cognitive impairment that mostly occurs in 1–3 days after surgery. POCD is more likely to appear 1 week after surgery and last for several months and even longer (Newman et al., 2001; Pérez-Belmonte et al., 2019). There is a specific diagnosis criterion for POD but POCD does not have a recognized one. However, after the recommendation of nomenclature of perioperative cognitive change, POD and POCD are integrated into PNDs and obtain their new and specific definitions and criteria (Evered et al., 2018; Daiello et al., 2019). They share some common risk factors in which age and the level of education are independent and crucial (Monk et al., 2008). But the diminished “cognitive reserve” may be the real reason hidden behind them. POD is related to cognitive dysfunction in a short period after surgery (Sauër et al., 2017; Daiello et al., 2019). At the same time, delirium is likely to interface with dementia on multiple levels, and POD implies a decline in cognitive reserve, predicting a decline in cognitive function and dementia in the future (Nadelson et al., 2014; Fong et al., 2015). Preoperative cognitive impairment such as MCI and early dementia will facilitate the occurrence of POCD (Kline et al., 2012). It has been proposed that POCD may be associated with the occurrence and development of post-operative dementia, based on that anesthesia can lead to the oligomerization and deposition of amyloid β-protein (Fodale et al., 2010), which can promote the pathological pathway of dementia. However, according to an 11-year prospective study, there is no significant correlation between POCD and the occurrence of dementia (Steinmetz et al., 2013). So the relationship between post-operative complications and dementia is still unclear and further exploration will be needed to unmask it.

Evaluation Development of POCD

In 1955, Bedford (1995) first reported a series of neurological complications, such as impaired directional function and forgetfulness of friends and household, in the elderly who had no abnormalities before surgery. Since then, more and more researchers have gradually paid more attention to it, but overall, research has been slow for a long time due to the lack of targeted assessment tools. Later, Moller et al. (1993) took patients with general anesthesia and local anesthesia as the same subjects, which may influence the incidence of POCD (Rasmussen et al., 2003). He also evaluated preoperative and post-operative cognitive function through the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) and continuous reaction time (RT) tests and used questionnaires to assess long-term cognitive function after surgery. Willner and Struve (1970) and Willner et al. (1976) used psycho-interviews and conceptual-level analogy test to evaluate preoperative and post-operative cognitive functions. Mattlar et al. (1991) used evaluation scales and NPTs for cognitive evaluation and used different test combinations at different stages. Chung et al. (1989) applied mini-mental state examination (MMSE) to assess cognitive function 6 h after surgery and 24 h after surgery. Murkin et al. (1995) carried out perioperative NPT and established a healthy control group. Cutoffs for “abnormal” functioning were operationally defined as change scores that were exceeded by 95% of the normative control group, and cognitive dysfunction was defined as impaired performance within one or more of the tests. Due to the limitations of the research methods at this stage, there were great differences among the results derived from all of the researchers in this field at this stage (Rasmussen et al., 2001).

In 1998, Moller et al. conducted a large-scale prospective study in which they used a rigorously screened NPT battery including visual–verbal learning test, concept shifting test, Stroop color–word interference test, pen and paper memory scanning test, letter-digital coding, and four-box test to evaluate the cognitive status and took Z-scores as the criterion of POCD (Moller et al., 1998). After that, other sets of NPT batteries derived from the previously mentioned tests began to be widely used for clinical evaluation. Clinical researchers are increasingly accepting the NPT-based Z-scores as the diagnosis of POCD. With the development of modern science and technology, cognitive evaluation assessment has evolved from traditional pen-and-paper tools to computer-based tests and then to utilizing lightweight and convenient tablet devices, which come with a digital set of assessment tools matched with it. Theoretically, the more NPTs there are in a battery, the more comprehensive and sensitive the evaluation of cognitive function is. Simultaneously, the testing time will increase accordingly, and the compliance of patient will be poor. Multiple NPTs exist with overlapping cognitive domains among them. Thus, NPTs with fewer overlapping portions that offer basic coverage of cognitive domains should be chosen to shorten the test time. Table 1 presents the batteries of NPTs used in clinical studies.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. NPT batteries in clinical studies.

Common NPTS and Their Characteristics

A List of Primary Screening Tests Commonly Used for POCD

Clinically, MMSE and Montreal Cognitive Assessment Scale (MoCA) are used for the initial screening of patients, but these tools are not suitable for follow-up evaluation of patients.

Mini-Mental State Examination

Mini-mental state examination is currently the scale of evaluating cognitive function widely used in the clinical environment. It includes time and spatial orientation, language ability, instant memory, delayed recall, attention, calculation, visual–spatial ability, and executive function. The scale is simple and easy to conduct, which is suitable for the evaluation of elderly patients. The characteristic of covering the majority of cognitive domains makes it suitable for screening cognitive function in the population. However, every coin has two sides. Consequently, it demonstrates low sensitivity and the ceiling effect when this scale is used to detect impairment of a single cognitive domain and mild changes. The total points are 30, and 23/24 is the original cutoff score (Folstein et al., 1975), but the corresponding sensitivity is low. As the cutoff value increases, the sensitivity gradually ascends, and the specificity gradually descends. Taking 26/27 as the boundary point can attain proper sensitivity and specificity.

Montreal Cognitive Assessment Scale

Nasreddine et al. developed MoCA in Montreal, Canada, in 1995, for health professionals to assess mild cognitive dysfunction. This scale evaluates cognitive function from visuospatial, executive function, naming, memory, attention, language, abstraction, and orientation. Furthermore, it is commonly used for screening MCI. A score of 24/25 out of 30 is recommended as the cutoff score for MCI (sensitivity: 80.48%, specificity: 81.19%), and MoCA is more suitable for screening MCI compared with MMSE (Nasreddine et al., 2005; Ciesielska et al., 2016).

NPTs Are Commonly Used to Evaluate Cognitive Status

Visual/Auditory Verbal Learning Test

The visual/auditory verbal learning test is a task of verbal memory designed to measure the ability to learn new verbal information and retrieve it from memory. In other words, the test is mainly to assess working memory. Specialists visually display through a booklet or screen or read 15 low-associated words. This process is repeated five times. After each process, the patient is asked to recall as many words as possible, and after 20 min of the last presentation or reading, the subject is told to recall as many words as possible (Brand and Jolles, 1985). The verbal memory test is particularly sensitive to the early stages of cognitive impairment, and can effectively distinguish between MCI and normal condition. The test is simple and easy to practice. Using two different but equivalent sets of words during preoperative and post-operative assessments can alleviate learning effects. The visual-verbal learning test is also a subtest of the recommended test battery for the International Study of Post-operative Cognitive Dysfunction (ISPOCD). In the literature, it is reported that the telephone version of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVL) has demonstrated good known-groups effectiveness in distinguishing subjective cognitive impairment and amnestic MCI (aMCI) and is significantly correlated with the regular Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (Jagtap et al., 2020). The verbal learning test could be influenced by many factors, such as the speed of presentation, level of education, age, gender, vision, and hearing.

Stroop Color Interference Test

The test was originally developed by Stroop in 1935. It is used to evaluate selective attention, information processing speed, execution, and anti-interference ability. This initial version is composed of three white cards with every card containing 50 items. This test includes four parts: in part 1, the subject is asked to read the randomized color names (green, blue, red, brown, and purple) printed in black. For part 2, the subject needs to read the same color names as part 1 printed in incongruously colored ink (blue, green, yellow, and red). Next in part 3, the subject has to name the same number of correspondingly colored patches. Finally, the subject should name the color of the ink printed in the card used in part 2 disregarding the color name. The time consumed during each part is recorded as scores (Houx et al., 1993). A lot of other versions are developed based on it and these tests are different in the color and the number of items. Stroop word-reading reflected the speed of visual search. Stroop color-naming reflected working memory as well as the speed of visual search. Stroop color-word reflected working memory, conflict monitoring, and speed of visual search (Periáñez et al., 2021). Because the test mainly reflects the attention of the subject, it is also often used for cognitive evaluation in patients with multiple sclerosis or schizophrenia, which can affect selective attention (Morrow, 2013). There are also some limitations of it: (1) Because the test is mainly carried out visually, patients with vision impairment or red–green colorblindness will not complete the test or get inaccurate test scores. (2) For illiterate patients, the third part of the test will not be able to effectively assess the selective attention of the patient, which is prone to a false negative. Some researchers recommend taking the numeral version of the Stroop test as an alternative to the Stroop color interference test (Kulaif and Valle, 2008), but its effectiveness still needs a great deal of research and studies to confirm.

Concept Shifting Test

A kind of NPT based on the trail making test (TMT) to assess the conceptual transfer and executive function in a patient. This test consists of four basic parts. The test material is a paper with four large circles, in which 16 small circles are grouped in a larger circle. The four large circles are assigned in random order with digits, letters, both digits and letters, and with nothing. The participants have to cross out the digits in numerical order, the letters in alphabetical order, the digits and letters in alternating order, and the empty circle in a clockwise orientation. Concept shifting test is usually used in common with Stroop test to perform executive evaluations, but it is reported that the test has low test–retest reliability (Rasmussen and Siersma, 2004). The test is susceptible to gender, age, and education level, while the preferred hand does not affect test performance. In clinical and research domains, the scores participants acquired from this test will be compared to normal data to evaluate cognitive function.

Paper and Pencil Memory Screening Test

The test is improved based on the Sternberg memory screening test. It can evaluate working memory, visual scanning speed, and information-processing speed. Compared with the digit symbol test, it focuses more on the evaluation of working memory. In this test, the patient is first given a blank paper containing one to four letters, and after 5 s, it is replaced with a rectangular test sheet containing 120 random letters. The subject is asked to delete the previous four letters. The tools composing this test are simple, and the test time is short, ~5 min, taking the consumed time as the measurement is simpler than the scoring system of the original screening test. But the test also has some limitations of its own. For example, it is not suitable for subjects who are not familiar with the letters, and the test is also easily affected by age and education level (Van Der Elst et al., 2007).

Grooved Pegboard Test (GPT)

Grooved pegboard test can be used to evaluate the manual flexibility, fast visual–motor coordination, and psychomotor speed in the patient. The assessment tool is a metal plate composed of an array of 25 holes with differently positioned slots and pegs with a little ridge on the side. Only when the ridge is in the same orientation of the slot can the peg be inserted. The subject needs to insert these pegs into the holes in order as quickly as possible until all the holes have been filled, first with the dominant hand and then the non-dominant one. The conductor records the required time of each hand as the two parts of the scoring system. Studies have pointed out that the test has good test–retest reliability from 7 days to 3 months after surgery. This test can also be used for motor function in patients with Parkinson's disease and can adequately reflect the level of clinical assessment of upper-limb control, stiffness, and motor retardation (Sage et al., 2012). Women complete it faster than men. The higher the level of education is, the better the performance is, and the preferred hand has a better performance in contrast with the other hand. GPT also has a new version (removing task) corresponding to the standard version (placement task), and the new version is more susceptible to gender and use of preferred hand effects (Bryden and Roy, 2005). Adding cognitive or motor tasks to the GPT process can better assess manual flexibility and cognitive ability (Espenes et al., 2020).

Trail Making Test

Trail making test is one of the most widely used NPTs. This test includes two parts. Each part consists of 25 circles on a sheet of paper. The circle of TMT part A (TMT-A) contains consecutive numbers from 1 to 25, and participants need to connect them as quickly as possible in numerical order by making pencil lines. The circle of TMT part B (TMT-B) contains consecutive numbers from 1 to 13 and consecutive letters from A to L, and subjects need to connect numbers and letters in alternating order. The test yields two scores that are the needed time of these two parts. TMT-A provides a measurement of visual-scanning speed, while TMT-B additionally involves assessment of executive function. The score difference between TMT-B and TMT-A (TMT B-A) and the ratio of the two scores (TMT B/A) enhance the measurement of executive function. TMT is sensitive to a variety of diseases with neurological deficits; therefore, TMT is suitable as a screening tool for the evaluation of nervous system integrity and individual cognitive function. TMT would be easily affected by age and education level. Education level has a greater correlation with TMT-B. Correspondingly, its derived scores (TMT B-A and TMT B/A) are less affected by age and education level (Cavaco et al., 2013). Many people are not familiar with letters, so the original TMT-B would not be a fitting tool. To solve this problem, researchers developed some other versions, such as the version for Chinese (Lu and Bigler, 2000), but the corresponding norm data and the validity require additional research for further clarification.

Boston Naming Test (BNT)

Boston naming test is a subset of the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE). It is an extensive object-naming test that can be used to evaluate the ability of the patient to find and name words. The standard version of BNT (BNT-S) consists of 60 line drawings of common objects arranged in descending order of word frequency. Participants are asked to verbally speak out the name of the drawings, and some semantic or phonemic cues can be given to subjects when they are stuck in the test process. The scores consist of the number of correct items without cueing, the number of cues given, and the number of correct responses with each kind of cue. At the same time, the short versions of BNT containing 15 or 30 items were derived from it to reduce test time for patients. It is reported that the short BNT has a strong correlation with the BNT-S in outpatient evaluation (Attridge et al., 2020). All brief BNTs retain the function to distinguish between normal subjects and dementia subjects but compared with the full BNT, the short versions are weaker in distinguishing between normal and MCI subjects (Katsumata et al., 2015).

A Complete Set of Cognitive Testing Tools

National Institutes of Health (NIH) Examiner

Executive function is an important part of cognitive assessment, but there is no consensus on the testing methods and operating procedures of executive function. Therefore, The National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke had developed an NPT suited for executive function. This test group involves multiple areas associated with executive function: working memory, inhibition, scene switching, fluency, planning, insight, social cognition, and behavior, combining the scores of the subsets to form an executive composite score using the item response theory. The test group is modular, modifiable, efficient, and suitable for a wide range of age groups and cognitive levels. It has English and Spanish versions and includes computer-based and traditional paper-and-pencil testing formats (Kramer et al., 2014).

NIH Toolbox-Cognitive Battery

National Institutes of Health Toolbox is a comprehensive set of neuro-behavioral measurements, of which cognitive battery is the cognitive assessment part. The assessing process is based on a tablet computer and is simple and easy to implement. The battery consists of tests of seven constructs, including five fluid cognition (executive function, attention, episodic memory, working memory, and processing speed) and two crystallized cognition (vocabulary and word reading). This test group can maintain its stability among people of different age groups, races, genders, and education levels (Ma et al., 2020). It also has good reliability and validity in the evaluation of the healthy elderly (Scott et al., 2019). The decrease of the high-scores rate of the battery reflects the decline of cognitive function in participants with greater education and crystallized ability (Karr and Iverson, 2020). There are some other computer-based NPTs listed in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Computer-based NPTs.

Modern NPTs Based on the Internet and Artificial Intelligence

An online application (Lunardini et al., 2020) based on a tablet has attracted more attention, and its built-in voice assistant can realize fully autonomous evaluation. The NPT based on video conference can realize remote evaluation, which is of great significance for people in remote areas, but there are some limitations, for example, it cannot provide an evaluation of cognitive functions in comparison to hands-on tests. At the same time, whether video-related factors will impact on reliability and validity of the test have not been fully confirmed. Some initial evidence (Brearly et al., 2017) demonstrates the NPT mediated by language is less affected by video-related factors and particularly suitable as video-related subsets. Web-based evaluation methods are conducive to recruiting subjects and large-scale data collection and will be a potential development direction of cognitive evaluation in the future. Researchers are trying to use biomedical informatics for managing big data from advanced assessment methods, which can make more effective use of data sources (Miller, 2019).

With the advent of the era of intelligence, artificial intelligence has gradually been applied to the field of cognitive assessment. Kang et al. (2019) used artificial network algorithms to predict the impairment of cognitive function by summarizing multi-center NPT data, and both its predictive sensitivity and specificity were above 95%. Artificial intelligence is also applied in the field of Alzheimer's disease (AD) and has become a more powerful method of extracting reliable predictors and automatically classifying different phenotypes (Battista et al., 2020). The traditional cognitive-evaluation process is easily influenced by changes in the surrounding environment, and Virtual Reality (VR) technology can provide an immersive feeling as well as keep other interference relatively constant at each testing stage. Because VR has many advantages over traditional NPTs, investigators are paying more and more attention to the application of VR for cognitive testing and making neuropsychological decisions. Amato et al. (2020) used the NPT battery “CONVIRT,” fitted with VR for the evaluation of cognitive dysfunction caused by alcohol, which showed high sensitivity and test–retest reliability.

Although these NPTs can more accurately reflect the domain-specific change and severity of cognitive impairment, in the real clinical environment, the natural decline in the functional state in patients, or emotional and physical conditions influenced by surgery and disease itself, will lead to the patients not being in a suitable state to conduct NPT (Hoffmann, 2020). For example, patients with decreased concentration levels or abulia may not be fit for the relatively complex and time-consuming batteries, so the relatively short mental tests such as MMSE or MoCA will be more suitable for the majority of such patients.

Diagnosis of POCD

Cognitive function can be evaluated through scales associated with intelligence, memory, cognition and others, clinical manifestations, activities of daily living (ADLs), subjective cognitive concern, NPT batteries, and computer-based cognitive evaluation systems. The subjective cognitive complaint is more sensitive to the patient with richer cognitive reserves. Evaluative tools are the core of diagnosing POCD, and the timing choice of evaluation is also an important part of it. If these tools are conducted too early, it could confuse POCD with POD. However, there is no uniform and recognized timing. As is shown in related researches, the baseline test is usually carried out 1 week before the operation. The early post-operative cognitive test is mostly performed 7 days after the operation. Moreover, the medium- and long-term post-operative cognitive tests often take place at 3 and 12 months after surgery. At this stage, evaluating POCD based on NPT is still the mainstream.

Z-Score Method

The calculative method for the Z-score of a single test is to subtract from the follow-up score the baseline test score and the mean change on test in the control group, then divide the result by the SD of the change in the control group. The approach yields a combined Z-score for each subject by dividing the sum of Z-scores of all subsets of one person by the SD of the total Z-score in the control group. POCD is defined as at least two Z-scores in individual tests or the combined Z-score is 1.96 or more (Zhang et al., 2018). Some studies also take 2 as the cutoff Z-score, and the diagnosing criterion of POCD is that the combined score, or the Z-scores of at least two subsets, or 20% of the individual NPTs are 2 or more (Rudolph et al., 2008; Rodriguez et al., 2010). The Z-score method usually needs to recruit the control group coincident with the same inclusion/exclusion criteria. Some researchers have proposed a different method without setting a control group, which is just replacing the average and SD of the traditional Z-score with the average and SD of the experimental group. The combined Z-score is the average of all test Z-scores for a single subject, and a negative combined Z-value means a decline in cognitive status (Yocum et al., 2009).

SD Method

The difference of a single test between the post-operative and preoperative scores is compared with the SD of the baseline scores of the corresponding test. POCD is defined as the changing scores of no less than one test of the testing battery, which are one SD or more (Royse et al., 2011; Klinger et al., 2019). At the same time, exceeding 1 SD is regarded as mild cognitive decline, 1.5 SDs is the moderate cognitive decline and 2 >SDs are severe cognitive decline. Due to the lack of the non-anesthetic/surgical control group, it is impossible to analyze the impact of learning effects and physical cognitive decline caused by aging on NPTs. The SD represents individual differences that are easily affected by multiple factors. If individual variability is great, the false-negative rate will increase, otherwise, the false-positive rate could be high.

Percentage Method

The percentage method is similar to the SD method. The percentage of the difference between baseline and follow-up scores in the corresponding test is taken as the criterion for POCD. Because there is no control group and no unified standard for the percentage, the reliability of this definition is not satisfying. With the emergence of some new and recognized methods, the frequency of this diagnosing method gradually decreases, and it is not used as the main judging criterion of POCD anymore.

After POCD is integrated into PNDs, the diagnosis of POCD is based on subjective cognitive concern, objective NPTs, and ADLs. At the same time, in terms of NPTs, the difference between baseline and follow-up tests is no longer the compared index. Researchers just need to conduct the post-operative tests and compare them to normal data, which is more comprehensive and in line with the clinical diagnostic criteria. But additional research needs to be conducted to obtain normative reference values that match the different levels of demographic factors at the same time, such as gender, age, and education level.

Characteristics and Limitations of NPT

The three crucial parts of cognitive-function evaluation are objectively neuropsychological measurement, self-cognitive concern, and a cognitive concern of an informant. NPT belongs to objective assessment, and is not only the main evaluating method of POCD, but is also widely used in the evaluation of cognitive impairment caused by AD, vascular dementia, or Parkinson's disease. An ideal NPT should meet some requirements, such as high sensitivity, specificity, test–retest reliability, and inter-tester reliability. In addition, the overall evaluative process should be simple and easy as well as take a short period of time. Other requirements for an ideal NPT include patients with good compliance, the NPT has alternative versions or a low learning effect, the presence of normative data of this test, the NPT is sensitive to mild changes of cognitive function, the test has corresponding versions of different kinds of languages, and the NPT should not be easily affected by other non-cognitive factors.

With the development of clinical research, more and more attention is paid to limitations of NPT: (1) The diagnosis of POCD requires repeated evaluations of cognitive function and some NPTs, without alternative versions can cause learning effects in repeated tests. This can cover up cognitive impairment or even lead to post-operative cognitive improvement. (2) NPT is prone to a ceiling effect in people with rich cognitive reserves, which reduces its sensitivity. (3) NPT requires professionals to perform in a standard test environment and interpret the test results, which has limitations in clinical applications. (4) A more comprehensive evaluation of the cognitive domains often requires joint application of multiple tests to cover as many cognitive domains as possible. In addition, the time-consuming evaluation process can reduce the compliance of the subject and performer so that the test result cannot reflect the true level of cognition. (5) Different NPTs can cover different cognitive domains. The joint implementation of different NPTs evaluates multiple cognitive domains, but a test does not only involve one cognitive domain so that there will be cognitive overlap among different tests. (6) NPT is easily affected by culture, education level, language, and so on. (7) At present, the normal data of a variety of NPTs are not enough, and the existing data mostly come from smaller clinical samples. (8) The traditional evaluative process has some of the problems of ecological validity, and particular areas are underrepresented, especially those involving high-level cognitive skills and social skills.

Performance Validity Tests (PVTs)

The NPT score is the main indicator to reflect the cognitive function of the subject. However, the testing scores produced by the subjects in the standard process may not truly represent the real cognitive function and may be influenced by several factors, most notably inadequate task engagement. Invalid results may affect the judgment of cognitive function of the examinee, which can cause some serious repercussions. The national neuropsychological organizations (Bush et al., 2005; Heilbronner et al., 2009) advocated the inclusion of PVTs in routine NPTs many years ago. PVTs have independent and embedded versions. The independent version is still used as the diagnostic criteria for performance validity and includes Medical Symptom Validity Test (MSVT), Test of Memory Malingering-Trail I (TOMM TI), Advanced Clinical Solutions Word Choice Test (WCT), and Dot Counting Test (DCT), etc. The diagnostic indicators of the embedded version are derived from existing NPTs, so it has the dual role of reflecting the cognitive function and test validity. Embedded PVTs are becoming more and more popular in clinical and research environments.

Outlook

Neuropsychological testing is the main evaluation method of POCD. The integration of traditional neuropsychological evaluation with neurophysiological indicators of EEG or imaging examination can further study the relationship between the human brain and behavior (Marcopulos and Łojek, 2019). Demographic characteristics will affect the validity of NPTs, and this influence can be reduced by establishing norms corresponding to different characteristic levels. With the development of society, digital evaluation tools have attracted more and more attention. Compared with the traditional tools, they have the advantages of high efficiency, accurate result recording, convenient extraction of test results, and less influence by examiners. However, digital tools are the lock of the norms, which may limit its clinical application to a certain extent. Elderly patients have low acceptance of digital testing and lack independent test guidance and supervision, and these factors will also affect the widespread use of digital assessment tools.

Modern evaluation tools will gradually become the assessing mainstream, but the important status of classic evaluation methods still cannot be ignored. The corresponding norms of different tests are not enough, and testing tools are not systematic, which could lead to many inconveniences in mutual quoting. In the future, it will be valuable that there can be more research on these domains, and professionals can establish a global neuropsychological database combined with artificial intelligence. At the same time, with the development of science and technology, the application of new and highly specific digital evaluation methods can make various research or clinical applications more convenient and efficient, and will also obtain more accurate evaluation results.

Author Contributions

JL, KH, BZhu, BZhou, LL, and XW: equal contribution for the literature search, writing, and correcting of this review article. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was funded by Zhejiang Provincial Natural Science Foundation of China (LY19H25001), Medical Health Science and Technology Project of Zhejiang Provincial Health Commission (2019RC266 and 2021KY1043), Ningbo Medical Science and Technology Project (2019Y16), Ningbo Science and Technology Innovation 2025 major special Project (2019B10035), and the Affiliated Hospital of Medical School of Ningbo University Youth Talent Cultivation Program (FYQM-LC-202002).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Abildstrom, H., Rasmussen, L. S., Rentowl, P., Hanning, C. D., Rasmussen, H., Kristensen, P. A., et al. (2000). Cognitive dysfunction 1-2 years after non-cardiac surgery in the elderly. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 44, 1246–1251. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-6576.2000.441010.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amato, I., Nanev, A., Piantella, S., Wilson, K. E., Bicknell, R., Heckenberg, R., et al. (2020). Assessing the utility of a virtual-reality neuropsychological test battery, 'CONVIRT', in detecting alcohol-induced cognitive impairment. Behav. Res. Methods. 132(Pt 10), 2850–2870. doi: 10.3758/s13428-020-01485-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Attridge, J., Zimmerman, D., Rolin, S., and Davis, J. J. (2020). Comparing Boston naming test short forms in a rehabilitation sample. Appl. Neuropsychol. Adult 1–6. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2020.1811984. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Backx, R., Skirrow, C., Dente, P., Barnett, J. H., and Cormack, F. K. (2020). Comparing web-based and lab-based cognitive assessment using the cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery: a within-subjects counterbalanced study. J. Med. Intern. Res. 22:e16792. doi: 10.2196/16792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, M. T., Moring, J. C., Hale, W. J., Mintz, J., Young-McCaughan, S., Bryant, R. A., et al. (2018). Acute assessment of traumatic brain injury and post-traumatic stress after exposure to a deployment-related explosive blast. Mil. Med. 183, e555–e563. doi: 10.1093/milmed/usy100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Battista, P., Salvatore, C., Berlingeri, M., Cerasa, A., and Castiglioni, I. (2020). Artificial intelligence and neuropsychological measures: the case of Alzheimer's disease. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 114, 211–228. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.04.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bedford, P. D. (1995). Adverse cerebral effects of anaesthesia on old people. Lancet 269, 259–263.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Berger, M., Murdoch, D. M., Staats, J. S., Chan, C., Thomas, J. P., Garrigues, G. E., et al. (2019). Flow cytometry characterization of cerebrospinal fluid monocytes in patients with postoperative cognitive dysfunction: a pilot study. Anesth. Analg. 129, e150–e154. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000004179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, N., and Jolles, J. (1985). Learning and retrieval rate of words presented auditorily and visually. J. Gen. Psychol. 112, 201–210. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1985.9711004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brearly, T. W., Shura, R. D., Martindale, S. L., Lazowski, R. A., Luxton, D. D., Shenal, B. V., et al. (2017). Neuropsychological test administration by videoconference: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol. Rev. 27, 174–186. doi: 10.1007/s11065-017-9349-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryden, P. J., and Roy, E. A. (2005). A new method of administering the grooved pegboard test: performance as a function of handedness and sex. Brain Cogn. 58, 258–268. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bush, S. S., Ruff, R. M., Tröster, A. I., Barth, J. T., Koffler, S. P., Pliskin, N. H., et al. (2005). Symptom validity assessment: practice issues and medical necessity NAN policy & planning committee. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 20, 419–426. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2005.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahn-Hidalgo, D., Estes, P. W., and Benabou, R. (2020). Validity, reliability, and psychometric properties of a computerized, cognitive assessment test (Cognivue(®)). World J. Psychiatry 10, 1–11. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v10.i1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavaco, S., Gonçalves, A., Pinto, C., Almeida, E., Gomes, F., Moreira, I., et al. (2013). Trail Making Test: regression-based norms for the Portuguese population. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 28, 189–198. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acs115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, F., Lavelle, P. A., McDonald, S., Chung, A., and McDonald, N. J. (1989). Cognitive impairment after neuroleptanalgesia in cataract surgery. Anesth. Analg. 68, 614–618. doi: 10.1213/00000539-198905000-00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciesielska, N., Sokołowski, R., Mazur, E., Podhorecka, M., Polak-Szabela, A., and Kedziora-Kornatowska, K. (2016). Is the montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) test better suited than the mini-mental state examination (MMSE) in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) detection among people aged over 60? Meta-analysis. Psychiatr. Polska 50, 1039–1052. doi: 10.12740/PP/45368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daiello, L. A., Racine, A. M., Yun Gou, R., Marcantonio, E. R., Xie, Z., Kunze, L. J., et al. (2019). Postoperative delirium and postoperative cognitive dysfunction: overlap and divergence. Anesthesiology 131, 477–491. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000002729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deiner, S., Liu, X., Lin, H. M., Sieber, F., Boockvar, K., Sano, M., et al. (2019). Subjective cognitive complaints in patients undergoing major non-cardiac surgery: a prospective single centre cohort trial. Br. J. Anaesth. 122, 742–750. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2019.02.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, Y., Wang, R., Li, S., Zhu, X., Wang, T., Wu, J., et al. (2021). Methylene blue reduces incidence of early postoperative cognitive disorders in elderly patients undergoing major non-cardiac surgery: an open-label randomized controlled clinical trial. J. Clin. Anesth. 68:110108. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinane.2020.110108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espenes, J., Hessen, E., Eliassen, I. V., Waterloo, K., Eckerström, M., Sando, S. B., et al. (2020). Demographically adjusted trail making test norms in a Scandinavian sample from 41 to 84 years. Clin. Neuropsychol. 34, 110–126. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2020.1829068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evered, L., Silbert, B., Knopman, D. S., Scott, D. A., DeKosky, S. T., Rasmussen, L. S., et al. (2018). Recommendations for the nomenclature of cognitive change associated with anaesthesia and surgery-20181. J. Alzheimer's Dis. 66, 1–10. doi: 10.3233/JAD-189004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fodale, V., Santamaria, L. B., Schifilliti, D., and Mandal, P. K. (2010). Anaesthetics and postoperative cognitive dysfunction: a pathological mechanism mimicking Alzheimer's disease. Anaesthesia 65, 388–395. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2044.2010.06244.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fong, T. G., Davis, D., Growdon, M. E., Albuquerque, A., and Inouye, S. K. (2015). The interface between delirium and dementia in elderly adults. Lancet Neurol. 14, 823–832. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00101-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geng, Y. J., Wu, Q. H., and Zhang, R. Q. (2017). Effect of propofol, sevoflurane, and isoflurane on postoperative cognitive dysfunction following laparoscopic cholecystectomy in elderly patients: A randomized controlled trial. J. Clin. Anesth. 38, 165–171. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinane.2017.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goh, S. C., Saw, A. E., Kountouris, A., Orchard, J. W., and Saw, R. (2021). Neurocognitive changes associated with concussion in elite cricket players are distinct from changes due to post-match with no head impact. J. Sci. Med. Sport 24, 420–424. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2020.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heilbronner, R. L., Sweet, J. J., Morgan, J. E., Larrabee, G. J., and Millis, S. R. (2009). American academy of clinical neuropsychology consensus conference statement on the neuropsychological assessment of effort, response bias, and malingering. Clin. Neuropsychol. 23, 1093–1129. doi: 10.1080/13854040903155063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffmann, M., (ed.). (2020). Clinical Mentation Evaluation: A Connectomal Approach to Rapid and Comprehensive Assessment. Florida, doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-46324-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffmann, M., Schmitt, F., and Bromley, E. (2009). Comprehensive cognitive neurological assessment in stroke. Acta Neurol. Scand. 119, 162–171. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2008.01101.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houx, P. J., Jolles, J., and Vreeling, F. W. (1993). Stroop interference: aging effects assessed with the stroop color-word test. Exp. Aging Res. 19, 209–224. doi: 10.1080/03610739308253934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jagtap, S., Dawson, D. R., Vandermorris, S., Anderson, N. D., Davids-Brumer, N., Dar, M., et al. (2020). Known-groups and convergent validity of the telephone rey auditory verbal learning test total learning scores for distinguishing between older adults with amnestic cognitive impairment and subjective cognitive decline. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 36:acaa085. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acaa085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, M. J., Kim, S. Y., Na, D. L., Kim, B. C., Yang, D. W., Kim, E. J., et al. (2019). Prediction of cognitive impairment via deep learning trained with multi-center neuropsychological test data. BMC Med. Inform. Decis. Making 19:231. doi: 10.1186/s12911-019-0974-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karr, J. E., and Iverson, G. L. (2020). Interpreting high scores on the nih toolbox cognition battery: potential utility for detecting cognitive decline in high-functioning individuals. Neuropsychology. 34, 764–773. doi: 10.1037/neu0000691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katsumata, Y., Mathews, M., Abner, E. L., Jicha, G. A., Caban-Holt, A., Smith, C. D., et al. (2015). Assessing the discriminant ability, reliability, and comparability of multiple short forms of the boston naming test in an Alzheimer's disease center cohort. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disorders 39, 215–227. doi: 10.1159/000370108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. P., Pirraglia, E., Cheng, H., De Santi, S., Li, Y., Haile, M., et al. (2012). Surgery and brain atrophy in cognitively normal elderly subjects and subjects diagnosed with mild cognitive impairment. Anesthesiology 116, 603–612. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e318246ec0b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klinger, R. Y., Cooter, M., Bisanar, T., Terrando, N., Berger, M., Podgoreanu, M. V., et al. (2019). Intravenous lidocaine does not improve neurologic outcomes after cardiac surgery: a randomized controlled trial. Anesthesiology 130, 958–970. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000002668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, J. H., Mungas, D., Possin, K. L., Rankin, K. P., Boxer, A. L., Rosen, H. J., et al. (2014). NIH EXAMINER: conceptualization and development of an executive function battery. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 20, 11–19. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713001094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kulaif, T., and Valle, L. E. (2008). Alternative to the stroop color-word test for illiterate individuals. Clin. Neuropsychol. 22, 73–83. doi: 10.1080/13854040601186964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langer, T., Santini, A., Zadek, F., Chiodi, M., Pugni, P., Cordolcini, V., et al. (2019). Intraoperative hypotension is not associated with postoperative cognitive dysfunction in elderly patients undergoing general anesthesia for surgery: results of a randomized controlled pilot trial. J. Clin. Anesth. 52, 111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinane.2018.09.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W. X., Luo, R. Y., Chen, C., Li, X., Ao, J. S., Liu, Y., et al. (2019). Effects of propofol, dexmedetomidine, and midazolam on postoperative cognitive dysfunction in elderly patients: a randomized controlled preliminary trial. Chin. Med. J. 132, 437–445. doi: 10.1097/CM9.0000000000000098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, L., and Bigler, E. D. (2000). Performance on original and a Chinese version of trail making test part B: a normative bilingual sample. Appl. Neuropsychol. 7, 243–246. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN0704_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lunardini, F., Luperto, M., Romeo, M., Basilico, N., Daniele, K., Azzolino, D., et al. (2020). Supervised digital neuropsychological tests for cognitive decline in older adults: usability and clinical validity study. JMIR mHealth uHealth 8:e17963. doi: 10.2196/17963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, Y., Carlsson, C. M., Wahoske, M. L., Blazel, H. M., Chappell, R. J., Johnson, S. C., et al. (2020). Latent factor structure and measurement invariance of the NIH toolbox cognition battery in an Alzheimer's disease research sample. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 27, 412–425. doi: 10.1017/S1355617720000922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcopulos, B., and Łojek, E. (2019). Introduction to the special issue: are modern neuropsychological assessment methods really “modern”? Reflections on the current neuropsychological test armamentarium. Clin. Neuropsychol. 33, 187–199. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2018.1560502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattlar, C. E., Engblom, E., Vesala, P., Vänttinen, E., and Knuts, L. R. (1991). The proportion of patients with cognitive impairment after coronary artery bypass surgery: an 8-month follow-up study. Psychother. Psychosom. 55, 145–150. doi: 10.1159/000288422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Memória, C. M., Yassuda, M. S., Nakano, E. Y., and Forlenza, O. V. (2014). Contributions of the computer-administered neuropsychological screen for mild cognitive impairment (CANS-MCI) for the diagnosis of MCI in Brazil. Int. Psychogeriatr. 26, 1–9. doi: 10.1017/S1041610214000726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, J. B. (2019). Big data and biomedical informatics: Preparing for the modernization of clinical neuropsychology. Clin. Neuropsychol. 33, 287–304. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2018.1523466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moller, J. T., Cluitmans, P., Rasmussen, L. S., Houx, P., Rasmussen, H., Canet, J., et al. (1998). Long-term postoperative cognitive dysfunction in the elderly ISPOCD1 study. ISPOCD investigators. International Study of Post-Operative Cognitive Dysfunction. Lancet 351, 857–861. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)07382-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moller, J. T., Svennild, I., Johannessen, N. W., Jensen, P. F., Espersen, K., Gravenstein, J. S., et al. (1993). Perioperative monitoring with pulse oximetry and late postoperative cognitive dysfunction. Br. J. Anaesth. 71, 340–347. doi: 10.1093/bja/71.3.340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monk, T. G., Weldon, B. C., Garvan, C. W., Dede, D. E., van der Aa, M. T., Heilman, K. M., et al. (2008). Predictors of cognitive dysfunction after major noncardiac surgery. Anesthesiology 108, 18–30. doi: 10.1097/01.anes.0000296071.19434.1e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrow, S. A. (2013). Normative data for the Stroop color word test for a North American population. The Canadian journal of neurological sciences. Le J. Can. Sci. Neurol. 40, 842–847. doi: 10.1017/S0317167100015997

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murkin, J. M., Martzke, J. S., Buchan, A. M., Bentley, C., and Wong, C. J. (1995). A randomized study of the influence of perfusion technique and pH management strategy in 316 patients undergoing coronary artery bypass surgery. II. Neurologic and cognitive outcomes. J. Thorac. Cardiovasc. Surg. 110, 349–362. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5223(95)70230-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nadelson, M. R., Sanders, R. D., and Avidan, M. S. (2014). Perioperative cognitive trajectory in adults. Br. J. Anaesth. 112, 440–451. doi: 10.1093/bja/aet420

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bedirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, M. F., Kirchner, J. L., Phillips-Bute, B., Gaver, V., Grocott, H., Jones, R. H., et al. (2001). Longitudinal assessment of neurocognitive function after coronary-artery bypass surgery. N. Engl. Med. J. 344, 395–402. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200102083440601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pérez-Belmonte, L. M., Florido-Santiago, M., Osuna-Sánchez, J., Barbancho, M. A., Millán-Gómez, M., Jímenez-Navarro, M. F., et al. (2019). Screening versus brief domain-specific tests to assess long-term postoperative cognitive dysfunction after concomitant aortic valve replacement and coronary artery bypass grafting. J. Cardiovasc. Nurs. 34, 511–516. doi: 10.1097/JCN.0000000000000596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Periáñez, J. A., Lubrini, G., García-Gutiérrez, A., and Ríos-Lago, M. (2021). Construct validity of the stroop color-word test: influence of speed of visual search, verbal fluency, working memory, cognitive flexibility, and conflict monitoring. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 36, 99–111. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acaa034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Press Y. Punchik B. Kagan E. Berzak A. Freud T. Dwolatzky T. (2021). Methylphenidate for mild cognitive impairment: an exploratory 3-day, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Front. Med. 8:594228. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2021.594228

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quan, C., Chen, J., Luo, Y., Zhou, L., He, X., Liao, Y., et al. (2019). BIS-guided deep anesthesia decreases short-term postoperative cognitive dysfunction and peripheral inflammation in elderly patients undergoing abdominal surgery. Brain Behav. 9:e01238. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasmussen, L. S., Johnson, T., Kuipers, H. M., Kristensen, D., Siersma, V. D., Vila, P., et al. (2003). Does anaesthesia cause postoperative cognitive dysfunction? A randomised study of regional versus general anaesthesia in 438 elderly patients. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 47, 260–266. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-6576.2003.00057.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasmussen, L. S., Larsen, K., Houx, P., Skovgaard, L. T., Hanning, C. D., Moller, J. T., et al. (2001). The assessment of postoperative cognitive function. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 45, 275–289. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-6576.2001.045003275.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasmussen, L. S., and Siersma, V. D. (2004). Postoperative cognitive dysfunction: true deterioration versus random variation. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 48, 1137–1143. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-6576.2004.00502.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzo, J. R., Hudson, T. E., Dai, W., Birkemeier, J., Pasculli, R. M., Selesnick, I., et al. (2016). Rapid number naming in chronic concussion: eye movements in the King-Devick test. Ann. Clin. Transl. Neurol. 3, 801–811. doi: 10.1002/acn3.345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodriguez, R. A., Rubens, F. D., Wozny, D., and Nathan, H. J. (2010). Cerebral emboli detected by transcranial Doppler during cardiopulmonary bypass are not correlated with postoperative cognitive deficits. Stroke 41, 2229–2235. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.590513

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Royse, C. F., Andrews, D. T., Newman, S. N., Stygall, J., Williams, Z., Pang, J., et al. (2011). The influence of propofol or desflurane on postoperative cognitive dysfunction in patients undergoing coronary artery bypass surgery. Anaesthesia 66, 455–464. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2044.2011.06704.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudolph, J. L., Marcantonio, E. R., Culley, D. J., Silverstein, J. H., Rasmussen, L. S., Crosby, G. J., et al. (2008). Delirium is associated with early postoperative cognitive dysfunction. Anaesthesia 63, 941–947. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2044.2008.05523.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sage, M. D., Bryden, P. J., Roy, E. A., and Almeida, Q. J. (2012). The relationship between the grooved pegboard test and clinical motor symptom evaluation across the spectrum of Parkinson's disease severity. J. Parkinson's Dis. 2, 207–213. doi: 10.3233/JPD-2012-012093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauër, A. C., Veldhuijzen, D. S., Ottens, T. H., Slooter, A. J. C., Kalkman, C. J., and van Dijk, D. (2017). Association between delirium and cognitive change after cardiac surgery. Br. J. Anaesth. 119, 308–315. doi: 10.1093/bja/aex053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, E. P., Sorrell, A., and Benitez, A. (2019). Psychometric properties of the NIH toolbox cognition battery in healthy older adults: reliability, validity, and agreement with standard neuropsychological tests. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 25, 857–867. doi: 10.1017/S1355617719000614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silbert, B., Evered, L., Scott, D. A., McMahon, S., Choong, P., Ames, D., et al. (2015). Preexisting cognitive impairment is associated with postoperative cognitive dysfunction after hip joint replacement surgery. Anesthesiology 122, 1224–1234. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinmetz, J., Christensen, K. B., Lund, T., Lohse, N., and Rasmussen, L. S. (2009). Long-term consequences of postoperative cognitive dysfunction. Anesthesiology 110, 548–555. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e318195b569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinmetz, J., Siersma, V., Kessing, L. V., and Rasmussen, L. S. (2013). Is postoperative cognitive dysfunction a risk factor for dementia? A cohort follow-up study. Br. J. Anaesth. 110 Suppl 1, i92–i97. doi: 10.1093/bja/aes466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, P., Goodman, S., Sommer, B. R., Maloney, W., Huddleston, J., and Lemmens, H. J. (2017). The effect of desflurane versus propofol anesthesia on postoperative delirium in elderly obese patients undergoing total knee replacement: a randomized, controlled, double-blinded clinical trial. J. Clin. Anesth. 39, 17–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinane.2017.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Der Elst, W., Van Boxtel, M. P., Van Breukelen, G. J., and Jolles, J. (2007). Assessment of information processing in working memory in applied settings: the paper and pencil memory scanning test. Psychol. Med. 37, 1335–1344. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vedel, A. G., Holmgaard, F., Siersma, V., Langkilde, A., Paulson, O. B., Ravn, H. B., et al. (2019). Domain-specific cognitive dysfunction after cardiac surgery. A secondary analysis of a randomized trial. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 63, 730–738. doi: 10.1111/aas.13343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willner, A. E., Rabiner, C. J., Wisoff, B. G., Fishman, J., Rosen, B., Hartstein, M., et al. (1976). Analogy tests and psychopathology at follow-up after open heart surgery. Biol. Psychiatry 11, 687–696. doi: 10.1037/e668292012-505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willner, A. E., and Struve, F. A. (1970). An analogy test that predicts EEG abnormality. Use with hospitalized psychiatric patients. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 23, 428–437. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1970.01750050044007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodhouse, J., Heyanka, D. J., Scott, J., Vincent, A., Roebuck-Spencer, T., Domboski-Davidson, K., et al. (2013). Efficacy of the ANAM general neuropsychological screening battery (ANAM GNS) for detecting neurocognitive impairment in a mixed clinical sample. Clin. Neuropsychol. 27, 376–385. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2012.762427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yocum, G. T., Gaudet, J. G., Teverbaugh, L. A., Quest, D. O., McCormick, P. C., Connolly, E. S. Jr., et al. (2009). Neurocognitive performance in hypertensive patients after spine surgery. Anesthesiology 110, 254–261. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181942c7a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Shan, G. J., Zhang, Y. X., Cao, S. J., Zhu, S. N., Li, H. J., et al. (2018). Propofol compared with sevoflurane general anaesthesia is associated with decreased delayed neurocognitive recovery in older adults. Br. J. Anaesth. 121, 595–604. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2018.05.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: anesthesia, surgery, neuropsychological assessment, cognitive dysfunction, perioperative

Citation: Liu J, Huang K, Zhu B, Zhou B, Ahmad Harb AK, Liu L and Wu X (2021) Neuropsychological Tests in Post-operative Cognitive Dysfunction: Methods and Applications. Front. Psychol. 12:684307. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.684307

Received: 24 March 2021; Accepted: 06 May 2021;
Published: 04 June 2021.

Edited by:

Michele Scandola, University of Verona, Italy

Reviewed by:

Michael Hoffmann, University of Central Florida, United States
Livia Stocco Sanches Valentin, University of São Paulo, Brazil

Copyright © 2021 Liu, Huang, Zhu, Zhou, Ahmad Harb, Liu and Wu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lin Liu, MTA0Nzk4MDkmI3gwMDA0MDtxcS5jb20=; Xiang Wu, Znl3dXhpYW5nMiYjeDAwMDQwO25idS5lZHUuY24=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.