Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 30 November 2021
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Bullying, Cyberbullying, and Dating Violence: State of the Art, Evaluation Instruments and Prevention and Intervention Proposals View all 6 articles

A Meta-Analytic Review of Moral Disengagement and Cyberbullying

  • Department of Psychology, School of Education Science, Liaocheng University, Liaocheng, China

With the development of technology, cyberbullying prevalence rates are increasing worldwide, and a growing body of the literature has begun to document cyberbullying behavior. Moral disengagement is often considered a key correlate factor in cyberbullying. This article aims to conduct a meta-analysis review of the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying and some psychosocial and cultural variables. Based on the PRISMA method, a random-effects meta-analysis is employed in this study to obtain reliable estimates of effect sizes and examine a range of moderators (age, gender, measure method, and cultural background). Relevant studies, published from 2005 to February 30, 2021, were identified through a systematic search of the Web of Science, ScienceDirect, SpringerLink, Pubmed, EBSCO, and Wiley Online Library. Finally, 38 studies (N=38,425) met the inclusion criteria. The meta-analysis conclusion demonstrated that moral disengagement positively correlated medium intensity with cyberbullying (r=0.341). Age, gender, and cultural background had moderated the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying.

Introduction

With the development of science and technology, Internet communication technology has been continuously popularized and applied. Meanwhile, mobile phones, computers, and other network communication devices have become an indispensable part of people’s daily life. According to the 2021 Internet World Statistics, the number of global Internet users has reached 5.16 billion as of March 31, 2021. As the number of Internet users is growing, cyberbullying based on online media is also increasing year by year, and it has become a vital issue of concern worldwide (Gini et al., 2014a; Lee and Shin, 2017; Kowalski et al., 2018; Zhu et al., 2021).

Cyberbullying is typically defined as online aggression behavior, which is intentionally and repeatedly conducted in an electronic platform (e.g., email, blogs, instant messages, and text messages) against a person who cannot easily defend him or herself (Kowalski et al., 2014; Chan et al., 2021). The main forms of cyberbullying include online abuse, online intimidation, online isolation, disclosure of privacy, and online disguised identity (Menesini and Spiel, 2012; Zych et al., 2019a). The media where cyberbullying occurs are also diverse, including instant messaging, emails, web pages, chat rooms, social networking sites, digital images, and online games (Kowalski et al., 2018; Paciello et al., 2020). What’s more, recent meta-analyses indicated that the prevalence of cyberbullying among school-age children ranges from 13.99 to 57.5% (Bussey et al., 2015a; Zhu et al., 2021), while the incidence of cyberbullying among young adults range from 8 to 28% (Francisco et al., 2015; Selkie et al., 2015; Kowalski et al., 2018).

Cyberbullying has the characteristics of anonymity, virtuality, and concealment. Compared with face-to-face bullying, cyberbullying occurs in the online environment, in which victims cannot quickly recognize the identity of the cyberbullies (Gini et al., 2014a; Wang and Ngai, 2020; Zhu et al., 2021). Perpetrators of cyberbullying often perceive themselves to be anonymous (Wang and Ngai, 2020). Most cyberbullying messages are sent in the form of nicknames, generating an opportunity for cyberbullies to hide (Wang et al., 2016; Yang et al., 2018). Moreover, individuals can observe the influence of their behavior on the victim in face-to-face bullying. However, the virtual environment of cyberspace makes it impossible for cyberbullying perpetrators to have a direct way to understand the impact of their behavior on the victim (Sourander et al., 2010; Kowalski et al., 2014). For some perpetrators, the awareness that they have hurt the victim is enough to prevent further bullying. Cyberbullying is not restricted by time and space and is more likely to cause severe physical and psychological harm to individuals (Kowalski et al., 2014, 2018). Cyberbullying can lead to undesirable behaviors and health-related issues, resulting in depression, anxiety, stress, and adverse emotional problems. Moreover, it can lead to suicide problems in extreme cases (Lam and Li, 2013; Kowalski et al., 2014, 2018).

Moral Disengagement and Cyberbullying

Firstly, Bandura et al. (1996) proposed the concept of “Moral Disengagement” based on social cognition theory. It refers to helping individuals redefine their cognitive-behavioral tendencies, thus making them feel less guilt and shame to victims. This explains why people do not feel pain and self-accusation even when they commit cruel acts of harm. Moral disengagement is a cognitive mechanism, which can be divided into eight mechanisms: moral defense mechanism, euphemistic labeling mechanism, responsibility transfer mechanism, favorable comparison mechanism, responsibility dispersion mechanism, result in distortion mechanism, dehumanization mechanism, and blame attribution mechanism (Bauman, 2010; Meter and Bauman, 2016). Moral disengagement is an important cognitive basis for the generation of individual aggression (Gini et al., 2014a; Kowalski et al., 2014; Zych et al., 2019a). Several studies have shown a strong connection between moral disengagement and bullying behaviors (Gini et al., 2014a; Kowalski et al., 2014; Zych et al., 2019a; Gaffney et al., 2019). Individuals can redefine their bullying behavior through the moral disengagement mechanism. It is an effective predictor of aggression and cyberbullying behavior (Bandura et al., 1996; Pornari and Pornari, 2010; Kokkinos et al., 2016; Luo and Bussey, 2019). For example, to avoid their negative self-evaluation and shame (Bauman, 2010; Meter and Bauman, 2016), they consider that their cyberbullying actions are less harmful to the victim and the victim should be punished.

The effect of moral disengagement on traditional bullying is clear (Paciello et al., 2020; Romera et al., 2021a; Travlos et al., 2021), while the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying remains controversial (Lo Cricchio et al., 2021). Firstly, the characteristics of offline and online moral disengagement and cyberbullying are different. Individuals engaging in cyberbullying can perpetrate cyberbullying behavior 24h a day, 7days a week. During the day or night, they can create websites, send messages, or post pictures about others on the Internet at any time (Kowalski et al., 2014, 2018). Traditional bullying occurs most frequently face to face during school days (Paciello et al., 2020). Cyberbullying is not limited by time or place, as it may occur at any time and can reach the victim anywhere. Cyberbullying material is shared online. It is easy for people to share, retweet, and repeat bullying messages.

These materials are hard to remove so that the bullying can last for a long time (Wang et al., 2009; Zych et al., 2019b). Thousands of people may view insulting posts online, while only several may view bullying incidents at school (Menesini and Spiel, 2012; Paciello et al., 2020). Cyberbullying, which has a much greater potential audience than traditional bullying, has a more severe impact on victims (Martínez et al., 2019; Paciello et al., 2020). Compared to offline, individuals with lower moral levels are more likely to engage online and engage in cyberbullying (Perren and Gutzwiller-Helfenfinger, 2012; Orue and Calvete, 2019; Romera et al., 2021b).

Secondly, the influence mechanism of moral disengagement on traditional bullying and cyberbullying is different. Cyberspace is invisibility, publicity, and shareability, which does not have space and time boundaries (Wang and Ngai, 2020). In such a virtual network society, individuals can ignore the social norms and social pressures from the real world. Thus, their cyberbullying behavior is more likely to be associated with a higher level of moral disengagement (Postmes and Spears, 1998; Busching and Krahé, 2015).

The virtual online world seems to be characterized by a degree of disinhibition (Suler, 2004; Wright, 2014), which is a crucial social environment for moral disengagement (Bandura et al., 1996; Bauman, 2010; Meter and Bauman, 2016). At the same time, under the conformity and the accessibility of cyberspace, cyberbullying is increasingly being used as an emotional outlet by more and more people (Gini et al., 2018). Moreover, the social media environment might accelerate the emergence of moral disengagements, such as diffusion of responsibility, blame attribution of the victims, and result in distortion (Meter and Bauman, 2016). In this case, individuals can freely explain their behavior to defend themselves (Runions and Bak, 2015; Kowalski et al., 2018). The virtual online world lacks social norms, supervision mechanisms, and moral evaluation systems. Therefore, it is difficult for people to form a “heterogeneous morality” influenced by external norms. These aspects, in turn, can increase the likelihood of individuals engaging in cyberbullying behaviors.

Additionally, although there were some sporadic studies on the effect of moral disengagement on cyberbullying, its effect sizes were inconsistent across different studies. For example, Lazuras et al. (2019) measured the correlation coefficient between moral disengagement and cyberbullying in Greek and Italian participants, which was −0.150 and 0.35, respectively. Wang et al. (2016) calculated the correlation coefficient between moral disengagement and cyberbullying, 0.52 and 0.28 for male and female participants, respectively. The correlation coefficient between moral disengagement and cyberbullying was 0.16 and 0.47 in Meter and Bauman (2016) and Bussey et al. (2020), respectively.

Therefore, it is necessary to integrate a large number of relevant works of the literature to explore the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying.

Moderators Between Moral Disengagement and Cyberbullying

To examine the meta-analysis relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying, we also examined whether these relationships varied depending on moral disengagement measuring tools, age, gender, and cultural background.

Regarding measuring tools, different research tools may have different impacts on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. The original scale of moral disengagement was a 32-item scale developed by Bandura et al. (1996), which was used to measure the degree of moral disengagement, including eight moral disengagement mechanisms. The items were assessed using a five-point Likert Scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. This scale has been widely used in Chinese samples and has good reliability and validity (Wang et al., 2019c, 2020). Adolescent Version of Moral Disengagement Scale (MDS; Bandura et al., 1996) was used to assess the acceptance of moral exemption for harmful conduct. The scale consists of 24 items to evaluate six moral disengagement mechanisms, including moral justification, advantageous comparison, distorting consequences, displacement of responsibility, diffusion of responsibility, and attribution of blame. Pelton et al. (2004) tested the structure, reliability, and correlation of the MDS (Bandura et al., 1996) in the United States. The study found that MDS has similar factor structures, internal consistency, and demographic results in the US participants. Furthermore, the role of moral disengagement in the correlation between parenting and child behavior was examined. Gini et al. (2014b) developed Classroom Collective Moral Disengagement Scale for adolescents, which refers to shared group beliefs that morally justify negative actions. It is promising that the scale is a measure for research concerning group-level morality. Ribeaud and Eisner (2010) conducted related research on the items in the MDSs, and the results suggested a correlation between them (r=0.51). Some researchers revealed a large degree of overlap in the concept of items measured by different scales. Related research demonstrated that the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying is affected by the common method bias (Gini et al., 2014a). The effect of measuring tools on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying is uncertain. Therefore, different moral disengagement measuring scales on the relationship between them should be investigated.

Regarding age, the purpose of measuring this moderator was to explore the changes in the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying behavior across different ages. At present, some researchers have reported age differences between them, while the research participants were mainly focused on adolescents and children (Gini et al., 2014a; Kowalski et al., 2018; Zhu et al., 2021). As well, the studies on cyberbullying indicated that the rate of cyberbullying among adolescents was higher than that among children. Robson and Witenberg (2013) suggested that age had a significant predictive effect on the participation of cyberbullying. The proportion of 14–15years old students participating in cyberbullying was higher than that of 12–13years old students. Gini et al. (2014a) discovered that teenagers (12–18years old) had higher levels of cyberbullying than children (8–11years old). A longitudinal study on the Internet bullying behavior of German teenagers also revealed that cyberbullying behavior gradually increased with the growth of age (Scharkow et al., 2014). Adolescent cyberbullying was more common in high school (Álvarez-García et al., 2018; Calmaestra et al., 2020). Nevertheless, some relevant studies pointed out that there was little analysis and research on the cyberbullying behavior of adult participants, and the cyberbullying behavior of adult participants needs to be deeply explored (Chan et al., 2021). Therefore, we propose the hypothesis that age has a significant moderating effect on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. The degree of cyberbullying of adult participants between moral disengagement and cyberbullying is higher than that of adolescent participants.

In previous studies, the issue of the gender between moral disengagement and cyberbullying has always been the focus of scholars’ research, with three completely different views. Firstly, there is no connection in gender between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. They considered that cyberbullying happened in cyberspace is similar (Wang et al., 2019a), and there is no difference in gender, so the correlation between moral disengagement and aggressive behavior is not significantly different between boys and girls (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001; Martinez-Pecino and Durán, 2019; Marr and Duell, 2020). Nevertheless, some researchers consider that there are gender differences in a specific form of cyberbullying. For example, girls usually use emails or chat rooms for cyberbullying (Zych et al., 2019a), while boys often employ text messages or online games for cyberbullying (Wang et al., 2016; Romera et al., 2021a). They thought that individuals of different genders have different preferences for bullying behavior (Kowalski et al., 2018). Secondly, the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying was stronger for females than for males (Kowalski and Limber, 2007; Kowalski et al., 2014; Marcum et al., 2014). The results of the meta-analysis by Kowalski et al. (2014) suggested that gender could significantly moderate the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. Specifically, the correlation coefficient between moral disengagement and cyberbullying increases as the proportion of women in the sample increases. Girls are prone to hidden aggression. It is reported that girls’ aggression is more covert rather than overt because it uses note-sharing, “hate books,” isolation from peer groups, and various forms of anonymous call (Burnham et al., 2011; Marr and Duell, 2020). Thirdly, the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying was stronger for males than that for females (Erdur-Baker and Kavsut, 2010; Wang et al., 2016; Calmaestra et al., 2020; Gao et al., 2020). Boys show fewer moral feelings (e.g., guilt and empathy) than girls (Bussey et al., 2015b), who are a lower desire for personal relationship building, which would be associated with a greater engagement in cyberbullying. In contrast, girls, they desire positive relations with others may tend to limit their engagement in cyberbullying behaviors, even when they have higher levels of moral disengagement (Samnani et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2016). Based on the above research, it is necessary to explore further the role of gender in the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. Therefore, we hypothesize that gender has a significant moderating effect on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying.

Finally, we researched the cultural background. Cross-cultural research on cyberbullying is often reported by researchers. However, there is no comparative analysis of cultural background differences in cyberbullying globally due to the small number of previous studies and the small sample size (Kowalski et al., 2014). Some researchers suggested that cultural differences may be reflected in cyberbullying behaviors under Hofstede’s cross-cultural analysis model. Hofstede divides the cross-cultural model into five dimensions: power distance, long-term orientation index, uncertainty avoidance, masculinity or feminality, and individualism or collectivism. In the 2010 study, he added a sixth dimension: Indulgence versus Restraint. Kowalski et al. (2014) discovered that the relationship between cyberbullying and loneliness, self-esteem, and moral disengagement in the North American samples was higher than that in the European and Australian samples, considering that cyberbullying behavior has differences in individualism/collectivism between North America and other places. Zhu et al. (2021) found that cyberbullying had country differences. The incidence of cyberbullying in the United States of America is 15.5–31.4%, and the incidence in Israel is 30–45%. China has the highest incidence of cyberbullying, ranging from 6 to 46.3%. Canada has the lowest incidence of cyberbullying at 7.99%. These results are related to cultural backgrounds. Shapka and Law (2013) demonstrated that the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying among East Asian teenagers was higher than that among European teenagers. The eastern cultural background belongs to collectivism, while the western culture belongs to individualism. In collectivist cultures, people like doing things together. It is rare for people to be the first to engage in cyberbullying, and they are often not the leaders in cyberbullying incidents. Usually, when someone does this cyberbullying, people will follow suit. Thus, the number of cyberbullies has gradually increased. In collectivist cultures, the individual’s moral disengagement mechanisms are more likely to be activated, and the number of cyberbullies usually exceeds the number of victims because cyberbullies often act in groups (Kowalski et al., 2018; Paciello et al., 2020). Furthermore, group cyberbullying is more common in the collectivist culture. Cyberbullying generally happens among peer groups, rarely one-on-one (Cassidy et al., 2013; Killer et al., 2019). Therefore, we hypothesize that cultural background has a significant moderating effect on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying.

The Current Research

Gini et al. (2014a) conducted the first meta-analysis of the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. However, only four research samples of moral disengagement and cyberbullying were included, composed of only children and adolescents. Wang et al. (2014) meta-analyzed moral disengagement and cyberbullying, and only three sample data on cyberbullying were included. Kowalski et al. (2014) conducted a meta-analysis of the relationship between cyberbullying and cyberbullying victims, self-esteem, compassion, substance abuse, life satisfaction, school security, anger, loneliness, and academic achievement, with only seven data samples, revealing that the correlation coefficient between moral disengagement and cyberbullying was 0.27. The subjects of this study are mostly children and adolescents under the age of 18, making it not comprehensive enough. Moderating effect analysis was not performed owing to the limitation of small samples at that time. However, Kowalski et al. (2018) discovered differences in the prevalence of cyberbullying among different age groups, such as children, adolescents, and adults. Research on adulthood is notably lacking. More research is needed to investigate the effect of age on cyberbullies. Zhu et al. (2021) considered that cyberbullying has age, gender, and regional cultural differences, while the reasons for these differences need to be further explored.

In the past, most studies analyze various variables, such as moral disengagement, empathy, and depression on cyberbullying, resulting in an insufficient sample size for presenting the specific impact of moral disengagement on cyberbullying. Therefore, in this study, the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying is mainly analyzed, as well as adult samples in our research, to compare the differences between adult and adolescent participants in the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying.

This meta-analysis attempts to solve two major questions. (1) What is the effect size of the correlation coefficient between moral disengagement and cyberbullying? (2) Whether measuring tools, age, gender, and cultural background affect the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying? This study aims to cover the latest research and the most extensive database. To explore the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying, we searched published papers from 2005 to February 2021 on several databases. The measuring tools, gender, age, and cultural background were to be analyzed as a moderating variable.

Materials and Methods

Search Strategy

The systematic literature search strategy is based on the PRISMA statement (Moher et al., 2009; Yap and Jorm, 2015). Papers were searched in several electronic databases, including the Web of Science, ScienceDirect, SpringerLink, Pubmed, and Wiley Online Library. A wide range of search terms was provided to ensure that the included articles were comprehensive and specific. Relevant studies contained at least one keyword in the title, abstract, and/or keywords from each of the two aspects: moral disengagement and cyberbullying (see Table 1). Wildcards and logical operators were adopted to minimize the number of missed documents in database searches by ensuring that we searched the most extensive literature. Moreover, we looked at the contents of major journals in the field and manuallyexamined the citations of highly cited studies on the research issues.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Keywords of two search aspects.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

The criteria for inclusion and exclusion in this study were described as follows. (1) The research must be empirical research on moral disengagement and cyberbullying. Specific survey data should be reported, and pure theoretical research, such as literature review and field research, would be excluded. (2) The literature must report a clear sample size and the correlation coefficient r between moral disengagement and cyberbullying, or other complete data that can be converted into an effect size r. (3) The scales used in the literature must be complete and specific, and the MDS and cyberbullying scale must be reported with good reliability and validity. The details of the employed screening process are illustrated in Figure 1, in which a total of 38 studies (n=38,425) were included in our final review.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow chart diagram showing the process of study selection for inclusion in the systematic review on moral disengagement and cyberbullying.

Coding Procedures

The literature meeting the meta-analysis inclusion criteria were coded as literature information (the first author and publication time), sample size(N), effect size(r), age of participants (adolescents vs. adults), moral disengagement measurement questionnaire (Bandura original vs. Bandura revision vs. others), cultural background (Collectivism vs. Individualism), and the proportion of female participants. Among them, subjects aged <18years old and≥18years old were coded as adolescents and as adults, respectively. The cultural background was coded as the collectivism or individualism dimension according to Hofstede’s cross-cultural model (Hofstede and Minkov, 2010). China (Regarding Hofstede’s study, the score for individualism is 25. The lower the score, the greater the collectivism; the higher the score, the greater the individualism. The same as below), Arab countries (38), Turkey (37), Greece (35), South Korea (18), Spain (51), and Iran (41) are coded as Collectivism; the United States (91), Australia (90), England (89), Canada (80), Netherlands (80), Italy (76), and Germany (67) are coded as Individualism. The effect size of each independent sample in the literature was coded only once. If literature contained multiple independent samples, they were coded separately; if the effect size of boys and girls was reported independently in the literature, they were separately coded. This produced multiple independent effect sizes. The details are listed in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Study characteristics.

Sample Characteristics

Two types of sample characteristics were coded in the present study: (1) gender was coded according to the percentage of girls included in the sample; (2) age was coded for adolescents (range of mean age: 6–18years old) and adults (range of mean age: 18years old above). These were coded to examine whether the strength of the association between moral disengagement and cyberbullying varied across the participant samples.

Study Design and Outcome Characteristics

Regarding the study design and outcome characteristics, the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying was first examined in the present study. Then, the moderating variable was coded. Considering that there are fewer studies on the cultural background between moral disengagement and cyberbullying, classification in the present study was made using Hofstede’s cultural model according to collectivism and individualism. The cultural background was divided into collectivism and individualism. Previous studies have adopted various scales to examine the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. The classification of the Moral Disagreement Scale was divided into three main categories (Bandura Original, Bandura Revised, and Others). The original Bandura scale was the initial scale developed by Bandura et al. (1996), and the revised Bandura scale was developed for different countries and regions with the promotion and revision of the scale. The other scales were self-compiled by other researchers, and all the scales possessed good reliability and validity. Thus, three different categories of moral disengagement tools were employed in the present study (Bandura Original vs. Bandura Revised vs. Others). Furthermore, the age was divided into adolescent and adult. In addition to the three classified variables (age, moral disengagement tools, and cultural background), the proportion of women was adopted as a moderating variable, and gender was coded as a continuous variable.

Data Extraction

The two authors coded documents simultaneously, and the coding process was completed independently without communication. After the independent coding, two researchers compared the coding documents and cross-checked the results. The identical coding rate was 97.2. For the differences in the screening and data extraction processes between the two authors, they addressed the problem and finished the final code documents. All authors strictly adhered to the inclusion criteria guidelines.

Meta-Analytic Procedure

The meta-analysis strategy used comprehensive meta-analysis CMA 3.0 (CMA; Higgins and Green, 2005; Borenstein et al., 2013; Higgins et al., 2019). In the primary analysis, the overall effect size was represented by r to make the report clearer. Cohen (1992) concluded that the effect size r=0.10 is small, r=0.30 is medium, and r=0.50 is large. These guidelines are employed to assess the effect size of relationships reported in the meta-analysis. The Pearson correlation coefficient r is taken as the effect size of the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying because it is easy for Pearson r to explain the effect size indicator of the relationship between the variables. If Pearson r was not reported, the effect size r should be calculated by other available data in the study. CMA converted all effect sizes r (Hedges and Olkin, 1985) to calculate the combined effect size. Then, the Q statistics (statistical testing of heterogeneity) and the I2 index (representing the amount of heterogeneity) were used to evaluate the effect sizes in each study (Higgins et al., 2019). The range of I2 values was 0–100%, of which 25% represents low heterogeneity, 50% represents medium intensity heterogeneity, and 75% represents high heterogeneity (Higgins et al., 2003).

Furthermore, a moderator analysis (measuring tools, participant age, gender, and cultural background) was conducted, and the level of influence of each moderating factor was estimated in this study. All analyses adopted the random-effects model (Hedges and Vevea, 1998). The random-effects model was selected to integrate the effect size to reduce the chance of the Type I error (Borenstein et al., 2010). By convention, the criterion for statistical significance was usually set as a value of p less than 0.05 (Borenstein et al., 2009), and data on the 95% confidence interval of the effect size were given. Publication bias indicates whether the published research literature can systematically and comprehensively represent the research population in this field (Rothstein et al., 2005). In the study, the Q test was performed to test the heterogeneity of the data, and three test methods (Funnel plot, Fail-safe Number (Nfs), and Egger’s regression intercept method) were used to test whether the publication bias exists.

Results

Study Characteristics

After excluding studies according to our predefined criteria, a total of 32 articles, 38 effect sizes (N=38,425 participants) were included in the analyses (Figure 1 was a flow chart depicting reasons for article exclusions). An overview of all included studies is presented in Table 2. We have excluded some articles following the inclusion criteria, and all articles were peer-reviewed publications. The primary studies were conducted between 2005 and February 30, 2021. We could not include any earlier articles. Owing to the popularity of the Internet, the concept of cyberbullying only appeared after 2005. Most studies were performed in collectivist culture countries (k=24), and some were conducted in individualistic culture countries (k=14). The participants of the samples were adolescents (k=33) and adults (k=5). Moreover, all studies had correlational studies (k=38), with no experimental designs.

Effect Size and Homogeneity Tests

The random-effects model was used for the test of the main effects (as shown in Table 3). The overall correlation coefficient between moral disengagement and cyberbullying was 0.341 (p<0.001). According to the criteria above, the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying was a medium correlation in magnitude. Most effect sizes ranged between 0 and 0.5. As revealed from Table 2, the Q-value of the effective value of the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying reached a significant level after the heterogeneity test (p<0.001), indicating that the effect size in the meta-analysis was heterogeneous. The I2 value was 95.351 (Table 3). It suggested that the effect size of moral disengagement and cyberbullying was highly heterogeneous. Thus, a random effect model should be used for analysis in this study.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary and the moderating effect test between moral disengagement and cyberbullying.

Publication Bias

First, a funnel plot was performed to measure the publication bias of this meta-analysis, as exhibited in Figure 2. The funnel plot demonstrated that the research literature on moral disengagement and cyberbullying was uniformly and symmetrically distributed on two sides of the total effect size, and most of the research data were mainly concentrated in the middle and upper part of the funnel plot, reflecting that there was a little possibility of publication bias in this meta-analysis. However, the funnel plot was only used for the preliminary examination of publication bias from an intuitive perspective, and Rosenthal’s Classic Fail-Safe N and Egger’s regression intercept method were employed to perform a more accurate inspection. According to Rosenthal, the Fail-Safe N factor was greater than 5k+10 (k is the number of studies), indicating that the meta-analysis publication bias was effectively controlled (Rothstein et al., 2005). Egger’s regression intercept method is usually performed in a hypothesis test on whether the intercept is 0. If it is not significant, there is no publication bias (Egger et al., 1997). In this study, the Fail-Safe N coefficient (Nfs) of cyberbullying was 30,579, which was much larger than 5k+10=200, suggesting no publication bias. Meanwhile, Egger’s regression showed that the intercept value was 2.246 (p>0.05), further confirming that there was no publication bias in this study. Therefore, the published research literature included in this study can systematically and comprehensively represent the research population in this field.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The funnel plots are in this meta-analysis.

Moderator Analyses

Four moderator analyses were conducted, respectively, for the sample age group (Adults vs. Adolescents), moral disengagement measure tools (Bandura original vs. Bandura revision vs. Others), cultural background (Collectivism culture vs. Individualism culture), and gender (Proportion of women).

Before our meta-analyses, a qualitative and quantitative review of these four moderator variables was performed. The heterogeneity test results suggested that the overall effect size of the included literature was highly heterogeneous, demonstrating there had a significant moderating effect (Table 3). Regarding measuring tools, there was no significant moderating effect. The MDS types presented no moderating effect on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying (Qb=1.429, p>0.05). Given the previous analysis, the MDS was divided into three categories. However, different MDSs all exhibited a good Coefficient of Internal Consistency. Therefore, different MDSs may not have an impact on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. The participant’s age had a significant moderating effect on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying (Qb=5.532, p=0.019), with the effect size of the adult group (r=0.488) significantly higher than that of the adolescent group (r=0.308). Gender had a significant moderating effect on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying (Qb=80.330, p=0.000). As the proportion of female subjects in the study increases, the effect size increases. The cultural background had a significant moderating effect on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying (Qb=5.792, p=0.016), with the effect size of the collectivist culture (r=0.380) higher than that of the individualism culture (r=0.270).

Discussion

In this study, a meta-analysis was conducted to quantitatively summarize the empirical research on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. The results suggested that moral disengagement and cyberbullying had a positive correlation of medium intensity, implying that cyberbullying behaviors will be more frequent for individuals with higher moral disengagement.

Individuals with morally disengaged lack self-censorship are more callous and tend to engage in cyberbullying (Fang et al., 2020). The reason why some students cyberbully is just that they are bored, looking for fun, and entertaining themselves (Kyriacou and Soteriou, 2015; Ramadan, 2019). Bandura (2002) argued that the process of moral disengagement centers on redefining harmful conduct as honorable by moral justification. It focuses on the agency of action, enabling the perpetrators to minimize their role in causing harm by diffusion and displacement of responsibility, to minimize or distort the harm resulting from detrimental actions. This makes the group of cyberbullying easier to escape responsibility in a moral disengagement (Meter and Bauman, 2016). In the online world, perpetrators of cyberbullying often perceive themselves to be anonymous. Individuals will say and do things anonymously, rather than in face-to-face interactions. The anonymity of the network significantly opens up the pool of potential perpetrators of cyberbullying. Cyberbullying information is often released by nicknames person, making it impossible to identify and find cyberbullies quickly. It is easy to use the moral disengagement mechanisms to generate cyberbullying, leaving victims in a passive position and unable to effectively combat cyberbullying (Wang et al., 2016, 2020). In traditional bullying, bullying often happens face to face. When the victim is separated from the bully, it can prevent being bullied and reduce the harm caused by the bullying behavior to the victim (Bussey et al., 2015a). However, cyberbullying, which is not restricted by time and space, can happen anytime and anywhere (Lazuras et al., 2019; Hoareau et al., 2020). The pictures, videos, and text messages of cyberbullying others posted on the Internet are permanent. This information will not disappear with time or memory degradation. Moreover, it is also easier to cause the spread of cyberbullying information with the convenience and popularization of functions, such as “sharing” and “reposting.” People “sharing” and “reposting” information without discriminating between right and wrong in cyberspace are a secondary injury to victims (Selkie et al., 2015; Chan et al., 2021). When cyberbullying becomes a collective activity, everyone’s responsibilities are reduced. The disengagement of collective morality also encourages cyberbullying, causing more harm to the victims.

For the measuring tools, there was no moderating effect on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. The measurement of moral disengagement had no significant effect on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. Related research demonstrated that the differences in MDSs are caused by common method bias (Gini et al., 2014a; Kowalski et al., 2018). In recent years, with the deepening of research and the awakening of people’s moral consciousness, different MDSs have been continuously revised in the research (Gini et al., 2014a). Current MDSs generally have good content validity and structural validity, and the scales used possess good reliability and validity (Wang et al., 2014; Zhou et al., 2019). The dimensions of the MDSs adapted by researchers in different countries were similar (Kowalski et al., 2014). Consequently, there was no moderating effect on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying.

We also discovered that age has significantly moderated the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. This relationship was higher among adults than among adolescents. Compared to younger students, adults were more likely to engage in moral disengagement and cyberbullying (Gini et al., 2014a; Kowalski et al., 2018). Some researchers revealed that adults and college students, without academic pressure and parental supervision, had more access to social media and spent more time online than teenagers (Berger, 2007; Kowalski et al., 2014; Chen et al., 2016). Thus, they were more likely to be morally disengaged without supervision, resulting in more cyberbullying (Kowalski et al., 2014; Martinez-Pecino and Duran, 2019; Marr and Duell, 2020). Some researchers pointed out that since adults acquired more skills in using the Internet, they could adopt various ways to bully others on the Internet. More adults may also realize that cyberbullying is more accessible and safer than direct bullying. Therefore, they are more likely to morally disengage in the online environment and engage in cyberbullying (Chen et al., 2016). Furthermore, as they grow older, bullies begin to realize that their direct bullying behavior is not in accordance with social norms, and they need to pay a certain price or be severely punished (Kowalski et al., 2018; Chan et al., 2021). Thus, individuals tend to use indirect forms of bullying to maintain their image and avoid punishment. Cyberbullying is a kind of indirect bullying behavior (Gini et al., 2014a). Its anonymity and concealment are more conducive to the moral disengagement of elderly bullies and enhance their cyberbullying behavior (Zych et al., 2019a). With the growth of age, the number of cyberbullies increased.

Gender had a significant moderating effect on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. As the proportion of women increased, the correlation coefficient between moral disengagement and cyberbullying increased. This relationship is higher among females than among males. Females prefer indirect bullying while males tend to direct bullying (Alhajji et al., 2019). Cyberbullying is an indirect form of bullying. Cyberbullying does not require face-to-face contact, and its invisibility may attract girls’ “hidden aggression culture” (Busching and Krahé, 2015; Pereira and Matos, 2016). Particularly, their moral disengagement mechanisms are activated in cyberspace. Girls who cyberbully may hide behind a mask of anonymity. They try to intimidate those who are physically stronger than they are, or who have more advantages than they have, or who are unable to compete with them in real life, leading to cyberbullying (Kowalski et al., 2014; Calmaestra et al., 2020; Gao et al., 2020). Meanwhile, some traditional bullying victims may use the Internet to attack others in retaliation. Some cyberbullying girls whose power in the real society is weak were likely to be victims of traditional bullying (Raskauskas et al., 2010; Robson and Witenberg, 2013). Then, they vented emotions by attacking others on the Internet. The uniqueness of the Internet and the anonymity of the Internet increased the activation of the moral disengagement mechanism and protected girls’ cyberbullying behavior, resulting in more cyberbullying (Marr and Duell, 2020). Moreover, many researchers considered that women use emerging digital communication platforms more than men, making them more likely to develop unhappy relationships online. Hence, the risk of online quarrels and conflicts increases, leading to more moral disengagement and cyberbullying (Marr and Duell, 2020).

Moreover, the correlation coefficient between moral disengagement and cyberbullying under the background of collectivism was higher than that in individualism, according to Hofstede’s cross-cultural model. In a collectivist culture, the behavior of an individual was often dependent on and inseparable from the collective behavior (Wang et al., 2019b). In collective behavior, it is more conducive for individuals to make moral justification for their immoral behaviors, blur and distort their immoral behaviors, and attribute their faults to others (Allison and Bussey, 2016; Li et al., 2021). In this way, they can evade responsibility and transfer responsibility, reducing everyone’s sense of responsibility and responsible attitude toward cyberbullying (Gini et al., 2014b, 2015). In a collectivist environment, it is easy for individuals to produce moral disengagement mechanisms. When the group engages in cyberbullying, the individual may also provide help to the collective behavior under the influence of the group, such as group fighting (Allison and Bussey, 2017). The higher the possibility of moral disengagement with anonymity in a network environment, the more likely it is to increase the individual’s cyberbullying behavior (Fernández-Antelo and Cuadrado-Gordillo, 2019; Fang et al., 2020). However, individuals cannot get collective support and often have a sense of insecurity in the individualistic culture. Thus, it is not easy to activate the moral disengagement mechanism, weakening the occurrence of cyberbullying behavior (Bjärehed et al., 2019, 2021). Thus, cyberbullies tend to be fewer in individualistic cultures.

Contributions

This systematic review offered three crucial contributions. First, the effect size between moral disengagement and cyberbullying was explored. This provided a further study of cyberbullying for academic literature. Moreover, several moderators of these relationships were tested to illuminate further the effect size of the moderating variable on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. Anonymity, invisibility, and disinhibition in the network environment were more likely to be the mechanisms inducing moral disengagement and cyberbullying.

Second, cross-cultural research was performed on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying, and discovered that the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying in the collectivist cultural background was higher than that in the individualism cultural background. In the collectivist culture, the actions of individuals were often influenced by the group. The indirect network environment has induced more moral disengagement mechanisms, leading to conformity and imitation in the groups and cyberbullying behavior. Moreover, individual behavior was often hard to be affected by collective behavior in the cultural context of individualism.

Third, the influence of participants’ characteristics on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying was researched. Specifically, gender had a moderating effect on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. This filled up the blanks in past research. In the previous studies, the role of gender as a demographic variable in cyberbullying behavior was controversial. In this study, the method of female ratio was adopted to reveal that the effect size between moral disengagement and cyberbullying increases as the proportion of women increases. Women’s unique characteristics and hidden network atmosphere give birth to women’s cyberbullying behavior. Next, age had significantly moderated the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. The effect size of adults was significantly higher than that of adolescents. The main reason for this phenomenon is that adults have more time to use the Internet and master more Internet skills, allowing them to use the Internet more to engage in cyberbullying.

Limitations and Future Directions

(1) In this study, eligible meta-analysis articles were screened and included. However, some conference papers and dissertations were still not available due to copyright restrictions, causing a small amount of data to be omitted. Some papers have no direct reporting effect size. Since the method of transformation was adopted to include the effect size, there may be a certain error. Therefore, the search for the original data in the article should be expanded in future research.

(2) Some studies pointed out that longitudinal researches should be designed to examine the gender correlation between moral disengagement and cyberbullying, and gender-atypical samples cannot be included in the analysis (Navarro et al., 2016). Gender typicality has been commonly used as an indicator of participants’ conformity to gender congruent attributes and traits (Jackson et al., 2020). Gender typicality is the self-perceived similarity to other members of the same gender category that is more abstracted and synthesizes diverse information about one’s gender typing (Navarro et al., 2016). Gender-atypical is the opposite of gender typicality. For example, atypical sex boys, who prefer to be more like girls in some aspects, such as personality traits, activity preferences, academic pursuits, and occupational preferences. Future studies of cyberbullying should consider including measures of gender typicality and gender identity to more fully account for gender effects (Perry et al., 2019; Jackson and Bussey, 2020).

(3) Apart from cross-sectional study on moral disengagement and cyberbullying, longitudinal research on their relationship can also be increased. Personal experience, years of network usage, and parental rearing styles may also have an impact on moral disengagement and cyberbullying. These variables can be studied in future research. By tracking the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying, as men and women grow older, we can determine who is more aggressive in different circumstances, girls or boys (Leduc et al., 2018). Furthermore, experimental studies will be designed to verify the theory of causality with moral disengagement as the independent variable and cyberbullying as the dependent variable, so as to determine whether moral disengagement is a necessary factor of cyberbullying.

(4) At present, there are not sufficient studies to confirm the impact of the dimensionality mechanism of moral disengagement on cyberbullying, and future studies need further subdivide the relationship between specific moral disengagement mechanisms and cyberbullying. (5) Due to the limitations of the sample age range, the moderating effect across the age range of our samples in this study does not mean that cyberbullying is directly proportional to the increase of age, which requires the inclusion of older subjects and more in-depth studies in further research. (6) Most of the existing research data were mostly self-reporting methods, which may be affected by the social desirability effect (Wang et al., 2016) and increase the overall effect size. In future research, a multi-angle cyberbullying report method (including parents, friends, and teachers) should be employed (Calvete et al., 2010; Beran et al., 2012).

Conclusion

(1) There was a medium positive correlation between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. (2) The measuring tools did not have a moderating effect on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying behavior. (3) Age played a significant role in moderating the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. The effect size between moral disengagement and cyberbullying in the adult group was significantly higher than that in the adolescent group. (4) Gender played a significant role in moderating the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying. The correlation coefficient between moral disengagement and cyberbullying increased with the increase in the proportion of women in the sample. (5) Cultural background had a significant moderating effect on the relationship between moral disengagement and cyberbullying, and the correlation coefficient between moral disengagement and cyberbullying in the collectivist cultural background was higher than that in the individualism cultural background.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

LZ designed the study and wrote the protocol. JY conducted the statistical analysis and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the final manuscript. All authors made equal contributions to the final version for submission.

Funding

This research was supported by the Social Science Planning Research Project of Shandong Province, China (Grant No. 17CZLJ03), and the Postgraduate Education and Teaching Reform Research Project of Shandong Province, China (Grant No. sdyjg21200).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

*References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.

Alhajji, M., Bass, S., and Dai, T. (2019). Cyberbullying, mental health, and violence in adolescents and associations with sex and race: data from the 2015 youth risk behavior survey. Glob. Pediatr. Health 6, 1–9. doi: 10.1177/2333794X19868887

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allison, K. R., and Bussey, K. (2016). Cyber-bystanding in context: A review of the literature on witnesses’ responses to cyberbullying. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 65, 183–194. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2016.03.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Allison, K. R., and Bussey, K. (2017). Individual and collective moral influences on intervention in cyberbullying. Comput. Hum. Behav. 74, 7–15. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.04.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Almeida, A., Correia, I., Marinho, S., and Garcia, D. (2012). “Virtual but no less real: A study of cyberbullying and its relations to moral disengagement and empathy.” in Cyberbullying in the Global Playground: Research from International Perspectives. eds. Li, Q., Cross, D., and Smit, P. K. (Chichester, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell Press), 223–244.

Google Scholar

Álvarez-García, D., Núñez, J. C., García, T., and Barreiro-Collazo, A. (2018). Individual, family, and community predictors of cyber-aggression among adolescents. Eur. J. Psychol. Appl. L. 10, 79–88. doi: 10.5093/ejpalc2018a8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (2002). Selective Moral Disengagement in the Exercise of Moral Agency. J. Moral Educ. 31, 101–119. doi: 10.1080/0305724022014322

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., and Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 364–374. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.364

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bartolo, M. G., Palermiti, A. L., Servidio, R., Musso, P., and Costabile, A. (2019). Mediating processes in the relations of parental monitoring and school climate with cyberbullying: the role of moral disengagement. Eur. J. Psychol. 15, 568–594. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v15i3.1724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bauman, S. (2010). Cyberbullying in a rural intermediate school: An exploratory study. J. Early Adolesc. 30, 803–833. doi: 10.1177/0272431609350927

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beran, T. N., Rinaldi, C., Bickham, D. S., and Rich, M. (2012). Evidence for the need to support adolescents dealing with harassment and cyber-harassment: Prevalence, progression, and impact. School Psychol. Int. 33, 562–576. doi: 10.1177/0143034312446976

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, K. S. (2007). Update on bullying at school: science is forgotten? Dev. Rev. 27, 90–126. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2006.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjärehed, M., Thornberg, R., Wänström, L., and Gini, G. (2019). Individual moral disengagement and bullying among Swedish fifth graders: the role of collective moral disengagement and pro-bullying behavior within classrooms. J. Interpers. Viol. 7, 1–25. doi: 10.1177/0886260519860889

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjärehed, M., Thornberg, R., Wänström, L., and Gini, G. (2021). Moral disengagement and verbal bullying in early adolescence: a three-year longitudinal study. J. Sch. Psychol. 84, 63–73. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2020.08.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., and Rothstein, H. (2009). Effect Sizes Based on Means. Int. Meta Analysis Chap. 4, 21–32. doi: 10.1002/9780470743386.ch4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., and Rothstein, H. R. (2010). A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Met. 1, 97–111. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burnham, J. J., Wright, V. H., and Houser, R. A. (2011). Cyberbullying: emergent concerns for adolescents and challenges for school counselors. J. Sch. Counsel. 9, 1–31.

Google Scholar

Busching, R., and Krahé, B. (2015). The girls set the tone: gendered classroom norms and the development of aggression in adolescence. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 41, 659–676. doi: 10.1177/0146167215573212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bussey, K., Fitzpatrick, S., and Raman, A. (2015a). The role of moral disengagement and self-efficacy in cyberbullying. J. Sch. Viol. 14, 30–46. doi: 10.1080/15388220.2014.954045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Bussey, K., Luo, A., Fitzpatrick, S., and Allison, K. (2020). Defending victims of cyberbullying: The role of self-efficacy and moral disengagement. J. Sch. Psychol. 78, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2019.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bussey, K., Quinn, C., and Dobson, J. (2015b). The moderating role of empathic concern and perspective taking on the relationship Between moral disengagement and aggression. Merrill-Palmer Q. 61, 10–29. doi: 10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.61.1.0010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calmaestra, J., Rodríguez-Hidalgo, A. J., Mero-Delgado, O., and Solera, E. (2020). Cyberbullying in adolescents from Ecuador and Spain: prevalence and differences in gender, school year and ethnic-cultural background. Sustainability 12:4597. doi: 10.3390/su12114597

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calvete, E., Orue, I., Estevez, A., Villardon, L., and Padilla, P. (2010). Cyberbullying in adolescents: modalities and aggressors’ profile. Comput. Hum. Behav. 26, 1128–1135. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.03.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassidy, W., Faucher, C., and Jackson, M. (2013). Cyberbullying among youth: A comprehensive review of current international research and its implications and application to policy and practice. Sch. Psychol. Int. 34, 575–612. doi: 10.1177/0143034313479697

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, T. K. H., Cheung, C. M. K., and Lee, Z. W. Y. (2021). Cyberbullying on social networking sites: A literature review and future research directions. Inf. Manag. 58:103411. doi: 10.1016/j.im.2020.103411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, L., Ho, S. S., and Lwin, M. O. (2016). A meta-analysis of factors predicting cyberbullying perpetration and victimization: From the social cognitive and media effects approach. New Media Soc. 19, 1194–1213. doi: 10.1177/1461444816634037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112, 155–159. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Cuadrado-Gordillo, I., and Fernández-Antelo, I. (2019). Analysis of moral disengagement as a modulating factor in adolescents’ perception of cyberbullying. Front. Psychol. 10:1222. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., and Minder, C. E. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 315, 629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erdur-Baker, O., and Kavsut, F. (2010). Cyberbullying: a new face of peer bullying. Eur. J. Educ. Res. 12, 109–125. doi: 10.1177/1461444809341260

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Fang, J., Wang, X., Yuan, K., Wen, Z., Yu, X., and Zhang, G. (2020). Callous-unemotional traits and cyberbullying perpetration: The mediating role of moral disengagement and the moderating role of empathy. Pers. Individ. Differ. 157:109829. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.109829

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Fernández-Antelo, I., and Cuadrado-Gordillo, I. (2019). Moral disengagement as an explanatory factor of the polyivictimization of bullying and cyberbullying. Int. J. Env. Res. Pub. He. 16:2414. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16132414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Francisco, S. M., Simao, A. M. V., Ferreira, P. C., and Martins, M. J. D. D. (2015). Cyberbullying: the hidden side of college students. Comput. Hum. Behav. 43, 167–182. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.10.045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaffney, H., Farrington, D. P., Espelage, D. L., and Ttofi, M. M. (2019). Are cyberbullying intervention and prevention programs effective? A systematic and meta-analytical review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 45, 134–153. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2018.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Gao, L., Liu, J., Wang, W., Yang, J., Wang, P., and Wang, X. (2020). Moral disengagement and adolescents’ cyberbullying perpetration: student-student relationship and gender as moderators. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 116:105119. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105119

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gini, G., Pozzoli, T., and Bussey, K. (2014b). Collective moral disengagement: initial validation of a scale for adolescents. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 11, 386–395. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2013.851024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gini, G., Pozzoli, T., and Bussey, K. (2015). The role of individual and collective moral disengagement in peer aggression and bystanding: a multilevel analysis. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 43, 441–452. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9920-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gini, G., Pozzoli, T., and Hymel, S. (2014a). Moral disengagement among children and youth: a meta-analytic review of links to aggressive behavior. Aggress. Behav. 40, 56–68. doi: 10.1002/ab.21502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gini, G., Thornberg, R., and Pozzoli, T. (2018). Individual moral disengagement and bystander behavior in bullying: the role of moral distress and collective moral disengagement. Psychol. Viol. 10, 38–47. doi: 10.1037/vio0000223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedges, L. V., and Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Hedges, L. V., and Vevea, J. L. (1998). Fixed- and random-effects models in meta-analysis. Psychol. Methods 3, 486–504. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.3.4.486

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P. T., and Green, S. (2005). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Chichester, UK: Wiley Press.

Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P. T., Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M. J., et al. (2019). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Press.

Google Scholar

Higgins, J., Thompson, S., Deeks, J., and Altman, D. (2002). Statistical heterogeneity in systematic reviews of clinical trials: A critical appraisal of guidelines and practice. J. Health Serv. Res. Policy 7, 51–61. doi: 10.1258/1355819021927674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., and Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 327, 557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Hoareau, N., Bagès, C., Allaire, M., and Guerrien, A. (2020). The role of psychopathic traits and moral disengagement in cyberbullying among adolescents. Crim. Behav. Ment. Health 29, 321–331. doi: 10.1002/cbm.2135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofstede, G. J., and Minkov, M. (2010). Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind. 3rd Edn. New York: McGraw-Hill Press.

Google Scholar

Jackson, E. F., and Bussey, K. (2020). Under pressure: differentiating expectations regarding stereotypic masculine and feminine behavior. Sex Roles 83, 303–314. doi: 10.1007/s11199-019-01113-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, E. F., Bussey, K., and Trompeter, N. (2020). Over and above gender differences in cyberbullying: relationship of gender typicality to cyber victimization and perpetration in adolescents. J. Sch. Viol. 19, 623–635. doi: 10.1080/15388220.2020.1808790

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Jung, D. Y., and Park, J. H. (2020). Effect of moral disengagement on cyberbullying perpetration in middle school students and the moderating role of self-control. Fam. Environ. Res. 58, 61–74. doi: 10.6115/fer.2020.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Killer, B., Bussey, K., Hawes, D. J., and Hunt, C. (2019). A meta-analysis of the relationship between moral disengagement and bullying roles in youth. Aggress. Behav. 45, 450–462. doi: 10.1002/ab.21833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokkinos, C. M., Voulgaridou, I., and Markos, A. (2016). Personality and relational aggression: moral disengagement and friendship quality as mediators. Pers. Individ. Differ. 95, 74–79. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.02.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kowalski, R. M., Giumetti, G. W., Schroeder, A. N., and Lattanner, M. R. (2014). Bullying in the digital age: A critical review and meta-analysis of cyberbullying research among youth. Psychol. Bull. 140, 1073–1137. doi: 10.1037/a0035618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kowalski, R. M., and Limber, S. P. (2007). Electronic bullying among middle school students. J. Adolesc. Health 41, 22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.08.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kowalski, R. M., Limber, S. P., and McCord, A. (2018). A developmental approach to cyberbullying: prevalence and protective factors. Aggress. Violent Behav. 45, 20–32. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2018.02.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kyriacou, M. C., and Soteriou, G. (2015). Quality and postharvest performance of watermelon fruit in response to grafting on interspecific cucurbit rootstocks. J. Food Qual. 38, 21–29. doi: 10.1111/jfq.12124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, L. T., and Li, Y. (2013). The validation of the E-victimisation scale(E-VS) and the E-bullying scale(E-BS) for adolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 29, 3–7. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.06.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Lazuras, L., Brighi, A., Barkoukis, V., Guarini, A., Tsorbatzoudis, H., and Genta, M. L. (2019). Moral disengagement and risk prototypes in the context of adolescent cyberbullying: findings from two countries. Front. Psychol. 10:1823. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leduc, K., Conway, L., Gomez-Garibello, C., and Talwar, V. (2018). The influence of participant role, gender, and age in elementary and high-school children’s moral justifications of cyberbullying behaviors. Comput. Hum. Behav. 83, 215–220. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.01.044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, C., and Shin, N. (2017). Prevalence of cyberbullying and predictors of cyberbullying perpetration among Korean adolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 68, 352–358. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.11.047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipsey, M. W., and Wilson, D. B. (2001). The role of method in treatment effectiveness research: evidence from meta-analysis. Psychol. Methods 6:413. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.6.4.413

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Q., Luo, Y., Hao, Z., Smith, B., Guo, Y., and Tyrone, C. (2021). Risk Factors of Cyberbullying Perpetration Among School-Aged Children Across 41 Countries: A Perspective of Routine Activity Theory. Int. J. Bull. Prevent. 3, 168–180. doi: 10.1007/s42380-020-00071-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lo Cricchio, M. G., García-Poole, C., Te Brinke, L. W., Bianchi, D., and Menesini, E. (2021). Moral disengagement and cyberbullying involvement: A systematic review. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 18, 271–311. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2020.1782186

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, A., and Bussey, K. (2019). The selectivity of moral disengagement in defenders of cyberbullying: contextual moral disengagement. Comput. Hum. Behav. 93, 318–325. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.12.038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Lyu, W., and Zhang, J. (2017). The influence of childhood psychological maltreatment on mainland China college students’ cyberbullying: The mediating effect of moral disengagement and the moderating effect of moral identity. Eurasia J. Math Sci. Tech Ed. 13, 7581–7590. doi: 10.12973/ejmste/80302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcum, C. D., Higgins, G. E., Ricketts, M. L., and Wolfe, S. E. (2014). Hacking in High School: Cybercrime Perpetration by Juveniles. Deviant Behav. 35, 581–591. doi: 10.1080/01639625.2013.867721

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Marín-López, Z. I., Ortega-Ruiz, R., Monks, C. P., and Llorent, V. J. (2020). Empathy online and moral disengagement through technology as longitudinal predictors of cyberbullying victimization and perpetration. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 116:105144. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marr, K. L., and Duell, M. N. (2020). Cyberbullying and cybervictimization: does gender matter? Psychol. Rep. 0, 1–19. doi: 10.1177/0033294120916868

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez, I., Murgui, S., Garcia, O. F., and Garcia, F. (2019). Parenting in the digital era: protective and risk parenting styles for traditional bullying and cyberbullying victimization. Comput. Hum. Behav. 90, 84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinez-Pecino, R., and Durán, M. (2019). I Love You but I Cyberbully You: The Role of Hostile Sexism. J. Interpers. Viol. 34, 812–825. doi: 10.1177/0886260516645817

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menesini, E., and Spiel, C. (2012). Introduction: cyberbullying: development, consequences, risk and protective factors. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 9, 163–167. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2011.652833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Meter, D. J., and Bauman, S. (2016). Moral disengagement About cyberbullying and parental monitoring: effects on traditional bullying and victimization via cyberbullying involvement. J. Early Adolesc. 38, 303–326. doi: 10.1177/0272431616670752

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6, 1–6. doi: 10.3736/jcim20090918

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Moses, H. T. (2013). The Relationship between the Processes of Moral Disengagement and Youth Perceptions of Cyberbullying Behaviors during their Final Semester of High School. dissertation/doctor’s thesis. USA: University of Florida.

Google Scholar

Navarro, R., Larrañaga, E., and Yubero, S. (2016). Gender identity, gender-typed personality traits and school bullying: victims, bullies and bully-victims. Child Indic. Res. 9, 1–20. doi: 10.1007/s12187-015-9300-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Orue, I., and Calvete, E. (2019). Psychopathic traits and moral disengagement interact to predict bullying and cyberbullying Among adolescents. J. Interpers. Viol. 34, 2313–2332. doi: 10.1177/0886260516660302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Paciello, M., Tramontano, C., Nocentini, A., Fida, R., and Menesini, E. (2020). The role of traditional and online moral disengagement on cyberbullying: do externalizing problems make any difference? Comput. Hum. Behav. 103, 190–198. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.09.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelton, J., Gound, M., Forehand, R., and Brody, G. (2004). The moral disengagement scale: extension with an American minority sample. J. Psychopathol. Behav. 26, 31–39. doi: 10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007454.34707.a5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pereira, F., and Matos, M. (2016). Cyber-stalking victimization: what predicts fear among Portuguese adolescents? Eur. J. Crim. Policy Res. 22, 253–270. doi: 10.1007/s10610-015-9285-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Perren, S., and Gutzwiller-Helfenfinger, E. (2012). Cyberbullying and traditional bullying in adolescence: differential roles of moral disengagement, moral emotions, and moral values. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 9, 195–209. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2011.643168

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, D. G., Pauletti, R. E., and Cooper, P. J. (2019). Gender identity in childhood: A review of the literature. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 43, 289–304. doi: 10.1177/0165025418811129

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Pornari, C. D., and Pornari, C. D. (2010). Peer and cyber aggression in secondary school students: the role of moral disengagement, hostile attribution bias, and outcome expectancies. Aggress. Behav. 36, 81–94. doi: 10.1002/ab.20336

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Postmes, T., and Spears, R. (1998). Deindividuation and antinormative behavior: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 123, 238–259. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.123.3.238

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Ramadan, A. T. F. (2019). Moral disengagement and parental monitoring as predictors of cyberbullying among first-year secondary school students. Int. J. Psycho-Edu. Sci. 8, 95–103.

Google Scholar

Raskauskas, J. L., Gregory, J., Harvey, S. T., Rifshana, F., and Evans, I. M. (2010). Bullying among primary school children in New Zealand: relationships with prosocial behaviour and classroom climate. Educ. Res. 52, 1–13. doi: 10.1080/00131881003588097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ribeaud, D., and Eisner, M. (2010). Are Moral Disengagement, Neutralization Techniques, and Self-Serving Cognitive Distortions the Same? Developing a Unified Scale of Moral Neutralization of Aggression. Int. J. Confl. Violence 4, 298–315. doi: 10.1080/14616742.2010.513130

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Robson, C., and Witenberg, R. T. (2013). The influence of moral disengagement, morally based self-esteem, age, and gender on traditional bullying and cyberbullying. J. Sch. Viol. 12, 211–231. doi: 10.1080/15388220.2012.762921

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romera, E. M., Ortega-Ruiz, R., Runions, K., and Camacho, A. (2021a). Bullying perpetration, moral disengagement and need for popularity: examining reciprocal associations in adolescence. J. Youth Adolesc. 50, 2021–2035. doi: 10.1007/s10964-021-01482-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romera, E. M., Ortega-Ruiz, R., Runions, K., and Falla, D. (2021b). Moral disengagement strategies in online and offline bullying. Psychosoc. Interv. 30, 85–93. doi: 10.5093/pi2020a21

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothstein, H. R., Sutton, A. J., and Borenstein, M. (2005). “Publication bias in meta-analysis.” in In the Publication Bias in Meta-Analysis: Prevention, Assessment and Adjustments. eds. Rothstein, H. R., Sutton, A. J., and Borenstein, M. (Chichester, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd Press), 1–7.

Google Scholar

Runions, K. C., and Bak, M. (2015). Online moral disengagement, cyberbullying, and cyber-aggression. Cyberpsych. Beh. Soc. N. 18, 400–405. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0670

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samnani, A. K., Salamon, S. D., and Singh, P. (2014). Negative affect and counterproductive workplace behavior: The moderating role of moral disengagement and gender. J. Bus. Ethics 119, 235–244. doi: 10.1007/s10551-013-1635-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scharkow, M., Festl, R., and Quandt, T. (2014). Longitudinal patterns of problematic computer game use among adolescents and adults–a 2-year panel study. Addiction 109, 1910–1917. doi: 10.1111/add.12662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selkie, E. M., Kota, R., Chan, Y. F., and Moreno, M. (2015). Cyberbullying, depression, and problem alcohol use in female college students: A multisite study. Cyberpsych. Beh. Soc. N. 18, 79–86. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapka, J. D., and Law, D. M. (2013). Does one size fit all? Ethnic differences in parenting behaviors and motivations for adolescent engagement in cyberbullying. J. Youth Adolesc. 42, 723–738. doi: 10.1007/s10964-013-9928-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sourander, A., Klomek, A. B., Ikonen, M., Lindroos, J., Luntamo, T., Koskelainen, M., et al. (2010). Psychosocial risk factors associated with cyberbullying among adolescents. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 720–728. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.79

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suler, J. (2004). The Online Disinhibition Effect. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 7, 321–326. doi: 10.1089/1094931041291295

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Travlos, A. K., Tsorbatzoudis, H., Barkoukis, V., and Douma, I. (2021). The effect of moral disengagement on bullying: testing the moderating role of personal and social factors. J. Interpers Viol. 36, 2262–2281. doi: 10.1177/0886260518760012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Iannotti, R. J., and Nansel, T. R. (2009). School bullying among adolescents in the United States: physical, verbal, relational, and cyber. J. Adolesc. Health 45, 368–375. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Wang, X., Lei, L., Liu, D., and Hu, H. (2016). Moderating effects of moral reasoning and gender on the relation between moral disengagement and cyberbullying in adolescents. Pers. Individ. Differ. 98, 244–249. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Wang, L., and Ngai, S. Y. (2020). The effects of anonymity, invisibility, asynchrony, and moral disengagement on cyberbullying perpetration among school-aged children in China. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 119:105613. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105613

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Wang, X., Wang, W., Qiao, Y., Gao, L., Yang, J., and Wang, P. (2020). Parental phubbing and adolescents’ cyberbullying perpetration: A moderated mediation model of moral disengagement and online disinhibition. J. Interpers. Viol. :886260520961877. doi: 10.1177/0886260520961877 [Epub ahead preprint]

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Wang, X., Yang, J., Wang, P., and Lei, L. (2019c). Childhood maltreatment, moral disengagement, and adolescents’ cyberbullying perpetration: fathers’ and mothers’ moral disengagement as moderators. Comput. Hum. Behav. 95, 48–57. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.01.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., Yang, J., and Yang, L. (2014). A meta-analysis of the relationship between moral disengagement and aggressive behavior. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1092–1102. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.01092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Wang, X., Yang, L., Yang, J., Wang, P., and Lei, L. (2017). Trait anger and cyberbullying among young adults: A moderated mediation model of moral disengagement and moral identity. Comput. Hum. Behav. 73, 519–526. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.03.073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M. J., Yogeeswaran, K., Andrews, N. P., Hawi, D. R., and Sibley, C. G. (2019a). How common is cyberbullying Among adults? Exploring gender, ethnic, and age differences in the prevalence of cyberbullying. Cyberpsych. Beh. Soc. N. 22, 736–741. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2019.0146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Wang, X., Zhao, F., Yang, J., and Lei, L. (2019b). School climate and adolescents’ cyberbullying perpetration: A moderated mediation model of moral disengagement and friends’ moral identity. J. Interpers. Viol. 36, NP9601–NP9622. doi: 10.1177/0886260519860089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, M. F. (2014). Longitudinal investigation of the associations between adolescents’ popularity and cyber social behaviors. J. Sch. Viol. 13, 291–314. doi: 10.1080/15388220.2013.849201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Yang, X., Wang, Z., Chen, H., and Liu, D. (2018). Cyberbullying perpetration among Chinese adolescents: The role of interparental conflict, moral disengagement, and moral identity. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 86, 256–263. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yap, M. B. H., and Jorm, A. F. (2015). Parental factors associated with childhood anxiety, depression, and internalizing problems: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 175, 424–440. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.01.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Zhou, Y., Zheng, W., and Gao, X. (2019). The relationship between the big five and cyberbullying among college students: the mediating effect of moral disengagement. Curr. Psychol. 38, 1162–1173. doi: 10.1007/s12144-018-0005-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, C., Huang, S., Evans, R., and Zhang, W. (2021). Cyberbullying among adolescents and children: a comprehensive review of the global situation, risk factors, and preventive measures. Front. Public Health 9:634909. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.634909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zych, I., Farrington, D. P., and Ttofi, M. M. (2019b). Protective factors against bullying and cyberbullying: A systematic review of meta-analyses. Aggress. Violent Behav. 45, 4–19. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2018.06.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Zych, I., Gómez-Ortiz, O., Touceda, L. F., Nasaescu, E., and Llorent, V. J. (2019a). Parental moral disengagement induction as a predictor of bullying and cyberbullying: mediation by children’s moral disengagement, moral emotions, and validation of a questionnaire. Child Indic. Res. 13, 1065–1083. doi: 10.1375/acri.38.3.298

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: moral disengagement, cyberbullying, meta-analysis, moderating effect, cross-culture

Citation: Zhao L and Yu J (2021) A Meta-Analytic Review of Moral Disengagement and Cyberbullying. Front. Psychol. 12:681299. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.681299

Received: 16 March 2021; Accepted: 02 November 2021;
Published: 30 November 2021.

Edited by:

Manuel Gil-Mediavilla, Universidad Isabel I de Castilla, Spain

Reviewed by:

Juan Calmaestra, University of Cordoba, Spain
Shane Connelly, University of Oklahoma, United States

Copyright © 2021 Zhao and Yu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lijun Zhao, lijunzhao@lcu.edu.cn

These authors share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.