Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 29 July 2021
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Psychometric Analysis of a Postulated Set of Evolved Human Motives

\nRobert Aunger
Robert Aunger1*Dugald FosterDugald Foster2Val Curtis&#x;Val Curtis1
  • 1Environmental Health Group, Department of Disease Control, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London, United Kingdom
  • 2College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Exeter, United Kingdom

Many different general systems of human motives have been postulated in the psychological literature. However, as yet, no consensus on which motives should be nominated, nor how many there are, has emerged. Recently, we deduced the existence of a number of motives using a logical argument derived from evolutionary theory; that humans have evolved an independent psychological “engine” to respond to each kind of evolutionary problem set by a dimension of the human niche, or life-way. Here, we confirm the existence of 14 out of 15 of these postulated motives using factor analysis on a web-based sample of 500 respondents from the UK: Lust, Hunger, Fear, Disgust, Attract, Love, Nurture, Hoard, Create, Affiliate, Status, Justice, Curiosity, and Play. The items which loaded most strongly for each factor confirmed the expected core value of each motive. Comfort did not emerge, perhaps because it is more about satisfying specific physiological requirements than a cluster of activities linked semantically by the concept of attaining “comfort.” We believe this analysis can form the foundation of a scale for use in applied psychological work ranging from personality testing to personnel selection to public health program design.

Introduction

Understanding motivation is key to understanding our own behavior and that of others. However, psychologists have yet to agree as to the existence of separable motives (Barrett, 2017), how many there are, nor on the identity of individual motives. Many different systems of human motives have been postulated in the psychological literature, from its earliest days (James, 1890) to the present (Talevich et al., 2017; Chierchia et al., 2020; Desmet and Fokkinga, 2020; Ko et al., 2020). Scholars have used a wide variety of approaches to identify sets of motivational constructs (sometimes called by names such as ergs, drives, needs or goals). For example, Murray came up with a list of 23 “psychogenic” needs including for water, achievement, seclusion, order, and exposition as part of his theory of personality (Murray, 1938). Maslow famously postulated a hierarchy of 12 needs including affiliation, achievement and security from his experience as a primatologist (Maslow, 1943). Cattel carried out factor analysis on agglomerations of linguistic terms to identify ten motives including affiliation, nurturance and safety (Cattell, 1957). Similarly, Chulef and colleagues, Chierchia and colleages, and Talevich and colleagues used hierarchical clustering to reduce the large number goals, values and needs identified in the history of psychological literature on human motivation to a more manageable list based on semantic similarity judgments (Chulef et al., 2001; Talevich et al., 2017; Chierchia et al., 2020). Reiss and Havercamp reviewed lists generated by previous scholars to derive a set of 16 basic desires including curiosity and citizenship (Reiss and Havercamp, 1998). Some authors picked a single motive which they argued must be central to any explanation of human behavior—such as the need for achievement (McClelland et al., 1953), power (Winter, 1973), or affiliation (Schachter, 1959; Baumeister and Leary, 1995). Hybrid methods have also been used. Desmet and Fokkinga began with Maslow's hierarchical typology of needs, but also included candidates identified by several other studies that satisfied a set of five criteria for only identifying “fundamental” needs. From this list the authors used a variety of techniques, such as collapsing similar needs, to arrive at a consensual list (Desmet and Fokkinga, 2020).

Each of these approaches to the identification of human motives has shortcomings. For example, it is unlikely that English language categories mirror psychological categories precisely, nor that all kinds of motivation are expressed linguistically. Combining and selecting from the work of previous authors without strict criteria lacks rigor and postulating individual or small sets of motives fails to provide a complete picture of the domain of human motivation.

An alternative means of identifying human motives is to define them a priori based on some argument from theory. For example, Bugental recently identified a logical suite of social relationship types that must be motivated (Bugental, 2000), while Kenrick and colleagues relied on a combination of functional and evolutionary arguments to update Maslow's hierarchy of needs (Kenrick et al., 2010), producing a set of four motives including safety/security and reciprocity. However, these sets are incomplete because they are restricted to the social realm. Humans, while being the most social species on the planet, must still avoid accidents, and find food and shelter to survive, and so are motivated to achieve physical and biological goals as well as social ones.

To identify a complete and comprehensive set of distinct human motives, we deduced their existence using a logical argument derived from evolutionary theory (Aunger and Curtis, 2013). Our supposition was that the human way of life is associated with the need to satisfy a range of evolutionary goals tied to reproduction and survival. Essentially, each dimension of the human niche requires us to perform behaviors that achieve a specific kind of objective. For example, all species need to reproduce; human do this sexually, which requires individuals to seek out and mate with, partners—a motive we call Lust. However, our offspring are also dependent on long-term care to survive. Providing this care involves a pair-bond. The motive to maintain such a pair-bond we call Love.

These objectives are associated with distinct end-states that produce evolutionary benefits for those that achieve them, such as copulations and surviving offspring in the two cases just discussed. We then noted that the evolutionarily ultimate end-states of reproduction and survival can be achieved more or less directly by a particular behavior. Behavior can benefit the individual directly (in terms of actually reproducing through copulation or aiding somatic survival—as when avoiding predation). But behavior can also result in end-states that are only indirectly related to these ultimate goals, by producing improvements in the individual's physical, biological or social situation, or even more indirectly, by investing in psychological abilities to improve their situation in future. That is, individuals can engage in Creating a more supportive environment by constructing shelter (to protect from potential predation), or planting crops to be eaten later. Alternatively, a person can engage in repeated practice of a hard-to-learn skill such as hunting animal prey (which we call Play), that can benefit one's hunting efficiency subsequently. In this way, 15 kinds of end-states were identified: ranging from consumption of nutritious food, to recognition of status by a social interactant, to investing in knowledge of where fruit trees are blossoming or predators are lying in wait, and so on. We further postulated that there should be a specific kind of “mental engine” to drive the behaviors leading to the achievement of each of these objectives, since each one can involve recognition of different situations as opportunities or threats of a particular kind, as well as identification of the most likely sequence of behaviors necessary to take advantage of the opportunity or avoid the specific kind of threat present. These situations and response sequences can be quite distinct (e.g., a mating opportunity bears little resemblance to attack by a predator, and the behavioral responses are opposite). The need to quickly identify the correct response can lead to psychological specialization in the form of a modular mechanism. We call these psychological mechanisms “motives.” They are: Lust, Hunger, Comfort, Fear, Disgust, Attract, Love, Nurture, Hoard, Create, Affiliate, Status, Justice, Curiosity, and Play (see Table 1 for a brief definition of each). If the motives that we postulate each result in a unique kind of behavioral bias in response to particular kinds of stimulus, we should be able to identify these motives using standard psychometric techniques.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The evolved human motives.

In this study we therefore employed confirmatory factor analysis to identify factors related to these motives on a web-based sample of individuals from the United Kingdom. We analyse and discuss the results, and conclude by arguing that the (slightly modified) set of motives that we have thus identified should be used generally by psychologists to understand motivated human behavior.

Materials and Methods

We used confirmatory factor analysis to conduct a psychometric study of human motivation. Factor analysis is a statistical procedure which is used to identify dimensions along which datasets vary systematically. It takes a suite of statements (called “items”) and reduces them to a smaller set of factors that share a common response pattern from a population of respondents. These factors can then be interpreted (Brown, 2014). Psychometric factor analysis takes advantage of the fact that individuals are likely to vary in the weight they attach to achieving different kinds of objectives, even though we expect everyone (within the normal range of psychological functioning) to place some value on all of these outcomes (Thurstone, 1954; DeVellis, 2016). Confirmatory, rather than exploratory, factor analysis, is appropriate when attempting to verify the existence of a pre-defined set of factors.

Item Generation and Selection

The study authors used the definitions of the evolutionary function of the 15 postulated motives to generate a large number of potential items, which were then subjected to preliminary data collection and analysis. How well each item reflected the central evolutionary function of the motive was discussed, and items eliminated by consensus. An online version of the 120 selected item list was hosted by Webexperiment.com for 6 months, which generated a sample of 147 respondents. In order to broaden the demographic and geographic range of the sample, we advertised the same list via Facebook for a week, accumulating a total of 419 responses (including the original 147). In a first step, several items were removed from analysis due to significant skew. We used Maximum Likelihood analysis with Varimax rotation, as this was an exploratory phase, and restricted solutions to between 11–20 factors. No distinct factors were found for Hoard, Disgust, Comfort or Play, and those for Create and Status were not very clearly reflective of their nature. No significant cross-correlations were found.

For the second round of data collection and analysis, additional items were generated for motives for which there were too few items for reliable estimation and for motives which did not have a factor at all. This resulted in a list of 10 candidate items for each motive (see Table 1; 10 items being commonly used as a reasonable size for a stimulus set to identify a latent variable such as a factor) (Marsh et al., 1998; Brown, 2014). The additional items were produced independently and together in several new brain-storming sessions, and then agreed to a final selection by consensus, based again on how closely the item was seen to reflect the central purpose of each motive. Each item was phrased as a statement, or value attached to a statement (Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975). This might be expressed as a factual consequence of behavior (“I don't have many friends;” “I read more fiction than non-fiction”), a behavioral tendency (e.g., “I wouldn't mind getting close to the edge of a cliff;”), a liking of a certain kind of reward from behavior (“Doing the little things that are needed to make sure a child is safe and secure give me satisfaction”), or experiencing a specific kind of consequence from behavior (“[Seeing] gory images make me feel faint”). Although they do not ask respondents to report about their behavior directly, each is a good indicator of some aspect of behavior. For example, not having many friends is an indicator of not seeking out affiliations. The complete list of 150 items is listed in Appendix.

Study Transparency

The hypothesis and analytical strategy of this study were pre-registered on the Open Science Framework prior to data collection (pre-registration available from https://osf.io/hr8s6/register/5771ca429ad5a1020de2872e). Statistical scripts (in R) used in data cleaning, manipulation and analysis are available from the authors. The data themselves have been archived at https://ukdataservice.ac.uk.

Sample Size

Based on the number of model parameters (survey items), the size of expected loadings (where expectations were derived from prior studies of a similar kind), and known model size, we determined on a sample size of 500 (MacCallum et al., 1999). Ultimately our sample consisted of 510 responses due to an extra 10 participants freely provided by the online survey service.

Ethics

The study protocol was approved by the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine Ethics Committee prior to data collection (internal application number 5734).

Data Collection

Data were collected by the Qualtrics company (www.Qualtrics.com) using Qualtrics v.10/18. To maximize representativeness, we requested that individuals from their large empanelled population be recruited until age, gender and regional quotas matched proportions from recent UK census data. Respondents were tasked with completing an on-line questionnaire consisting of a study consent form, 150 items (statements), and demographic information. Respondents were paid £4.00 for completing this task. Data were collected in two rounds between September and October 2018. In the first (pilot) round, 50 responses were collected in order to check for data quality issues, and to decide on a cut-off point for survey completion times. No changes to items were made. In the second round, responses from 460 participants were collected, resulting in a total sample of 510 for final analysis. Following standard Qualtrics practice, participants with completion times under two thirds of the median completion time from the pilot round (11 mins) were dropped (for responding too fast) and replaced by an extra participant. Participants that incorrectly answered a random attention check were also replaced.

Each item consisted of a statement to which respondents provided answers using a 5-point Likert scale (“Strongly disagree,” “Somewhat disagree,” “Neither agree nor disagree,” “Somewhat agree,” “Strongly agree”). Items were presented in random order on the website, but in the same order to all respondents.

Data Analysis

All analyses were conducted using R v3.5.0 (R Core Team, 2018) and RStudio v1.1.447 (RStudio Team, 2016).

Exploratory Analyses

In order to assess whether it was reasonable to extract our predicted number of factors (15), we conducted an exploratory factor analysis of the Qualtrics data. This involved using a random subset of the data (n = 255) to estimate eigenvalues from a scree plot using the psych package in R (Revelle, 2017).

Confirmatory Analyses

We then conducted Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) to test whether a theoretically derived model of the covariance among variables provided a good fit to the data. Due to the likelihood that the factors are inter-correlated rather than orthogonal (that is, we expect there to be internal structure to the relationships among the motives), allowing non-orthogonal estimation made more sense. For the CFA analyses we therefore used the lavaan package in R (Rosseel, 2012). Our a priori factor structure was derived from our pre-registered model which included 15 factors, representing the 15 different postulated motives.

Following common practice, we compared models using both the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) and the Comparative Fit Index (CFI). Models were considered to provide a good fit to the data if they showed an RMSEA ≤ 0.08 and a CFI ≥ 0.9. After evaluating the model fit, we calculated overall consistency value (α) and cross-correlation indices.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Table 2 shows the characteristics of our sample, which displayed standard demographic characteristics consistent with the British population as a whole.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Sample characteristics.

Exploratory Factor Analysis

A scree plot analysis (see Figure 1) showed that we should be able to extract around 13 factors from simulated datasets (based on the point at which the “elbow” in the plot for the simulated and actual FA data occur) (Cattell, 1966).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scree plot analysis.

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Next, in line with the study pre-registration protocol, we ran a CFA analysis of the complete 15-factor model on the full sample of 510 respondents (see Table 3, left hand side for results). This analysis showed that all but one factor produced reasonable factor loadings. However, the overall measures of fit (i.e., the CFI and RMSEA values) for this model were not sufficiently robust (0.317 and 0.138, respectively). We therefore decided to investigate further.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. CFA model results.

14-Factor Model

We next ran a CFA with one fewer factor than the full model, as allowed by the study pre-registration, in an attempt to improve the model fit by removing the poorly-fitting factor (“Comfort”). This 14-factor model exhibited much better measures of fit (CFI = 0.736; RMSEA = 0.088; see Table 3, right hand side), especially given the large number of factors included in the estimation process.

We also looked at the correlation matrix between factors to ascertain the relationship structure among factors (see Table 4), and calculated the internal consistency (measured as coefficient alpha) for each factor (see Table 5). Overall consistency values (α) for the 10 items per factor range from 0.55 (considered to be relatively low reliability) to 0.86 (reasonably good reliability). Internal consistency is relatively low for Comfort (which didn't actually produce an interpretable factor), Hoard and Curiosity.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Factor cross-correlations.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Internal consistency values by factor.

Finally, we took the three top-loading items per factor and re-ran the CFA analysis on this reduced questionnaire (see Table 6 for a listing of the relevant items, together with the proportion of the sample that agreed with the item statement in the original 10-item analysis). As expected, this analysis showed better measures of fit than the full questionnaire (CFI = 0.866; RMSEA = 0.063).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Reduced Human Motives Scale*.

Discussion

In previous work, we postulated that 15 different motives evolved to bias human behavior toward achieving goals that helped our ancestors to survive and reproduce in our ancestral niche. Here, we sought to test our theoretical predictions empirically by exploring whether our candidate motives can be identified through dimension-reducing (psychometric) techniques. The confirmatory factor analysis suggested that 14 of our 15 hypothesized human motives are dissociable and discreet. Measures of fit (CFI, RMSEA, internal consistency) were acceptable or good.

The fifteenth motive, Comfort, could not be identified as a robust factor using this dataset. This could be because our items simply did not correctly identify the “core” issue associated with this motive (the items which loaded heavily on this factor concerned being lazy—having a lie in, staying in dressing gown all day—rather than sensitivity to pain, hot/cold, touch or loud noises). However, we think it more likely that this hypothesized motive may not be a unitary construct but, in fact, represent a variety of primitive and reflexive responses to physiological stimuli such as light, heat, acidity, wetness or pain. The fact that such perceptions require the use of different senses may mean there is no unified psychological mechanism to be picked up by a factor analysis.

Below we reflect on what the results of the analysis can tell us about each of the other postulated motives, with special reference to the top three loading items, as these figure in the reduced 42-item scale we hope others will take forward (see Table 6). We begin by outlining the motives related to somatic needs.

Hunger: Most questions about the hunger motive had high factor loadings and high degrees of agreement, with the exception of “I can go without eating for ages and not think about it.” The three highest loading items concerned the enjoyment of eating, shopping for and anticipation of meals, though caring about food, setting aside time to eat and the pleasure of eating also had high factor loadings. From an evolutionary perspective it seems uncontroversial to suggest that the hunger motive works to drive behavior that provides the immediate, or anticipated, rewards of eating. Food seeking drives (sometimes called instincts or needs) were common in earlier motive schemas—e.g., (James, 1890) and (Maslow, 1943)—but tends not to feature in more recent ones.

Fear: Only two fear-related questions had high factor loadings; these were dislike of roller coasters and being able to stand up to a threat from someone else. Unwillingness to go skydiving had moderate factor loadings. There were minor gender differences in the high loading factors and younger people scored more highly on fear (with the exception of the roller-coaster item). Most of the other questions related to imaginary events or non-specific threats that many people may not have actually experienced, such as encounters with predators. It seems that the higher loading questions may concern the unpleasant nature (negative reward) of fearful events that have actually been experienced. Fear or safety and security feature in most motives schemas (Aunger and Curtis, 2013).

Disgust: The three highest factor loading items concerned food: “I would be disgusted to find mold on some food I was eating,” “Smelling milk that has gone off makes me nauseous,” and “I always keep my kitchen free from any germs”. The fourth food-related item (“I would not eat any food that had passed its sell-by date”) also loaded highly. Though there was strong agreement about not sharing toothbrushes or not cleaning someone's infected wound, these items had lower factor loadings. Again, it seems that the most strongly loading items on this factor concerned events where participants were likely to have had direct experience of unpleasant effects of contact with disgusting stimuli, potentially being made nauseous or sick in connection with foodstuffs. This is consistent with the well-known “Garcia effect” (Garcia and Koelling, 1966), which accounts for strong food aversions based on bad experiences with food.

A set of needs are linked to the need for mortal individuals to reproduce themselves. In humans, this includes the need to solve problems associated with sexual reproduction and dependent offspring.

Lust: 61% of people agreed or strongly agreed that the sheer pleasure of sex is one of life's great rewards; 61% also agreed or strongly agreed that they hoped that they would still be having sex when they got old. These questions had the highest factor loadings, alongside liking to experiment with sexual positions. The questions with lower factor loadings were less directly concerned with the pleasures of sex and more about potentially socially tabooed activities such as one-night stands, early loss of virginity and use of pornography. The core of the lust motive seems thus to be most closely concerned with the directly rewarding nature of sexual activity. This is consistent with evolution having designed the lust motive to drive behavior that maximizes the pleasure derived from this crucial behavior.

Attract: Seven out of 10 factors loaded highly (LF > 0.6), suggesting that the analysis has captured an important dissociable psychological factor. The three highest loading questions were: “I like to dress provocatively,” “My friends would say I'm a flirt” and “I like to hang out where I might meet desirable partners,” which have close links to actually finding of a sexual partner. Other high loading items concerned more remote solutions; getting an operation to enhance one's appearance, dieting and exercise, or learning about mating strategies through self-education. Taken together, these items mention a wide range of tactics for attracting the attention and interest of potential partners. Few researchers have proposed attract as a separate motive from love and lust, though Chulef et al. suggest physical appearance is a goal (Chulef et al., 2001). But we believe this result indicates that there is an intermediate goal between immediate satisfaction of sexual cravings (Lust) and long-term pair-bonding (Love).

Love: The three highest loading items for the postulated “Love” factor concerned the pleasures of having a life partner (“I am happiest when I am with a person I love,” “I'd rather spend time with my partner than do anything else,” and “Finding your ideal life partner is the best thing that can happen to you”). Three other questions concerning valuing and investing in a partner also loaded highly. Less central were issues concerning dependability and cheating. Again, the core of this motive seems to concern the rewarding aspects of being in a loving relationship. In human evolutionary history, with highly dependent offspring, reproduction tended to be more successful with two parents, so a strong motive to invest in forming and maintaining a pair bond over a long period would have been adaptive (Rotkirch, 2018). Since the high loading items cover willingness to sacrifice for, the central importance of the pair-bond, and a variety of rewards from being in, and maintaining, such a relationship, this factor should adequately represent all the aspects of this important motivation.

Nurture: Uniquely, almost all of the items loaded highly on this factor. though the top three were “Doing the little things that are needed to make sure a child is safe and secure give me satisfaction,” “The smile of a child is one of the most beautiful things on the planet,” and “Being a parent is the most important role one can play in life.” Others concerned a willingness to defend a child under threat, despite great potential cost, and a willingness to do alloparenting just for pleasure. Questions that loaded poorly concerned caring for other relatives and the importance of a career versus having children. The results suggest that nurture is one of the most evolutionarily important motives, given that it should be tightly correlated with reproductive success, and that, given the amount of sustained investment that is required to rear a child, the rewards of nurturance must be correspondingly high. What might be missing is expression of the desire to see a child successfully reared to a (high status) adulthood, as the ultimate reward of good nurturing.

There is also a suite of motives related to human social life.

Affiliate: We proposed that those ancestors with a strong desire for gaining social acceptance would have had an adaptive advantage in the highly social human niche. The highest loading factors (which loaded negatively) were: “I spend a lot of time keeping in contact with my friends,” “I can't say I know a lot of people,” and “I prefer to work in a team,” which present a somewhat heterogeneous set of indicators of the core value, which suggest we did not identify the core value in this case. On reflection, we did not include any items that concerned the immediate rewards of social behavior (e.g., “I'm happy when I'm with a close friend”), which we suspect might have worked better at identifying the unique qualities of this motive, nor did we identify directly the benefits of working together, collaboratively, which should be central to the appeal of this motive. The items were instead mostly about feelings associated with being in groups or in some cases difficulties that might be associated with trying to maintain relationships.

Status: Our proposal from theory was that ancestors who found behaviors related to improving their social position rewarding would have been likely to have enhanced success in securing access to crucial resources. There were seven items with a factor loading above 0.5. These included “I enjoy showing off things that tell people I'm important,” “Holding a well-respected position in society is important to me,” and “Much of what I do is designed to improve my social position”. Items that loaded poorly on this factor concerned being competitive and being in charge. Whilst most of the questions clearly did load together, we provided few items about the rewards of being deferred to or socially recognized (e.g., “It's nice to be admired,” “I'm happy to be complemented when I've done good work”) which may have been more central to this motive. One question, that loaded strongly, comes close, however, by saying “People in my social group look up to me”.

Justice: There was strong agreement in our data concerning morally-related questions, and clear support in the pattern of responses for our central hypothesis that the Justice motive promotes third party punishment. This is the central mechanism underlying morality in evolutionary models and the consequent ability it confers to cooperate on a large scale, which uniquely characterizes human sociality (Fehr and Fischbacher, 2004; Jordan et al., 2016). The top three loading items were “I would scold anyone who was inconsiderate to others,” “I get angry when I see someone take advantage of others” and “I am not afraid to stand up for the right thing”. Lower loading items concerned attitudes to politics, criminality and direct revenge (“an eye for an eye”).

Other motives concern goals that improve an individual's situation with respect to the physical or biological environment.

Hoard: The items that loaded highest on this factor were “I always like to keep plenty of spare items around just in case I need them,” “I feel secure when I'm surrounded by stuff that might come in handy,” and “I'm always buying things that I don't really need”. The highest loading question corresponds closely the hypothesized purpose of the motive in driving behavior that ensured that resources were available for times of scarcity. The top two items refer to the immediate rewards of owning “stuff,” whilst the lower loading items are more distal or abstract (e.g., saving up for the future). Recent motives schemas tend not refer to “hoard” as a motive, though Nohria et al. suggested possession of resources to be a drive (Nohria et al., 2001). Starch and McDougall suggested similar constructs (McDougall, 1908; Starch, 1923).

Create: We hypothesized that ancestors who found constructing things such as tools and housing rewarding would have improved their niches, thus putting themselves and their families into a relatively good position for survival and reproduction. The highest loading factors were consistent with this supposition. For example, “I constantly make small improvements to the things I own,” “I like coming up with new inventions,” and “I would like to build my own house” loaded most strongly. There was also strong agreement and high loading for the item concerning the appreciation of good workmanship. Low loading items concerned tidying and watching plants grow. Again, the core of this factor seems to revolve around the pleasurable rewards of constructive behavior. Few recent schemas except Chuleff include constructs related to “create,” though Starch (Starch, 1923), Murray (Murray, 1938) and Maslow (Maslow, 1943) propose needs for aesthetics, beauty and order, which might be seen as an evolved appreciation for highly constructed environments.

Finally, a couple of motives describe how individuals can improve their own mental representations of the world around them or develop skills that enable them to better achieve the goals related to other motives.

Curiosity: If Curiosity is essentially about updating one's mental map of the world and storing knowledge about where opportunities and threats lie, as we postulate (Aunger and Curtis, 2013), then it makes sense that the top scoring three items on this factor concerned the direct pleasure of finding things out. These items were: “I am fascinated by going to places I haven't visited before,” “I get a lot of pleasure from discovering how things work,” and “It would be a great thrill to discover something no one has ever known before.” Closely linked, and weighted, was the claim that “I am interested in everything,” which is a somewhat more vague, and less generously rewarded, statement of the same tendency. Again, central to the curiosity motive seems to relate to directly experienced, pleasurable rewards, rather than meeting abstract and distal objectives (studying the genetics of flies, reading fiction or non-fiction).

Play: Items concerning the pleasurable rewards of experimental play behavior loaded most highly on this factor. “I love to learn new skills,” “I've always enjoyed play acting,” and “I enjoy contemplating new ideas.” The importance of having fun also loaded highly. The lower loading items concerned losing oneself in reading, sport as a major part of life and playing pranks, which appear to be more distal or abstract aspects of the play motive. There was no difference in the items by gender and little by age, though the play-acting item was agreed to by more younger people. The findings support the notion that the immediate “fun” rewards of experimental, skill-building activity reinforce playful behavior, which would have been adaptive for humans learning to live in their ancestral niches.

Relationships Between Motives

We can also look at pair-wise relationships between factors by estimating their inter-correlation (see Table 4). Nearly all inter-correlations between the factors are statistically significant, presumaby due to the large sample size; the exceptions being Lust and Disgust (p = 0.5), Lust and Nurture (p = 0.2), Fear and Disgust (p = 0.2) and Disgust and Curiosity (p = 0.9). It is obviously interesting to note that Lust does not “mix” with Disgust or Nurture (confusing Lust with either of these can certainly be counter-productive), while Disgust and Curiosity differ at a fundamental level in behavioral terms (one being avoidant, the other involving approach).

We can also look at those correlations which are absolutely large (i.e., |r| > 0.6) for indicators of interesting relationships. Play has the highest average correlation with other motives, and relationships with six others at values >0.6: Fear, Hoard, Create, Status, Justice, and Curiosity. The correlation with Create is negative, indicating a difference between practicing a skill and actually producing something. You also can't Play safely unless you are (at least somewhat) Fearful, and can't Hoard the resources needed to engage in practice, while Curiosity can help motivate Playful behavior. The significant relationship with Justice is interesting, suggesting that a concern with fairness can be associated with learning social skills through Play.

On the other hand, Status and Affiliation appear to be opposites (due to a strong negative correlation): aggressively pursuing higher status within a group can apparently work against efforts to be a “good citizen” or member of that group. Curiosity is also inconsistent with a desire to Create a better environment, perhaps because exploration distracts from the focus needed to make something here and now.

Comparison With Other Schemas

Many alternative motive schemas have been published throughout the history of psychology. Whilst these approaches have produced many similar candidate motives [indeed, those we have defined have been among the more popular ones throughout the history of study on this topic (Aunger and Curtis, 2013)], they also demonstrate considerable disagreement. Part of this lack of agreement concerns what can and cannot be classed as a motive in the first place. We have argued that motives should be seen as a set of evolved mechanisms that achieve goals over the relatively short term through action sequences guided by dopaminergic responses (Aunger and Curtis, 2015). This distinguishes motives from more ancient automated reflexes, and from more recently evolved cognitively planned objectives which require the involvement of consciousness and foreword thinking, as means of controlling the production of behavior. Hence pain avoidance, for example, is ancient and reflexive, whilst “autonomy” (Deci and Ryan, 2000) and “self-actualisation” (Maslow, 1943) are more recently evolved consciously elaborated objectives. As a consequence, we would not class them as motives. Our data support this notion, showing that more abstract and distal objectives do not load so closely to the immediately “rewarding core” of each motive.

Three previous efforts used evolutionary logic to produce lists of human motives, as we did: (Schwartz, 1992; Bernard et al., 2005; Kenrick et al., 2010). Schwartz is by far the most widely used of these schemes. He used a similar logic to ours in his original study of “universal human values” (Schwartz, 1992). He first argued that human values arise because individuals are biological organisms, engage in coordinated social interaction, and that their groups have survival and welfare needs. From these “universal human requirements,” he further deduced eight “motivational types” (prosocial, restrictive conformity, enjoyment, achievement, maturity, self-direction, security, and power), to which he suggested adding tradition, stimulation and spirituality (the last of which was not empirically supported). He then further argued that these ten values could be organized under four higher-level categories, and also arranged in a circumflex, based on possible pairings to achieve these higher goals (which suggested which values a particular value had on either side of itself).

Unfortunately, the results of these three efforts were considerably different from each other, and from ours. Table 7 compares these lists. While there is some overlap, a number of discrepancies also arise between the three typologies. These discrepancies can be largely accounted for by the different starting-points of the authors. Schwartz's intention was to develop a list of values that could be used to compare cultures and behavioral orientations across the world. Kenrick et al. began with a desire to update Maslow's hierarchy of human needs, and Bernard et al. with an a priori claim that human motives relate to five ever-expanding realms: “(a) the self-protection domain of the single system; (b) the mating domain of the dyadic system; (c) the relationship maintenance and parental care domain of the small, kin system; (d) the coalition domain of the large, nonkin system; and (e) the “memetic” domain of the large, symbolic, cultural system.” As a consequence, Bernard et al. tend to include more cultural (“memetic”) motives, while Kenrick et al. leave out the motives to improve mental abilities (Curiosity, Play), as well as Justice and Lust, because these don't appear in Maslow's triangle. Schwartz's orientation toward values rather than motives per se means that his list contains generic constructs such as “stimulation” and “achievement,” which characterize any goal-oriented activity, but simultaneously lacks specific, basic needs such as sex or love.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Four evolutionary motive typologies.

Our own starting point was to provide an account of the universal, fundamental goals any individual should exhibit—that is, we began our investigation with the desire to identify the means by which humans would need to survive and reproduce, given the features of the human niche (Aunger and Curtis, 2013). This means our list covers much of the ground of the others, including social needs, but not the needs of groups, considered as agents independent of the individuals within them (e.g., group survival is not considered a separate need, as it was by Schwartz). We believe this is a strong foundation on which to build such an important claim about human nature, because evolutionary theory is so well-supported, as the intellectual foundation of the discipline of biology, and by implication psychology, given the fruitful and robust application of evolutionary thinking to psychology already over the past 50 years or so. Certainly, deducing the set of human motives from straightforward theoretical principles should be preferable to inducing them from some select set of data (as in a linguistic corpus) or group of previous studies, as others have done.

Of course, the evolutionary orientation does not distinguish this study from the others just mentioned. Neither does the fact that we use psychometric techniques to validate our list (All of these others have done the same: Schwartz and Boehnke, 2004; Bernard and Lac, 2014; Neel et al., 2016). Using a dimension-reduction statistical technique like factor analysis can produce outcomes that are interpretable from a wide range of starting points. Rather, what we believe we have accomplished here is to have produced empirical support for the existence of a particular set of motives—a very specific choice from among the wide variety of previously postulated motives—that were chosen on the basis of their consistency with a single, theoretically strong proposition that is more general yet parsimonious than the foundations of these other studies, based as it is simply on the claim that human motivation has evolved to solve the problems set by the dimensions of the human niche.

Limitations

This study was restricted to people living in Great Britain. Obviously, it is desirable when making claims about the universal nature of human motivation to base that argument on evidence that is less WEIRD (i.e., from a Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic population) (Henrich et al., 2010). Replication with a multi-cultural sample would therefore be desirable. The limited sample size might also have constrained the ability of factor analysis to strongly identify 15 different factors, suggesting that a larger sample might identify the Comfort motive effectively (also suggested by the scree plot analysis). This possibility should also be tested.

Conclusion

In this paper, we report the results from a study designed to validate the existence of a theoretically-derived set of motives using confirmatory factor analysis. This effort was largely successful, in that 14 of the postulated 15 motives could be rigorously identified. That is, we find statistical support for the notion that there are (at least) 14 simultaneously dissociable human motives which drive behavior that would have been adaptive in our ancestral past. Lust, Attraction, Love and Nurture drive behaviors that are directly associated with reproduction. Hunger, Fear and Disgust fuel and protect the body. Create and Hoard lead individuals to improve their physical niche, whilst Affiliation, Status and Justice lead individuals to improve their social niche. Curiosity and Play motivate individuals to improve their own knowledge and skills, making them better able to profit from their physical and social niche.

Further, each identified motive had much the same meaning as originally hypothesized from theory. That is, each should direct behaviors for achieving exactly the sorts of functions that we expected. For example, Love concerns staying in close proximity to, and spending time with, the pair-bonded individual, while Hoard is about ensuring one is surrounded by all the materials required to survive and prosper in emergency situations.

Our results shed additional light on the operations of these motives. For motives where we provided items that were immediately hedonically rewarding (or punishing) these tended to form the core of the motive. This corresponds closely to a neuroscientific conception of motivation where the experience of past rewards leads to anticipatory motivational value for objects and events generated through the dopamine system (Reeve and Lee, 2019), and hence increased likelihood of repeating that behavior (i.e., reinforcement learning). We cannot say that this is true for all of the motives in this schema because we did not generate hedonically rewarding items for each motive for test. Future work should remedy this omission. However, the pattern is seen strongly in our data. It therefore seems likely that this insight can be used to reliably distinguish nominations of motives: if they aren't accompanied by relatively short-term accomplishment of states through goal-directed behavior that are associated with rewards, then they aren't likely to qualify as motives. Hence, securing a cultural legacy (as suggested by Bernard et al. as a motive) becomes unlikely, due to its requirement for long-term accomplishments, and is more appropriately thought of as a life-plan, while achieving a physiological state of comfort may be pursued so quickly and automatically that it is properly conceived as a reflex. This could be why one of our postulated motives, Comfort, did not emerge from the data. We suggest this is because the goals associated with being “comfortable” are more about satisfying specific physiological requirements than a cluster of activities linked semantically by the concept of attaining “comfort”.

We see the advance made here as consisting in the combination of a rigorous empirical demonstration of the existence of these factors as independent psychological constructs, where the factors represent a set of motives that have been deduced from a single proposition of great generality, which is itself derived from strong theory. This study has demonstrated that 14 motives are mentally constructed in a similar way by a diverse set of people from the UK. It does not prove that that these motives have an influence on behavior, or that they are the only motives which exist (as there might be others in addition to those identified here). However, this set was derived from a strong theoretical basis, and is in many senses common-sensical, as many of these motives have been put forward in previous studies, and are consistent with everyday interpretations of what is important in life. This study simply adds luster to the possibility that a relatively small set of motives can explain much of human behavior.

Having established the existence of these motives, we next examined their inter-relationships, showing that, in terms of instigating behavior, a number of motives appear to be mutually reinforcing (e.g., Status and Attractiveness; Play and Curiosity), while others are generally antagonistic (e.g., Status and Affiliation). Future work should investigate motives' relationships with other sorts of evidence, following the example of recent work in this area linking motive aspects (such as approach/avoid tendencies) and brain area activation (Schultheiss et al., 2021) or examining the perceived real-world importance of different types of motives (Ko et al., 2020). We also validated a more manageable scale (a reduced list of the three highest loading items for each of the 14 identified motives, shown in bold in Table 6), showing that it had similar psychometric qualities to the full questionnaire. We hope that this reduced scale can be used in applied psychological work ranging from personality testing to personnel selection to market testing and public health program design.

Code Availability

R code for the factor analysis is available from the authors.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: The data has been deposited at the UK Data Service (https://ukdataservice.ac.uk).

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine Ethics Committee prior to data collection (internal application number 5734). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

RA and VC conceived of the study. RA led writing of the paper, VC contributed to the writing. DF conducted all statistical analyses and contributed to the description of the data collection and analysis processes. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

I would like to acknowledge the assistance of the journal's reviewers, who helped make substantial improvements to the manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.680229/full#supplementary-material

References

Aunger, R., and Curtis, V. (2013). The anatomy of motivation: an evolutionary-ecological approach. Biol. Theory 8, 49–63. doi: 10.1007/s13752-013-0101-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aunger, R., and Curtis, V. (2015). Gaining Control: How Human Behaviour Evolved. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199688951.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F. (2017). How Emotions are Made: The Secret Life of the Brain. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

Baumeister, R. F., and Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernard, L. C., and Lac, A. (2014). Testing a multidimensional model of putative evolved human motives. Motiv. Emot. 38, 47–64. doi: 10.1007/s11031-013-9360-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernard, L. C., Mills, M., Swenson, L., and Walsh, R. P. (2005). An evolutionary theory of human motivation. Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. 131, 129–184. doi: 10.3200/MONO.131.2.129-184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. A. (2014). Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research. New York, NY: Guilford Publications.

Google Scholar

Bugental, D. (2000). Acquisition of the algorithms of social life: a domain-based approach. Psychol. Bull. 126, 187–219. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cattell, R. B. (1957). Personality and Motivation Structure and Measurement. New York, NY: World Book.

Google Scholar

Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate Behav. Res. 1, 245–276. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr0102_10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chierchia, G., Przyrembel, M., Lesemann, F. P., Bosworth, S., Snower, D., and Singer, T. (2020). Navigating motivation: a semantic and subjective atlas of 7 motives. Front. Psychol. 11:568064. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.568064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chulef, A. S., Read, S. J., and Walsh, D. A. (2001). A hierarchical taxonomy of human goals. Motiv. Emot 3, 191–232. doi: 10.1023/A:1012225223418

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inq. 11, 227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desmet, P., and Fokkinga, S. (2020). Beyond Maslow's pyramid: introducing a typology of thirteen fundamental needs for human-centered design. Multimodal Technol. Interact. 4:38. doi: 10.3390/mti4030038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVellis, R. F. (2016). Scale Development: Theory and Applications. New York, NY: SAGE Publications.

Google Scholar

Fehr, E., and Fischbacher, U. (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 63–87. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(04)00005-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fishbein, M., and Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Garcia, J., and Koelling, R. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychon. Sci. 4, 123–124. doi: 10.3758/BF03342209

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature 466:29. doi: 10.1038/466029a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. Boston: Henry Holt. doi: 10.1037/10538-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordan, J. J., Hoffman, M., Bloom, P., and Rand, D. G. (2016). Third-party punishment as a costly signal of trustworthiness. Nature 530, 473–476. doi: 10.1038/nature16981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., Neuberg, S. L., and Schaller, M. (2010). Renovating the pyramid of needs: contemporary extensions built upon ancient foundations. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 5, 292–314. doi: 10.1177/1745691610369469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ko, A., Pick, C. M., Kwon, J. Y., Barlev, M., Krems, J. A., Varnum, M. E. W., et al. (2020). Family matters: rethinking the psychology of human social motivation. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15, 173–201. doi: 10.1177/1745691619872986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacCallum, R. C., Widaman, K. F., Zhang, S., and Hong, S. (1999). Sample size in factor analysis. Psychol. Methods 4:84. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.4.1.84

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., Balla, J. R., and Grayson, D. (1998). Is more ever too much? The number of indicators per factor in confirmatory factor analysis. Multivar. Behav. Res. 33, 181–220. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3302_1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychol. Rev. 50, 370–396. doi: 10.1037/h0054346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., and Lowell, E. L. (1953). The Achievement Motive. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts. doi: 10.1037/11144-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDougall, W. (1908). An Introduction to Social Psychology. London: Methuen. doi: 10.1037/12261-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in Personality. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Neel, R., Kenrick, D. T., White, A. E., and Neuberg, S. L. (2016). Individual differences in fundamental social motives. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 110, 887–907. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nohria, N., Lawrence, P., and Wilson, E. (2001). Driven: How Human Nature Shapes Our Choices. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

R Core Team (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Reeve, J., and Lee, W. (2019). “Motivational neuroscience,” in The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation, ed R. M. Ryan (Oxford: Oxford University Press). doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190666453.013.20

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiss, S., and Havercamp, S. M. (1998). Toward a comprehensive assessment of fundamental motivation: factor structure of the Reiss Profiles. Psychol. Assess. 10:97. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.97

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Revelle, W. R. (2017). psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research. Amsterdam: Elseiver

Google Scholar

Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling and more. Version 0.5–12 (BETA). J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotkirch, A. (2018). “Evolutionary family sociology,” in Oxford Handbook of Evolution, Biology and Society (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 451–478. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190299323.013.39

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

RStudio Team (2016). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. RStudio, Inc. Boston, MA. Available online at: http://www.rstudio.com

PubMed Abstract

Schachter, S. (1959). The Psychology of Affiliation: Experimental studies of the sources of gregariousness. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Google Scholar

Schultheiss, O. C., Schwemmer, O. S., and Khalaidovski, K. (2021). Motives and laterality: exploring the links. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 7, 133–165. doi: 10.1007/s40750-021-00165-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 25, 1–65. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60281-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. H., and Boehnke, K. (2004). Evaluating the structure of human values with confirmatory factor analysis. J. Res. Pers. 38, 230–255. doi: 10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00069-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Starch, D. (1923). Principles of Advertising. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Talevich, J., Read, S., Walsh, D., Iyer, R., and Chopra, G. (2017). Toward a comprehensive taxonomy of human motives. PLoS ONE 12:e0172279. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0172279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thurstone, L.L. (1954). The Measurement of Values. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Winter, D. G. (1973). The Power Motive. New York, NY: The Free Press.

Google Scholar

Keywords: motive, motivation, evolutionary pscyhology, factor analysis, behavior determination

Citation: Aunger R, Foster D and Curtis V (2021) Psychometric Analysis of a Postulated Set of Evolved Human Motives. Front. Psychol. 12:680229. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.680229

Received: 15 March 2021; Accepted: 28 June 2021;
Published: 29 July 2021.

Edited by:

Fernando Barbosa, University of Porto, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Stephen J. Read, University of Southern California, United States
Martina Blašková, Police Academy of the Czech Republic, Czechia

Copyright © 2021 Aunger, Foster and Curtis. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Robert Aunger, Um9iZXJ0LmF1bmdlciYjeDAwMDQwO2xzaHRtLmFjLnVr

This paper is dedicated to the valued memory of VC who passed away on 19.10.2020, and who was fascinated by the problem of human motivation

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.