Introduction
Defensive behavior is defined as a behavioral response to threatening situations for survival and body safety of oneself and others (Pulkkinen, 1987; Wrangham, 2018). Defending is a prosocial behavior defined by the intent to help victimized individuals (Eisenberg and Spinrad, 2014; Lambe and Craig, 2020). Lambe and Craig (2020) proposed that defensive behavior can be conceptualized as a multidimensional behavior, suggesting that solution-focused and aggressive defending are ways in which individuals defend directly while reporting to authority and comforting are ways in which people defend indirectly supporting the victim. Together, these four behaviors reflect different ways in which individuals can defend others from victimization. Reflecting on the social value of defensive behavior, some questions come to mind and need to be answered.
Generally, prosocial behaviors are defined as intervening actions that are preceded by the direct observation or inference of another's a negative state (Dunfield and Kuhlmeier, 2013; Warneken, 2013; Dunfield, 2014; Eisenberg and Spinrad, 2014; Tomasello, 2019). Prosocial behaviors rest on the ability (i) to recognize that another is having a negative experience, (ii) to understand what an appropriate response would entail, and (iii) to act with the motivation to intervene. Dunfield (2014) claimed that within the general domain of prosocial behaviors there are three distinct types (helping, sharing, and comforting), which are responses to three negative states: instrumental need when individuals cannot complete a goal-directed action; material desire, when individuals do not have access to resources; emotional distress when individuals experience a negative emotional state. Each of these negative states can be alleviated by a different prosocial behavior, such as helping, sharing, or comforting, respectively. These prosocial behaviors rely on different social-cognitive assessments, such as goals for the helping, desires for the sharing, and emotions for the comforting. These three types of behavior show unique ages of onset, dissociable developmental trajectories, and distinct associations with individual difference factors early in life. Recent research has emphasized the multidimensional nature of prosocial behavior and has fueled the current debate on the psychological mechanisms underlying prosocial actions in early childhood (Dunfield, 2014; Paulus, 2014, 2018). According to Dunfield's model (2014), we believe that defending action can be among prosocial behaviors as a subtype for the following reasons: (a) it is triggered by the recognition that an individual is in a state of emotional distress, and (b) it is aimed to alleviate this negative state. To alleviate the emotional distress, defending action involves a specific negative action against the aggressor to protect an individual victimized. Unlike the comforting action, defending behavior implies a direct action against the aggressor as a consequence of a negative evaluation of the aggressor's antisocial action. Furthermore, there is no doubt that defensive behavior, like other prosocial behaviors, contributes to social well-being, strengthening the positive peer group experiences across development (Dirks et al., 2018).
What is the underlying motivation for defensive behaviors? What elicits this behavior? How are these behaviors triggered in the first years of life? The present article aims to answer these questions by reviewing recent evidence on defending behaviors in the second year of life.
Defending Behaviors
We think that defending requires the ability (i) to identify both another's an emotional state and the cause of the negative state (Saarni et al., 2007), (ii) to evaluate the aggressor's behavior as antisocial (Van de Vondervoort and Hamlin, 2016), and (iii) to recognize the negative behavior (defending action) toward to antisocial agent as positive (Hamlin et al., 2011).
Firstly, from the first days of life newborns demonstrate to respond to other's distress with the distress of their own (Sagi and Hoffman, 1976; Davidov et al., 2021), and within the first year of life, infants differentiate positive and negative emotions (Field et al., 1982, 1983; Walker-Andrews, 1997; Montague and Walker-Andrews, 2001; Farroni et al., 2007; Flom and Bahrick, 2007; Grossmann, 2010; Flom et al., 2018; for a review see also Ruba and Repacholi, 2020). Furthermore, several studies demonstrated that infants are significantly attuned to emotionally salient events, such as seeing someone in pain after hurting themselves (Zahn-Waxler et al., 1992). During these events, infants at 8 months will show increasingly observable nonverbal empathic responses, such as concern (e.g., Zahn-Waxler et al., 1992; Roth-Hanania et al., 2011), and later, at around 14 months, they will show more prosocial behaviors, such as comforting and helping (e.g., Warneken and Tomasello, 2007; Svetlova et al., 2010; Dunfield et al., 2011). Interestingly, Vaish et al. (2009) found that at 18 months, toddlers are more likely to engage in prosocial behaviors toward a victim, also in the absence of any distressing cues exhibited by the victim herself. Thus, at this age prosocial behaviors are elicited not only by the valence of the victim's emotional expressions but also simply by the valence of the events.
Secondly, based on some previous findings of infants' expectations, it is clear that preverbal infants possess specific socio-cognitive abilities to assess defensive behaviors. Infants showed to recognize the goals of an action performed by geometrical figures (Csibra, 2008) or human hands (Woodward, 1998), to discriminate between positive and negative interactions involving two agents (Premack and Premack, 1997), and to evaluate a puppet's helping or hindering action toward another one (Hamlin et al., 2007, 2011; Hamlin, 2013). Kanakogi et al. (2013) found that 10-month-old infants may discriminate the victim from the aggressor, showing a personal preference for the victim rather than for the aggressor. From the second year of life, toddlers can infer dominance from both agents' body sizes (Thomsen et al., 2011) and type of social interactions (Mascaro and Csibra, 2012). Taken together, these findings demonstrated an early capacity to distinguish social roles in laboratory conditions using artificial stimuli, often computer-presented animations showing a negative interaction between two agents. When infants were presented with an animation showing a triadic relation, they expected the third agent to intervene (placing himself in the middle between the victim and aggressor; Kanakogi et al., 2017) or to defend with a harmful action against the aggressor (e.g., pushing away; Geraci, 2020a). We distinguish defending behaviors from intervening behaviors only for different behavioral responses that are performed to stop aggressive interaction between two agents: the intervener stops the aggressive interaction with a less negative action against the aggressor, and the defender attacks the aggressor with a more negative action against the aggressor. The purpose of both actions is to protect the victim from the aggressor, but the behavior adopted is different. Furthermore, reflecting on the different behavioral responses of the defender and the intervener, it would seem that the defender desires to punish the aggressor more strongly (Hamlin et al., 2011; Meristo and Surian, 2014; Marshall et al., 2021).
On intervening behaviors, Kanakogi et al. (2017) investigated the developmental trajectory of perceiving and understanding protective third-party interventions in 6- and 10-month-old infants. Infants were presented with an event involving three agents: an aggressor strongly hitting a victim while a bystander watched from within an enclosure. This event concluded when the bystander interfered in that aggressive interaction by inserting itself or not inserting itself. In a subsequent reaching choice task, 6-month-old infants showed a personal preference for the interfering agent rather than the non-interfering agent. Additional experiments revealed the psychological processes underlying infants' choices: 6-month-olds understood the interfering agent want to protect the victim from the aggressor, but only 10-month-olds approved this intervention after considering the intentions of the interfering agent that were coherent with their prosocial expectations. These results revealed that 10-month-olds understand the intention behind intervening to prevent a victim from being harmed. On third-party defensive behaviors, a recent study found that 20-month-olds tend to reward and prefer those who defend a victim from an aggressor over who does not defend, and expect a bystander to approach the defender puppet rather than the non-defender (Geraci, 2020a). In Experiment 1, toddlers after seeing a puppy puppet played the role of victim, showed a preference for the defensive puppet over the non-defensive puppet, and also, they attributed the same preference to a bystander. Experiment 2 provides converging evidence in support of the first experiment, demonstrating that toddlers' expectations and evaluations of defensive behaviors are applied to victims independent of their size or age. Experiment 3 revealed that toddlers' evaluations guide awarding behaviors. These results revealed that toddlers understand and also evaluate defensive behaviors toward third parties, in line with previous findings on early social evaluations of helping behaviors (Hamlin et al., 2007; Hamlin, 2013; Van de Vondervoort and Hamlin, 2016; Surian and Franchin, 2017a), distributive behaviors (Geraci and Surian, 2011; Sloane et al., 2012; Surian and Franchin, 2017b; Ziv and Sommerville, 2017) and intervening behaviors (Kanakogi et al., 2017; Geraci, 2020b).
Other research investigates whether young children engage in defending actions. Previous studies found that preschool children defend themselves by protesting, arguing, and tattling (Dunn and Munn, 1987; Ingram and Bering, 2010) when they perceived themselves to be the victims of moral violations, such as property rights or physical harm. Also, 3-year-old children intervene in third-party moral transgressions, tattling on the transgressor, and act prosocially toward the victim (Vaish et al., 2011). In a review, Ingram (2014) claimed that during development, children tend to reduce physical aggression (direct aggression) that puts them at risk for retaliation and start to prefer tattling (indirect aggression). Very little is known about the emergence of direct aggression aimed to defend individuals victimized. Future studies are needed to investigate when infants or toddlers start to produce actively defending actions in their family toward siblings or their first peer groups.
Conclusion
At this point, it is important to differentiate defending actions from helping, sharing, and comforting actions (Dunfield, 2014), considering their different eliciting factors (i.e., goal attribution > helping; material desire > sharing; emotional distress > comforting; emotional distress + punitive motivation > defending). The recent findings can be consistent with Hoffman's theoretical account (1982, 2000), according to which social, emotional, and cognitive development is an integral part of prosocial development. The ability to represent other's emotional distress alone is not sufficient for functional behavioral responses aimed to alleviate others' negative emotional states. Furthermore, the understanding of negative emotional states and their causes could involve an intuitive, automatic, and emotion-based evaluation (Van de Vondervoort and Hamlin, 2016), before generating a motivation to defend a victim from an attacker by engaging in aggressive behaviors. In this perspective, defensive behaviors could be considered a subtype of prosocial behaviors that can be elicited by recognizing another's emotional distress in Dunfield's smodel (2014), but in this case, it is necessary to evaluate the attacker as dangerous and evil to expect a bystander to engage a defensive behavior with an active action against the aggressor (Geraci, 2020a). Finally, defense behavior strongly inspires an evolutionary explanation of the emergence of a moral sense in terms of cooperation between our ancestors for their survival (Hamlin, 2013). Future studies should investigate toddlers' active behavioral engagements in defending actions to better explain better the emergence of prosocial behaviors, the link between the toddlers' aggressive behaviors (Vaish et al., 2009), and expectations of defending actions in third-party contexts (Geraci, 2020a).
Author Contributions
All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Csibra, G. (2008). Goal attribution to inanimate agents by 6.5-month-old infants. Cognition 107, 705–717. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.08.001
Davidov, M., Paz, Y., Roth-Hanania, R., Uzefovsky, F., Orlitsky, T., Mankuta, D., et al. (2021). Caring babies: concern for others in distress during infancy. Dev. Sci. 24:e13016. doi: 10.1111/desc.13016
Dirks, M. A., Dunfield, K. A., and Recchia, H. E. (2018). “Prosocial behavior with peers: intentions, outcomes, and interpersonal adjustment,” in The Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups. (Second Edition), eds W. M. Bukowski, B. E., Laursen, and K. H. Rubin (Guilford, CT: Guilford Press), 243-264.
Dunfield, K., Kuhlmeier, V. A., O'Connell, L., and Kelley, E. (2011). Examining the diversity of prosocial behavior: helping, sharing, and comforting in infancy. Infancy 16, 227–247. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00041.x
Dunfield, K. A. (2014). A construct divided: prosocial behavior as helping, sharing, and comforting subtypes. Front. Psychol. 5:958. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00958
Dunfield, K. A., and Kuhlmeier, V. A. (2013). Classifying prosocial behavior: Children's responses to instrumental need, emotional distress, and material desire. Child Dev. 84, 1766–1776. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12075
Dunn, J., and Munn, P. (1987). Development of justification in disputes with mother and sibling. Dev. Psychol. 23:791. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.23.6.791
Eisenberg, N., and Spinrad, T. L. (2014). “Multidimensionality of prosocial behavior: rethinking the conceptualization and development of prosocial behavior,” in Prosocial Development: A Multidimensional Approach, eds L. M. Padilla-Walker and G. Carlo (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 17–39. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199964772.003.0002
Farroni, T., Menon, E., Rigato, S., and Johnson, M. (2007). The perception of facial expressions in newborns. Europ. J. Dev. Psych. 4, 2–13. doi: 10.1080/17405620601046832
Field, T., Woodson, R., Cohen, D., Greenberg, R., Garcia, R., and Collins, K. (1983). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by term and preterm neonates. Infant Behav. Dev. 6, 485–489. doi: 10.1016/s0163-6383(83)90316-8
Field, T., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., and Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expression by neonates. Science 218, 179–181. doi: 10.1126/science.7123230
Flom, R., and Bahrick, L. (2007). The development of infant discrimination of effect in multimodal and unimodal stimulation: the role of intersensory redundancy. Dev. Psych. 43, 238–252. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.238
Flom, R., Bahrick, L., and Pick, A. (2018). Infants discriminate the affective expressions of their peers: the roles of age and familiarization time. Infancy 23, 692–707. doi: 10.1111/infa.12246
Geraci, A. (2020a). How do toddlers evaluate defensive actions toward third parties? Infancy 25, 910–926. doi: 10.1111/infa.12367
Geraci, A. (2020b). How evaluation of protective third-party interventions and the relationship context interact at 21 months. Europ. J. Dev. Psych. 17, 556–577. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2019.1680360
Geraci, A., and Surian, L. (2011). The developmental roots of fairness: infants' reactions to equal and unequal distributions of resources. Dev. Sci. 14, 1012–1020. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01048.x
Grossmann, T. (2010). The development of emotion perception in face and voice during infancy. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 28, 219–236. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-3585-3_5
Hamlin, J. K. (2013). Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: evidence for an innate moral core. Curr. Direct. Psych. Sci. 22, 186–193. doi: 10.1177/0963721412470687
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., and Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature. 450, 557–559. doi: 10.1038/nature06288
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., and Mahajan, N. (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 19931–19936. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1110306108
Ingram, G. P. (2014). From hitting to tattling to gossip: an evolutionary rationale for the development of indirect aggression. Evolut. Psych. 12:147470491401200205. doi: 10.1177/147470491401200205
Ingram, G. P., and Bering, J. M. (2010). Children's tattling: The reporting of everyday norm violations in preschool settings. Child Dev. 81, 945–957. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01444.x
Kanakogi, Y., Inoue, Y., Matsuda, G., Butler, D., Hiraki, K., and Myowa-Yamakoshi, M. (2017). Preverbal infants affirm third-party interventions that protect victims from aggressors. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 1–7. doi: 10.1038/s41562-016-0037
Kanakogi, Y., Okumura, Y., Inoue, Y., Kitazaki, M., and Itakura, S. (2013). Rudimentary sympathy in preverbal infants: preference for others in distress. PLoS ONE. 8:e65292. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0065292
Lambe, L. J., and Craig, W. M. (2020). Peer defending as a multidimensional behavior: development and validation of the defending behaviors scale. J. School Psych. 78, 38–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2019.12.001
Marshall, J., Yudkin, D. A., and Crockett, M. J. (2021). Children punish third parties to satisfy both consequentialist and retributive motives. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 361–368. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-00975-9
Mascaro, O., and Csibra, G. (2012). Representation of stable social dominance relations by human infants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 6862–6867. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113194109
Meristo, M., and Surian, L. (2014). Infants distinguish antisocial actions directed towards fair and unfair agents. PLoS ONE 9:e110553. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110553
Montague, D., and Walker-Andrews, A. (2001). Peekaboo: a new look at infants' perception of emotion expressions. Dev. Psych. 37, 826–838. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.37.6.826
Paulus, M. (2014). The emergence of prosocial behavior: Why do infants and toddlers help, comfort, and share?. Child Dev. Persp. 8, 77–81. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12066
Paulus, M. (2018). The multidimensional nature of early prosocial behavior: a motivational perspective. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20, 111–116. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.09.003
Premack, D., and Premack, A. J. (1997). Infants attribute value±to the goal-directed actions of self-propelled objects. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 848–856.
Pulkkinen, L. (1987). Offensive and defensive aggression in humans: a longitudinal perspective. Aggr. Behav. 13, 197–212. doi: 10.1002/1098-2337(1987)13:4<197::AID-AB2480130404>3.0.CO;2-E
Roth-Hanania, R., Davidov, M., and Zahn-Waxler, C. (2011). Empathy development from 8 to 16 months: early signs of concern for others. Infant Behav. Dev. 34, 447–458. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.04.007
Ruba, A. L., and Repacholi, B. M. (2020). Do Preverbal Infants Understand Discrete Facial Expressions of Emotion? Emotion Review. 12, 235–250. doi: 10.1177/1754073919871098
Saarni, C., Campos, J. J., Camras, L. A., and Witherington, D. (2007). “Emotional development: Action, communication, and understanding,” in Handbook of Child Psychology, Vol. 3. doi: 10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0305
Sagi, A., and Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in the new- born. Dev. Psych. 12, 175–176. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.12.2.175
Sloane, S., Baillargeon, R., and Premack, D. (2012). Do infants have a sense of fairness? Psychol. Sci. 23, 196–204. doi: 10.1177/0956797611422072
Surian, L., and Franchin, L. (2017a). Toddlers selectively help fair agents. Front. Psych. 8:944. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00944
Surian, L., and Franchin, L. (2017b). Infants reason about deserving agents: a test with distributive actions. Child Dev. 44, 49–56. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.009
Svetlova, M., Nichols, S. R., and Brownell, C. A. (2010). Toddlers' prosocial behaviours: from instrumental to empathic to altruistic helping. Child Dev. 81, 1814–1827. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01512.x
Thomsen, L., Frankenhuis, W. E., Ingold-Smith, M., and Carey, S. (2011). Big and mighty: Preverbal infants mentally represent social dominance. Science 331, 477–480. doi: 10.1126/science.1199198
Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., and Tomasello, M. (2009). Sympathy though affective perspective taking and its relation to prosocial behaviours in toddlers. Dev. Psych. 45, 534–543. doi: 10.1037/a0014322
Vaish, A., Missana, M., and Tomasello, M. (2011). Three-year-old children intervene in third-party moral transgressions. Br. J. Dev. Psych. 29, 124–130. doi: 10.1348/026151010X532888
Van de Vondervoort, J. W., and Hamlin, J. K. (2016). Evidence for intuitive morality: preverbal infants make sociomoral evaluations. Child Dev. Persp. 10, 143–148. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12175
Walker-Andrews, A. (1997). Infants' perception of expressive behaviors: differentiation of multimodal information. Psychol. Bull. 121, 437–456. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.121.3.43
Warneken, F. (2013). Young children proactively remedy unnoticed accidents. Cognition 126, 101–108. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.09.011
Warneken, F., and Tomasello, M. (2007). Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age. Infancy 11, 271–294. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00227.x
Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition 69, 1–34. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4
Wrangham, R. W. (2018). Two types of aggression in human evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, 245–253. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1713611115
Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., and Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Dev. Psych. 28, 126–136. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.126
Keywords: prosociality, emotional distress, evaluation, sociomoral development, defensive behaviors
Citation: Geraci A and Franchin L (2021) Is Defensive Behavior a Subtype of Prosocial Behaviors? Front. Psychol. 12:678370. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.678370
Received: 09 March 2021; Accepted: 19 May 2021;
Published: 23 June 2021.
Edited by:
Klaus Libertus, University of Pittsburgh, United StatesReviewed by:
Yasuhiro Kanakogi, Kyoto University, JapanKristen Ann Dunfield, Concordia University, Canada
Copyright © 2021 Geraci and Franchin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Alessandra Geraci, alessandra.geraci@unitn.it