Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 28 May 2021
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

The Indirect Effect of Death Anxiety on Experienced Meaning in Life via Search for Meaning and Prosocial Behavior

  • 1Department of Psychology, Faculty of Education, Guangxi Normal University, Guilin, China
  • 2Department of Neuroscience, Faculty of Medicine, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden

This study investigated the relationship between death anxiety and experienced meaning in life. Six hundred and forty-eight Chinese college students were surveyed using the Death Anxiety Scale, the Prosocial Behavior Scale, and the Meaning in Life Scale. The results showed that death anxiety predicted experienced meaning through three pathways: the first one was through search for meaning singly; the second one was through prosocial behavior singly; and the third one was through search for meaning and prosocial behavior serially, which accounted for the highest proportion of the total effect. This study highlights the positive side of death anxiety.

Introduction

According to the terror management theory, humans can be aware of their own vulnerability and inevitability of death due to their advanced cognitive abilities, which produces overwhelming anxiety and fear (Greenberg et al., 1986). Given natural and manmade disasters are unavoidable in human life, in current age with highly developed network media, news and information related to various prominent deaths can be seen everywhere at any time (Bovero et al., 2020). However, death salience is not always negative, and emergency events can often bring more prosocial behaviors (Lowe and Fothergill, 2003; Bauer et al., 2016). For example, earthquake victims are more likely to participate in voluntary activities, such as blood donation and rescue (Wang and Wu, 2020). What makes researchers curious is why individuals engage in prosocial behavior in the face of death threat despite its emotional burden. In this study, it is believed that death anxiety will lead people to have a strong motivation to seek meaning in life. Because prosocial behavior meets the need for meaning, people are more willing to engage in prosocial behavior, after which they will experience higher sense of meaning in life. In other words, there is a serial indirect effect: Death anxiety leads to higher motivation to seek meaning in life, which leads to more prosocial behaviors and finally higher experienced sense of meaning.

First of all, death anxiety can promote people to seek meaning in life. The shattered assumption proposes that death crushes people's basic assumptions about the world and the self: The world is good, safe, kind, and meaningful (Janoff-bulman, 1992). Instead, death reminds people that the world is unordered, unsafe, unpredictable, and uncontrollable. Therefore, death salience makes people experience the feelings of loss and damage of meaning in life (Burke et al., 2010). According to the deficit correcting hypothesis, the motivation to search for meaning in life stems from the lack of the presence of meaning (Baumeister, 1991). As a result, when facing life threats, individuals have the psychological needs to find the meaning in life (Baumeister, 1991; Juhl and Routledge, 2014). This idea is supported by empirical evidence. For example, based on a series of studies about rape victims, breast cancer patients, and people infected with HIV, Taylor (1983) pointed out that search for meaning is often the first step in the process of adaptation to life-threatening events. Davis and colleagues found that 90% of bereaved college students were immersed in thinking about death and repeatedly searching for the cause of the event (Davis et al., 1995). Two months after 9/11, two-thirds of surveyed American adults reported that they were searching for meaning in life (Updegraff et al., 2008). Laboratory studies also showed that under the condition of mortality salience, individuals tended to search for meaning in life by defending their cultural worldview (Hayes et al., 2010; Pyszczynski et al., 2010).

Second, prosocial behavior is an important source of meaning in life, which can be influenced by various contextual (e.g., rules, norms, and social network, for a recent review, see Simpson and Willer, 2015), emotional (e.g., parental warmth and support, for a recent review, see Spinrad and Gal, 2018), and cognitive factors [e.g., theory of mind, for a recent meta-analysis, see Imuta et al. (2016)]. On the one hand, according to self-determination theory (Deci and Ryan, 2000), prosocial behavior can increase the sense of meaning in life by satisfying the basic psychological needs. For example, a person can decide to participate in volunteer activities autonomously and realize he/she has the ability to help others and establish relationships with others (Baumeister and Vohs, 2002; Hoffman et al., 2020). On the other hand, because cultural worldview is a fundamental source of meaning and prosocial behavior is generally praised and encouraged by most cultural worldviews, it is natural to expect that prosocial behavior can bring about a sense of meaning for people (Klein, 2017). Consistent with this, Van Tongeren and colleagues found that prosocial behavior had a strong predictive effect on the sense of meaning no matter prosocial behavior was measured by questionnaires or manipulated in experiments (Van Tongeren et al., 2016). Results from the experience sampling method also provides supporting evidence that prosocial behavior can enhance people's experienced sense of meaning (Hofmann et al., 2014). Therefore, prosocial behavior is a major source of obtaining meaning in life (Wong, 1998; Van Tongeren et al., 2016).

Therefore, based on past experimental work that death reminders inspire search for meaning and nonexperimental findings that engaging in prosocial behavior is a robust source of meaning, we predict that death anxiety will be associated with prosocial behavior through search for meaning and prosocial behavior will in turn corresponds with higher sense of meaning.

Methods

Participants and Procedures

The study was reviewed and approved by the Department of Psychology, Faculty of Education, Guangxi Normal University. Participants provided their informed consent to participate in this study. Standard biosecurity and institutional safety procedures have been adhered to.

In total, 680 college students were randomly selected for an online survey, in which 32 were excluded due to invalid responses, yielding an efficiency rate of 95.29%. Among the 648 remaining participants, 329 were males and 319 were females. The mean age was 22.26 years (SD = 4.10). Three hundred and fifty-five participants were from the rural area and 293 were from the urban area.

Measures

Death Anxiety

The Chinese version of the Death Anxiety Scale (Templer, 1970) was used to measure death anxiety (e.g., “I am very afraid of death,” “Seeing the dead body makes my hair stand on end”). The original scale consists of 15 items, of which six is reverse scored. Because reverse-scored items often elicit less accurate responses and unintended consequences for the scale structure (Kulas et al., 2019), we only selected the nine forward-scored items. Participants responded using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). The items showed a single-factor structure and yielded high internal consistency (Cronbach'α = 0.82). The mean score was adopted, with higher scores indicating higher death anxiety.

Prosocial Behavior

The 15-item Prosocial Behavior Scale (Yang et al., 2016) was used to measure prosocial behavior (e.g., “I would like to do something for the class,” “I would like to donate money and materials to the disaster area”). This scale used a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree). The mean score was adopted, with higher scores indicating higher prosocial behavior. The Cronbach's α of the scale in this study was 0.94.

Meaning in Life

The Chinese version of the Meaning in Life Scale (Steger et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2016) was adopted. Five items were used to measure experienced meaning (e.g., “I have found a purpose for my life to satisfy myself”), and the other five to measure search for meaning (e.g., “I am searching for meaning in my life.”). Participants responded using a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree). In this study, Cronbach's α of the experienced meaning scale was 0.72, and Cronbach's α of the search for meaning scale was 0.88.

Subjective Socioeconomic Status

Participants were shown a ladder indicating different levels of socioeconomic status (1 = the lowest level; 10 = the highest level). They were asked to indicate on which level they stand.

Data Analyses

SPSS 25.0 software was used for correlation analysis. PROCESS 3.3 (Model 6) was used to conduct the analysis for the serial indirect effect.

Results

Correlation Analysis

The results of correlation analysis are shown in Table 1. Death anxiety, search for meaning, prosocial behavior, and presence of meaning were significantly correlated with each other. Gender and subjective socioeconomic status were significantly correlated with experienced meaning, therefore it is necessary to control these two demographic variables in subsequent test of the indirect effect.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and Pearson's correlations for study variables.

Serial Indirect Effect

There were significant correlations among death anxiety, search for meaning, prosocial behavior, and experienced meaning, which met the statistical requirements for further analysis of the indirect effect of search for meaning and prosocial behavior. After controlling for gender and subjective socioeconomic status, we analyzed the indirect effect of search for meaning and prosocial behavior on the relationship between death anxiety and experienced meaning. The regression analysis results (see Table 2) showed that death anxiety had a significant positive predictive effect on experienced meaning (β = 0.207, SE = 0.037, 95% CI [0.133, 0.280]). When death anxiety, search for meaning, and prosocial behavior were included into the regression equation together, the predictive effect of death anxiety on experienced meaning became insignificant (β = 0.003, SE = 0.033, 95% CI [−0.062, 0.068]). Death anxiety directly predicted search for meaning (β = 0.354, SE = 0.037, 95% CI [0.283, 0.426]) and prosocial behavior (β = 0.079, SE = 0.029, 95% CI [0.021, 0.136]). Search for meaning positively predicted prosocial behavior (β = 0.670, SE = 0.030, 95% CI [0.612, 0.728]). Both search for meaning (β = 0.154, SE = 0.044, 95% CI [0.067, 0.242]) and prosocial behavior (β = 0.472, SE = 0.044, 95% CI [0.385, 0.558]) positively predicted experienced meaning.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Regression analysis of variables in the model.

The indirect effect analysis showed that search for meaning and prosocial behavior fully accounted for the relationship between death anxiety and experienced meaning, with a total indirect effect value of 0.204. Specifically, the indirect effect was composed of three indirect paths: the indirect effect 1 (Effect = 0.055) from the path of death anxiety → search for meaning → experienced meaning; the indirect effect 2 (Effect = 0.037) from the path of death anxiety → prosocial behavior → experienced meaning; and the indirect Effect 3 (Effect = 0.112) from the path of death anxiety → search for meaning → prosocial behavior → experienced meaning. As shown in Table 3, the three indirect effects accounted for 26.58%, 17.88%, and 54.13% of the total effect, respectively. The bootstrap 95% confidence intervals of the above indirect effects did not contain zero, indicating that the three indirect effects all reached the significant level. There were also significant differences between these three indirect effects. The bootstrap 95% confidence intervals of the difference between the indirect effect 1 and the indirect effect 2 (comparison 1) and the difference between the indirect effect 1 and the indirect effect 3 (comparison 2) contained zero, indicating that there was neither significant difference between the indirect effect 1 and the indirect effect 2, nor between the indirect effect 1 and the indirect effect 3. The bootstrap 95% confidence interval of the difference between the indirect effect 2 and the indirect effect 3 (comparison 3) did not contain zero, indicating that the difference between the indirect effect 2 and the indirect effect 3 was significant. The detailed path model is shown in Figure 1.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Indirect effect analysis of search for meaning and prosocial behavior.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Path diagram of the serial indirect effect. **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05.

Discussion

This study examined the relationship between death anxiety and experienced meaning in life and explored its potential mechanisms. Indirect effect analysis showed that search for meaning and prosocial behavior fully accounted for the relationship between death anxiety and experienced meaning. Furthermore, the indirect effect included three paths: The indirect effect of search for meaning, the indirect effect of prosocial behavior, as well as the serial indirect effect of search for meaning and prosocial behavior. This study provided a preliminary answer to the question about how people maintain presence of meaning when they experience death anxiety and why death anxiety increases prosocial behavior.

Indirect Effects

Death anxiety can predict experienced meaning through search for meaning singly. Steger and colleagues suggested two paths regarding the sequential relationship between search for meaning and experienced meaning: search to presence and the presence to search (Steger et al., 2008). The results of this study are more in line with the search to presence path, namely, heightened motivation to search for meaning leads to higher sense of experienced meaning. A recent longitudinal study also found similar results, such that search for meaning predicted within-person increases in experienced meaning at a later time (Newman et al., 2018).

Death anxiety can also predict experienced meaning through prosocial behavior singly, which is consistent with the terror management theory. That is, in order to alleviate death anxiety, people need to get a sense of value in the cultural context to which they belong and think of themselves as a meaningful and valuable member of the culture (Rosenblatt et al., 1989). Given prosocial behavior is generally valued by most cultures, people are prone to exhibiting prosocial behavior because it can be considered as a way of supporting one's own cultural worldview. Accordingly, ample studies have demonstrated death anxiety could increase prosocial behavior. For example, laboratory studies showed that launching mortality salience promoted charitable behavior (Joireman and Duell, 2007; Chen et al., 2020). Field studies found that participants passing through funeral parlors had a more positive attitude toward charitable organizations (Jonas et al., 2002).

Importantly, the current study found that search for meaning and prosocial behavior serially accounted for the link between death anxiety and experienced meaning. In the first step of this process, death anxiety positively predicts search for meaning, which is in line with the meaning maintenance model (Heine et al., 2006). That is, human beings are creatures that create meaning and seek meaning, and the pursuit of meaning in life is the best way to alleviate the fear of death (Zhang et al., 2019). Because death destroys the possibility of achieving the basic goals of life and the overall belief about meaning in life (Barnett et al., 2018), after a traumatic event involving death threat people tend to reevaluate the event and construct a new meaning (Park and George, 2013). There are many ways to repair the damage of meaning caused by death anxiety. Prosocial behavior is a prominent way of meaning repairing because it is associated with increased sense of meaning in life, which is referred to as the “lingering aroma effect” of prosocial behavior (Van Tongeren et al., 2016; Klein, 2017). Therefore, through search for meaning, death anxiety predicts prosocial behavior, which in turn is associated with higher experienced meaning.

Limitation and Prospect

Martin Heidegger, in his book Being and Time, puts forward the countdown method of the meaning in life for people to face the issue of unavoidable death: To live toward death. That is, since the day of birth, people are approaching death every minute and every second (Heidegger et al., 2010). This study verified the positive relationship between death anxiety and experienced meaning through a cross-sectional method and provided evidence for the path of death anxiety → search for meaning → prosocial behavior → experienced meaning, which sheds light on how to realize “to live toward death.” Although it adds to the current literature, there are still some limitations. First, the research method is relatively simple. In order to better explore the causal relationship between these variables, future studies can employ the longitudinal study method and/or manipulate death anxiety (e.g., increasing mortality salience) in the lab. For example, the classic open-ended short-answer questions or the pictural and audio materials related to death can be used to launch mortality salience (Burke et al., 2010). Prosocial behavior can also be measured by other methods such as the dictator paradigm or the on-site donation paradigm. Second, some studies found that death anxiety could lead to in-group preference and out-group derogation (Rosenblatt et al., 1989). Therefore, future studies can explore whether there are differences in individuals' prosocial behavior toward in-group and out-group when they experience death anxiety.

Conclusion

This study adopts the questionnaire survey method to investigate the influence of death anxiety on experienced meaning and its underlying psychological mechanisms. It draws the following conclusions: (1) death anxiety predicts experienced meaning via search for meaning; (2) death anxiety predicts experienced meaning via prosocial behavior; and (3) death anxiety positively predicts experienced meaning through a serial indirect effect of search for meaning and prosocial behavior.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Department of psychology, Faculty of Education, Guangxi Normal University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

BC developed the study concept. Testing and data collection were performed by BC, JC, and JF. BC, JC, and JD performed the data analysis and drafted the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Funding

BC was supported by the General Project of Humanities and Social Science Research of the Ministry of Education (20XJA190001), Guangxi philosophy and social science planning research project (20FSH005), and project of Guangxi Ethnic Education Development Research Center (2020MJYB004). JD was supported by the Swedish Research Council (2018-06664).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.673460/full#supplementary-material

References

Barnett, M. D., Anderson, E. A., and Marsden, A. D. (2018). Is death anxiety more closely linked with optimism or pessimism among older adults? Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 77, 169–173. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2018.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauer, M., Blattman, C., Chytilova, J., Henrich, J., Miguel, E., and Mitts, T. (2016). Can war foster cooperation? J. Econ. Perspect. 30, 249–274. doi: 10.1257/jep.30.3.249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Meanings of Life. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Vohs, K. D. (2002). The pursuit of meaningfulness in life. Handbook Posit. Psychol. 1, 608–618.

Google Scholar

Bovero, A., Tosi, C., Botto, R., Fonti, I., and Torta, R. (2020). Death and dying on the social network: an Italian survey. J. Social Work End-of-Life Palliat. Care 16, 266–285. doi: 10.1080/15524256.2020.1800552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, B. L., Martens, A., and Faucher, E. H. (2010). Two decades of terror management theory: a meta-analysis of mortality salience research. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 14, 155–195. doi: 10.1177/1088868309352321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, G., Crossland, C., and Huang, S. (2020). That could have been me: director deaths, CEO mortality salience, and corporate prosocial behavior. Manage. Sci. 66, 3142–3161. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.2019.3348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, C. G., Lehman, D. R., Wortman, C. B., Silver, R. C., and Thompson, S. C. (1995). The undoing of traumatic life events. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 21, 109–124. doi: 10.1177/0146167295212002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inquiry 11, 227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., and Solomon, S. (1986). “The causes and consequences of a need for self-esteem: a terror management theory,” In: Public Self and Private Self , ed R. F. Baumeister (New York, NY: Springer), 189–212. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4613-9564-5_10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, J., Schimel, J., Arndt, J., and Faucher, E. H. (2010). A theoretical and empirical review of the death-thought accessibility concept in terror management research. Psychol. Bull. 136, 699–739. doi: 10.1037/a0020524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heidegger, M., Stambaugh, J., and Schmidt, D. J. (2010). Being and Time. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

Google Scholar

Heine, S. J., Proulx, T., and Vohs, K. D. (2006). The meaning maintenance model: on the coherence of social motivations. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 10, 88–110. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1002_1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, E., Gonzalez-Mujica, J., Acosta-Orozco, C., and Compton, W. C. (2020). The psychological benefits of receiving real-life altruism. J. Hum. Psychol. 60, 187–204. doi: 10.1177/0022167817690280

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, W., Wisneski, D. C., Brandt, M. J., and Skitka, L. J. (2014). Morality in everyday life. Science 345, 1340–1343. doi: 10.1126/science.1251560

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Imuta, K., Henry, J. D., Slaughter, V., Selcuk, B., and Ruffman, T. (2016). Theory of mind and prosocial behavior in childhood: a meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol. 52, 1192–1205. doi: 10.1037/dev0000140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janoff-bulman, R. (1992). “Shattered assumptions: towards a new psychology of trauma,” in Living With Terror (New York, NY: Free Press), p. 256.

Google Scholar

Joireman, J., and Duell, B. (2007). Self-transcendent values moderate the impact of mortality salience on support for charities. Pers. Individ. Dif. 43, 779–789. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jonas, E., Schimel, J., Greenberg, J., and Pyszczynski, T. (2002). The scrooge effect: evidence that mortality salience increases prosocial attitudes and behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28, 1342–1353. doi: 10.1177/014616702236834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juhl, J., and Routledge, C. (2014). The effects of trait self-esteem and death cognitions on worldview defense and search for meaning. Death Stud. 38, 62–68. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2012.718038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, N. (2017). Prosocial behavior increases perceptions of meaning in life. J. Posit. Psychol. 12, 354–361. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2016.1209541

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kulas, J. T., Klahr, R., and Knights, L. (2019). Confound it! Social desirability and the “reverse-scoring” method effect. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 35, 855–867. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000459

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowe, S., and Fothergill, A. (2003). “A need to help: emergent volunteer behavior after September 11th,” in Beyond September 11th: An Account of Post-Disaster Research, ed J. L. Monday (Boulder: University of Colorado), 293–314.

Google Scholar

Newman, D. B., Nezlek, J. B., and Thrash, T. M. (2018). The dynamics of searching for meaning and presence of meaning in daily life. J. Pers. 86, 368–379. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, C. L., and George, L. S. (2013). Assessing meaning and meaning making in the context of stressful life events: measurement tools and approaches. J. Posit. Psychol. 8, 483–504. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2013.830762

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Koole, S., and Solomon, S. (2010). “Experimental existential psychology: coping with the facts of life,” in Handbook of Social Psychology, 5th Edn, Vol. 1, eds S. Fiske, D. Gilbert, and G. Lindzey (London: John Wiley and Sons), 724–757. doi: 10.1002/9780470561119.socpsy001020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., and Lyon, D. (1989). Evidence for terror management theory: I. The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 681–690. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, B., and Willer, R. (2015). Beyond altruism: sociological foundations of cooperation and prosocial behavior. Ann. Rev. Sociol. 41, 43–63. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-073014-112242

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spinrad, T. L., and Gal, D. E. (2018). Fostering prosocial behavior and empathy in young children. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20, 40–44. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S., and Kaler, M. (2006). The meaning in life questionnaire: assessing the presence of and search for meaning in life. J. Couns. Psychol. 53, 80–93. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.53.1.80

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steger, M. F., Kashdan, T. B., Sullivan, B. A., and Lorentz, D. (2008). Understanding the search for meaning in life: personality, cognitive style, and the dynamic between seeking and experiencing meaning. J. Pers. 76, 199–228. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00484.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events: a theory of cognitive adaptation. Am. Psychol. 38, 1161–1173. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.38.11.1161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Templer, D. I. (1970). The construction and validation of a death anxiety scale. J. Gen. Psychol. 82, 165–177. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1970.9920634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Updegraff, J. A., Silver, R. C., and Holman, E. A. (2008). Searching for and finding meaning in collective trauma: results from a national longitudinal study of the 9/11 terrorist attacks. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95, 709–722. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.95.3.709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Tongeren, D. R., Green, J. D., Davis, D. E., Hook, J. N., and Hulsey, T. L. (2016). Prosociality enhances meaning in life. J. Posit. Psychol. 11, 225–236. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2015.1048814

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, W. C., and Wu, X. C. (2020). Mediating roles of gratitude, social support and posttraumatic growth in the relation between empathy and prosocial behavior among adolescents after the Ya'an earthquake. Acta Psychol. Sin. 52, 307–316. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00307

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X. Q., You, Y. Y., and Zhang, D. J. (2016). Psychometric properties of the meaning in life questionnaire-chinese version (MLQ-C) in Chinese university students, and its relationship with psychological quality. J. Southwest Univ. 38, 161–167.

Google Scholar

Wong, P. T. P. (1998). “Implicit theories of meaningful life and the development of the personal meaning profile,” in The Human Quest for Meaning: A Handbook of Psychological Research and Clinical Applications, eds P. T. P. Wong, and P. S. Fry (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc), 111–140.

Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Zhang, M., and Kou, Y. (2016). The revalidation and development of the prosocial behavior scale for adolescent. Chin. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 10, 135–150.

Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Peng, J., Gao, P., Huang, H., Cao, Y., Zheng, L., et al. (2019). Relationship between meaning in life and death anxiety in the elderly: self-esteem as a mediator. BMC Geriatr. 19:308. doi: 10.1186/s12877-019-1316-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: terror management theory, experienced meaning, search for meaning, prosocial behavior, death anxiety

Citation: Chang B, Cheng J, Fang J and Dang J (2021) The Indirect Effect of Death Anxiety on Experienced Meaning in Life via Search for Meaning and Prosocial Behavior. Front. Psychol. 12:673460. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.673460

Received: 27 February 2021; Accepted: 12 April 2021;
Published: 28 May 2021.

Edited by:

Leonardo Carlucci, University of Studies G. d'Annunzio Chieti and Pescara, Italy

Reviewed by:

Andrew A. Abeyta, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, Camden, United States
Manuel Martí-Vilar, University of Valencia, Spain

Copyright © 2021 Chang, Cheng, Fang and Dang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Junhua Dang, dangjunhua@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.