Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 22 July 2021
Sec. Educational Psychology

Which Divergent Thinking Index Is More Associated With Problem Finding Ability? The Role of Flexibility and Task Nature

  • 1Department of Gifted Education, Arabian Gulf University, Manama, Bahrain
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Nebraska Omaha, Omaha, NE, United States
  • 3Ministry of Education, Manama, Bahrain

Problem finding (PF) and divergent thinking (DT) are considered to be indicators of creative potential. Previous studies, with different goals, suggest a positive correlation between PF and DT. However, none of these works have explicitly examined which index of DT is more associated with PF. The current investigation examined the association between PF and three main indexes of DT: fluency, flexibility, and originality. It also tested whether such a relation differs based on task nature (verbal vs. figural). The sample consisted of 90 sixth graders who completed three tests: (a) a verbal DT test, (b) a figural DT test, and (c) a PF test. Correlational analysis showed that flexibility was highly correlated with PF in the verbal DT test, whereas originality was significantly correlated with PF in the figural test. Results of the path analysis confirmed the results from correlational analyses and showed that verbal flexibility strongly predicted PF fluency, flexibility, and originality more than any other variable. Likelihood ratio test showed that using 1 or 3% cutoff for scoring originality did not significantly altered the results in both figural and verbal DT (vs. PF), while the likelihood ratio test showed significant differences between the figural and verbal DT. Finally, predictor variables in the verbal DT accounted for 40–58% of the variance in PF skills, whereas predictor variables in the figural DT accounted for 28–37% of the variance in PF skills. As suggested by experts in the field of PF, the role of flexibility in PF is a fertile area to be considered in future studies.

Introduction

The significance of problem finding (PF) is widely recognized, and it is considered the first step in creative problem-solving efforts (e.g., Wallas, 1926; Osborn, 1953; Mumford et al., 1991; Treffinger and Isaksen, 2005). Some scholars have even considered PF more important than problem solving (e.g., Einstein and Infeld, 1938; Wertheimer, 1945; Mackworth, 1965; Csikszentmihalyi, 1988). A recent meta-analysis revealed a significant positive correlation between PF and creativity (Abdulla et al., 2020), of which the effect size is nearly the same as that reported in another meta-analysis (Kim, 2008) regarding the correlation between divergent thinking (DT) and creative achievement.

The history of studying PF began with the seminal work of Csikszentmihalyi and Getzels (1971), who aimed to assess PF in art students. Since then, the field of creativity research has displayed a growing interest in PF. However, this important cognitive process has not received as much attention from researchers in the field of creativity relative to DT or ideation. Figure 1 outlines the number of hits when the three terms (problem solving, DT, and PF) were searched from 1960 to 2020 in the following databases: ERIC, Academic Search Complete, Academic Search Premium, and ProQuest Central. Advanced searches were used, including the following combinations of keywords: (a) problem solving AND creativity OR creative, (b) DT AND creativity OR creative, and (c) all terminologies of PF reported in Abdulla and Cramond (2018; see page 202) AND creativity OR creative. The search only included titles, and redundant works were eliminated.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Number of publications focusing on problem finding, DT, and problem solving in the past five decades.

Although the results clearly indicate that research on PF is growing (e.g., Abdulla et al., 2020; Rubenstein et al., 2020a,b; AlSaleh et al., 2021), and that it is increasing at a higher rate relative to other research, the number of papers overall is still relatively small. This is considered a problem for several reasons. First, PF is an essential element in all well-known models of the creative process, such as Wallas’s four-stage model (Wallas, 1926), which includes (a) preparation, (b) incubation, (c) illumination, and (d) verification; Osborn-Parnes’s creative problem-solving model (Parnes, 1967), which includes (a) fact finding, (b) solution finding, and (c) acceptance finding; Basadur’s (1995) model which includes (a) PF, (b) problem solving, and (c) solution implementation, and Mumford et al.’s (1991) model of creative problem solving, which includes (a) problem construction, (b) information gathering, (c) concept selection, (d) conceptual combination, (e) idea generation, (f) idea evaluation, and (g) implementation planning. It should be noted that the role of PF in creative problem solving does not stop when the novel problem is identified. Rather, the problem solver might revisit and reformulate the problem after learning more about it (Runco, 1994b).

Second, some definitions of creativity explicitly include the concept of PF (e.g., Torrance, 1966; Cropley, 2011; Runco, 2014), and descriptions of how the concept is part of the creativity complex (Runco, 1994b). Third, advancing innovation and creativity for the benefit of business and society depends on people who can sense gaps in knowledge, discover ill-defined problems, and solve them in a novel way. Ma’s (2009) meta-analysis indicated that PF was the greatest predictor of creativity, which supports this last assertion. Additional support can be found in the work by Scott et al. (2004) which suggests that training of the PF process is the most effective in improving creativity.

However, this is not to say that other cognitive processes involved in creativity and creative thinking are less important than PF. The argument posited by this point is that PF is an essential cognitive ability that deserves more attention from researchers in the field of creativity.

After six decades of systematically studying PF, researchers have a deeper understanding of the relationship between PF and creativity as a whole and with specific cognitive processes, such as inter and intrapersonal evaluation (Mumford et al., 1991; Runco, 1994a,b; Basadur, 1995), problem solving (Arlin, 1975; Bouchard and Drauden, 1976; Carson and Runco, 1999), selective encoding and combination, selective comparison (Brugman, 1995), information search (Harms et al., 2020), and DT (e.g., Csikszentmihalyi and Getzels, 1971; Arlin, 1975; Artley et al., 1980; Wakefield, 1985; Chand and Runco, 1993; Hoover, 1994).

Of these cognitive processes, creativity literature suggests that DT is associated with PF–and many researchers have sought to explore this association (Arlin, 1975; Wakefield, 1985; Hoover and Feldhusen, 1990; Chand and Runco, 1993; Ambrosio, 1994; Hoover, 1994; Reiter-Palmon et al., 1997; Runco and Acar, 2010). However, these studies have yielded conflicting results (see Table 1), which could have been caused by (a) the DT and PF assessments that were used in the studies, (b) the DT skills that were targeted (i.e., fluency, flexibility, and originality), and (c) the nature of the task (verbal vs. figural tasks). The present study focuses on the latter two issues. Section “Literature Review: PF and DT” below illuminates the major findings from previous studies that have investigated the association between PF and DT. The section also demonstrates how the results differed according to the DT index that was studied and the nature of the tasks in the DT tests that were employed.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Selected studies focusing on the relationship between DT and PF.

Literature Review: PF and DT

After examining previous studies that investigated the association between PF and DT, this study noted that (a) most studies used a verbal DT when investigating the association, (b) fluency was the most studied DT index regarding the relationship between DT and PF (compared with flexibility, originality, and elaboration), (c) few studies examined the possible differences of the association in terms of nature of the DT task (i.e., verbal vs. figural), and (d) no studies explicitly aimed to understand which DT index is more strongly associated with PF.

The only study that considered both verbal and non-verbal DT tests in DT’s relationship with PF was Getzels and Smilansky (1983), who investigated the content and quality of problems that high school students formulated. Getzels and Smilansky’s (1983) main findings indicated that verbal originality (r = 0.23) and verbal flexibility (r = 0.18) were significantly associated with PF, and that no significant correlation was found with verbal fluency. Figural fluency, flexibility, and originality did not significantly correlate with PF, indicating that the association between DT and PF differs based on task nature.

Studies that employed verbal DT tests included Arlin (1975); Artley et al. (1980), Lee and Cho (2007), and Runco and Okuda (1988). Arlin’s (1975) study–which aimed to propose a cognitive process model of PF–reported a positive relationship between PF quality and adaptive flexibility (r = 0.26), and a negative relationship between PF quality and spontaneous flexibility (r = −0.09) and fluency (r = −0.18).

Artley et al. (1980) examined the association between verbal fluency, which was measured according to the Torrance Test of Creative Thinking (TTCT) and PF fluency. The results revealed a moderately significant correlation (r = 0.47) between the two variables. Artley et al. (1980) concluded that PF is independent from creativity, as assessed by DT tests. Runco and Okuda (1988) reached a similar conclusion after administering verbal DT and verbal PF tests: the correlation between the two variables was positively significant (r = 0.32). They concluded that “problem-finding skills is by definition statistically independent of problem-solving” (Runco and Okuda, 1988, p. 217). Runco and Okuda (1988) recognized the importance of considering other DT indices–more specifically, flexibility and originality–and they called for future research that examines the association between these other DT indices and PF. Finally, in a more recent study, Lee and Cho (2007) examined the association between PF using ill-structured problem situation tasks and TTCT. The verbal form of the TTCT was used, and the scholars reported a composite score for the verbal battery. A negative correlation (r = −0.17) was found between the DT and PF.

Some studies used a figural DT, such as those by Ambrosio (1994) and Wakefield (1985). Ambrosio’s (1994) study examined the PF differences between freshman and senior undergraduate students using the figural form of the TTCT. Ambrosio (1994) reported a negative relationship between DT and PF quality (r = −0.08) and a significant positive relationship between DT and PF quantity (r = 0.41). Ambrosio (1994) interpreted these results as demonstrating that DT is distinct from PF. Finally, Wakefield (1985) studied the association between figural DT (i.e., Wallack and Kogan’s pattern meaning and line meaning tests) and figural PF. Only fluency was scored for the DT and PF tasks. Wakefield (1985) reported a high correlation between DT and PF (r = 0.75) and concluded that “freedom to discover and solve problems appears to be the primary condition of creative performance” (p. 268). Moreover, Wakefield (1985) asserted that responses to presented problems (such as those used in DT tests) are marginally related to creativity, unlike PF tasks.

PF and DT: Summary

As discussed above, and as shown in Table 1, only one study examined the relationship between PF and elaboration (i.e., Arlin, 1975), and only two studies examined the relationship between PF and flexibility (Arlin, 1975; Hoover, 1994). These study results suggest that the specific DT measure and index used (verbal or figural; fluency, flexibility, or originality) can affect the results (Hornberg and Reiter-Palmon, 2017; Reiter-Palmon et al., 2019). Further, much of the research that focuses on DT tasks tended to use fluency (Runco and Acar, 2012). Therefore, one of the present study’s contributions is the evaluation of flexibility and originality, in addition to fluency. Flexibility could be especially critical for PF. According to Mumford et al. (1994, p. 30), “Flexibility in applying selection and screening strategies may contribute to individual differences in PF skills.” In addition to evaluating the relationships with different DT indices, the present study also determined whether the relationship between DT and PF differs according to the nature of the DT task (verbal or figural). Finally, the present study also employed a more advanced path analysis method to understand the association between DT and PF. Based on the literature review presented in this section, the present study’s research hypotheses were determined as the following:

H1: The relationship between DT and PF differs according to the DT index (i.e., fluency, flexibility, and originality).

H2: The relationship between DT and PF will differs based on the nature of the DT task (figural or verbal).

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedures

The participants included 90 students in the sixth grade (42 boys and 48 girls). We randomly collected data from two public schools after receiving the official approval from the Directorate for Scientific Research at the Ministry of Education. Prior to data collection, each participant was asked to read and sign a consent form prepared by the authors of this study.

Before the tests were administered, the first author visited the two schools and spent one class period (40 min) with the students to create familiarity with them (no DT or PF activities were discussed). By the end of this meeting, the author informed the study participants of a subsequent visit in the next week to administer some activities that could show the students’ creativity. The tests were administered in the second class/session (8:45 am), as recommended by the teachers. The participants were informed that they will obtain the results of their “creative activity score” after the authors had scored them. The tests were conducted in a game-like condition (untimed), following the results of a recent meta-analysis that showed that the untimed condition significantly improves students’ performance in DT tests (Said-Metwaly et al., 2020).

Instruments

Uses Test

We administered three tasks from the Uses test (Wallach and Kogan, 1965): (a) uses for a spoon, (b) uses for a wheel, and (c) uses for a toothbrush. The directions for the task are as follows:

In this game, I am going to name an object–any kind of object, like a light bulb or the floor–and it will be your job to tell lots of different ways that the object could be used. Any object can be used in a lot of different ways. Remember, think of all the different ways you could use the object that I mentioned (Wallach and Kogan, 1965, p. 31).

Responses to the Uses test were scored for fluency, flexibility, and originality. A total of 199 different ideas were generated for the spoon task, 124 different ideas for the wheel task, and 181 different ideas for the toothbrush task. Fluency was defined as the number of different ideas related to a given stimulus. Flexibility was defined as the number of different categories or shifts between ideas. Based on participants’ responses, 12 categories were created for the spoon task, 11 for the wheel task, and eight for the toothbrush task (see Table 2). Finally, since some studies found that using different cutoff criteria can results in different findings, originality was scored based on both, 1% cutoff criterion and 3% cutoff criterion (Reiter-Palmon et al., 2019).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Flexibility categories for verbal and figural tests.

Figural Test

We administered the Figural test from the Runco Creativity Assessment Battery (2020), which consists of three tasks, similar to the Wallach and Kogan Line Meaning test (Wallach and Kogan, 1965, p. 36). The Figural test includes three shapes: Spiral, Blocks, and Lines. The directions are as follows:

Look at the figure below. What do you see? List as many things as you can that this figure might be or represent. This is NOT a test. Think of this as a game and have fun with it! The more ideas you list, the better (Runco et al., 2016, p. 6).

The Figural test was also scored for fluency, flexibility, and originality. A total of 105 different ideas were generated for the first task (Spiral), 111 for the second task (Blocks), and 53 for the third task (Lines). For flexibility, 10 categories were created in the Spiral task, 11 categories in the Blocks task, and 11 categories in the Lines task. Originality was scored using the same method as the Uses test (i.e., 1 and 3% cutoffs).

Problem Generation Test

We used the Problem Generation (PG) test to assess participants’ PF ability (Okuda et al., 1991)1. The PG test consists of three open-ended tasks that require participants to list as many problems as they can. Participants were asked to generate as many problems as they can (real or hypothetical) without any instruction to evaluate any of their ideas, or that their ideas will be evaluated. The three PG tasks are related to (a) home and school, (b) life situations, and (c) health and well-being. An example of a PG task is as follows:

List problems along with your friends, peers, or schoolmates (any individual who is approximately the same age as your-self). These problems can be real, or they can be hypothetical and imaginary. Do not limit yourself; the more problems you can list, the better. You can think of problems that exist now or those that might exist in future.

The PG test was scored for fluency, flexibility, and originality. The participants generated 235, 253, and 269 different ideas in the first, second, and third tasks, respectively. For flexibility, 11 categories were created in the first task (home and school), 12 categories in the second task (life situation), and 13 categories in the third task (health and well-being). The same method was utilized for scoring originality in the PG test (i.e., 1 and 3% cutoffs).

Finally, the students were asked to fill out a demographic questionnaire with items on their age and sex.

Results

Reliability

Both the interclass correlation (ICC) and internal consistency reliability were calculated for the three tests that were used in the study. As mentioned earlier, DT verbal and figural tests (as well as PG tests) were scored for fluency, flexibility, and originality. Table 3 lists the reliability coefficients for the three tests. For the ICC, a two-way mixed-model absolute agreement was calculated.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Interclass and internal consistency reliability coefficients of study instruments.

As moderate reliability coefficients were observed in the figural test, this study examined each task of the figural test. The lines task demonstrated lower reliability coefficients in the figural test. After removing the lines test, the reliability coefficients in the figural test improved. Therefore, the lines task scores were eliminated in all subsequent analyses.

Correlation Analysis

As outlined in Table 4, the pattern of correlations between DT and PF differed in the verbal and figural tests. In the verbal DT, the highest correlation was found between verbal flexibility and PF (r = 0.71–0.76, p < 0.001), followed by verbal fluency and PF (r = 0.68–0.71, p < 0.001) and verbal originality and PF (r = 0.55–0.61, p < 0.001). This pattern remained the same regardless of whether the 1% cutoff or 3% cutoff was used as the originality criterion.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Correlations between study variables (n = 90).

Further, the magnitude of the correlation between figural DT and PF was much lower than that of verbal DT. More specifically, the highest correlation was found between figural originality and PF (r = 0.27–0.31, p < 0.05), followed by figural flexibility and PF (r = 0.15–0.20) and figural fluency and PF (r = 0.14–0.18). Again, the different cutoff criteria for originality did not alter the results.

Path Analysis

While correlational analysis allows for a better understanding of the bivariate relationship between DT and PF, examining all the relationships at the same time is important as well. After creating the models and selecting the required path directions, the maximum likelihood method was used to estimate the parameters of the structural equation model. The present study then analyzed how verbal and figural DT (fluency, flexibility, and originality at the 3% cutoff) predicted PF (fluency, flexibility, and originality at the 3% cutoff), and how verbal and figural DT (fluency, flexibility, and originality at the 1% cutoff) predicted PF (fluency, flexibility, and originality at the 1%). The four path analysis models are illustrated in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Path analysis models for the study variables. V, verbal; F, figural; PF, problem finding; FLU, fluency; FLE, flexibility; ORG, originality.

As displayed in Figure 2, the standardized path coefficients (β) and t values were observed to be between the DT indices and PF. These values indicate that the model adequately fits the data. After examining the fit indices χ2/df, RMSEA, GFI, AGFI, and NFI, the values indicated that the models adequately fits the data. Table 5 showed the four models.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Goodness-of-fit indices for the different models.

First, Model 1 indicated that the predictor variables accounted for 56, 58, and 43% of the variance in PF fluency, flexibility, and originality, respectively. The results verified the model and confirmed that verbal flexibility strongly affected: (a) PF flexibility (β = 0.81, p < 0.001), (b) PF fluency (β = 0.78, p < 0.001), and (3) PF originality (β = 0.59, p < 0.001). Finally, verbal originality showed significant effects on: (a) PF originality (β = 0.54, p < 0.001) and (b) PF fluency (β = 0.39, p < 0.001).

Second, Model 2 showed that the predictor variables accounted for 56, 40, and 53% of the variance in PF fluency, flexibility, and originality, respectively. The results verified the model and confirmed that verbal flexibility strongly affected: (a) PF flexibility (β = 0.78, p < 0.001), (b) PF fluency (β = 0.78, p < 0.001), and (3) PF originality (β = 0.25, p < 0.001). Finally, verbal originality has a significant effect on: (a) PF originality (β = 0.44, p < 0.001) and (b) PF fluency (β = 0.37, p < 0.001).

Third, Model 3 showed that the predictor variables accounted for 35%, 37%, and 35% of the variance in PF fluency, flexibility, and originality, respectively. The results verified the model and confirmed that figural flexibility strongly affected: (a) PF originality (β = 0.37, p < 0.001), (b) PF fluency (β = 0.33, p < 0.001), and (3) PF flexibility (β = 0.27, p < 0.001). Figural originality has a significant effect on: (a) PF flexibility (β = 0.39, p < 0.001), and (b) PF fluency (β = 0.16, p < 0.05).

Finally, Model 4 indicated that the predictor variables accounted for 33%, 30%, and 28% of the variance in PF fluency, flexibility, and originality, respectively. The results verified the model and confirmed that figural flexibility strongly affected: (a) PF flexibility (β = 0.32, p < 0.001), (b) PF fluency (β = 0.30, p < 0.001), and (3) PF originality (β = 0.13, p < 0.05). Finally, figural originality has a significant effect on: (a) PF flexibility (β = 0.24, p < 0.001), and (b) PF originality (β = 0.22, p < 0.001; see Table 6).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Path analysis of DT indices with PF.

Likelihood ratio test showed that there were no significant differences between Models 1 and 2, (Δχ2 = 2.19, Δdf = 2, p = 0.335). Furthermore, there were no significant differences between Models 3 and 4, (Δχ2 = 1.10, Δdf = 2, p = 0.073), indicating that using different cutoff scores for originality (1 vs. 3%) did not affect the results. As for the differences between verbal and figural DT, the results indicated that there were significant differences between Models 1 and 3 (DT Verbal vs. DT Figural; 3% cutoff originality) in favor of Verbal DT (Δχ2 = 2.77, Δdf = 2, p = 0.008). Moreover, there were significant differences between Models 2 and 4 (DT Verbal vs. DT Figural; 1% cutoff originality) in favor of Verbal DT (Δχ2 = 2.73, Δdf = 2, p = 0.001). It can thus be concluded that the nature of the task significantly influenced the relationship between PF and DT, such that verbal tasks were more strongly related to PF.

Discussion

Studies on PF have suggested that some cognitive processing skills are related to PF, and have also paid attention to DT along with PF. Previous research has examined the association between PF and DT, however, none of these studies have explicitly aimed to examine multiple DT indices and tasks and the relative association with PF. The results of our study showed that flexibility, which was neglected in previous works on PF and DT, was highly correlated with PF compared with fluency and originality, especially when verbal tasks were administered. In the path models, flexibility of verbal DT was the best predictor of the PF task performance. Arlin (1975) described problem finders as “flexible thinkers.” Our findings support this claim and suggest that good problem finders are characterized by an ability to shift between ideas and keep formulating different ideas to find a novel problem that needs to be solved. However, more studies are needed to elucidate fully the role of flexibility in PF.

The other important finding in the present study is the difference in the relation between PF and DT based on the task nature. We found that PF can be better assessed using verbal tasks than figural ones. This pattern of results included both the correlational analysis and the path models. This finding is partly supported by Abdulla et al. (2020), in which PF in the writing domains is highly correlated with creativity (r = 0.36) compared with art (r = 0.20). One issue that future studies might consider is whether highly capable problem finders are overrepresented in writing vs. in other domains. It is possible that the strong correlations between PF and DT in the writing domain is in part a result of both requiring writing as the mode of generated responses. Previous research suggests that the task and index of measurement is important in understanding these types of relationships (Reiter-Palmon et al., 2019; Reiter-Palmon and Schoenbeck, 2020). Additional research evaluating whether these relationships hold with other verbal DT measures such as consequences as well as other figural DT measures would help clarify this issue.

The path model also suggests that originality of verbal DT responses was important for predicting PF. Specifically, originality was predictive of PF fluency and PF originality. Interestingly, the results from the path analysis suggest that verbal DT fluency was either minimally predictive or non-significant. When it was predictive, the relationship that was obtained was negative, which suggests that when other dimensions of DT scoring are included, fluency is less important or even may have a negative effect. This further indicates that it is not sufficient to evaluate DT fluency when trying to understand the relationship between DT and other creativity measures and processes, such as PF.

Limitations

It is important to note that PF is not a single process. Many scholars have differentiated known PF skills, such as problem definition, problem identification, problem construction, and problem discovery (e.g., Getzels, 1982; Mumford et al., 1994; Runco, 1994b). The differences between these and other PF-related skills lie in (a) how well- or ill-defined a problem is and (b) the degree to which ideation and evaluation are required (Abdulla and Cramond, 2018). In our study, PF was operationalized as problem discovery, which has been considered as the highest level of PF hierarchy (Abdulla and Cramond, 2018). We asked the participants to produce as many ideas as they could think of for different and novel problems related to home and school, life situations, and health and well-being. They were explicitly instructed to think of real or hypothetical problems, and to think of problems that exist or those that might exist in the future. Previous work on instructions indicates that the focus of instructions can have a significant impact on the outcomes associated with DT measures (Runco et al., 2005; Nusbaum et al., 2014; Acar et al., 2020). The focus of the instructions is on both different and novel ideas may be one reason why flexibility was such an effective predictor. This point is crucial because different results might be attributed to the level of ideation and evaluation required in the PF task (Lee and Cho, 2007). Thus, our findings might not be generalized to all PF tasks, except those that require problem discovery where there is limited role for evaluation. The nature of the specific PF task, focusing on generation, may also indicate some degree of common method bias. This would be particularly the case if fluency was the only metric that was used for the study. However, since flexibility and originality were evaluated for both the DT and PF task, this is less of a concern. This study adds to our understanding of the relationship between PF and DT. Future research should address some of the limitations noted here such as including additional PF tasks. In addition, evaluating the effects of task instructions for both PF and DT tasks by varying task instructions would be beneficial. Finally, additional research should evaluate the relationships in other populations such as adolescents and adults.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Directorate for Scientific Research at the Ministry of Education, Bahrain. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

AMA originated the research idea and wrote the manuscript with support from RR-P. AMA wrote the introduction and the results. RR-P wrote the discussion and reviewed the introduction. ZS collected the data and scored the assessments (two divergent thinking tests and a problem construction test). AMA and ZS calculated in interclass correlation reliability. AAA wrote the method section and analyzed the data. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://creativitytestingservice.com

References

Abdulla, A. M., and Cramond, B. (2018). The creative problem finding hierarchy: a suggested model for understanding problem finding. Creat. Theor. Res. Appl. 5, 197–229. doi: 10.1515/ctra-2018-0019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abdulla, A. M., Paek, S. H., Cramond, B., and Runco, M. A. (2020). Problem finding and creativity: a meta–analytic review. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 14, 3–14. doi: 10.1037/aca0000194

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Acar, S., Runco, M. A., and Park, H. (2020). What should people be told when they take a divergent thinking test? A meta-analytic review of explicit instructions for divergent thinking. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 14, 39–49. doi: 10.1037/aca0000256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

AlSaleh, A., Abdulla Alabbasi, A. M., Ayoub, A., and Hafsyan, A. S. M. (2021). The effects of birth order and family size on academic achievement, divergent thinking, and problem finding among gifted students. J. Educ, Gifted Young Sci. 9, 67–75. doi: 10.17478/jegys.864399

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ambrosio, A. L. (1994). Social problem finding: a cognitive-developmental perspective. Diss. Abstr. Int. Section A 54:3700.

Google Scholar

Arlin, P. K. (1975). A Cognitive process model of problem finding. Educ. Horizons Winter 54, 99–106. doi: 10.4324/9780203859827-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Artley, N. L., Van Horn, R., Friedrich, D. D., and Carroll, J. L. (1980). The relationship between problem finding, creativity and cognitive style. Creat. Child Adult Q. 5, 20–26.

Google Scholar

Basadur, M. (1995). Optimal ideation-evaluation ratios. Creat. Res. J. 8, 63–75. doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj0801_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouchard, T. J., and Drauden, G. (1976). Discovery-oriented behavior and problem solving. J. Psychol. 92, 243–248. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1976.9921362

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brugman, G. (1995). The discovery and formulation of problems. Eur. Educ. 27, 38–57. doi: 10.2753/EUE1056-4934270138

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carson, D. K., and Runco, M. A. (1999). Creative problem solving and problem finding in young adults: interconnections with stress, hassles, and coping abilities. J. Creat. Behav. 33, 167–190. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.1999.tb01195.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chand, I., and Runco, M. A. (1993). Problem finding skills as components in the creative process. Pers. Individ. Differ. 14, 155–162. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90185-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cropley, A. J. (2011). “Definitions of creativity,” in Encyclopedia of Creativity, eds M. A. Runco and S. R. Pritzker (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 511–524.

Google Scholar

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). Solving a problem is not finding a new one: a reply to Simon. New Ideas Psychol. 6, 183–186. doi: 10.1016/0732-118x(88)90003-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csikszentmihalyi, M., and Getzels, J. W. (1971). Discovery-oriented behavior and the originality of creative products: a study with artists. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 19, 47–52. doi: 10.1037/h0031106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Einstein, A., and Infeld, L. (1938). The Evolution of Physics. New York NY: Simon and Schuster.

Google Scholar

Frederiksen, N., and Evans, F. R. (1974). Effects of models of creative performance on ability to formulate hypotheses. J. Educ. Psychol. 66, 67–82. doi: 10.1037/h0035808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Getzels, J. W. (1982). “The problem of the problem,” in New Directions for Methodology of Social and Behavioral Science: Question Framing, ed. R. Hogarth (San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass), 37–49.

Google Scholar

Getzels, J. W., and Smilansky, J. (1983). Individual differences in pupil perceptions of school problems. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 53, 307–316. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1983.tb02563.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guilford, J. (1966). Measurement and creativity. Theory Into Prac. 5, 186–202.

Google Scholar

Harms, M., Reiter-Palmon, R., and Derrick, D. C. (2020). The role of information search in creative problem solving. Psychol. Aesth. Creat. Arts 14, 367–380. doi: 10.1037/aca0000212

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoover, S. M. (1994). Scientific problem finding in gifted fifth-grade students. Roeper Rev. 16, 156–159. doi: 10.1080/02783199409553563

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoover, S. M., and Feldhusen, J. F. (1990). The scientific hypothesis formulation ability of gifted ninth-grade students. J. Educ. Psychol. 82, 838–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.82.4.838

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hornberg, J., and Reiter-Palmon, R. (2017). “Creativity and the big five personality traits: is the relationship dependent on the creativity measure?,” in Cambridge Handbooks in Psychology. The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity and Personality Research, eds G. J. Feist, R. Reiter-Palmon, and J. C. Kaufman (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 275–293. doi: 10.1017/9781316228036.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaarsveld, S., Lachmann, T., and van Leeuwen, C. (2012). Creative reasoning across developmental levels: convergence and divergence in problem creation. Intelligence 40, 172–188. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2012.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, K. H. (2008). Meta-analyses of the relationship of creative achievement to both IQ and divergent thinking test scores. J. Creat. Behav. 42, 106–130. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.2008.tb01290.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H., and Cho, Y. (2007). Factors affecting problem finding depending on degree of structure of problem situation. J. Educ. Res. 101, 113–123. doi: 10.3200/JOER.101.2.113-125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, H.-H. (2009). The effect size of variables associated with creativity: a meta-analysis. Creat. Res. J. 21, 30–42. doi: 10.1080/10400410802633400

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackworth, N. H. (1965). Originality. Am. Psychol. 20, 51–66. doi: 10.1037/h0021900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mumford, M. D., Mobley, M. I., Uhlman, C. E., Reiter-Palmon, R., and Doares, L. M. (1991). Process analytic models of creative capacities. Creat. Res. J. 4, 91–122. doi: 10.1080/10400419109534380

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mumford, M. D., Reiter-Palmon, R., and Redmond, M. R. (1994). “Problem construction and cognition: applying problem representations in ill-defined domains,” in Problem Finding, Problem Solving, and Creativity, ed. M. A. Runco (Norwood NJ: Ablex Publishing), 3–39.

Google Scholar

Nusbaum, E. C., Silvia, P. J., and Beaty, R. E. (2014). Ready, set, create: what instructing people to “be creative” reveals about the meaning and mechanisms of divergent thinking. Psychol. Aesth. Creat. Arts 8, 423–432. doi: 10.1037/a0036549

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okuda, S. M., Runco, M. A., and Berger, D. E. (1991). Creativity and the finding and solving of real-world problems. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 9, 45–53. doi: 10.1177/073428299100900104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osborn, A. F. (1953). Applied Imagination. New York NY: Scribner’s.

Google Scholar

Parnes, S. (1967). Creative Behavior Guidebook. New York NY: Scribner.

Google Scholar

Reiter-Palmon, R., and Schoenbeck, M. (2020). “Creativity equals creativity- or does it? How creativity is measured influences our understanding of creativity,” in Handbook of Research Methods of Creativity, eds M. Stierand and V. Dorfler (Cheltenham: Elgar Publishing).

Google Scholar

Reiter-Palmon, R., Forthmann, B., and Barbot, B. (2019). Scoring divergent thinking tests: a review and systematic framework. Psychol. Aesth. Creat. Arts 13, 144–152. doi: 10.1037/aca0000227

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiter-Palmon, R., Mumford, M. D., O’Connor Boes, J., and Runco, M. A. (1997). Problem construction and creativity: the role of ability, cue consistency and active processing. Creat. Res. J. 10, 9–23. doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj1001_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubenstein, L. D., Callan, G. L., Speirs Neumeister, K., and Ridgley, L. M. (2020a). Finding the problem: how students approach problem identification. Think. Skills Creat. 35:100635. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2020.100635

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubenstein, L. D., Callan, G. L., Speirs Neumeister, K., Ridgley, L. M., and Hernández Finch, M. (2020b). How problem identification strategies influence creativity outcomes. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 60:101840. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101840

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Runco Creativity Assessment Battery (2020). Creativity Testing Services. Available online at: https://www.creativitytestingservices.com/products.

Google Scholar

Runco, M. A. (1994b). “Conclusions concerning problem finding, problem solving, and creativity,” in Problem Finding, Problem Solving, and Creativity, ed. M. A. Runco (Norwood NJ: Ablex Publishing), 272–290.

Google Scholar

Runco, M. A. (2014). Creativity: Theories and Themes: Research, Development, and Practice. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Runco, M. A. (Ed.) (1994a). Creativity Research. Problem Finding, Problem Solving, and Creativity. Norwood NJ: Ablex Publishing.

Google Scholar

Runco, M. A., and Acar, S. (2010). Do tests of divergent thinking have an experiential bias? Psychol. Aesth. Creat. Arts 4, 144–148. doi: 10.1037/a0018969

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Runco, M. A., and Okuda, S. M. (1988). Problem discovery, divergent thinking, and the creative process. J. Youth Adol. 17, 211–220. doi: 10.1007/BF01538162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Runco, M. A., Abdulla, A. M., Peak, S., Aljasim, F. A., and AlSuwaidi, H. N. (2016). Which test of divergent thinking is best? Creat. Theor. Res. Appl. 3, 4–18. doi: 10.1515/ctra-2016-0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Runco, M. A., and Acar, S. (2012). Divergent thinking as an indicator of creative potential. Creat. Res. J. 24, 66–75. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2012.652929

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Runco, M. A., Illies, J. J., and Reiter-Palmon, R. (2005). Explicit instructions to be creative and original: a comparison of strategies and criteria as targets with three types of divergent thinking tests. Korean J. Think. Problem Solving 15, 5–15.

Google Scholar

Said-Metwaly, S., Fernández-Castilla, B., Kyndt, E., and Van den Noortgate, W. (2020). Testing conditions and creative performance: meta-analyses of the impact of time limits and instructions. Psychol. Aesth. Creat. Arts 14, 15–38. doi: 10.1037/aca0000244

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, G., Leritz, L. E., and Mumford, M. D. (2004). Types of creativity training: approaches and their effectiveness. J. Creat. Behav. 38, 149–179. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.2004.tb01238.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torrance, E. P. (1966). Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: Norms Technical Manual (Research Edition). Princeton, NJ: Personnel Press.

Google Scholar

Treffinger, D. J., and Isaksen, S. G. (2005). creative problem solving: the history, development, and implications for gifted education and talent development. Gifted Child Q. 49, 342–353. doi: 10.1177/001698620504900407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wakefield, J. F. (1985). Towards creativity: problem finding in a divergent-thinking exercise. Child Stud. J. 15, 265–270.

Google Scholar

Wallach, M. A., and Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of Thinking in Young Children: A Study of The Creativity-Intelligence Distinction. New York NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

Google Scholar

Wallas, G. (1926). The Art of Thought. New York NY: Harcourt Brace and World.

Google Scholar

Wertheimer, M. (1945). Productive Thinking. Barranquilla: Harper.

Google Scholar

Keywords: divergent thinking, ill-structured problems, creativity, task nature, problem finding, fluency, flexibility, originality

Citation: Abdulla Alabbasi AM, Reiter-Palmon R, Sultan ZM and Ayoub AEA (2021) Which Divergent Thinking Index Is More Associated With Problem Finding Ability? The Role of Flexibility and Task Nature. Front. Psychol. 12:671146. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.671146

Received: 23 February 2021; Accepted: 25 June 2021;
Published: 22 July 2021.

Edited by:

Gregory Callan, Utah State University, United States

Reviewed by:

Martin Storme, Université Paris Descartes, France
Wenjing Yang, Southwest University, China

Copyright © 2021 Abdulla Alabbasi, Reiter-Palmon, Sultan and Ayoub. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ahmed M. Abdulla Alabbasi, YWhtZWRtZGFAYWd1LmVkdS5iaA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.