Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 17 May 2021
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19): The Mental Health, Resilience, and Communication Resources for the Short- and Long-term Challenges Faced by Healthcare Workers View all 43 articles

Commentary: Self-Oriented Empathy and Compassion Fatigue: The Serial Mediation of Dispositional Mindfulness and Counselor's Self-Efficacy

  • School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, United Kingdom

A Commentary on
Self-Oriented Empathy and Compassion Fatigue: The Serial Mediation of Dispositional Mindfulness and Counselor's Self-Efficacy

by Zhang, L., Ren, Z., Jiang, G., Hazer-Rau, D., Zhao, C., Shi, C., et al. (2021). Front. Psychol. 11:613908. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.613908

Zhang et al. (2021), in their recent article exploring “the association between self-oriented empathy and compassion fatigue” (p. 1) stated that

“Compassion fatigue (CF) is an empathetic reaction resulting from frequently witnessing the emotional or physical suffering of others or repeatedly listening to a person suffering from mental or physical dysfunction” (p. 1).

The authors measured “self-oriented empathy” using Davis' original Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) scale (Davis, 1983) and stated,

“The personal distress subscale measures self-oriented empathy, that is, distress and discomfort elicited by witnessing another person's suffering. High scores of the personal distress subscale indicate a high tendency to experience self-oriented empathy when observing the suffering of others” (p. 4).

If one returns to Davis' original Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) scale development (Davis, 1983), the following is stated about the subscales under consideration,

“The Empathic Concern (EC) scale assesses “other-oriented” feelings of sympathy and concern for unfortunate others, and the Personal Distress (PD) scale measures “self-oriented” feelings of personal anxiety and unease in tense interpersonal settings” (p. 114) (emphasis added).

The Personal Distress subscale (Davis, 1980) contains items such as

Being in a tense emotional situation scares me.

I am usually pretty effective in dealing with emergencies (-).

I tend to lose control during emergencies.

When I see someone who badly needs help in an emergency, I go to pieces (p. 96).

These items do not reflect “empathy” or “self-oriented empathy.” Empathy is defined in many ways throughout the literature, but is generally considered to have two components; cognitive empathy (intellectually understanding another person's emotions and perspective) and affective or emotional empathy (being affected by and sharing another's emotions, both positive and negative). In other words, empathy relates to feeling with another. Empathy is necessary for empathic concern and compassion (wishing to or acting to alleviate suffering) which are other-oriented; feeling for another (Klimecki and Singer, 2011; Strauss et al., 2016). Decety (2020), reviewing the use of the term “empathy” in medicine states,

“Empathy is a broad construct that refers to the ability to sense other people's emotions, coupled with the ability to imagine what someone else might be thinking or feeling” (p. 563).

The author is unaware of any published definition of the concept of “self-oriented empathy” which appears to be a new term coined by Zhang et al. It should not be confused with “self-compassion” (Neff, 2003) which is associated with lower levels of both burnout (Gerber and Anaki, 2020; Hashem and Zeinoun, 2020) and secondary traumatic stress (Neff et al., 2020).

Compassion fatigue is defined by Stamm (2010) as having 2 components.

“The first part concerns things such like exhaustion, frustration, anger and depression typical of burnout. Secondary Traumatic Stress is a negative feeling driven by fear and work-related trauma. Some trauma at work can be direct (primary) trauma. In other cases, work-related trauma be a combination of both primary and secondary trauma” (p. 8).

Decety (2020) summarizes this as,

“Compassion fatigue is the physical and mental exhaustion and emotional withdrawal experienced by individuals who care for sick or traumatized people over an extended period of time” (p. 563).

In a very recent publication (Eng et al., 2021) which validated a new measure, The Compassion Fatigue Inventory, three factors were found to contribute to compassion fatigue:

1. Reduced Compassion—My will to help has declined; I feel irritated when patients complain

2. Social Life—I have started to withdraw from social interactions; I have noticed that my patience in personal relationships has dwindled

3. Workplace—I feel that my workplace provides care that is in accordance with my values (rev) (p. 13)

It did not include any items appearing to measure what one could consider to be self-oriented empathy or compassion. The authors concluded that,

“Even though Compassion fatigue had a high correlation with both burnout and STS [Secondary Traumatic Stress], the results suggest a narrower conceptualization of compassion fatigue” (p. 1).

Therefore, it can be seen that personal distress is just that, personal distress, and not self- oriented empathy. In other words, it reflects a tendency to be emotionally overwhelmed because of poor emotional regulation of negative affective empathy (Hofmeyer et al., 2019), inadequate self-other differentiation (Klimecki and Singer, 2011), or the nature of top-down control not/used such as cognitive appraisal of the situation (Lamm et al., 2007a,b). This leads to the argument that compassion fatigue should be called “Empathic distress fatigue” (Klimecki and Singer, 2011; Hofmeyer et al., 2019) where high levels of inadequately modulated empathy for distress, or a lack of ability to respond prosocially with compassion (Duarte and Pinto-Gouveia, 2017), result in personal distress and self-oriented attempts to reduce one's own suffering. This is apparently different to being overwhelmed to the point of coping badly as implied by the nature of the IRI items.

Zhang et al. (2021) also found that, “Self-oriented empathy was positively associated with compassion fatigue” (p. 1). This makes absolute sense; personal distress, as measured by the IRI, not self-oriented empathy, is associated with compassion fatigue.

In summary, the use of the term “self-oriented empathy” in this paper (Zhang et al., 2021) is confusing and the paper would be enhanced if it were re-written to reflect the fact that it is personal distress that is associated with compassion fatigue.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Davis, M. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog Sel. Doc. Psychol. 10, 85–104.

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 44, 113–126. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J. (2020). Empathy in medicine: what it is, and how much we really need it. Am. J. Med. 133, 561–566. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2019.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duarte, J., and Pinto-Gouveia, J. (2017). Empathy and feelings of guilt experienced by nurses: a cross-sectional study of their role in burnout and compassion fatigue symptoms. Appl. Nurs. Res. 35, 42–47. doi: 10.1016/j.apnr.2017.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eng, L., Nordström, J., and Schad, E. (2021). Incorporating compassion into compassion fatigue: the development of a new scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 1–21. doi: 10.1002/jclp.23113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerber, Z., and Anaki, D. (2020). The role of self-compassion, concern for others, and basic psychological needs in the reduction of caregiving burnout. Mindfulness 12, 741–750. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01540-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hashem, Z., and Zeinoun, P. (2020). Self-compassion explains less burnout among healthcare professionals. Mindfulness 11, 2542–2551. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01469-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmeyer, A., Kennedy, K., and Taylor, R. (2019). Contesting the term ‘compassion fatigue: integrating findings from social neuroscience and self-care research. Collegian 27, 232–237. doi: 10.1016/j.colegn.2019.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimecki, O., and Singer, T. (2011). “Empathic distress fatigue rather than compassion fatigue? Integrating findings from empathy research in psychology and social neuroscience,” in Pathological Altruism, eds B. Oakley, A. Knafo, G. Madhavan, and D. S. Wilson (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 368–383. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199738571.003.0253

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., and Decety, J. (2007a). The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 42–58. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamm, C., Nusbaum, H. C., Meltzoff, A. N., and Decety, J. (2007b). What are you feeling? Using functional magnetic resonance imaging to assess the modulation of sensory and affective responses during empathy for pain. PLoS ONE 2:e1292. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neff, K. D. (2003). Self-compassion: an alternative conceptualization of a healthy attitude toward oneself. Self Identity 2, 85–101. doi: 10.1080/15298860309032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neff, K. D., Knox, M. C., Long, P., and Gregory, K. (2020). Caring for others without losing yourself: an adaptation of the mindful self-compassion program for healthcare communities. J. Clin. Psychol. 76, 1543–1562. doi: 10.1002/jclp.23007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stamm, B. H. (2010). The Concise ProQOL Manual. Procatello, ID. Available online at: https://proqol.org/ (accessed April 20, 2021).

Strauss, C., Lever Taylor, B., Gu, J., Kuyken, W., Baer, R., Jones, F., et al. (2016). What is compassion and how can we measure it? A review of definitions and measures. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 47, 15–27. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Ren, Z., Jiang, G., Hazer-Rau, D., Zhao, C., Shi, C., et al. (2021). Self-oriented empathy and compassion fatigue: the serial mediation of dispositional mindfulness and counselor's self-efficacy. Front. Psychol. 11:613908. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.613908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: compassion fatigue, Interpersonal Reactivity Index, self-oriented empathy, personal distress, empathy

Citation: Chapman HR (2021) Commentary: Self-Oriented Empathy and Compassion Fatigue: The Serial Mediation of Dispositional Mindfulness and Counselor's Self-Efficacy. Front. Psychol. 12:665632. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.665632

Received: 08 February 2021; Accepted: 21 April 2021;
Published: 17 May 2021.

Edited by:

Federica Galli, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Reviewed by:

Gil Goldzweig, Academic College Tel Aviv-Yaffo, Israel

Copyright © 2021 Chapman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Helen R. Chapman, hchapman@lincoln.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.