Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 08 April 2021
Sec. Organizational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Impact of AI-Enabled Technologies in E-commerce and Omnichannel Retailing. View all 5 articles

Smart Speaker Recommendations: Impact of Gender Congruence and Amount of Information on Users' Engagement and Choice

  • 1Departamento de Financiación e investigación Comercial: UDI de Marketing, Autonomous University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
  • 2Departamento de Marketing, Operações e Gestão Geral, Instituto Universitário de Lisboa (ISCTE) Business School, University Institute of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal
  • 3Departamento de Dirección de Marketing e Investigación de Mercados Área de Comercialización e Investigación de Mercados, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain

The relevance of smart speakers is steadily increasing, allowing users perform several daily tasks. From a commercial perspective, smart speakers also provide recommendations of products and services that may influence the consumer decision-making process. However, previous studies have mainly focused on the adoption of smart speakers, but there is a lack of proper guidelines that help design the way these devices should offer their consumption recommendations. Based on a stimulus-organism-response approach, we analyze how two features of smart speakers' recommendations (the gender congruence between the customer and the speaker, and the length of the message) influence on the effectiveness of such recommendations (i.e., visiting intentions) through its impact on user engagement and attitude. Data was collected from a sample of undergrad students in Spain using an experiment design that focused on a restaurant recommendation, and analyzed using partial least squares. On the one hand, our results suggests that gender congruence generates user engagement with the smart speaker. On the other hand, message length is positively related to attitudes towards the restaurant, at a declining rate. In addition, while better attitudes lead to higher visiting intentions, the influence of engagement on visiting intentions is partially mediated via attitudes. Thus, our findings contribute to understand the antecedents of users' engagement with smart speakers, as well as its impact on the customers' willingness to follow smart speakers' recommendations, constituting a base to analyze the impact of artificial intelligence solutions aimed to smooth the transitions of a customer through the stages of purchase process.

Introduction

Smart speakers help individuals to perform a range of simple tasks in everyday life, such as reporting the weather forecast, switching off the light, or playing music. They can also recommend stores, products, services, etc., fitting customer requirements. These devices based on artificial intelligence have the ability to interact and converse with humans (e.g., Belanche et al., 2020). Specifically, smart speakers offer several applications in the hospitality industry, such ordering room service (e.g., Marriot use of Echo devices in select hotel rooms), booking a rental car or hotel, tracking flight prices and status or suggesting nearby restaurants (Hornick and Santhanam, 2018). With a 20% expected annual growth rate and projected sales of more than 500 million units worldwide in 2024 (Wadhwani and Gankar, 2018), the potential influence of smart speakers recommendations is huge. However, customers cannot process voice-based information as efficiently as visual or even text-based information, mostly because of a lack of accuracy and user-system interaction (Lee and Pan, 2010), so smart speakers need to offer engaging recommendations that generate favorable attitudes toward the recommended product or service as well as purchase or visiting intentions.

Past research has not analyzed yet how the engagement process evolves between humans and smart speakers; particularly, how such engagement influences the customer attitude and intention toward the recommendation of a store, brand, or firm (Loureiro et al., in press). Smart speaker developers lack proper guidelines helping them design the way these devices should offer their recommendations to customers. This research aims to fill this gap. Specifically, we analyze how two features of smart speaker recommendations (namely gender congruence between customer and speaker, and amount of information in the message) influence the effectiveness of such recommendations.

To do so, we follow the S(Stimuli)-O(Organism)-R(response) framework, which we apply to restaurant recommendations. The stimuli (attributes or features) comprise drivers affecting users' cognitive and emotional states (organism), whose responses may be either approach or avoidance (responses) (Roschk et al., 2017). For this research, the stimulus includes the smart speaker recommendation features. The organism comprises user engagement with the smart speaker and attitude toward the recommended restaurant. The response is the approach, that is, the visiting intention (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Theoretical framework.

Regarding the recommendation, we consider two important features: the amount of information provided by the smart speaker and the gender congruence between the smart speaker and the customer. The amount of information is defined as how much information is provided in the smart speaker recommendation message. This aspect is very relevant in information processing (Bagozzi et al., 2016). Gender congruence occurs when the gender of the information source (the smart speaker in this case) is the same as that of the customer. The influence of an information source may differ depending on the gender of both the information source and the receiver of the message (e.g., Casaló and Escario, 2016).

Customer engagement with a smart speaker captures customer interactions with a firm through this device (Bilro and Loureiro, 2020). Customer engagement as “a psychological state that occurs by virtue of interactive, co-creative customer experiences with a focal agent object” (Brodie et al., 2011), such as a smart speaker. Hollebeek et al. (2014) note that, during interaction between the customer and the firm – represented here by the smart speaker – customer engagement embraces cognitive, emotional, and behavioral activities (paying attention to the interaction, having feelings toward the firm through the smart speaker, and actually using the device). Engaged customers tend to feel satisfaction (Brodie et al., 2013; Fehrer et al., 2018), become loyal (Hollebeek, 2011; Dwivedi, 2015), and say positive things about the firm (Vivek et al., 2012; Hollebeek et al., 2014; McLean and Wilson, 2019).

Customer attitudes are one of the main outcomes generated by product/service recommendations (Casaló et al., 2015a; Ruiz-Equihua et al., 2020). Attitudes capture the assessments that customers make regarding a behavior (Wu and Chen, 2005) – visiting a recommended restaurant in this case. Customer attitudes arising from a recommendation determine their intention to follow such recommendation. According to the Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen, 1991), behavioral intentions reflect a person's willingness to perform a specific behavior. Therefore, the intention to visit the recommended restaurant may be a good indicator of the actual customer behavior as behavioral intentions imply that customers will likely behave in a specific way (McKnight et al., 2002).

Consistent with the S-O-R framework, we first propose that amount of information and gender congruence influence both customer engagement with the smart speaker and attitudes toward the recommended restaurant.

The amount of information provided by the smart speaker may be perceived as a sign of its interactivity, which may cause greater engagement (Sundar, 2007; Blasco-Arcas et al., 2013; Liao et al., 2019). In addition, customers may pay more cognitive attention to messages with a higher amount of information, resulting in a greater engagement with the device. Longer messages recommending products are considered more useful (Yang et al., 2017) and generate more sales (Chevalier and Mayzlin, 2006) than shorter ones. Therefore, we expect that the more information reinforcing a purchase decision, the more favorable the attitude toward the recommended product will be. We expect a similar effect on customer engagement, as the information delivered by the smart speaker aims to develop attention and affection, and trigger an interaction with the smart speaker (Hollebeek et al., 2014, 2019). However, the elaborateness of the information generated by firms does not produce a positive effect on customers if it is too short or too long (Hernández-Ortega et al., 2020). Therefore, we expect that this will occur at a declining rate, even possibly reaching a saturation point (Kim et al., 2001).

H1: Amount of information is positively associated with customer engagement with the smart speaker at a declining rate.

H2: Amount of information is positively associated with customer attitudes toward the recommended restaurant at a declining rate.

Previous research indicates that the influence of an information source depends on its similarity with the message receiver (Casaló and Escario, 2016). Specifically, customers perceiving a higher similarity with the information source (i.e., the smart speaker) tend to consider that the source's opinions are congruent with their own personal values (Casaló et al., 2011), being influenced more easily by such opinions. Particularly, gender congruence between the message sender and receiver generates more positive perceptions about the sender (Jordán, 2015); for example, a higher trust, leading customers to engage with the congruent source. Gender congruence also serves to internalize the transmitted message (Casaló et al., 2013), provoking a more favorable attitude toward it. Thus, we expect that:

H3: Gender congruence is positively associated with customer engagement with the smart speaker.

H4: Gender congruence is positively associated with customer attitude toward the recommended restaurant.

Customer engagement and attitude toward the recommended restaurant represent the organism of the S-O-R framework. Customers engaged with a smart speaker will think a lot about the information provided by the smart speaker, will spend a lot of time in their interactive activity, and will be happy and proud to use the smart speaker (Hollebeek et al., 2014). Since engaged individuals are more likely to develop more favorable attitudes (Vivek et al., 2012), we argue that these engaged customers are expected to have a favorable assessment (Wu and Chen, 2005) toward the recommended restaurant, leading to the following hypothesis:

H5: Customer engagement with the smart speaker is positively associated with customer attitude toward the recommended restaurant.

As the S-O-R framework explains, an organism creates a response. Traditionally, response is associated with behavioral intentions (Roschk et al., 2017). Prior studies have considered behavior intentions (representing the willingness to visit, re-purchase, or recommend to others) as outcomes of engagement (e.g., Hollebeek, 2011; Vivek et al., 2012) and of favorable attitudes (Casaló et al., 2015a; Ruiz-Equihua et al., 2020). While engagement implies a strong psychological connection that may lead to positive outcomes such as behavioral intentions, attitude plays a relevant role in forming consumer preferences (Moriuchi, 2019). In this vein, we propose that customer engagement with the smart speaker and attitude toward the recommended restaurant influence customer visiting intentions:

H6: Customer engagement with the smart speaker is positively associated with the intention to visit the recommended restaurant.

H7: Customer attitudes toward the recommended restaurant are positively associated with the intention to visit the recommended restaurant.

For the sake of completeness, we include two control variables in our model: customer gender and expertise with smart speakers: women and men process information in a different way (Venkatesh and Morris, 2000); experience with the smart speaker makes customers more familiar with and more knowledgeable about the smart speaker (Sun and Zhang, 2006). Therefore, gender and experience may influence customers' beliefs, evaluations, and intentions.

Methods

We tested our hypotheses about the effect of smart speaker recommendations in customer behaviors using a 3 (short, medium and large amount of information) × 2 (male smart speaker voice vs. female smart speaker voice) experimental design. Respondents include undergraduate “Business Administration” students from Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, second and fourth year, who participated in the study during March 2020 (before COVID-19 mobility restrictions), in exchange for course credits (n = 270; Table 1 contains sample demographics). We presented them a situation where they got a new smart speaker; after installing it and familiarizing themselves with its functions, they asked for a recommendation for dinner at a downtown restaurant. First, we manipulated the amount of information through three restaurant recommendations of low, medium and high duration. The shortest message included three basic attributes to reach the restaurant (name, address, and schedule), lasting 4 s. The longest message contained nine attributes (adding rating, price, cuisine type, and extra features, namely wi-fi, credit card accepted, and accessibility), lasting 20 s and providing a high amount of information without overloading respondents (Lee and Lee, 2004). Between both anchors, the intermediate message included six attributes (no extra features), lasting 12 s. Second, we manipulated gender congruence by modifying the voice of the smart speaker (female vs. male). We randomly assigned respondents to each condition and asked them to answer a survey containing questions about their engagement with smart speakers, attitudes toward the restaurant, and visiting intentions. We also included realism and manipulation check questions. We guaranteed participant anonymity and induced a psychological separation between our variables by including questions not related to the research goals to avoid common method bias problems (Podsakoff et al., 2003). The questionnaire was implemented in Qualtrics and self-administered by participants.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample profile.

Measures

We employed scales from prior research in our study for engagement, customer attitude, visiting intentions, and experience with smart speakers (Wu and Chen, 2005; Hollebeek et al., 2014; Casaló et al., 2015b; Matzler et al., 2016). We adapted these scales to our research context, measured amount of information and customer gender congruence using single-item scales. Respondents also reported their gender.

We measured all our variables as first-order construct, except customer engagement with the smart speaker. Consistent with its three-dimensional conceptualization, which includes cognitive processing, affection and activation (Hollebeek et al., 2014) we measured it as a type II reflective-formative second-order construct (Ringle et al., 2012). Respondents assessed the first-order constructs of our research using seven-point Likert-type scales, except for gender (male/female/prefer not to answer).

Results

We estimate our model using partial least squares (PLS). First, PLS is an appropriate method to develop theories in exploratory research, as in our case. Second, PLS can properly handle sample size such as the one in our study. Third, PLS can properly estimate type II constructs, such as customer engagement with the smart speaker in our research. Given that customer engagement with the smart speaker is an endogenous construct in our research, we estimate our model using a two-stage approach (Ringle et al., 2012).

Manipulation Check and Common Method Bias Assessment

We measured scenarios' realism and credibility using the following items taken from Bagozzi et al. (2016): “the scenario is realistic,” “the scenario is credible,” “how likely is it that the smart speaker would give you advice like the one that you hear here?” (all items were measured using a 7-point Likert scale). The items provided a reliable measure of realism and credibility (Cronbach's α = 0.71), which were computed as the average of the three items. The results confirmed the suitability of the scenarios. The mean of the measure was 5.45 (standard deviation = 1.11, hereafter, SD), significantly >4 – the central point of the scale (t = 80.66, p < 0.01).

Additionally, we tested the manipulation of the amount of information using one item: “the quantity of information provided by the smart speaker is …,” with answers ranging from 1, “insufficient” to 7, “excessive.” The scenarios obtained a mean of 4.25 (SD = 0.98), 4.91 (SD = 0.83), and 5.24 (SD = 0.94) for the low, medium, and high information conditions, respectively. The respondents perceived the information scenarios as different (tlow−medium = −4.85, p < 0.01; tmedium−high = −2.48, p < 0.05; tlow−high = −6.94, p < 0.01), hence confirming a successful manipulation.

We also assessed the manipulation of gender congruence. The item used was “smart speaker gender is …” with answers ranging from 1, “different from mine” to 7, “equal to mine.” Respondents reported a higher congruence when exposed to a voice corresponding to their gender than when not exposed (Mmales−different = 2.43, SD = 1.90, vs. Mmales−same = 4.73, SD = 2.47; Mfemales−different = 1.55, SD = 1.28, vs. Mfemales−same = 5.18, SD = 2.37), being such differences significant (tmales = 5.97, p < 0.01; tfemales = 10.92, p < 0.01). Therefore, our respondents successfully perceived our gender congruence manipulation.

Finally, we assessed whether common method bias is a problem in our study by evaluating variance inflation factors (Kock and Lynn, 2012). They all range between 1.01 and 2.24, below the recommended 3.3 threshold.

Measurement Model

We first evaluated the reliability and convergent validity of the first order construct in our model (Table 2). Cronbach's alpha ranges between 0.79 and 0.94, above the 0.7 ordinary threshold value (Nunnally, 1978). Composite reliability oscillates between 0.88 and 0.95. The loadings of constructs indicators are all above 0.7, supporting then indicator reliability. The average variance extracted (AVE) varies between 0.69 and 0.84, hence higher than the cut-off value of 0.5 suggested by Fornell and Larcker (1981).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Measurement items.

We next evaluated the discriminant validity of our variables using three criteria. We first evaluated whether the loadings of each indicator are higher for its assigned variable than for other variables. Second, we apply the Fornell and Larcker (1981) criterion. We checked whether the square root of the latent variables' AVE is higher that their correlations with other variables (Table 3). Finally, we computed the heterotrait-monotrait ratio (HTMT) of the correlations and checked whether they are lower than 0.85 to support discriminant validity (Clark and Watson, 1995; Kline, 2011). The cross-loadings of our items, the Fornell and Larcker's (1981) criterion, and the HTMT values support the discriminant validity of our variables.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Discriminant validity analysis.

After evaluating the measurement model of our first-order constructs, we re-estimated the model incorporating the latent scores of cognitive, affective, and activation as customer engagement indicators. Subsequently, we assessed the measurement model of customer engagement with the smart speaker through the significance of its indicators (calculated employing a non-parametric bootstrapping procedure with 10,000 subsamples, no sign change). They are all significant at a 95% level. Their variance inflation factors vary between 1.16 and 1.50, ensuring the required lack of collinearity in the measurement of customer engagement (Diamantopoulos and Winklhofer, 2001).

Structural Model

Regarding our structural model, we first assessed the global fit of the model through its standardized root mean residual (SRMR). The SRMR of our model is 0.05, below 0.08, thus indicating an adequate global fit (Hu and Bentler, 1998). Subsequently, we assessed how our model explains each endogenous variables through adjusted-R2. These are for 0.13 engagement,0.19 for attitude, and 0.51 for visiting intentions, showing a medium fit for visiting intentions, and small fit for attitude and engagement (Chin, 1998). Next, the Q2 values for customer engagement (0.05), attitude (0.13), and visiting intentions (0.41) indicate medium predictive relevance for engagement and attitude, and high for visiting intentions (Hair et al., 2017).

Finally, we calculated the significance of our path estimates. We employed a non-parametric bias corrected and accelerated bootstrapping procedure with 10,000 sub-samples, no sign change. We showed our path estimates, t estimates and p-values in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Structural relationships.

H1 proposes that the amount of information has a positive effect on customer engagement, at a declining rate. Neither the linear path estimate (0.06; p-value: 0.28) nor the quadratic path estimate (−0.00; p-value: 0.83) that capture the effect of amount of information on engagement are significant. Therefore, we reject H1. The length of the recommendation provided by the smart speaker does not influence the engagement of the customer with this device.

H2 posits that the amount of information has a positive effect on customer attitude, at a declining rate. Both the linear path estimate (0.27; p-value: <0.01) and the quadratic path estimate (−0.09; p-value: 0.02) that capture the effect of amount of information on customer attitude are significant. Hence, our results support H2. Particularly, our results indicate that the more information provided to the customer, the higher the attitude toward the recommended restaurant. The negative sign of the quadratic effect captures the declining impact of the amount of information; the amount of information provokes better attitudes, but this effect is lower as the amount of information grows.

H3 suggests that gender congruence positively influences customer engagement with the smart speaker. Our results support H3 only at a 90% level (0.09; p-value: 0.09). Individuals tend to engage more with smart speakers when they consider that the smart speaker has the same gender as them.

H4 evaluates whether gender congruence positively influences attitude toward the recommended restaurant. According to our results, this influence does not exist (−0.02; p-value: 0.59). We reject H4. The gender of the smart speaker voice does not influence attitude toward the recommended restaurant.

H5 indicates that customer engagement with the smart speaker is positively associated with attitude toward the recommended restaurant. Our results support H5 (0.31; p-value: <0.01). The more engaged with the smart speaker the customer is, the better attitude the smart speaker recommendation generates.

According to H6, customer engagement with the smart speaker increases the intention to visit the recommended restaurant. Our results support H6 at a 90% level (0.09; p-value: 0.06). Customer engagement with the smart speaker has a direct influence on intention to follow the recommendation made by the device.

Finally, H7 proposes that attitudes toward the restaurant are positively associated with the intention to visit the recommended restaurant. Our results support H7 (0.68; p-value: <0.01). The higher the attitudes toward the recommended restaurant, the higher the intention to visit it.

Together, H5 and H7 suggest that engagement might also indirectly influence intention, mediated by attitudes. This indirect effect is indeed significant (0.21; p-value: <0.01), according to our bootstrapping procedure. Thus, the total effect of engagement on intention is also significant (0.30; p-value: <0.01).

Discussion and Implications

Following the well-established S-O-R model (e.g., Mehrabian and Russell, 1974; Donovan and Rossiter, 1982), this research studies how smart speaker features, namely the amount of information and gender congruence with the consumer, influence intention to follow the recommendation due to their impact on customer engagement with the smart speaker and on attitude toward the recommended restaurant. Aligned with what previous studies suggest (Sundar, 2007; Blasco-Arcas et al., 2013; Liao et al., 2019), our results indicate that gender congruence generates user engagement with the smart speaker. Amount of information is positively related with attitude toward the restaurant, at a declining rate, that is, this impact is lower as the amount of information increases. Better attitude leads to higher visiting intention. In contrast, the influence of engagement on visiting intention is mainly indirect, via attitude. These findings have important theoretical and practical implications.

From a theoretical perspective, this research contributes to previous literature in two ways. First, while amount of information seems to be related to attitude toward the recommended product (with a declining impact, similar to previous research findings, e.g., Hernández-Ortega et al., 2020), gender congruence is more related to engagement with the smart speaker. The reason behind this finding may be based on the key elements of communication (Chandler, 1994). The amount of information represents a characteristic of the message transmitted. In contrast, gender congruence depends on the characteristics of the sender and the receiver of the message. Therefore, the amount of information affects more a variable related to the content of the message (attitude toward the recommended product), and gender congruence affects more a variable related to the sender of the message (engagement with the smart speaker). The influence of congruence could be explained by the similarity between the customer and the smart speaker, which may derive in a greater consumer identification with the sender of the message (e.g., Schouten et al., 2020).

Second, engagement with the smart speaker may serve to develop affective feelings –and even behavioral intentions– toward the products/services recommended by the smart speaker. Specifically, the influence of engagement on visiting intentions is partially mediated by attitude. This result complements previous literature, which has mostly analyzed customer engagement with brands within technological environments (e.g., Hollebeek et al., 2019), suggesting that this engagement may have positive consequences for both the brand and the technology (e.g., McLean and Wilson, 2019; Bilro and Loureiro, 2020).

From a managerial perspective, smart speaker developers should be aware that they must ensure gender congruence with users. Users tend to be more engaged – meaning that they spend more time using the smart speaker, are happier and prouder, and gain interest regarding smart speakers – when their own gender fits the perceived voice of the smart speaker. Currently, the most popular smart speakers (e.g., Alexa, Siri, Cortana) do not allow their users selecting assistants' gender. Manufacturers should incorporate this feature. Additionally, the smart speaker should be able to capture the sensibility of users to give the amount of information to captivate users, rather than bore or irritate them. The intention to visit the restaurant will only be created in the user's mind if the smart speaker can develop a favorable attitude toward the restaurant, that is, when users think that visiting the restaurant is a good idea. Therefore, the learning process of the smart speaker should be such that it provides an adequate amount of information, and uses a voice that is congruent to the users to develop a sense of interaction and a favorable evaluation of the service provided.

Our study is not exempt from limitations, which constitute opportunities for further research. First, we have employed a convenience sample (i.e., students) in our study. Consequently, our findings circumscribe to our sample and cannot be generalized. Hence, further research could replicate our model employing representative samples. Similarly, we have just considered one recommended product/service, a restaurant. Generalizing our results would require testing our model with a variety of different products and services (e.g., high vs. low involvement, familiar vs. unfamiliar brands, etc.). The recommendation features could be also expanded by further research, thus providing a better comprehension of how message features influence individuals. This is especially relevant as the percentage of variance explained for attitudes and engagement, although adequate for studies that address novel topics such as ours, is not very high. For example, language style (e.g., figurative vs. literal) might influence the way in which consumers reacts to smart speaker recommendations (as in the case of online reviews; Wu et al., 2017). Additionally, we suggest exploring the concept of “coolness” (Warren et al., 2019) and whether or not a smart voice regarded as “cool” by users – due to the tone of voice and the kind of information provided – will generate more engagement and consequently reinforce the intention to visit a restaurant or other place. Further research could also focus on additional consumer characteristics, such as technology readiness (Belanche et al., 2020), to identify the potential drivers of engagement with smart speakers. Finally, we adopt a static perspective in our research. Adopting a dynamic perspective to study this phenomenon would allow understanding whether the effects of message features remain stable across time.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because they were gathered under no-distribution assurance. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to Daniel Ruiz-Equihua, ZGFuaWVsLnJ1aXplJiN4MDAwNDA7dWFtLmVz.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the study conception and design. Material preparation, data collection and analysis were prepared by LC, DR-E, and JR. The first draft of the manuscript was written by JR, and subsequently extended and refined by LC, SL, and DR-E. All the authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research benefited from the Professorship Excellence Program in accordance with the multi-year agreement signed by the Government of Madrid and the Autonomous University of Madrid (Line #3).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50, 179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagozzi, R. P., Belanche, D., Casaló, L. V., and Flavián, C. (2016). The role of anticipated emotions in purchase intentions. Psychol. Mark. 33, 629–645. doi: 10.1002/mar.20905

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belanche, D., Casaló, L. V., Flavián, C., and Schepers, J. (2020). Service robot implementation: a theoretical framework and research agenda. Serv. Ind. J. 40, 203–225. doi: 10.1080/02642069.2019.1672666

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bilro, R. G., and Loureiro, S. M. C. (2020). A consumer engagement systematic review: synthesis and research agenda. Spanish J. Mark. ESIC 24, 283–307. doi: 10.1108/SJME-01-2020-0021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blasco-Arcas, L., Buil, I., Hernandez-Ortega, B., and Sese, F. J. (2013). Using clickers in class. The role of interactivity, active collaborative learning and engagement in learning performance. Comput. Educ. 62, 102–110. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2012.10.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brodie, R. J., Hollebeek, L. D., Juric, B., and Ilic, A. (2011). Customer engagement: conceptual domain, fundamental propositions, and implications for research. J. Serv. Res. JSR 14, 252–271. doi: 10.1177/1094670511411703

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brodie, R. J., Ilic, A., Juric, B., and Hollebeek, L. D. (2013). Consumer engagement in a virtual brand community: an exploratory analysis. J. Bus. Res. 66, 105–114. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2011.07.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casaló, L. V., and Escario, J.-J. (2016). Intergenerational association of environmental concern: evidence of parents' and children's concern. J. Environ. Psychol. 48, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casaló, L. V., Flavián, C., and Guinalíu, M. (2011). Understanding the intention to follow the advice obtained in an online travel community. Comput. Hum. Behav. 27, 622–633. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.04.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casaló, L. V., Flavián, C., and Guinalíu, M. (2013). New members' integration: key factor of success in online travel communities. J. Bus. Res. 66, 706–710. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2011.09.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casaló, L. V., Flavián, C., Guinalíu, M., and Ekinci, Y. (2015a). Avoiding the dark side of positive online consumer reviews: enhancing reviews' usefulness for high risk-averse travelers. J. Bus. Res. 68, 1829–1835. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2015.01.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casaló, L. V., Flavián, C., Guinalíu, M., and Ekinci, Y. (2015b). Do online hotel rating schemes influence booking behaviors? Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 49, 28–36. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhm.2015.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chandler, D. (1994). The Transmission Model of Communication. Available online at: http://visual-memory.co.uk/daniel/Documents/short/trans.html (accessed December 22, 2020).

Google Scholar

Chevalier, J. A., and Mayzlin, D. (2006). The effect of word of mouth on sales: online book reviews. J. Mark. Res. 43, 345–354. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.43.3.345

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chin, W. W. (1998). “The partial least squares approach to structural equation modelling,” in Modern Methods for Business Research, ed G. A. Marcoulides (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 295–336.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Clark, L. A., and Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: basic issues in objective scale development. Psychol. Assess. 7:309. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.7.3.309

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamantopoulos, A., and Winklhofer, H. M. (2001). Index construction with formative indicators: an alternative to scale development. J. Mark. Res. 38, 269–277. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.38.2.269.18845

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donovan, R. J., and Rossiter, J. R. (1982). Store atmosphere: an environmental psychology approach. J. Retail. 58:34.

Google Scholar

Dwivedi, A. (2015). A higher-order model of consumer brand engagement and its impact on loyalty intentions. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 24, 100–109. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2015.02.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fehrer, J. A., Woratschek, H., Germelmann, C. C., and Brodie, R. J. (2018). Dynamics and drivers of customer engagement: within the dyad and beyond. J. Serv. Manag. 29, 443–467. doi: 10.1108/JOSM-08-2016-0236

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 18, 39–50. doi: 10.1177/002224378101800104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Hult, G. T. M., Ringle, C. M., and Sarstedt, M. (2017). A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM), 2nd Edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Hernández-Ortega, B., San Martín, H., Herrero, Á., and Franco, J. L. (2020). What, how and when? Exploring the influence of firm-generated content on popularity in a tourism destination context. J. Destin. Mark. Manag. 18:100504. doi: 10.1016/j.jdmm.2020.100504

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollebeek, L. D. (2011). Demystifying customer brand engagement: exploring the loyalty nexus. J. Mark. Manag. 27, 785–807. doi: 10.1080/0267257X.2010.500132

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollebeek, L. D., Glynn, M. S., and Brodie, R. J. (2014). Consumer brand engagement in social media: conceptualization, scale development and validation. J. Interact. Mark. 28, 149–165. doi: 10.1016/j.intmar.2013.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollebeek, L. D., Sprott, D. E., Andreassen, T. W., Costley, C., Klaus, P., Kuppelwieser, V., et al. (2019). Customer engagement in evolving technological environments: synopsis and guiding propositions. Eur. J. Mark. 53, 2018–2023. doi: 10.1108/EJM-09-2019-970

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hornick, S., and Santhanam, S. (2018). How smart speakers are poised to reinvent the travel industry. Harvard Business Review Digital Article, 2–5. Available online at: https://hbr.org/2018/08/how-smart-speakers-are-poised-to-reinvent-the-travel-industry (accessed January 1, 2021).

Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychol. Methods 3, 424–453. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.3.4.424

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordán, P. (2015). Un Estudio de la Confianza en el Líder de un Equipo de Trabajo virtual (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Zaragoza: University of Zaragoza.

Google Scholar

Kim, J. W., Lee, B. H., Shaw, M. J., Chang, H.-L., and Nelson, M. (2001). Application of decision-tree induction techniques to personalized advertisements on internet storefronts. Int. J. Electron. Commer. 5, 45–62. doi: 10.1080/10864415.2001.11044215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Kock, N., and Lynn, G. S. (2012). Lateral collinearity and misleading results in variance-based SEM: an illustration and recommendations. J. Assoc. Inf. Syst. 13, 546–580. doi: 10.17705/1jais.00302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, B., and Lee, W.-N. (2004). The effect of information overload on consumer choice quality in an on-line environment. Psychol. Mark. 21, 159–183. doi: 10.1002/mar.20000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, L.-S., and Pan, Y.-C. (2010). “Voice-based information retrieval - how far are we from the text-based information retrieval?” in Automatic Speech Recognition & Understanding (Moreno). doi: 10.1109/ASRU.2009.5372952

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liao, S. H., Chung, Y. C., and Chang, W. J. (2019). Interactivity, engagement, trust, purchase intention and word-of-mouth: a moderated mediation study. Int. J. Serv. Technol. Manag. 25, 116–137. doi: 10.1504/IJSTM.2019.098203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loureiro, S. M. C., Guerreiro, J., and Tussyadiah, I. (in press). Artificial intelligence in business: state of the art future research agenda. J. Bus. Res. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matzler, K., Strobl, A., Stokburger-Sauer, N., Bobovnicky, A., and Bauer, F. (2016). Brand personality and culture: the role of cultural differences on the impact of brand personality perceptions on tourists'; visit intentions. Tour. Manag. 52, 507–520. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2015.07.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKnight, D. H., Choudhury, V., and Kacmar, C. (2002). The impact of initial consumer trust on intentions to transact with a web site: a trust building model. J. Strateg. Inf. Syst. 11, 297–323. doi: 10.1016/S0963-8687(02)00020-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLean, G., and Wilson, A. (2019). Shopping in the digital world: examining customer engagement through augmented reality mobile applications. Comput. Human Behav. 101, 210–224. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehrabian, A., and Russell, J. A. (1974). An Approach to Environmental Psychology. Cambridge, MA, US: The MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Moriuchi, E. (2019). Okay, google!: an empirical study on voice assistants on consumer engagement and loyalty. Psychol. Mark. 36, 489–501. doi: 10.1002/mar.21192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunnally, J. C. J. C. (1978). Psychometric Theory, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ringle, C. M., Sarstedt, M., and Straub, D. (2012). Editor's comments: a critical look at the use of PLS-SEM. MIS Q. 36, iii–xiv. doi: 10.2307/41410402

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roschk, H., Loureiro, S. M. C., and Breitsohl, J. (2017). Calibrating 30 years of experimental research: a meta-analysis of the atmospheric effects of music, scent, and color. J. Retail. 93, 228–240. doi: 10.1016/j.jretai.2016.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruiz-Equihua, D., Romero, J., and Casaló, L. V. (2020). Better the devil you know? The moderating role of brand familiarity and indulgence vs. restraint cultural dimension on eWOM influence in the hospitality industry. J. Hosp. Mark. Manag. 29, 310–328. doi: 10.1080/19368623.2019.1630698

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schouten, A. P., Janssen, L., and Verspaget, M. (2020). Celebrity vs. Influencer endorsements in advertising: the role of identification, credibility, and Product-Endorser fit. Int. J. Advert. 39, 258–281. doi: 10.1080/02650487.2019.1634898

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, H., and Zhang, P. (2006). The role of moderating factors in user technology acceptance. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 64, 53–78. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2005.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundar, S. S. (2007). “Social psychology of interactivity in human-website interaction,” in The Oxford Handbook of Internet Psychology, eds A. N. Joinson, K. Y. A. McKenna, T. Postmes, and U.-D. Reips (Oxford, England: Oxford University Press), 89–102.

Google Scholar

Venkatesh, V., and Morris, M. G. (2000). Why don't men ever stop to ask for directions? Gender, social influence, and their role in technology acceptance and usage behavior. MIS Q. 24, 115–139. doi: 10.2307/3250981

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vivek, S. D., Beatty, S. E., and Morgan, R. M. (2012). Customer engagement: exploring customer relationships beyond purchase. J. Mark. Theory Pract. 20, 122–146. doi: 10.2753/MTP1069-6679200201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wadhwani, P., and Gankar, S. (2018). Smart Speaker Market Size By Intelligent Virtual Assistant (Alexa, Google Assistant, Siri, Cortana, Others), by Application (Personal, Professional, Commercial), Industry Analysis Report, Regional Outlook, Growth Potential, Competitive Market Share and Fore. Available online at: https://www.gminsights.com/industry-analysis/smart-speaker-market (accessed January 1, 2021).

Google Scholar

Warren, C., Batra, R., Loureiro, S. M. C., and Bagozzi, R. P. (2019). Brand coolness. J. Mark. 83, 36–56. doi: 10.1177/0022242919857698

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, I. L., and Chen, J. L. (2005). An extension of trust and TAM model with TPB in the initial adoption of on-line tax: an empirical study. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 62, 784–808. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2005.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, L., Shen, H., Fan, A., and Mattila, A. S. (2017). The impact of language style on consumers′ reactions to online reviews. Tour. Manag. 59, 590–596. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2016.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, S.-B., Shin, S.-H., Joun, Y., and Koo, C. (2017). Exploring the comparative importance of online hotel reviews' heuristic attributes in review helpfulness: a conjoint analysis approach. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 34, 963–985. doi: 10.1080/10548408.2016.1251872

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: smart speakers, visiting intentions, customer attitudes, customer engagement, gender congruence, amount of information

Citation: Romero J, Ruiz-Equihua D, Loureiro SMC and Casaló LV (2021) Smart Speaker Recommendations: Impact of Gender Congruence and Amount of Information on Users' Engagement and Choice. Front. Psychol. 12:659994. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.659994

Received: 28 January 2021; Accepted: 09 March 2021;
Published: 08 April 2021.

Edited by:

Monica Cortinas, Public University of Navarre, Spain

Reviewed by:

Ángel Herrero, University of Cantabria, Spain
Nuria Huete-Alcocer, University of Castilla-La Mancha, Spain
Eva Lahuerta-Otero, University of Salamanca, Spain

Copyright © 2021 Romero, Ruiz-Equihua, Loureiro and Casaló. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jaime Romero, amFpbWUucm9tZXJvJiN4MDAwNDA7dWFtLmVz

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.