- Department of Computer Science, The University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
Psychology defines personality as the stable traits of an individual, and cognitive research suggests that a set of core beliefs is at the root of these traits. From this perspective, two major questions remain unanswered: (1) What are the core beliefs that make up personality? (2) How are they acquired? An interesting answer is provided by attachment theory, according to which attachment is at the basis of personality. The current theoretical formulation, however, does not sufficiently clarify the relationship between the two. Adopting a cognitive-clinical approach, we put forward a novel version of attachment theory, arguing that it can better account for the relationship between attachment and personality, thereby providing more convincing answers to questions (1) and (2). In particular, we propose that: (A) attachment information is acquired over seven dimensions; (B) the acquisition of each dimension is induced by a specific caregiving feature and (C) realized through a specific acquisition mechanism – imprinting. In a nutshell, we propose an Attachment-Personality Model (APM) according to which seven attachment dimensions constitute the knowledge core of personality. We finally discuss the significant implications of the model, especially its clinical application in terms of conception, assessment, and treatment of mental disorders. The model can be empirically tested, and we suggest three ways to do that.
Introduction
Psychologists usually recognize the significant explanatory power of attachment theory (Cassidy and Shaver, 1999, 2008, 2016) and the clinical effectiveness of cognitive psychotherapy (Beck, 2011; Beck and Haigh, 2014). We discuss here the intimate connection between these two areas of research and practice and use this connection as a basis to construct a novel model of personality.
Personality can be defined as the fairly stable psychological characteristics of an individual (McCrae and Costa, 2003; Corr and Matthews, 2009; Engler, 2013; Beck et al., 2015; Friedman and Schustack, 2015), which, according to a cognitive approach, are critically determined by the individual’s core knowledge – a set of core beliefs (Young, 2002; Dweck, 2008; Perdighe and Mancini, 2010; Beck et al., 2015; Osmo et al., 2018). This perspective, of course, raises two questions: (1) What are these core beliefs? (2) How are they acquired? With this respect, a particularly interesting aspect of attachment theory concerns the relationship between attachment and personality (Kobak, 1994; Carver, 1997; Hart, 2008; Lorenzini and Fonagy, 2013; Chiesa et al., 2017). As to question (2), the theory proposes that the core beliefs that make up one’s personality are formed through the relationship with one’s caregiver during childhood (Main et al., 1985; Platts et al., 2002; Sherman et al., 2015; Hesse, 2016; Marvin et al., 2016). Furthermore, according to the theory, this core knowledge involves three fundamental dimensions – disorganization, avoidance, and ambivalence (Bowlby, 1969/1982, 1973, 1980; Liotti, 2009; Paetzold et al., 2015; Feeney, 2016; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2016; Thompson, 2016). For example, the child who has a relationship with a cold caregiver develops the belief of being an unlovable person, while the child who has a relationship with a warm caregiver develops the belief of being a lovable person. These different beliefs are implicit, non-verbal, knowledge, which correspond to the different internal states the child can experience while relating to their caregiver in the avoidant dimension.
In this article, we argue that attachment-based approaches to personality are on the right track but need to be further expanded and elaborated upon. We propose four such amendments. First, we show that the nature of two of the three attachment dimensions described by attachment theory – avoidance and ambivalence – needs to be better specified. Second, we argue that four additional dimensions – phobicity, depressivity, somaticity, and obsessivity – are required to explain personality. Third, we propose a novel way to identify the caregiving features that induce the acquisition of each attachment dimension. Finally, we put forward an account of the specific mechanism through which attachment information is acquired: we argue that it is imprinting – a learning process evolutionarily preordained to be first performed in early sensitive periods – that gives attachment dimensions the quality of stable personality traits. In a nutshell, we propose a novel attachment-based model of personality, according to which the core knowledge underlying personality is made up by seven pieces of information imprinted through attachment interactions in response to certain caregiving features. We call this seven-dimensional Attachment Personality Model ‘APM.’
We will proceed as follows. In section one, we present the standard attachment view of personality. In section two, we illustrate how, despite its explanatory power, this view faces three major problems concerning personality, which involve: (1) stability, (2) intergenerational transmission, and (3) psychopathology. In section three, we argue that these three major problems faced by attachment theory can be solved if we supplement the theory by considering – overall – seven attachment dimensions that are induced by seven caregiving features and (unconsciously) acquired through imprinting. Importantly, we present five evidence-based arguments in favor of imprinting as the specific attachment acquisition mechanism. Finally, in section four, we discuss the implications and limitations of the proposed model.
Three caveats before we get started. Personality is a complex and controversial concept (Engler, 2013; Friedman and Schustack, 2015). We focus on the aspects of personality concerning acquired knowledge not because we think that this body of knowledge exhausts the nature of personality—we are well aware that genetical, biological, environmental, social, and cultural factors play a fundamental role in the constitution of personality—but simply because we think that core beliefs are a central part of personality and call for an explanation. In particular, by focusing on the acquired information underlying personality, our model can partially explain some of the differences in personality that do not arise from biological variables. We will therefore consider the influence of acquired information assuming a constant healthy biological substrate. Moreover, we do not suggest attachment knowledge to be the only acquired information that constitutes personality, but rather that it has a central role, given the early acquisition and crucial adaptation value.
Second, even though we situate our discussion at the interface of attachment theory and cognitive psychology, we consider our considerations completely compatible with psychoanalysis and most other clinical approaches. Finally, this article is mainly theoretical in scope: its aim is to develop a novel model which hasn’t be directly tested yet. Importantly, however, we intend to show that our proposal is indirectly supported by a large amount of evidence gathered from a variety of fields and is empirically productive, in that it generates a number of testable hypotheses.
Attachment Theory
In this section, we outline the main features of attachment theory – the standard version of it, as it is currently broadly accepted – and its view of personality.
Bowlby (1969/1982, 1973, 1980), father of attachment theory, maintained that attachment is the fundamental evolutionary mechanism that every human being is provided with – ”from the cradle to the grave” (Bowlby, 1969/1982, p. 208) – to obtain protection and care from a conspecific – a caregiver. For several years after birth, children need to be protected and cared for to survive and develop, and they are therefore inherently motivated to attach to a caregiver. Most often, the mother plays this fundamental role. However, despite the primacy of this early relationship, attachment remains prominent throughout life, and new attachment relationships with similar characteristics can be created at any age (Bowlby, 1969/1982; Allen, 2008; Kerns and Brumariu, 2016; Marvin et al., 2016; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2016; Fraley and Roisman, 2019). For example, when feeling vulnerable, an adult will look for protection and care from their partner as a child does from their caregiver, although usually in a different form. While the child cries and clings, the adult might call their loved one and relate a difficult day at work looking for sympathy.
According to attachment theorists, a fundamental aspect related to the formation of attachment relationships is the knowledge acquired through such relationships. This acquired knowledge—attachment theory also says—is a fundamental part of personality. Let us consider these two points in turn.
Attachment-Knowledge and Internal Working Models
Attachment knowledge has been described through the concept of Internal Working Models (IWMs), in which specific representations of the caregiver and the self—and hence of the attachment-caregiving relationship—are generated and stored (Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton and Munholland, 2008; Sherman et al., 2015; Marvin et al., 2016). We consider here these models as a whole and refer to them as the IWM. Attachment representations are taken to consists of three dimensions: disorganization, avoidance, and ambivalence (Bartholomew and Horowitz, 1991; Brennan et al., 1998; Fraley and Spieker, 2003; Liotti and Farina, 2011; Fraley et al., 2015; Paetzold et al., 2015; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2016). We call them ‘α-dimensions.’ All dimensions are acquired by each individual in different degrees. They are generally considered reciprocally (statistically) independent, although some correlation between avoidance and ambivalence has been found measuring adult attachment (Cameron et al., 2012; Fraley et al., 2015). An individual who is neither avoidant nor ambivalent is referred to as being secure. Otherwise stated, who is either avoidant or ambivalent is considered to be insecurely attached.
Attachment information is primarily represented at an implicit, non-verbal, level (Bowlby, 1969/1982; Schore, 2000), and can therefore be assessed only indirectly. Classical ways to do that are the Strange Situation Procedure (SSP) (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Main and Solomon, 1990) – a lab procedure to measure attachment behaviors in infants – and the Adult attachment interview (AAI) (George et al., 1985; Main and Hesse, 1990; Hesse, 2016) – a structured interview to evaluate the state of mind with regards to attachment in adults. Through these instruments, four different attachment styles – disorganized, secure, avoidant, and ambivalent – can be measured. We want to point out that we will use here the term ‘style’ in a proprietary way, focusing on attachment representations and dimensions. Attachment theory is complex, and researchers with different orientations tend to use different terms, which imply their focus on different aspects of attachment. In particular, attachment ‘pattern’ and ‘state of mind’ traditionally belong to the developmental area and ‘style’ to the social one. It is also worth noting that instruments developed in different areas, such as the AAI and adult attachment questionnaires, have shown minimal correlation (Crowell et al., 2016). These instruments differ in many respects, but the AAI – originally developed as a categorical tool – has then been found to be underpinned by dimensions (Roisman et al., 2007), like the questionnaires, thereby further supporting the dimensional perspective endorsed by this work.
Given the implicit nature of the fundamental attachment knowledge, its identification can be performed only indirectly and can be synthetized as follows. Disorganization is a representation of the caregiver’s frightfulness. In this case, the IWM represents the caregiver as being both a source of protection and a threat for the child, thereby leading to disorganized behaviors (Main and Solomon, 1990; Liotti, 2004, 2011; Lyons-Ruth and Jacobvitz, 2016). Avoidance is a representation of the caregiver’s loving attitude toward the child and their sensitivity to the child’s emotional life, while ambivalence represents the caregiver’s availability and reliability (Ainsworth et al., 1978; George et al., 1985; Hesse, 2008; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2016). As a result, an avoidant child expects their caregiver not to be interested in their inner life, and an ambivalent child expects their caregiver not to be there for them in case of need. Notably, each attachment style is primarily characterized by (implicit) information, which manifests itself in both behaviors and internal states. Given that an individual can appear to be more or less disorganized, avoidant, or ambivalent, the stored information has a clear continuous nature.
Attachment researchers agree that the represented α-dimensions crucially depend on the relation between the child and the caregiver. In particular, an insecure child’s representation of their caregiver is taken to be strongly related to the caregiver’s sensitivity – defined as how adequately and promptly the caregiver detects and satisfies the child’s needs (Ainsworth et al., 1978; De Wolff and van Ijzendoorn, 1997). Given our purposes, a crucial aspect for the information stored in the IWM is that it appears to be present across the lifespan and to manifest itself in one’s adult relations (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Parkes et al., 1993; Green et al., 2000; Fraley and Shaver, 2008; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2016). In other words, this information is the latent foundational part of the attachment styles that manifest themselves at any age (Ammaniti et al., 2000; Target et al., 2003; Stievenart et al., 2012). An avoidant adult tends to be dismissing and detached in their romantic relationships as much as an avoidant child tends to be over-autonomous and unemotional with their caregiver.
We will argue that attachment knowledge is not only present during the entire course of life but also stable, by virtue of its specific acquisition mechanism – imprinting. Bowlby (1969/1982) first indicated that the attachment of a child to their caregiver is realized through imprinting in an early sensitive period of life – that usually ends within the sixth month. After attaching to a specific caregiver, the child needs to acquire the information to build their IWM and dimensions. Such information – that allows them to best adapt to their caregiver – needs to be acquired within the first 24 months, otherwise they will suffer from terrible psychological dysfunctions (Marvin et al., 2016; Troller-Renfree and Fox, 2017). The period between 6 and 24 months can therefore be considered as sensitive for the acquisition of the α-dimensions – disorganization, avoidance, and ambivalence.
The fact that attachment knowledge persists across one’s life brings us to the second issue: the relation between attachment and personality.
Attachment-Knowledge and Personality
“From the viewpoint of the position adopted, adult personality is seen as a product of an individual’s interactions with key figures during all his years of immaturity, especially of his interactions with attachment figures.” (Bowlby, 1973, p. 208)
Although usually attachment theorists do not define personality explicitly, they recognize the strict relationship between attachment and personality, which have appeared evident since the foundation of the theory and have been confirmed by numerous studies (Bowlby, 1969/1982, 1973, 1980; Guidano, 1987, 1991; Noftle and Shaver, 2006; Guidano, 2007; Chopik et al., 2013; Levy et al., 2015; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2016; Karterud and Kongerslev, 2019; Rosa-Mendes et al., 2019; Young et al., 2019). Such relationship is so tight that attachment styles are often implicitly taken as being themselves personality characteristics (Bowlby, 1973; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2012).
At least four areas of attachment research touch on this subject. (1) The quality of early attachment has been found to have profound developmental consequences, for example, in terms of emotional regulation, self-esteem, resilience, social attitude, and competence (Sroufe, 2005; Thompson, 2016). In particular, emotional regulation appears to be directly related to attachment styles (Viddal et al., 2017; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2019). (2) In their romantic relationships, secure, avoidant, and ambivalent adults adopt specific strategies, being the secure considerably more adaptive compared to insecure ones – in terms of both personal satisfaction and relationship functioning (Fraley et al., 2011; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2016). (3) A great number of studies demonstrates the link between disorganized/insecure attachment and psychopathology, in particular personality disorders (Lorenzini and Fonagy, 2013; Stovall-McClough and Dozier, 2016; Chiesa et al., 2017). For example, convincing arguments and evidence show the causal role of early disorganization of attachment in the development of a borderline personality disorder (Liotti and Farina, 2011). (4) Finally, attachment styles are connected to mating strategies, which strongly characterize adult behavior (Chisholm, 1996; Simpson and Belsky, 2016).
The connection between attachment and personality emerges with particular clarity from the TAM (Temperament-Attachment-Mentalizing) model of personality elaborated by Karterud and Kongerslev (2019). These authors effectively synthesize the complexity of personality by identifying in T, A, and M the three major components of personality. Temperament is the innate biological basis of personality, attachment is what is learned early in life from the caregiver, and mentalizing is a higher-level cognitive ability grounded in attachment and developed throughout life. Therefore, according to the TAM model, attachment can be considered the fundamental acquired knowledge that constitutes personality.
Attachment Theory: Three Major Problems Concerning Personality
Notwithstanding its great achievements, attachment theory is widely recognized to be faced with three major general problems. In this section, we show how these three issues represent a problem also when trying to account for the relationship between attachment and personality – despite the clear link discussed above.
Problem P1: Intergenerational Transmission
A central issue in attachment theory is the intergenerational transmission of attachment styles. Both attachment and clinical sources support the style transmission from a generation to the next (Bowlby, 1969/1982, 1973, 1980; Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991; Bretherton, 1993; Van Ijzendoorn, 1995; Bernier et al., 2014; Sette et al., 2015; Verhage et al., 2016; Van Ijzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2019). According to this phenomenon, a child will most likely acquire the style of their main caregiver. By measuring the child’s style through the SSP and that of their caregiver through the AAI, most likely, we will have: (A) An avoidant child from an avoidant caregiver; (B) A secure child from a secure caregiver; and (C) An ambivalent child from an ambivalent caregiver. However, the identification of the caregiving features responsible for bringing about each attachment dimension remains an open problem. An extensive statistical analysis indicates caregiver’s sensitivity as not being the only cause of avoidance and ambivalence (Van Ijzendoorn, 1995; Verhage et al., 2016; Van Ijzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2019), and other caregiving features have been proposed to fill this so-called transmission gap (Whipple et al., 2011; Bernier et al., 2014; Van Ijzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2019). In other words, although sensitivity is the feature that is supposed to be involved in fostering attachment security, being sensitive seems not to be enough to raise a secure child. The standard theory has proven unable to fill this gap with its own means. As a result, the origin of attachment-personality remains uncertain.
Problem P2: Stability
Personality is (relatively) stable by definition. Therefore, if attachment is – as generally accepted – a fundamental part of personality, then attachment should also be (relatively) stable. This means that an individual’s attachment style over time should – in general – stay the same. According to this view, an avoidant child, for example, is expected to become an avoidant adult. However, this crucial aspect of attachment remains controversial: attachment studies that investigated its consistency across the lifespan found different results and, often, only modest stability (Waters et al., 2000; Fraley, 2002; McConnell and Moss, 2011; Pinquart et al., 2013; Kobak et al., 2016). In other words, according to the standard view, we face a contradiction: attachment is supposed to be, at the same time, a central part of personality and not really stable – while personality is. The standard theory seems to be caught in an unsolvable dilemma.
Problem P3: Psychopathology
Finally, the link between attachment and psychopathology is unanimously recognized. In particular, disorganization has been identified as a cause of dissociative pathologies (Liotti, 1992, 2004; Liotti and Farina, 2011; DeKlyen and Greenberg, 2016; Lyons-Ruth and Jacobvitz, 2016; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2016; Stovall-McClough and Dozier, 2016), and avoidance and ambivalence as generally correlated to most mental disorders (DeKlyen and Greenberg, 2016; Stovall-McClough and Dozier, 2016). A frightening caregiver favors the development of dissociative symptoms – such as depersonalization and derealization (i.e., an alteration of the individual’s perception of themselves or the world around respectively) – while an insensitive caregiver characterizes the childhood of most people who suffer, as an adult, from a common mental disorder – such as mood, anxiety, eating, obsessive disorders – suggesting a profound and durable effect of early attachment on the individual. Given its implications on a personal and social level, this problem is the most critical one. In fact, it has been accurately taken into account, and many clinicians believe that attachment should play a primary role in the therapeutic process (Obegi and Berant, 2010; Berry and Danquah, 2016). However, the underlying factors that connect insecure attachment to psychopathology remain unidentified. Again, the standard theory is stuck. It could only prove the implication of attachment in psychopathology with no further specification.
Attachment Theory Is in Trouble: What Should We Do?
Standard attachment theory identifies attachment as a fundamental part of personality. However, the theory confronts three critical problems that challenge the validity of this claim. (1) Sensitivity – the caregiving feature indicated as involved in the early attachment relationship – is not sufficient to explain the acquisition of the attachment knowledge by the child. (2) Attachment cannot be simultaneously a central part of personality and relatively unstable over life because personality is stable. (3) Psychopathology is deeply affected by attachment, but the theory is unable to specify how exactly.
These problems suggest two possibilities: (1) Attachment is not really a central part of personality; or (2) Attachment is a central part of personality, but we need to enhance the theory in order to fully account for this centrality. In the following sections, we show how the second option might well be the valid one and its remarkable implications.
Attachment Dimensions and the Imprinted Personality
This section is dedicated to outlining the APM enhancement of standard attachment theory to account for the relationship between attachment and personality. We start our argumentation assuming (in sections “α-Dimensions: The Three Basic Attachment Dimensions” and “β-Dimensions: The Four Additional Attachment Dimensions”) imprinting as the specific mechanism of attachment knowledge acquisition. Although the standard theory implicitly accepts imprinting as such a mechanism, it has never elaborated on the concept, and imprinting and its implications have never been adequately considered. Therefore, we then provide (in section “Imprinting and Sensitive Periods”) detailed arguments that support imprinting. Overall, we suggest that seven dimensions are first imprinted over two consecutive early sensitive periods:
1. Three α-dimensions – disorganization, avoidance, and ambivalence – over an α-period (between 6 and 24 months). As mentioned above, this has already been found by the standard theory.
2. Four β-dimensions – phobicity, depressivity, somaticity, and obsessivity – over a β-period (between 2 and 6 years). This is an APM proposal.
Although we focus here on the dimensions, the attachment to a specific caregiver is also realized through imprinting and is usually already accomplished within the first 6 months of life (Bowlby, 1969/1982; Marvin et al., 2016; Table 1).
According to our proposal, seven imprinted dimensions fully account for the centrality of attachment in personality. In particular, we argue that: (1) A redefinition of avoidance and ambivalence allows the APM to solve the intergenerational transmission gap (solution to problem P1, section “α-Dimensions: The Three Basic Attachment Dimensions”); (2) The introduction of four additional dimensions allows the APM to fully explain the causal relationship between attachment and psychopathology (solution to problem P3, section “β-Dimensions: The Four Additional Attachment Dimensions”); (3) Imprinting accounts for the stability of attachment and, therefore, personality (solution to problem P2, section “Imprinting and Sensitive Periods”). Importantly, this mechanism, although ensuring general stability, also allows for change at any age.
α-Dimensions: The Three Basic Attachment Dimensions
According to the standard theory, the three α-dimensions – disorganization, avoidance, and ambivalence – are first acquired during the α-period, which corresponds to most infancy (6-24 months).
Disorganization corresponds to the collapse of any organized strategy under enough stress and has been clearly linked to the experience of a frightening caregiver, which generates a ‘fear without solution’ (Main and Hesse, 1990; Main and Solomon, 1990). Such a caregiver can be perceived as directly or indirectly threatening. An important case of indirect threat for a child is a caregiver who is suffering from an ‘unresolved state of mind with respect to loss or trauma’ – a condition that is also more easily found in adults who were disorganized children (Lyons-Ruth and Jacobvitz, 2008; Liotti and Farina, 2011). Disorganized experiences have a profound impact on personality, as suggested by their connection with dissociative symptoms and psychopathology. Disorganization typically leads to strategies that aim to reach some form of organization. Such strategies can hide disorganization but often correspond to dysfunctional conditions, as further discussed in section “Implications for Psychopathology.” On the other hand, the connection of avoidance and ambivalence to caregiving features is still controversial with a transmission gap that remains to be filled.
A New Definition of Avoidance and Ambivalence
The APM proposes that the α-dimensions derive each from one corresponding caregiving feature (Table 2), which we term α-features. To account for such a causal link, we suggest the following – more specific – definition of avoidance and ambivalence starting from a precise definition of the corresponding α-features – that we refer to as sensitivity and responsiveness respectively:
• Sensitivity: the emotional connection offered by the caregiver. Sensitivity is the feature of love, the ‘emotional warmth,’ which is communicated by the caregiver to the child.
Responsiveness: the physical availability offered by the caregiver. Responsiveness is the feature of ‘being physically there when needed.’
• Avoidance: the subjective measure of the caregiver’s insensitivity. The more the caregiver is unloving, the higher is avoidance.
Ambivalence: the subjective measure of the caregiver’s responsiveness. The more the caregiver is physically unavailable when needed, the higher is ambivalence (for the child, the caregiver should and could be there but is not).
These definitions entail a two-channel-hypothesis – i.e., that two main (relatively) independent communication channels, one emotional and the other physical, are first relevant in the attachment relationship – with a one-to-one causal link between caregiving features and attachment dimensions:
1. Emotional-Channel. Sensitivity affects avoidance, and only avoidance: sensitivity is the emotional α-feature and avoidance the emotional α-dimension.
2. Physical-Channel. Responsiveness affects ambivalence, and only ambivalence: responsiveness is the physical α-feature and ambivalence the physical α-dimension.
The insensitive caregiver does not engage emotionally and encourages the child to deactivate attachment. As a result, the child tends to avoid interactions based on attachment. The unresponsive caregiver does not engage physically and encourages the child to hyper-activate attachment. As a result, the child tends to be worried about the caregiver’s availability (with evident emotional display).
Importantly, these definitions provide a clear correspondence between caregiving features and attachment dimensions. Insensitivity and avoidance match with each other by virtue of their emotional nature. The avoidant avoids the intolerable experience of facing a cold caregiver by deactivating attachment and focusing on other activities (Ainsworth et al., 1978; George et al., 1985; Hesse, 2008; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2016). A child will typically explore or play. When the insensitive caregiver is present, they do not connect emotionally and, as a result, asking comfort from them makes no sense. Unresponsiveness and ambivalence match with each other by virtue of their physical nature. The ambivalent hyper-activates attachment and worries whether their caregiver will be physically there for them (Ainsworth et al., 1978; George et al., 1985; Hesse, 2008; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2016). The unresponsive caregiver is unpredictably available but connects emotionally when present and, as a result, an emotional signal demanding their presence makes perfect sense. Interestingly, an insensitive-unresponsive caregiver will correspond to an avoidant-ambivalent child, but the two dimensions cannot be expressed simultaneously (since deactivation and hyper-activation are incompatible).
Hence, the provided definitions perfectly illustrate the basic characteristics of the attachment relationship. Their adequacy is further supported by the following four arguments.
(1) Statistical-independence argument. As discussed above, avoidance and ambivalence as defined in the literature are generally considered mutually (relatively) independent. We define avoidance and ambivalence more specifically – referring to the caregiver’s sensitivity and responsiveness – but still maintain that they are (relatively) independent. In fact, if they are, there must be two mutually (relatively) independent caregiving features, each of which induces one of them, and they also need to be identified.
The definitions given above support independence both between insensitivity and unresponsiveness and between avoidance and ambivalence. Indeed:
1. One is an emotional variable (insensitivity/avoidance) and the other a physical one (unresponsiveness/ambivalence).
2. Any combination of the emotional and physical variables is possible. In particular, a caregiver can independently provide any degree of emotional and physical care. For example, as emerges from a broad range of research (Bowlby, 1969/1982, 1973, 1980; Ainsworth et al., 1978; Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991; Schore, 1994; Sroufe, 1995; Guidano, 2007; Hesse, 2008; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2016), a caregiver can be physically there but emotionally disconnected and, conversely, they can usually be not physically there but emotionally connected when present.
The given definitions meet the requirement of reciprocal (relative) independence. By contrast, the literature has not focused on the mutual independence of caregiving features. Distinguishing between a purely emotional channel and a purely physical one allows us to ensure such independence.
(2) Developmental argument. We consider two points. (A) In order to develop adequately, for the child, both emotional connection and physical care are essential (Bowlby, 1969/1982, 1973, 1980; Stern, 1985; Schore, 1994; Sroufe, 1995; Leerkes and Wong, 2012; Marvin et al., 2016; Feldman, 2017). (B) As mentioned above, some caregiving features have been suggested as inducing avoidance and ambivalence. In particular, Bernier et al. (2014) found that sensitivity and autonomy support as defined by Whipple et al. (2011, p. 397), although not reciprocally independent, fully explain the generation of avoidance and ambivalence. This entails that it is possible to find two caregiving features on which avoidance and ambivalence entirely depend.
These data suggest that we should seek the cause of avoidance and ambivalence considering (A) the emotional and physical aspects of development and (B) two caregiving features. This is, indeed, the content of our proposal.
(3) Evolutionary argument. We have reason to believe that in the Environment of Evolutionary Adaptedness (EEA), the context of human evolution (Bowlby, 1969/1982), avoidance and ambivalence have been transmitted through two mutually independent channels – one emotional and the other physical. More precisely, according to an evolutionary argument, avoidance derives from the unwillingness to invest and ambivalence from the inability to invest in the offspring (Chisholm, 1996; Chisholm and Sieff, 2014).
On the one hand, unwillingness is an emotional feature that expresses intentional rejection. Evolutionarily, this is due to a harsh environment that makes the caregiver opt for investing in mating as opposed to parenting in order to maximize their reproductive success. In this condition, the child finds the best fit ignoring their caregiver and boosting their autonomy (avoidance).
On the other, inability is a physical feature that expresses the impossibility to be physically there. Evolutionarily, this is due to an unpredictable environment that occasionally forces the caregiver to attend to essential survival activities, thereby abdicating their role. In this condition, the child finds the best fit amplifying their need signals (ambivalence).
The evolutionary argument is supported by developmental evidence. Indeed, caregiver willingness and ability seem to be crucial information for young children as proved by the ability of 9-month-old infants to distinguish between the unwillingness and inability of an adult to hand them a toy (Behne et al., 2005).
(4) Neuroscientific argument. We have defined: (A) Insensitivity and avoidance as socio-emotional properties. As such, they primarily rely on gaze direction and facial expressions (relevant to emotional connection). (B) Unresponsiveness and ambivalence as socio-physical properties. As such, they primarily rely on reciprocal position (relevant to physical protection and care).
Two quite independent brain networks can be identified as underpinning these emotional and physical aspects. (A) On the one hand, the superior temporal sulcus, which is essential to detect gaze direction (Pelphrey et al., 2005; Hoehl et al., 2009; Itier and Batty, 2009; Carlin and Calder, 2013), is connected to the amygdala, which is key to reading facial emotions (Loughead et al., 2008; Whalen et al., 2013; Gothard, 2014; Wang et al., 2017). (B) On the other, the precuneus region is essential to deem the reciprocal position (Peer et al., 2015).
These neuroscientific data are consistent with two (relatively) independent channels, one emotional and the other physical, that underpin insensitivity/avoidance and unresponsiveness/ambivalence respectively.
Concluding this section, we want to stress that the focus on these two basic channels is proposed to help further understand and differentiate the complex nature of the two fundamental dimensions of avoidance and ambivalence and not to reduce it to such channels. In other words, the two channels are suggested to catch a fundamental distinction between the two dimensions and not to describe them exhaustively.
Solution to the ‘Intergenerational Transmission Problem’ (P1)
Overall, the above arguments converge to strongly support the two-channel-hypothesis. With these definitions, three attachment dimensions and three corresponding caregiving features are revealed to be clearly causally related: (1) a frightening caregiver induces disorganization, (2) an insensitive caregiver induces avoidance, and (3) an unresponsive caregiver induces ambivalence. Accordingly, each α-dimension can be thought of as corresponding to a core belief: “My caregiver is frightening,” “My caregiver is insensitive,” and “My caregiver is unresponsive” respectively (Table 3). Therefore, the APM indicates how attachment – and the related aspects of personality – are transmitted from a generation to the next, thereby solving the intergenerational transmission problem (P1). However, three dimensions are still insufficient to account for the relationship between attachment and psychopathology – we suggest these dimensions to be seven.
β-Dimensions: The Four Additional Attachment Dimensions
Analogously to the α-case, we propose a β-period corresponding to the preschool years (2–6 years) as sensitive for the imprinting of four β-dimensions – phobicity, depressivity, somaticity, and obsessivity (Table 1) – and that these dimensions also derive each from one corresponding caregiving feature, which we term β-features (Table 2). Indeed, as we discuss below, the caregiver can be:
1 Limiting: The caregiver regulates the child’s balance between attachment and exploration. When the caregiver is exploration-limiting, they induce a sense of vulnerability in the child. The attachment consequence is phobicity.
2 Unreachable: The caregiver should be emotionally reachable for the child. When the caregiver is emotionally unreachable, they induce a sense of defeat in the child. The attachment consequence is depressivity.
3 Defining: The caregiver regulates the child’s internal states, in particular sensations and emotions, and supports the child’s own definition of them. When the caregiver imposes their own definitions to the child, they induce a sense of somatic uncertainty in the child. The attachment consequence is somaticity.
4 Judgmental: The caregiver provides an ethical reference to the child. When the caregiver is judgmental to the child, they induce a sense of being wicked in the child. The attachment consequence is obsessivity.
Importantly, these features are indicated as the principal causes of the corresponding dimensions without excluding other possible influences on them. Moreover, we suggest that the β-dimensions are the typical cause of specific mental disorders. Indeed, analyzing vast clinical samples of patients suffering from the most common mental disorders – such as anxiety, mood, eating, and obsessive disorders – Guidano and Liotti found that these patients organized their pathological knowledge following four patterns – or ‘cognitive organizations’ (CO) (also called ‘personal meaning organizations’) (Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991, 2007; Table 4).
According to their studies, such COs favor the onset and maintenance of specific disorders – and, therefore, can be considered their cause. This research has been then confirmed by Guidano’s followers and extensively tested in clinical practice (Nardi and Bellantuono, 2008; Arciero and Bondolfi, 2009; Nardi et al., 2010). Furthermore, tools have been conceived to assess the four COs both in healthy and pathological conditions (Picardi et al., 2003; Nardi et al., 2012). Essentially, the organizations of knowledge identified by Guidano and Liotti can be considered higher-level descriptions of personality traits, and their careful analysis allows for the extraction of characterizing core beliefs that evidently correspond to those implied by the β-dimensions (Table 3). In other words, the β-dimensions can be considered the foundation of the COs (Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991, 2007; Nardi et al., 2010), and, therefore, such dimensions result to be causally related to psychopathology (Table 5).
Finally, we want to stress that the β-dimensions belong to every personality and, although they are connected to mental disorders, a high level of such dimensions does not entail psychopathology. They can be considered personality traits, whose excessive presence can facilitate a related disorder. Therefore, being phobic, depressive, somatic, or obsessive does not imply any pathology (and can actually favor adaptation). Similarly, a mental disorder typically related to these dimensions can have an etiology that is not related to them.
We outline each β-dimension in this section and provide evidence of a sensitive β-period in the next.
Phobicity and the Limiting Caregiver
We define phobicity as the subjective measure of how much the caregiver is limiting (in terms of the child’s exploration). The child needs to get their tendencies to attach and explore regulated by the caregiver, and the phobic child experiences a limitation of their exploration. In other words, the caregiver somehow forces the child to be closer to them than the child feels necessary. For example, the caregiver may be hyper-protective and tend to keep the child under their strict control. In this case, the child will tend to feel restricted and, when confronted with the task of autonomously exploring the environment, particularly in danger.
As a result, we propose, the preschool child gets imprinted an implicit knowledge that can be expressed by the core belief “I am in danger if my caregiver is not with me” and “My caregiver won’t let me go” (Table 3). This knowledge characterizes phobicity making the phobic particularly sensitive to the balance attachment-exploration and inclined to suffer from (1) separation anxiety when too far from the caregiver and (2) a sense of constriction when too close. In practice, phobicity is revealed by such sensitivity.
Our definition and proposal are supported by the following evidence.
Caregiving-attachment
The balance between protection and autonomy support is a fundamental caregiving task (Bowlby, 1973; Skinner et al., 2005; Bernier et al., 2014), and autonomy support has been found to be especially important in the preschool years for the following socio-emotional development (Matte-Gagné et al., 2015). The parental styles that induce phobicity have been found to be of two kinds, both of which result in limiting child exploration (Bowlby, 1973; Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991, 2007): (A) Direct: the caregiver is over-protective; (B) Indirect: the caregiver makes the child fear to lose them if they do not stay close – for example, by complaining about a serious illness.
Clinical
The caregiver limitation of exploration, in any of its forms, is causally related to the main clinical manifestations of phobicity – separation anxiety, agoraphobia, and panic disorder (Bowlby, 1973; Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991, 2007; Nardi et al., 2010). Indeed, despite the many intervening variables, the limitation of exploration suffered in childhood has been found correlated to adult panic disorder (Faravelli et al., 1991, 2010), and separation anxiety as a child to agoraphobia and panic attacks as an adult (Ayuso et al., 1989; Silove et al., 1995; Kossowsky et al., 2013).
Evolutionary
In a difficult EEA, the child that gets imprinted to stay close to the caregiver enhances their chances to survive, especially when they develop the ability of autonomous exploration (β-period, 2–6 years). Later, the phobic adult will tend to ensure they have their caregivers at hand, thereby improving their survival and reproductive chances in a harsh environment.
Depressivity and the Unreachable Caregiver
We define depressivity as the subjective measure of how much the caregiver is emotionally unreachable. The child needs to be able to reach the caregiver for emotional care, and the depressive child experiences a failure of their attempts to do so. In other words, the caregiver is, for some reason, emotionally unavailable to the child when the child tries to reach them. For example, the caregiver may be usually away from home. In this case, the child will tend to see their desire for emotional care frustrated and, therefore, to feel hopeless, defeated (Seligman, 1975; Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991, 2007).
As a result, we propose, the preschool child gets imprinted an implicit knowledge that can be expressed by the core belief “I won’t be able to reach my caregiver” (Table 3). This knowledge characterizes depressivity making the depressive particularly sensitive to the loss of the caregiver and inclined to suffer from (1) a sense of defeat and (2) depression when a loss is perceived. In practice, depressivity is revealed by such sensitivity.
Our definition and proposal are supported by the following evidence.
Caregiving-attachment
Ensuring emotional availability to the child is a fundamental caregiving task (Bowlby, 1980; Skinner et al., 2005). The emotional reachability of the caregiver has been found to be especially important in childhood (Kendler et al., 2000; Otowa et al., 2013), and particularly in the preschool years (Belden et al., 2007), for the following socio-emotional development. A cold and demanding parental style (affectionless control) has been identified as connected to the development of future depression (Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Parker, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991, 2007). Moreover, the most evident depressive parental characteristic is the long physical absence, especially their loss, which clearly entails emotional unreachability (Beck, 1967; Brown and Harris, 1978; Bowlby, 1980; Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Slavich et al., 2011; Otowa et al., 2014). Neuroscientific research confirms the connection between early loss and future depression (Panksepp, 1998) and the preschool years as a sensitive period (Panksepp and Biven, 2012).
Clinical
The caregiver emotional unreachability is causally related to the main clinical manifestation of depressivity – depression (Bowlby, 1980; Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991, 2007; Nardi et al., 2010). Accordingly, the lack of emotional reciprocity is considered to be a primary cause of the onset of depression in preschool children (Belden et al., 2007), and suffering long early separation or loss significantly increases the sensitivity to future loss as an antecedent to depression (Slavich et al., 2011).
Evolutionary
In a difficult EEA, the child that gets imprinted to give up attempting to reach an emotionally unavailable caregiver enhances their chances to survive by becoming self-reliant (Bowlby, 1980). A tendency to self-reliance starts to be feasible when the child begins to develop some independence (β-period, 2–6 years). Later, the depressive adult will have the advantage to be used to relying on themselves, thereby improving their survival and reproductive chances in a harsh competitive environment.
Somaticity and the Defining Caregiver
We define somaticity as the subjective measure of how much the caregiver is defining (in terms of the child’s internal states). The child needs to learn to recognize their internal states – primarily, the most somatic ones: sensations and emotions – from the caregiver, and the somatic child experiences, instead, an external definition. In other words, the caregiver somehow forces the child to adopt an internal state that does not match the child’s actual one. For example, the child might be suggested they feel pain when they do not (like in the case of Brenda, Guidano, 1987). In this case, the child will tend to feel confused about their own state and intruded upon by the caregiver. Somaticity is, therefore, an anomaly of the fundamental caregiving regulation of the child’s internal states, where the caregiver’s state does not correspond to the child’s state. This way, the child does not learn how to recognize their own internal states and becomes uncertain of them.
As a result, we propose, the preschool child gets imprinted an implicit knowledge that can be expressed by the core belief “I need my caregiver to tell me about myself” and “My caregiver will intrude on me” (Table 3). This knowledge characterizes somaticity making the somatic particularly sensitive to the definition of their own internal states and inclined to suffer from (1) uncertainty when not sufficiently defined and (2) a sense of intrusion when being excessively defined by the caregiver. In practice, somaticity is revealed by such sensitivity.
Our definition and proposal are supported by the following evidence.
Caregiving-attachment
The regulation of the child’s internal states is a fundamental caregiving task performed through behavioral and biological synchronicity between caregiver and child (Stern, 1985; Sroufe, 1995; Harrist and Waugh, 2002; Schore, 2005; Feldman, 2017; Hollenstein et al., 2017; Reindl et al., 2018). The specific socio-emotional task of the preschool years is to go from dyadic to self-regulation (Sroufe, 1995). In this period, synchronicity supports the acquisition of social skills (Harrist and Waugh, 2002) accompanied by the development of complex social emotions, such as shame and guilt, that signal the understanding of social standards and rules (Lewis, 2011; Botto and Rochat, 2018). The parental style that induces somaticity is intrusive defining-misattunement, which does not allow the child to reach proper security in self-definition (Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991, 2007). As a result, the child tends to remain uncertain of their own internal states and in need for definition from a caregiver. Sensations and emotions – the most somatic states – are the first to be involved, but gradually the caregiver’s attitude extends to more abstract ones such as preferences and opinions. Typically, the caregiver adheres to conventional standards, and the child strives to comply with them. Somaticity can lead to an enmeshed family, where members have very undefined self-borders and tend to have common emotions and opinions (Minuchin et al., 1978).
Clinical
The caregiver intrusive defining-misattunement is causally related to the main clinical manifestations of somaticity – eating disorders (Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991, 2007; Nardi et al., 2010). These disorders have been found (A) significantly correlated to attachment insecurity (Tasca and Balfour, 2014; Faber et al., 2018) and, in accordance with the somatic uncertainty about self-definition and tendency to compliance, (B) characterized by alexithymia (Schmidt et al., 1993; Westwood et al., 2017) and unassertiveness (Behar et al., 2006; Hartmann et al., 2010).
Evolutionary
In a difficult EEA, the child that gets imprinted to comply with the caregiver’s standards enhances their chances to survive by adopting a view of the situation that already proved functional instead of trying a new one. This becomes salient when the child develops the ability of autonomous exploration and starts to have broader social interactions (β-period, 2–6 years). The somatic adult will keep tending to comply with social standards, thereby improving their survival and reproductive chances in a harsh environment, where social compliance can make a key difference.
Obsessivity and the Judgmental Caregiver
We define obsessivity as the subjective measure of how much the caregiver is judgmental. The child needs to learn ethics – namely, what is considered right or wrong – from the caregiver, and the obsessive child experiences the imposition of the caregiver’s code of conduct and being significantly blamed for not abiding by it. In other words, the caregiver enforces a strict and arbitrary set of rules by systematically blaming the child for disobedience. The blame is always justified by claiming to cause terrible harm to someone and conveys the implicit message of the caregiver’s rejection of the child. It can assume different forms, such as verbal scold or physical punishment. For example, the child might be severely reproached for wearing wrinkled clothes (like in the case of Alison, Guidano, 1987). In this case, the child will tend to feel anguished by the possibility of misbehaving – even involuntarily – and to focus on acting correctly.
As a result, we propose, the preschool child gets imprinted an implicit knowledge that can be expressed by the core belief “I am wicked” (Table 3). This knowledge characterizes obsessivity making the obsessive particularly sensitive to ethical matters and inclined to suffer from (1) obsessions focused on their responsibility for causing harm and (2) compulsions to get rid of such intrusive ideas. In practice, obsessivity is revealed by such sensitivity.
Our definition and proposal are supported by the following evidence.
Caregiving-attachment
The moral guidance of the child is a fundamental caregiving task that has been widely studied, identifying three main related parenting styles: authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive (Baumrind, 1971, 2013; Robinson et al., 1995; Skinner et al., 2005). The acquisition of a code of conduct, accompanied by the emergence of guilt, is accomplished in the preschool years (Sroufe, 1995; Aksan and Kochanska, 2005; Lewis, 2011; Nicolais et al., 2017; Botto and Rochat, 2018) and has been found to have a significant impact on the next development of the child (Baumrind et al., 2010; Kochanska et al., 2010). The parental style that induces obsessivity is cold and severe authoritarianism (Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991, 2007; Lennertz et al., 2010; Timpano et al., 2010). Typically, one of the two parents is very active in imposing the rules, and the other is a passive accomplice.
Clinical
The caregiver aloof and strict moral guidance is causally related to the main clinical manifestation of obsessivity – the obsessive-compulsive disorder (Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991, 2007; Nardi et al., 2010). This disorder has been found (A) significantly correlated to attachment insecurity (Myhr et al., 2004; Doron et al., 2009; Ivarsson et al., 2010; Rezvan et al., 2012; Boysan and Çam, 2016) and, in accordance with the obsessive ethical focus, (B) characterized by guilt related to the violation of a moral rule (Shafran et al., 1996; Basile et al., 2013; Mancini and Gangemi, 2015).
Evolutionary
In a difficult EEA, the child that gets imprinted to respect the caregiver’s rules enhances their chances to survive by adopting a stricter and safer behavior. This becomes noticeable when the child develops locomotion and widens their social interactions, being both more cautious and a more reliable partner with a better reputation (β-period, 2–6 years). The obsessive adult, through conscientious application of their code, will reach relevant competencies (Hertler, 2015a,b), thereby improving their survival and reproductive chances in a harsh competitive environment.
Solution to the ‘Psychopathology Problem’ (P3)
The above arguments strongly suggest that: (A) The caregiver has four fundamental tasks: (1) regulating the child’s balance between attachment and exploration; (2) being emotionally reachable for the child; (3) regulating the child’s internal states and supporting the child’s own definition of them; (4) providing an ethical reference to the child. (B) These tasks become relevant during the preschool years. (C) The child needs to adapt to their caregiver’s particular way of accomplishing these tasks. (D) An optimal solution to this adaptation problem is to detect the caregiver’s attitude with respect to each of these tasks and rapidly and stably acquire such vital information – in other words, to imprint some core beliefs. Given their origin in the attachment relationship, these beliefs correspond to attachment dimensions and, given their fundamental socio-psychological role, they constitute the core of personality. (E) Particular kinds of accomplishments of these caregiving tasks induce the acquisition of core beliefs that can more easily lead to later dysfunctional states and behaviors – i.e., to psychopathology. (F) In particular, using the definitions that we propose: (1) A limiting caregiver induces phobicity, (2) an unreachable caregiver induces depressivity, (3) a defining caregiver induces somaticity, and (4) a judgmental caregiver induces obsessivity. Four caregiving features result to be causally related to four attachment dimensions.
Attachment theory could not find a specific connection between attachment and most psychopathologies because it investigated only the α-dimensions. In contrast, by considering the β-dimensions, the APM can indicate the specific causal relationship between attachment – and, therefore, personality – and the most common mental disorders (Table 5), thereby solving the problem (P1) of fully relating attachment to psychopathology.
Imprinting and Sensitive Periods
As we discuss below, imprinting is an evolutionarily preordained unconscious learning process, whose main characteristics are: (1) Taking place for the first time in sensitive periods during the early stages of life; (2) Being particularly resistant to change. In the previous sections, we argued that attachment is characterized by seven dimensions and assumed that they are first acquired through imprinting over two early sensitive periods: (1) The α-period (6–24 months), in which the α-dimensions – disorganization, avoidance, and ambivalence – are imprinted; and (2) The β-period (2–6 years, the preschool age), in which the β-dimensions – phobicity, depressivity, somaticity, and obsessivity – are imprinted. In this section, we make a case for imprinting as the specific attachment acquisition mechanism by drawing on five different areas: (1) Ethological; (2) Developmental-Attachment; (3) Clinical; (4) Evolutionary; and (5) Neuroscientific.
Ethological Argument
Imprinting and sensitive periods have been documented in various species of birds and mammals (Lorenz, 1937, 1981; Harlow, 1959; Hess, 1959; Shipley, 1963; Salzen, 1967; Table 6).
Goslings, for example, attach to their mother during a well-defined brief sensitive period shortly after hatching. Lorenz demonstrated that, when they catch some cues from the environment at the right time, they follow whoever matches the expected features. Famously, they attached to Lorenz himself. Through this – clearly evolutionarily preordained – process, the animal fixes in their mind the identity of their caregiver. Reasonably, given our psycho-social complexity, we-humans might have evolved a more elaborated version of imprinting compared to other animals. Indeed, we argue that we are not only preordained to fix in our mind the identity of specific caregivers, but also the most evolutionarily relevant attachment characteristics – the seven attachment dimensions.
Developmental and Attachment Argument
Bowlby (1969/1982) suggested the existence of imprinting and sensitive periods in humans noticing the striking similarities between our attachment and that of other animals, such as the geese studied by Lorenz (1937) or the rhesus monkeys studied by Harlow (1959). Since then, these concepts have been generally maintained by developmental-attachment research (Marvin et al., 2016), but the multiple intervening factors that can contribute to change seem to have caught much more attention than them. However, the α-period is unequivocally confirmed as sensitive for attachment by studies concerning early child-institutionalization that prove the long term effect of attachment experiences that occur within the first 2 years of life (Varin et al., 1996; Nelson et al., 2007; Zeanah et al., 2011; Fox, 2014; Troller-Renfree and Fox, 2017; Tables 1, 6). The effect of such experiences can be actually irreversible.
Regarding the β-period (preschool age), we can consider that:
1. Attachment develops beyond the second year of life (Bowlby, 1969/1982).
2. Although the child’s social context opens up to new interactions – in particular, with peers – (Nelson, 2007; Coplan and Arbeau, 2009; Marvin et al., 2016), the attachment relationship is still primary in the preschool years – since the child yet entirely depends on the caregiver for their survival (Crittenden, 2008; Marvin et al., 2016).
3. In the preschool years, caregiving is characterized by the four fundamental tasks provided by the caregiver’s β-features (cf. “Phobicity and the Limiting Caregiver,” “Depressivity and the Unreachable Caregiver,” Somaticity and the Defining Caregiver, and “Obsessivity and the Judgmental Caregiver”): (1) Protecting and supporting autonomy; (2) Being emotionally reachable; (3) Favoring self-definition; (4) Providing moral guidelines. These features represent four developmentally standard care-conditions to which the child needs to adapt.
All this is consistent with the imprinting of the four β-dimensions, which provides the child with vital information to adapt to the four human-specific attachment situations that become relevant during the β-period. As discussed above, we suggest that (Table 2):
1. The child adapts to an exploration-limiting caregiver by becoming phobic.
2. The child adapts to an emotionally unreachable caregiver by becoming depressive.
3. The child adapts to a state-defining caregiver by becoming somatic.
4. The child adapts to a judgmental caregiver by becoming obsessive.
Clinical Argument
Clinical research strongly supports the persistence of personality characteristics from childhood to adulthood and the correspondence between caregiving features and acquired attachment characteristics (Berne, 1972; Bowlby, 1973, 1980; Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991; Blatt and Levy, 2003; Guidano, 2007; Levy et al., 2015; Yakeley, 2018). The resistance to change is evident in psychopathology. In cognitive psychotherapy, the phenomenon is called ‘neurotic paradox’ (Perdighe and Mancini, 2010), which refers to the apparently inexplicable persistence of mental disorders despite the patients being aware of their self-damaging behaviors. The phenomenon is consistent with dysfunctional knowledge that is imprinted and can be hardly modified. This is exactly the characteristic of the seven attachment dimensions and, in fact, all major psychopathologies can regularly be linked to information acquired in attachment relationships, as shown by clinical accounts that report early attachment information (e.g., Bowlby, 1973, 1980; Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991; Oltmanns et al., 2012).
More specifically for the preschool age, as discussed above (cf. “Phobicity and the Limiting Caregiver,” “Depressivity and the Unreachable Caregiver,” Somaticity and the Defining Caregiver, and “Obsessivity and the Judgmental Caregiver”): (1) the β-dimensions are causally related to specific mental disorders; (2) The β-period is the earliest timeframe for the onset of such disorders; (3) These disorders tend to last throughout life; and (4) Parenting has been recognized as one of the major causes of their onset (Hopkins et al., 2013; Whalen et al., 2017). Since imprinting is the mechanism that underpins the long preservation of information, these data strongly suggest the imprinting of the β-dimensions in the preschool years.
Evolutionary Argument
Attachment has been designed by evolution as a fundamental adaptation mechanism. Bowlby (1969/1982; 1973; 1980) has first underlined the survival function of attachment for the child and, later, other authors (Chisholm, 1996; Chisholm and Sieff, 2014; Simpson and Belsky, 2016; Szepsenwol and Simpson, 2019; Young et al., 2019) have stressed the reproduction function of it for the adult. Indeed, the ultimate evolutionary goal of the individual is inclusive fitness (Hamilton, 1964), i.e., passing one’s gene to the following generations. These authors argue that the early attachment relationship provides cues of environmental characteristics – such as harshness and unpredictability (Ellis et al., 2009) – and the child unconsciously gathers these cues from the caregiver to set up an adequate strategy for later reproduction. This consideration suggests that attachment information acquired as a child is meant to be durable. Otherwise, it would lose its value for reproductive purposes. This is precisely what an imprinted IWM with its seven attachment dimensions is meant to provide.
In fact, this line of reasoning fits the preschool age. As discussed above (cf. “Phobicity and the Limiting Caregiver,” “Depressivity and the Unreachable Caregiver,” Somaticity and the Defining Caregiver,” and “Obsessivity and the Judgmental Caregiver”), beyond that in childhood, the β-dimensions have an adaptive function in adulthood: (1) Phobicity promotes protection through maintaining physical closeness; (2) Depressivity promotes the ability of being self-reliant; (3) Somaticity promotes familial cohesiveness through uniformity of thought and feeling; (4) Obsessivity promotes conscientiousness and competence through strict moral conduct. All these characteristics provide the adult with an advantage in a harsh and possibly unpredictable environment, thereby justifying imprinting the corresponding information from when it is available – the β-period indeed – to adulthood.
Neuroscientific Argument
The neural processes underlying attachment during sensitive periods have been identified and studied in different birds and non-human mammals (Knudsen, 2004; Sullivan and Holman, 2010; Landers and Sullivan, 2012; Knudsen, 2013; Nakamori et al., 2013; Roth et al., 2016; Feldman, 2017; Opendak et al., 2017; Opendak and Sullivan, 2019). These studies suggest the possible neural underpinnings of imprinting in the human brain. In particular, Knudsen (2004, 2013) describes how imprinting in sensitive periods corresponds to a shaping of neural networks that is durable but still admits of a later change through a particular interaction with the environment, which is exactly what is here hypothesized for the IWM. In accordance with Bowlby, it has also been established that, during a sensitive period, attachment to a caregiver occurs regardless of the care received, even when the caregiver is abusive (Sullivan and Holman, 2010; Landers and Sullivan, 2012; Opendak et al., 2017; Opendak and Sullivan, 2019), which further confirms the evolutionary programming of the process. Finally, lines of research across neuroscience and clinical psychology (Schore, 1994; Turnbull and Solms, 2003; Schore, 2009) identify the human brain areas involved in imprinting, with a sensitive period within the first 24 months (Tables 1, 6). In accordance with Bowlby, they stress the life-long durability of the phenomenon and its influence on personality.
Although neuroscience only recently has begun to address the attachment relationship directly in humans, some evidence has already been provided for a sensitive β-period. The studies of Rao et al. (2010) and Luby et al. (2016) found that maternal support in the preschool years is significantly correlated with hippocampal growth until adolescence, while maternal support in the following school years does not correlate with it. As a result, these authors explicitly propose that the preschool years are an attachment-related sensitive period. Moreover, Luby et al. (2016) found that preschool maternal support is correlated with emotional regulation in adolescence, with positive support linked to greater regulation ability. These data confirm the hypothesis of the preschool years as a sensitive period for attachment, with caregiving having a role in psychopathology. This is precisely what the APM proposes for the β-period and β-dimensions.
Solution to the ‘Stability Problem’ (P2)
The above five arguments converge to provide compelling support for imprinting as the specific attachment acquisition mechanism. The α-dimensions are imprinted in a sensitive α-period (6–24 months), and the β-dimensions in a sensitive β-period (2–6 years, the preschool age).
Therefore, the APM solves the dilemma of stability (P2) by clarifying that attachment – as it is expected of anything related to personality – is fundamentally stable.
Discussion
Focusing on its information component, personality can be seen as the set of stable traits that are critically determined by one’s core beliefs. According to this perspective, to determine personality, we need to identify its core beliefs and their origin. Standard attachment theory offers a privileged starting point to solve this issue by connecting personality to the core knowledge that each of us acquires in early attachment relationships. However, the theory encounters three major problems concerning the link between attachment and personality: (P1) Intergenerational transmission; (P2) Stability, and (P3) Psychopathology.
In this article, by adequately enhancing the standard theory, we propose a solution to these problems that leads to the full account for the relationship between attachment and personality. We consider a total of seven attachment dimensions and the mechanism of their acquisition – imprinting. In particular, we suggest the following solutions: (P1) A specific caregiving feature realizes the intergenerational transmission of each attachment dimension (Table 2); (P2) Imprinting provides attachment with fundamental stability (Table 1); (P3) Psychopathology is causally related to specific attachment dimensions (Table 5).
These solutions clarify the general stability and fundamental socio-psychological value of the seven attachment dimensions, suggesting them to be the knowledge core of personality (Table 3).
Implications
Overall, the APM entails significant consequences for attachment and personality theories and for the conception, assessment, and treatment of the most common mental disorders.
Implications for Attachment Theory
The APM proposes a revision of the current theory that is fully compatible with its achievements but, at the same time, can significantly enhance its explanatory power. In particular, the model suggests the relationship between caregiving features and attachment dimensions (P1), the basic stability of attachment due to imprinting (P2), and its connection to psychopathology (P3). We reach this result by focusing on the knowledge aspect of attachment and relying particularly on clinical research to explain the available data. This allows us to reduce complexity and identify key elements and their causal links. In contrast, standard attachment theory has been usually trying to take into account as many variables as possible relying mostly on statistical studies that cannot identify causal links. As a result, many variables have been considered, but the relationship between them has appeared often unclear.
In particular, we consider an IWM consisting of seven dimensions: 3 α-dimensions – disorganization, avoidance, and ambivalence – first imprinted in an α-period and four β-dimensions – phobicity, depressivity, somaticity, and obsessivity – first imprinted in a β-period (Table 1). We specify the definition of avoidance and ambivalence so that avoidance is transmitted via an emotional channel and ambivalence is transmitted via a physical channel. As a result, the α-dimensions are each induced by a specific caregiving feature. Furthermore, we introduce the four β-dimensions as each also induced by a specific caregiving feature. Consequently, we provide a complete mapping between caregiving and attachment (Table 2). According to the APM, the caregiving features correspond to fundamental tasks, among which the caregiver normally switches according to the situation, thereby inducing the acquisition or the expression of the corresponding attachment dimensions. On the other hand, given that they correspond to different caregiving features, the attachment dimensions are assumed to be independent, at least in terms of their function. However, since the same caregiver is usually responsible for the induction of multiple – if not all – dimensions, some characteristic patterns are to be expected. For example, when somaticity is the predominant dimension, then phobicity is also expected to be high because a defining caregiver will probably somehow limit their child’s exploration (Guidano, 1991). Finally, disorganization and the β-dimensions explain how attachment is linked to psychopathology (Table 5), as further discussed below.
We focus on dimensionality and imprinting as the central aspects of attachment that derive from its evolutionary predefinition. We propose that these seven dimensions specify the full range of adaptation-vital information, and imprinting provides the necessary stability to its acquisition – making it a durable and reliable adaptive base for the child and the future adult. In contrast, standard attachment theory has been exclusively concentrating on the information acquired within the first 2 years of life and on the possibility of its change. As a result, key aspects of attachment could not be identified.
Implications for Personality
Personality is a complex construct, and numerous models have been proposed from different perspectives. Between them, we focus here on one of the most influential and widely referred to: the Five-Factor Model (FFM) (or Big-Five) (McCrae and Costa, 2003). It consists of five traits – Openness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, Neuroticism – that have been statistically extracted from linguistic descriptions of personality characteristics. The model can be applied to healthy subjects but has also been linked to personality disorders (Widiger et al., 2017). An important feature of the FFM compared to the APM is its purely empirical nature with no endorsement of any specific etiological theory of personality. In other words, it is purely descriptive and cannot explain where the dimensions it postulates come from. In contrast, the APM is grounded in attachment theory – a theory of human relationships that finds in early experiences with the caregiver the foundation of personality, thereby offering an evident explanatory advantage. Moreover, in the condition of its applicability, we can expect the APM to have no less descriptive power than the FFM, since the set of α- and β-dimensions covers a broad range of personality features (Bowlby, 1969/1982, 1973, 1980; Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991, 2007; Nardi et al., 2010; Liotti and Farina, 2011; Hesse, 2016; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2016). Interestingly, empirical studies have found correlation between the fundamental attachment dimensions of avoidance and ambivalence and the Big Five (Noftle and Shaver, 2006; Fraley et al., 2011). In this regard, the most relevant finding is the negative correlation of avoidance with agreeableness and the positive correlation of ambivalence with neuroticism. These results match the typical avoidant detachment and ambivalent over-emotionality. Given their meaning, we can also expect some correspondence between the β-dimensions and the FFM traits. Somaticity, for example, is expected to be correlated with agreeableness, obsessivity with conscientiousness.
The connection between the APM and the FFM needs to be further investigated and, at the moment, only preliminary hypotheses can be formulated about it. In this respect, a central issue concerns the possibility of personality change throughout life. The APM proposes personality to be resistant to change, but empirical findings underpinned by the FFM indicate that changes are to be expected (Lucas and Donnellan, 2011; Oltmanns et al., 2019). For example, agreeableness and conscientiousness have been found to increase between age 20 and 60, while openness to decrease after 60, with important individual differences (Roberts and Mroczek, 2008). A ‘maturity principle’ has been proposed (Caspi et al., 2005; Lucas and Donnellan, 2011) according to which agreeableness and conscientiousness tend to increase and neuroticism to decrease over young and middle adulthood in relation to the assumption of mature social roles. The end of maturity (Oltmanns et al., 2019) has also been identified, corresponding to the decrease of extraversion, conscientiousness, and openness from late middle age. In particular, personality stability and change over the life course have been connected to identity processes (Roberts and Caspi, 2003). According to this perspective, continuity is related to identity strength, in terms of achievement and certainty, for example. Changes of identity that are characteristic of different stages of life – and related to different social roles (e.g., being independent, having a family, pursuing a career) – would explain corresponding changes of personality (Roberts et al., 2003, 2006). Although the apparent contrast, we can hypothesize that these findings are fully compatible with the general stability endorsed by the APM since the APM and the FFM focus on different constructs. First, the two models seem to measure different social variables related to personality. The APM is based on attachment, which is a social phenomenon limited to close relationships, while the above studies suggest that FFM measures are sensitive to broader social aspects. Second, the APM concerns the knowledge part of personality, whereas the high heritability of the FFM traits (Jang et al., 1996; Power and Pluess, 2015) suggests them to be more sensitive to biological variables and related changes over the lifespan. The indispensable empirical testing of the APM will provide further data to elaborate on this point.
Implications for Psychopathology
The implications of the APM for psychopathology are even stronger than those for personality.
Attachment disorders
As discussed above, the β-dimensions express core beliefs (Table 3) that clearly correspond to the cognitive organizations (CO) first identified by Guidano and Liotti (Table 4), who demonstrated the causal connection of the COs to the most common mental disorders. As a result, these dimensions are causally connected to specific psychopathologies (Table 5). The APM proposes the attachment origin of the β-dimensions, thereby connecting the vast and valuable clinical research related to the COs to a wider attachment framework. This link entails remarkable consequences. The two immediate ones are the following:
A. Any disorder that can be traced back to a β-dimension has an attachment etiology. These disorders are usually the most common ones – such as agoraphobia, depression, anorexia, bulimia, obsessive–compulsive disorder. Clearly, when a disorder has a β-dimension at its root, attachment plays a major role in its unfolding.
B. Attachment is also the principal mechanism underlying healing from these conditions. Since the change of a dimension is bound to attachment activation, the healing process implies the action of an attachment figure. In other words, when therapy is successful, the therapist offers through their – usually implicit – caregiving what has been called a ‘corrective emotional experience’ (Alexander and French, 1946; Mallinckrodt, 2010). The APM specifies the features of this experience – the successful intervention must have involved the patient’s pathological dimensions. Of course, the intervention can be tailored to each patient by addressing their specific pathological dimensions.
Therefore, the APM suggests that – in these cases – the attachment relationship is both (A) the key pathogenic factor and (B) the key healing one. Of course, the same considerations apply to disorganization and related dissociative disorders. Consequently, the model provides the basis for an etiological classification of psychopathology, with clear significant impact on assessment and treatment.
Pathological dynamics and treatment
The APM can be linked to the general motivational dynamics as described by Liotti’s motivational theory (Liotti and Monticelli, 2008; Liotti et al., 2017), which integrates attachment in a more general motivational framework. According to this view, human behavior can be described as underpinned by the continuous activation of built-in motivational systems, which drive us to pursue evolutionarily relevant goals – such as exploration, sex, attachment, caregiving, cooperation. If a pathology is related to attachment, the activation of the attachment motivational system will be potentially problematic. The APM suggests that five specific domains – corresponding to disorganization and the β-dimensions – can cause issues when attachment is activated: a situation will more easily elicit a pathological reaction if it concerns a dysfunctional dimension. For example, a threat will be more problematic if one is disorganized, a separation if one is phobic, a loss if one is depressive. Given that a pathological attachment dimension is connected to intolerable internal states (Bowlby, 1969/1982, 1973, 1980; Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991; Schore, 1994; Sroufe, 1995; Schore, 2000; Liotti and Farina, 2011; Brumariu, 2015), the individual who has such an issue will develop – from the preschool years – some strategies to avoid the activation of attachment in relation to the given dimension. These can be seen as strategies to regulate attachment activation – activating a non-attachment system to avoid that of attachment. For example, a child could activate caregiving with their mother, thereby taking care of her and inverting the attachment relationship (Bowlby, 1973). Regulation strategies have been identified for disorganization (Hennighausen and Lyons Ruth, 2005; Lyons-Ruth and Jacobvitz, 2008; Liotti, 2011; Liotti and Farina, 2011) and ambivalence and avoidance (Shaver and Mikulincer, 2002; Mikulincer et al., 2003; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2016). The APM suggests that they extend to the β-dimensions. This view is very well supported. Indeed, the dysfunctional attachment patterns described by Crittenden in the preschool years (Crittenden, 1995; 2008; Farnfield et al., 2010) are evident regulation strategies. Moreover, providing the attachment foundation to the COs, the APM can be immediately integrated with the clinical approaches stemmed from Guidano and Liotti’s research (Guidano, 2007; Nardi and Bellantuono, 2008; Arciero and Bondolfi, 2009; Liotti and Monticelli, 2014; Liotti et al., 2017). Finally, the concepts of attachment-related core belief and regulation strategy are analogous to those of interpersonal schema and interpersonal cycle (Safran and Segal, 1996; Dimaggio, 2015). As a consequence, as mentioned above, the APM is also perfectly compatible with the well-established metacognitive interpersonal therapy (Carcione et al., 2016; Dimaggio et al., 2017, 2019; Gordon-King et al., 2019). Therefore, the APM – in accordance also with the TAM model – suggests the attachment origin of our central psycho-social beliefs and is consistent with the fundamental clinical role of metacognition.
In conclusion, we can stress that, given the close relationship between psychopathology and attachment, mental disorders can offer us important insights into the nature of personality. Since one’s core beliefs are related to common psychopathologies, the study of such conditions can shed light on the structure and possible change of personality.
Limitations and Suggestions for Future Work
We suggest here the two main limitations of our model and possible ways to overcome them.
Scope
The APM refers to the aspects of personality related to imprinted attachment data. Therefore, when other variables – such as the biological ones – have a non-negligible influence on personality, such variables should be taken into account. We think that complexity should be added gradually to the model, by considering new variables – and upgrading the model itself – but only after adequate testing.
Testing
Although we found significant support for the hypotheses we formulate here, the APM is a novel theoretical model. As a consequence, it will need to be thoroughly empirically validated, especially through the operationalization and measurement of each dimension. In particular, we can consider the main hypotheses formulated by the APM – (H1) The existence of seven dimensions of attachment, each with specific characteristics; (H2) The correspondence between these dimensions and specific caregiving features; (H3) The role of the dimensions in personality and psychopathology – and suggest three different strategies (that we are already implementing) to test them.
Clinical
The most natural testing for the APM appears to be the clinical one, which can be performed not only directly, in clinical practice, but also indirectly, through the examination of case studies that include sufficient information about the attachment history of the patient (e.g., Bowlby, 1973, 1980; Guidano and Liotti, 1983; Guidano, 1987, 1991; Oltmanns et al., 2012). All the above main hypotheses are testable by analyzing clinical data, but especially H3 for its relevance in dysfunctional conditions. So far, this testing has fully confirmed the model. A systematic review of a larger number of cases is an optimal strategy for further testing.
Statistical
Questionnaires are a well-established assessment practice in attachment theory (Barone and Del Corno, 2006). The proposed dimensionality of attachment (H1) and correspondence between caregiving features and attachment dimensions (H2) (Table 2) can be empirically tested through a questionnaire administered to adults who answer about their current and past experiences.
Computational
Attachment relationships can be represented mathematically and modeled computationally (Buono et al., 2006; Amengual, 2009; Stevens and Zhang, 2009; Petters and Beaudoin, 2017) – for example, by representing mother and child in a software virtual environment. A mathematical model that is built in accordance with a given theory allows for the testing of its hypotheses. If the simulations do not reproduce the data gathered in real situations, the corresponding assumptions are disconfirmed. Since the APM integrates attachment into the whole motivational dynamics and offers a dimensional view of attachment knowledge, it is particularly suitable for computational implementation. However, given the complexity of pathological circumstances, this kind of testing seems to be more adequate to test H1 and H2 rather than H3. Interestingly, the simulation environment can work as a lab of synthetic psychology (Braitenberg, 1984; Dawson, 2004; Prescott and Camilleri, 2018), where not only attachment phenomena can be reproduced, but new situations can be generated to further test and extend the model.
Conclusion
We fully agree with those psychologists who believe that theoretical investigation is as important as the empirical one to the development of Psychology (Borghi and Fini, 2019). In this spirit, we present here a novel theory and hypotheses, urging their direct empirical testing. This work represents our attempt to contribute to the theoretical framework of clinical psychology and its connections to other fields of psychology and connected disciplines. Over the years, a great amount of evidence pointing to the fundamental role of attachment in the building of personality has been accumulating. The APM gives a precise form to this idea: During two consecutive sensitive periods early in life, humans acquire from their caregivers essential cues through which they set their fundamental socio-psychological variables corresponding to seven attachment dimensions. Seven core beliefs are imprinted to be the knowledge foundation of our personality, which will give us implicit directions throughout our lives. We are predefined adaptive machines designed by evolution to be unconsciously programmed by our caregivers.
Data Availability Statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.
Author Contributions
The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.
Funding
This work was part-supported by the EU Horizon 2020 programme through the FET Flagship Human Brain Project (HBP-SGA3, 945539).
Conflict of Interest
The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The author wants to express his deep gratitude to Prof. Tony J. Prescott and Dr. Luca Barlassina for their constant support and wise advice, without which this article would have never been written.
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
Aksan, N., and Kochanska, G. (2005). Conscience in childhood: old questions, new answers. Dev. Psychol. 41, 506–516.55.
Alexander, F., and French, T. M. (1946). Psychoanalytic Therapy: Principles and Application. New York, NY: Ronald Press.
Allen, J. P. (2008). “The Attachment System in Adolescence,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 2nd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 419–435.
Amengual, A. (2009). A Computational Model of Attachment Secure Responses in the Strange Situation”. Technical Report TR-09-002. Berkeley, CA: International Computer Science Institute.
Ammaniti, M., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Speranza, A. M., and Tambelli, R. (2000). Internal working models of attachment during late childhood and early adolescence: an exploration of stability and change. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2, 328–346. doi: 10.1080/14616730010001587
Arciero, G., and Bondolfi, G. (2009). Selfhood, Identity and Personality Styles. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Ayuso, J. L., Alfonso, S., and Rivera, A. (1989). Childhood separation anxiety and panic disorder: a comparative study. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 13, 665–671. doi: 10.1016/0278-5846(89)90053-5
Barone, L., and Del Corno, F. (eds) (2006). La valutazione Dell’attaccamento Adulto. Milan: Raffaello Cortina Editore.
Bartholomew, K., and Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: a test of a four-category model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61, 226–244. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.226
Basile, B., Mancini, F., Macaluso, E., Caltagirone, C., and Bozzali, M. (2013). Abnormal processing of deontological guilt in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Brain Struct. Funct. 219, 1321–1331. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0570-2
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Dev. Psychol. Monogr. 4, 1–103. doi: 10.1037/h0030372
Baumrind, D. (2013). “Authoritative parenting revisited: history and current status,” in Authoritative parenting: Synthesizing Nurturance and Discipline for Optimal Child Development, eds R. E. Larzelere, A. S. Morris, and A. W. Harrist (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 11–34. doi: 10.1037/13948-002
Baumrind, D., Larzelere, R. E., and Owens, E. B. (2010). Effects of preschool Parents’ power assertive patterns and practices on adolescent development. Parenting 10, 157–201. doi: 10.1080/15295190903290790
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, Experimental and Theoretical Aspects. New York, NY: Harper and Row.
Beck, A. T., Davis, D. D., and Freeman, A. (eds) (2015). Cognitive Therapy of Personality Disorders. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Beck, A. T., and Haigh, E. A. (2014). Advances in cognitive theory and therapy: the generic cognitive model. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 10, 1–24. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153734
Beck, J. S. (2011). Cognitive Behavior Therapy. Basics and Beyond. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Behar, A. R., Manzo, G. R., and Casanova, Z. D. (2006). [Lack of assertiveness in patients with eating disorders]. Rev. Med. Chil. 134, 312–319.
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2005). Unwilling versus unable: infants’ understanding of intentional action. Dev. Psychol. 41, 328–337. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.2.328
Belden, A. C., Sullivan, J. P., and Luby, J. L. (2007). Depressed and healthy preschoolers’ internal representations of their mothers’ caregiving: associations with observed caregiving behaviors one year later. Attach. Hum. Dev. 9, 239–254. doi: 10.1080/14616730701455395
Berne, E. (1972). What do You Say After You Say Hello? The Psychology of Human Destiny. New York, NY: Bantam Books.
Bernier, A., Matte-Gagné, C., Bélanger, M. E., and Whipple, N. (2014). Taking stock of two decades of attachment transmission gap: broadening the assessment of maternal behavior. Child Dev. 85, 1852–1865.
Berry, K., and Danquah, A. (2016). Attachment-informed therapy for adults: towards a unifying perspective on practice. Psychol. Psychother. 89, 15–32. doi: 10.1111/papt.12063
Blatt, S. J., and Levy, K. N. (2003). Attachment theory, psychoanalysis, personality development, and psychopathology. Psychoanal. Inq. 23, 102–150. doi: 10.1080/07351692309349028
Borghi, A. M., and Fini, C. (2019). Theories and explanations in psychology. Front. Psychol. 10:958. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00958
Botto, S. V., and Rochat, P. (2018). Sensitivity to the evaluation of others emerges by 24 months. Dev. Psychol. 54, 1723–1734. doi: 10.1037/dev0000548
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and Loss, Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and Anger. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and Loss, Vol. 3. Loss: Sadness and Depression. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Boysan, M., and Çam, Z. (2016). An investigation into the role of attachment insecurities in obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Br. J. Guid. Couns. 46, 566–581. doi: 10.1080/03069885.2016.1262533
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., and Shaver, P. R. (1998). “Self-report measurement of adult attachment: an integrative overview,” in Attachment Theory and Close Relationships, eds J. A. Simpson and W. S. Rholes (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 46–76.
Bretherton, I. (1993). “The roots and growing points of attachment theory,” in Attachment Across the Life Cycle, eds C. M. Parkes, J. Stevenson-Hinde, and P. Marris (New York, NY: Routledge), 17–40. doi: 10.4324/9780203132470-5
Bretherton, I., and Munholland, K. A. (2008). “Internal working models in attachment relationships: elaborating a central construct in attachment theory,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 2nd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 102–127.
Brown, G. W., and Harris, T. (1978). Social Origins of Depression. A Study of Psychiatric Disorder in Women. London: Tavistock Publications.
Brumariu, L. E. (2015). Parent–child attachment and emotion regulation. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2015, 31–45. doi: 10.1002/cad.20098
Buono, L., Chau, R., Lewis, G., Madras, N., Pugh, M., Rossi, L., et al. (2006). “Mathematical models of mother/child attachment,” In Proceedings of the 2006 Fields–MITACS Industrial Problems Workshop, Toronto.
Cameron, J. J., Finnegan, H., and Morry, M. M. (2012). Orthogonal dreams in an oblique world: a meta-analysis of the association between attachment anxiety and avoidance. J. Res. Pers. 46, 472–476. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.05.001
Carcione, A., Nicolò, G., and Semerari, A. (eds) (2016). Curare i Casi Complessi. La terapia Metacognitiva Interpersonale dei Disturbi di Personalità. Bari: Laterza.
Carlin, J. D., and Calder, A. J. (2013). The neural basis of eye gaze processing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 23, 450–455. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.014
Carver, C. S. (1997). Adult attachment and personality: converging evidence and a new measure. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 23, 865–883. doi: 10.1177/0146167297238007
Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., and Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: stability and change. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 453–484. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141913
Cassidy, J., and Shaver, P. R. (eds) (1999). Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Cassidy, J., and Shaver, P. R. (eds) (2008). Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Cassidy, J., and Shaver, P. R. (eds) (2016). Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Chiesa, M., Cirasola, A., Williams, R., Nassisi, V., and Fonagy, P. (2017). Categorical and dimensional approaches in the evaluation of the relationship between attachment and personality disorders: an empirical study. Attach. Hum. Dev. 19, 151–169. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2016.1261915
Chisholm, J. S. (1996). The evolutionary ecology of attachment organization. Hum. Nat. 7, 1–37. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_1964-1
Chisholm, J. S., and Sieff, D. F. (2014). “Live fast, die young: an evolved response to hostile environments,” in Understanding and Healing Emotional Trauma: Conversations with Pioneering Clinicians and Researchers, ed. D. F. Sieff (New York, NY: Routledge), 163–181.
Chopik, W. J., Edelstein, R. S., and Fraley, R. C. (2013). From the cradle to the grave: age differences in attachment from early adulthood to old age. J. Pers. 81, 171–183. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00793.x
Coplan, R. J., and Arbeau, K. A. (2009). “Peer interactions and play in early childhood,” in Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups, eds K. H. Rubin, W. M. Bukowski, and B. Laursen (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 143–161.
Corr, P. J., and Matthews, G. (eds) (2009). The Cambridge Handbook of Personality Psychology. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Crittenden, P. M. (1995). “Attachment and psychopathology,” in Attachment Theory: Social, Developmental, and Clinical Perspectives, eds S. Goldberg, R. Muir, and J. Kerr (Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press), 367–406.
Crittenden, P. M. (2008). Raising Parents: Attachment, Parenting and Child Safety. Cullompton: Willan.
Crowell, J. A., Fraley, R. C., and Roisman, G. I. (2016). “Measurement of individual differences in adult attachment,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 598–635.
Dawson, M. R. W. (2004). Minds And Machines: Connectionism And Psychological Modeling. Hoboken, NJ: Blackwell.
De Wolff, M. S., and van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: a meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Dev. 68, 571–591. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb04218.x
DeKlyen, M., and Greenberg, M. T. (2016). “Attachment and psychopathology in childhood,” in Handbook of Attachment. Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 3rd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 639–666.
Dimaggio, G. (2015). Awareness of maladaptive interpersonal schemas as a core element of change in psychotherapy for personality disorders. J. Psychother. Integr. 25, 39–44. doi: 10.1037/a0038770
Dimaggio, G., Popolo, R., and Salvatore, G. (2019). “Chapter 8 - Formulation of functioning for avoidant personality disorder in metacognitive interpersonal therapy,” in Case Formulation for Personality Disorders, ed. U. Kramer (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 137–160. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-813521-1.00008-4
Dimaggio, G., Salvatore, G., MacBeth, A., Ottavi, P., Buonocore, L., and Popolo, R. (2017). Metacognitive interpersonal therapy for personality disorders: a case study series. J. Contemp. Psychother. 47, 11–21. doi: 10.1007/s10879-016-9342-7
Doron, G., Moulding, R., Kyrios, M., Nedeljkovic, M., and Mikulincer, M. (2009). Adult attachment insecurities are related to obsessive compulsive phenomena. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 28, 1022–1049. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2009.28.8.1022
Dweck, C. S. (2008). Can personality be changed? The role of beliefs in personality and change. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 391–394. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00612.x
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., and Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: the impact of harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Hum. Nat. 20, 204–268. doi: 10.1007/s12110-009-9063-7
Faber, A., Dubé, L., and Knäuper, B. (2018). Attachment and eating: a meta-analytic review of the relevance of attachment for unhealthy and healthy eating behaviors in the general population. Appetite 123, 410–438. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.10.043
Faravelli, C., Di Paola, F., Scarpato, M. A., and Fioravanti, G. (2010). Parental attitudes of mothers of patients with panic disorder. J. Psychopathol. 16, 144–149. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-5618.2003.00023.x
Faravelli, C., Panichi, C., Pallanti, S., Paterniti, S., Grecu, L. M., and Rivelli, S. (1991). Perception of early parenting in panic and agoraphobia. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 84, 6–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1991.tb01411.x
Farnfield, S., Hautamäki, A., Nørbech, P., and Sahhar, N. (2010). DMM assessments of attachment and adaptation: procedures, validity and utility. Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 15, 313–328. doi: 10.1177/1359104510364315
Feeney, J. A. (2016). “Adult romantic attachment. Developments in the study of couple relationships,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 3rd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 435–463.
Feldman, R. (2017). The neurobiology of human attachments. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 80–99. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.11.007
Fox, N. A. (2014). What do we know about sensitive periods in human development and How Do we know it? Hum. Dev. 57, 173–175. doi: 10.1159/000363663
Fraley, C. R. (2002). Attachment stability from infancy to adulthood: meta-analysis and dynamic modeling of developmental mechanisms. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6, 123–151. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0602_03
Fraley, R. C., Heffernan, M. E., Vicary, A. M., and Brumbaugh, C. C. (2011). The experiences in close relationships—Relationship structures questionnaire: a method for assessing attachment orientations across relationships. Psychol. Assess. 23, 615–625. doi: 10.1037/a0022898
Fraley, R. C., Hudson, N. W., Heffernan, M. E., and Segal, N. (2015). Are adult attachment styles categorical or dimensional? A taxometric analysis of general and relationship-specific attachment orientations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 109, 354–368. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000027
Fraley, R. C., and Roisman, G. I. (2019). The development of adult attachment styles: four lessons. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 25, 26–30. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.008
Fraley, R. C., and Shaver, P. R. (2008). “Attachment theory and its place in contemporary personality research,” in Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, 3rd Edn, eds O. John and R. W. Robins (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 518–541.
Fraley, R. C., and Spieker, S. J. (2003). Are infant attachment patterns continuously or categorically distributed? A taxometric analysis of strange situation behavior. Dev. Psychol. 39, 387–404. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.3.387
Friedman, H. S., and Schustack, M. W. (2015). Personality: Classic Theories and Modern Research. New York, NY: Pearson.
George, C., Kaplan, N., and Main, M. (1985). The Adult Attachment Interview. Berkeley, CA: University of California.
Gordon-King, K., Schweitzer, R. D., and Dimaggio, G. (2019). Metacognitive interpersonal therapy for personality disorders: the case of a man with obsessive–compulsive personality disorder and avoidant personality disorder. J. Contemp. Psychother. 49, 39–47. doi: 10.1007/s10879-018-9404-0
Gothard, K. M. (2014). The amygdalo-motor pathways and the control of facial expressions. Front. Neurosci. 8:43. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00043
Green, J., Stanley, C., Smith, V., and Goldwyn, R. (2000). A new method of evaluating attachment representations in young school-age children: the Manchester Child Attachment Story Task. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2, 48–70. doi: 10.1080/146167300361318
Guidano, V. F. (1987). Complexity of the Self: A Developmental Approach to Psychopathology and Therapy. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Guidano, V. F. (1991). The Self in Process: Toward a Post-Rationalist Cognitive Therapy. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Guidano, V. F. (2007). Psicoterapia Cognitiva post-Razionalista. Una ricognizione dalla Teoria alla Clinica. Milan: FrancoAngeli.
Guidano, V. F., and Liotti, G. (1983). Cognitive Processes and Emotional Disorders. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. J. Theor. Biol. 7, 1–52. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4
Harlow, H. F. (1959). Love in infant monkeys. Sci. Am. 200, 68–75. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0659-68
Harlow, H. F., and Harlow, M. K. (1962). Social deprivation in monkeys. Sci. Am. 207, 136–150. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican1162-136
Harlow, H. F., and Suomi, S. J. (1970). Nature of love - simplified. Am. Psychol. 25, 161–168. doi: 10.1037/h0029383
Harlow, H. F., and Zimmermann, R. R. (1959). Affectional responses in the infant monkey. Science 130, 421–432. doi: 10.1126/science.130.3373.421
Harrist, A. W., and Waugh, R. M. (2002). Dyadic synchrony: its structure and function in children’s development. Dev. Rev. 22, 555–592. doi: 10.1016/s0273-2297(02)00500-2
Hart, S. (2008). Brain, Attachment, Personality: An Introduction to Neuroaffective. London: Karnac Books Ltd.
Hartmann, A., Zeeck, A., and Barrett, M. S. (2010). Interpersonal problems in eating disorders. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 43, 619–627. doi: 10.1002/eat.20747
Hennighausen, K. H., and Lyons Ruth, K. (2005). “Disorganization of behavioral and attentional strategies toward primary attachment figures: from biologic to dialogic processes,” in Attachment and Bonding: A New Synthesis, eds S. Carter, L. Ahnert, K. E. Grassmann, S. Hrdy, M. Lamb, S. Porges, et al. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 269–301.
Hertler, S. C. (2015a). The evolutionary logic of the obsessive trait complex: obsessive compulsive personality disorder as a complementary behavioral syndrome. Psychol. Thought 8, 17–34. doi: 10.5964/psyct.v8i1.125
Hertler, S. C. (2015b). Obsessive compulsive personality disorder as an adaptive anachronism: the operation of Phylogenetic inertia upon obsessive populations in western modernity. Psychol. Top. 24, 207–232.
Hesse, E. (2008). “The adult attachment interview. protocol, method of analysis, and empirical studies,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 2nd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 552–598.
Hesse, E. (2016). “The adult attachment interview: protocol, method of analysis, and empirical studies: 1985–2015,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 3rd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 553–597.
Hoehl, S., Reid, V. M., Parise, E., Handl, A., Palumbo, L., and Striano, T. (2009). Looking at eye gaze processing and its neural correlates in infancy—Implications for social development and autism spectrum disorder. Child Dev. 80, 968–985. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01311.x
Hollenstein, T., Tighe, A. B., and Lougheed, J. P. (2017). Emotional development in the context of mother–child relationships. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 17, 140–144. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.010
Hopkins, J., Lavigne, J. V., Gouze, K. R., LeBailly, S. A., and Bryant, F. B. (2013). Multi-domain models of risk factors for depression and anxiety symptoms in preschoolers: evidence for common and specific factors. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 41, 705–722. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9723-2
Itier, R. J., and Batty, M. (2009). Neural bases of eye and gaze processing: the core of social cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 843–863. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.02.004
Ivarsson, T., Granqvist, P., Gillberg, C., and Broberg, A. G. (2010). Attachment states of mind in adolescents with Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder and/or depressive disorders: a controlled study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 19, 845–853. doi: 10.1007/s00787-010-0120-x
Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., and Vernon, P. A. (1996). Heritability of the big five personality dimensions and their facets: a twin study. J. Pers. 64, 577–591. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1996.tb00522.x
Karterud, S. W., and Kongerslev, M. T. (2019). A temperament-attachment-mentalization-based (TAM) Theory of Personality and Its Disorders. Front. Psychol. 10:518. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00518
Kendler, K. S., Myers, J., and Prescott, C. A. (2000). Parenting and adult mood, anxiety and substance use disorders in female twins: an epidemiological, multi-informant, retrospective study. Psychol. Med. 30, 281–294. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799001889
Kerns, K. A., and Brumariu, L. E. (2016). “Attachment in Middle Childhood,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 3rd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 349–365.
Knudsen, E. I. (2004). Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1412–1425. doi: 10.1162/0898929042304796
Knudsen, E. I. (2013). “Early experience and sensitive periods,” in Fundamental Neuroscience, 4th Edn, eds L. R. Squire, D. Berg, F. E. Bloom, S. du Lac, A. Ghosh, and N. C. Spitzer (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 479–495. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-385870-2.00021-4
Kobak, R. (1994). Adult attachment: A personality or relationship construct? Psychol. Inq. 5, 42–44. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0501_7
Kobak, R., Zajac, K., and Madsen, S. (2016). “Attachment disruptions, reparative processes, and psychopathology: theoretical and clinical implications,” in Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications, 3rd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 25–39.
Kochanska, G., Koenig, J. L., Barry, R. A., Kim, S., and Yoon, J. E. (2010). Children’s conscience during toddler and preschool years, moral self, and a competent, adaptive developmental trajectory. Dev. Psychol. 46, 1320–1332. doi: 10.1037/a0020381
Kossowsky, J., Pfaltz, M. C., Schneider, S., Taeymans, J., Locher, C., and Gaab, J. (2013). The separation anxiety hypothesis of panic disorder revisited: a meta-analysis. Am. J. Psychiatry 170, 768–781. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12070893
Landers, M. S., and Sullivan, R. M. (2012). The development and neurobiology of infant attachment and fear. Dev. Neurosci. 34, 101–114. doi: 10.1159/000336732
Leerkes, E. M., and Wong, M. S. (2012). Infant distress and regulatory behaviors vary as a function of attachment security regardless of emotion context and maternal involvement. Infancy 17, 455–478. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00099.x
Lennertz, L., Grabe, H. J., Ruhrmann, S., Rampacher, F., Vogeley, A., Schulze-Rauschenbach, S., et al. (2010). Perceived parental rearing in subjects with obsessive–compulsive disorder and their siblings. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 121, 280–288. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01469.x
Levy, K. N., Johnson, B. N., Clouthier, T. L., Scala, J., and Temes, C. M. (2015). An attachment theoretical framework for personality disorders. Can. Psychol. 56, 197–207. doi: 10.1037/cap0000025
Lewis, M. (2011). “The self-conscious emotions,” in Encyclopedia on Early Development: Emotions eds R. E. Tremblay, M. Boivin, R. D. Peters, and M. Lewis (Montreal, CA: The Center of Excellence for Early Childhood Development), 6–9.
Liotti, G. (1992). Disorganized/disoriented attachment in the etiology of the dissociative disorders. Dissociation 5, 196–204.
Liotti, G. (2004). Trauma, dissociation, and disorganized attachment: Three strands of a single braid. Psychotherapy 41, 472–486. doi: 10.1037/0033-3204.41.4.472
Liotti, G. (2009). “Attachment and dissociation,” in Dissociation and the Dissociative Disorders: DSM-V and Beyond, eds P. F. Dell and J. A. O’Neil (New York, NY: Routledge), 53–65.
Liotti, G. (2011). Attachment disorganization and the controlling strategies: an illustration of the contributions of attachment theory to developmental psychopathology and to psychotherapy integration. J. Psychother. Integr. 21, 232–252. doi: 10.1037/a0025422
Liotti, G., and Farina, B. (2011). Sviluppi Traumatici. Eziopatogenesi, Clinica e Terapia della Dimensione Dissociativa. Milan: Raffaello Cortina Editore.
Liotti, G., Fassone, G., and Monticelli, F. (eds) (2017). L’evoluzione delle Emozioni e dei Sistemi Motivazionali. Milan: Raffaello Cortina Editore.
Liotti, G., and Monticelli, F. (eds) (2008). I sistemi motivazionali nel dialogo clinico. Milan, IT: Raffaello Cortina Editore.
Liotti, G., and Monticelli, F. (2014). Teoria e Clinica Dell’alleanza Terapeutica. Una Prospettiva Cognitivo-Evoluzionista. Milan: Raffaello Cortina Editore.
Lorenzini, N., and Fonagy, P. (2013). Attachment and personality disorders: a short review. FOCUS 11, 155–166. doi: 10.1176/appi.focus.11.2.155
Loughead, J., Gur, R. C., Elliott, M., and Gur, R. E. (2008). Neural circuitry for accurate identification of facial emotions. Brain Res. 1194, 37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.105
Luby, J. L., Belden, A., Harms, M. P., Tillman, R., and Barch, D. M. (2016). Preschool is a sensitive period for the influence of maternal support on the trajectory of hippocampal development. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 5742–5747. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1601443113
Lucas, R. E., and Donnellan, M. B. (2011). Personality development across the life span: longitudinal analyses with a national sample from Germany. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 847–861. doi: 10.1037/a0024298
Lyons-Ruth, K., and Jacobvitz, D. (2008). “Attachment disorganization: genetic factors, parenting contexts, and developmental transformation from infancy to adulthood,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 2nd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 666–697.
Lyons-Ruth, K., and Jacobvitz, D. (2016). “Attachment disorganization from infancy to adulthood. Neurobiological correlates, parenting contexts, and pathways to disorder,” in Handbook of Attachment. Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 3rd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press).
Main, M., and Hesse, E. (1990). “Parents’ unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant disorganized attachment status: Is frightened and/or frightening parental behavior the linking mechanism?,” in Attachment During the Preschool Years: Theory, Research and Intervention (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press), 161–182.
Main, M., Kaplan, N., and Cassidy, J. (1985). “Security in infancy, childhood and adulthood: a move to the level of representation,” in Growing Points of Attachment Theory and Research Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50, eds I. Bretherton and E. Waters (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press), 66–106. doi: 10.2307/3333827
Main, M., and Solomon, J. (1990). “Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth strange situation,” in Attachment in the Preschool Years: Theory, Research, and Intervention, eds M. T. Greenberg, D. Cichetti, and E. M. Cummings (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press), 121–160.
Mallinckrodt, B. (2010). The psychotherapy relationship as attachment: evidence and implications. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 27, 262–270. doi: 10.1177/0265407509360905
Mancini, F., and Gangemi, A. (2015). Deontological guilt and obsessive compulsive disorder. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 49, 157–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.05.003
Marvin, R. S., Britner, P. A., and Russell, B. S. (2016). “Normative development. The ontogeny of attachment in childhood,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 3rd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 273–290.
Matte-Gagné, C., Harvey, B., Stack, D. M., and Serbin, L. A. (2015). Contextual specificity in the relationship between maternal autonomy support and children’s socio-emotional development: a longitudinal study from preschool to preadolescence. J. Youth Adolesc. 44, 1528–1541. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0247-z
McConnell, M., and Moss, E. (2011). Attachment across the life span: factors that contribute to stability and change. Aust. J. Educ. Dev. Psychol. 11, 60–77.
McCrae, R. R., and Costa, P. T. (2003). Personality in Adulthood: A Five-Factor Theory Perspective. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Mikulincer, M., and Shaver, P. R. (2012). “Attachment theory expanded,” in The Oxford handbook of Personality and Social Psychology, eds K. Deaux and M. Snyder (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 467–492.
Mikulincer, M., and Shaver, P. R. (2016). Attachment in Adulthood: Structure, Dynamics, and Change. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Mikulincer, M., and Shaver, P. R. (2019). Attachment orientations and emotion regulation. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 25, 6–10. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.006
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., and Pereg, D. (2003). Attachment theory and affect regulation: the dynamics, development, and cognitive consequences of attachment-related strategies. Motiv. Emot. 27, 77–102.
Minuchin, S., Rosman, B. L., and Baker, L. (1978). Psychosomatic Families: Anorexia Nervosa in Context. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Myhr, G., Sookman, D., and Pinard, G. (2004). Attachment security and parental bonding in adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparison with depressed out-patients and healthy controls. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 109, 447–456. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0047.2004.00271.x
Nakamori, T., Maekawa, F., Sato, K., Tanaka, K., and Ohki-Hamazaki, H. (2013). Neural basis of imprinting behavior in chicks. Dev. Growth Differ. 55, 198–206. doi: 10.1111/dgd.12028
Nardi, B., Arimatea, E., Giovagnoli, S., Blasi, S., Bellantuono, C., and Rezzonico, G. (2012). The mini questionnaire of personal organization (MQPO): Preliminary validation of a new post-rationalist personality questionnaire. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 19, 78–90. doi: 10.1002/cpp.740
Nardi, B., and Bellantuono, C. (2008). A new adaptive and evolutionary conceptualization of the Personal Meaning Organization (PMO) framework. Eur. Psychother. 8, 5–16.
Nardi, B., Rezzonico, G., and Bellantuono, C. (2010). Toward a scientific framework for the Personal Meaning Organization (PMO) paradigm: neuroimaging and genetic studies. Quad. Ital. Psichiatr. 29, 81–88. doi: 10.1016/j.quip.2010.07.003
Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H., Fox, N. A., Marshall, P. J., Smyke, A. T., and Guthrie, D. (2007). Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young children: the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Science 318, 1937–1940. doi: 10.1126/science.1143921
Nelson, K. (2007). Young Minds in Social Worlds: Experience, Meaning, and Memory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Nicolais, G., Fazeli-Fariz Hendi, S., Modesti, C., and Presaghi, F. (2017). Early moral conscience: the development of a moral short played stories procedure. Infant Ment. Health J. 38, 391–405. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21640
Noftle, E. E., and Shaver, P. R. (2006). Attachment dimensions and the big five personality traits: associations and comparative ability to predict relationship quality. J. Res. Pers. 40, 179–208. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2004.11.003
Obegi, J. H., and Berant, E. (2010). Attachment Theory and Research in Clinical Work with Adults. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Oltmanns, J. R., Jackson, J. J., and Oltmanns, T. F. (2019). Personality change: longitudinal self-other agreement and convergence with retrospective-reports. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 118, 1065–1079. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000238
Oltmanns, T. F., Martin, M. T., Davison, G. C., and Neale, J. M. (2012). Case Studies in Abnormal Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Opendak, M., Gould, E., and Sullivan, R. (2017). Early life adversity during the infant sensitive period for attachment: programming of behavioral neurobiology of threat processing and social behavior. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 145–159. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.02.002
Opendak, M., and Sullivan, R. M. (2016). Unique neurobiology during the sensitive period for attachment produces distinctive infant trauma processing. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 7, 31276. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v7.31276
Opendak, M., and Sullivan, R. M. (2019). Unique infant neurobiology produces distinctive trauma processing. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 36:100637. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100637
Osmo, F., Duran, V., Wenzel, A., de Oliveira, I. R., Nepomuceno, S., Madeira, M., et al. (2018). The negative core beliefs inventory: development and psychometric properties. J. Cogn. Psychother. 32, 67–84. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.32.1.67
Otowa, T., Gardner, C. O., Kendler, K. S., and Hettema, J. M. (2013). Parenting and risk for mood, anxiety and substance use disorders: a study in population-based male twins. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 48, 1841–1849. doi: 10.1007/s00127-013-0656-4
Otowa, T., York, T. P., Gardner, C. O., Kendler, K. S., and Hettema, J. M. (2014). The impact of childhood parental loss on risk for mood, anxiety and substance use disorders in a population-based sample of male twins. Psychiatry Res. 220, 404–409. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.07.053
Paetzold, R. L., Rholes, W. S., and Kohn, J. L. (2015). Disorganized attachment in adulthood: theory, measurement, and implications for romantic relationships. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 19, 146–156. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000042
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundation of Human and Animal Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Panksepp, J., and Biven, L. (2012). The Archaeology of Mind: Neuroevolutionary Origins of Human Emotions. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.
Parker, G. (1983). Parental ‘affectionless control’ as an antecedent to adult depression: a risk factor delineated. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 40, 956–960. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1983.01790080038005
Parkes, C. M., Stevenson-Hinde, J., and Marris, P. (1993). Attachment Across the Life Cycle. New York, NY: Routledge.
Peer, M., Salomon, R., Goldberg, I., Blanke, O., and Arzy, S. (2015). Brain system for mental orientation in space, time, and person. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 11072–11077. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1504242112
Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., and McCarthy, G. (2005). Neural basis of eye gaze processing deficits in autism. Brain 128, 1038–1048. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh404
Perdighe, C., and Mancini, F. (eds) (2010). Elementi di Psicoterapia Cognitiva. Rome: Giovanni Fioriti Editore.
Petters, D., and Beaudoin, L. (2017). “Attachment modelling: from observations to scenarios to designs,” in Computational Neurology and Psychiatry, eds P. Érdi, B. Sen Bhattacharya, and A. L. Cochran (New York, NY: Springer), 227–271. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-49959-8_9
Picardi, A., Mannino, G., Arciero, G., Gaetano, P., Pilleri, M. F., Arduini, L., et al. (2003). Costruzione e validazione del QSP, uno strumento per la valutazione dello stile di personalità secondo la teoria delle “organizzazioni di significato personale”. Riv. Psichiatr. 38, 13–34.
Pinquart, M., Feußner, C., and Ahnert, L. (2013). Meta-analytic evidence for stability in attachments from infancy to early adulthood. Attach. Hum. Dev. 15, 189–218. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2013.746257
Platts, H., Tyson, M., and Mason, O. (2002). Adult attachment style and core beliefs: Are they linked? Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 9, 332–348. doi: 10.1002/cpp.345
Power, R. A., and Pluess, M. (2015). Heritability estimates of the Big Five personality traits based on common genetic variants. Transl. Psychiatry 5:e604. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.96
Prescott, T. J., and Camilleri, D. (2018). “The synthetic psychology of the self,” in Cognitive Architectures, eds M. I. Aldinhas Ferreira, J. Silva Sequeira, and R. Ventura (New York, NY: Springer), 85–104.
Rao, H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L., Korczykowski, M., Avants, B. B., et al. (2010). Early parental care is important for hippocampal maturation: evidence from brain morphology in humans. Neuroimage 49, 1144–1150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.003
Reindl, V., Gerloff, C., Scharke, W., and Konrad, K. (2018). Brain-to-brain synchrony in parent-child dyads and the relationship with emotion regulation revealed by fNIRS-based hyperscanning. Neuroimage 178, 493–502. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.060
Rezvan, S., Bahrami, F., Abedi, M., MacLeod, C., Doost, H. T. N., and Ghasemi, V. (2012). Attachment insecurity as a predictor of obsessive–compulsive symptoms in female children. Couns. Psychol. Q. 25, 403–415. doi: 10.1080/09515070.2012.736156
Roberts, B. W., and Caspi, A. (2003). “The cumulative continuity model of personality development: Striking a balance between continuity and change in personality traits across the life course,” in Understanding Human Development, eds U. M. Staudinger and U. Lindenberger (Boston, MA: Springer), 183–214. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-0357-6_9
Roberts, B. W., Caspi, A., and Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Work experiences and personality development in young adulthood. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 582–593. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.3.582
Roberts, B. W., and Mroczek, D. (2008). Personality trait change in adulthood. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 31–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x
Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., and Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol. Bull. 132, 1–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1
Robinson, C. C., Mandleco, B., Olsen, S. F., and Hart, C. H. (1995). Authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive parenting practices: development of a new measure. Psychol. Rep. 77, 819–830. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1995.77.3.819
Roisman, G. I., Fraley, R. C., and Belsky, J. (2007). A taxometric study of the adult attachment interview. Dev. Psychol. 43, 675–686. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.3.675
Rosa-Mendes, M., Pires, R., and Ferreira, A. S. (2019). Personality traits of the alternative DSM-5 model and the attachment dimensions in Portuguese adults. Pers. Individ. Differ. 143, 21–29. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.02.007
Roth, T. L., Barr, G. A., and Lewis, M. J. (2016). “Neurobiology and programming capacity of attachment learning to nurturing and abusive caregivers,” in Environmental Experience and Plasticity of the Developing Brain, ed. A. Sale (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley).
Safran, J. D., and Segal, Z. V. (1996). Interpersonal Process in Cognitive Therapy. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.
Salzen, E. A. (1967). Imprinting in birds and primates. Behaviour 28, 232–254. doi: 10.1163/156853967x00028
Schmidt, U., Jiwany, A., and Treasure, J. (1993). A controlled study of alexithymia in eating disorders. Compr. Psychiatry 34, 54–58. doi: 10.1016/0010-440X(93)90036-4
Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect Regulation and the Origin of the Self: The Neurobiology of Emotional Development. New York, NY: Routledge.
Schore, A. N. (2000). Attachment and the regulation of the right brain. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2, 23–47. doi: 10.1080/146167300361309
Schore, A. N. (2005). Attachment, affect regulation, and the developing right brain: linking developmental neuroscience to pediatrics. Pediatr. Rev. 26, 204–217. doi: 10.1542/pir.26-6-204
Schore, A. N. (2009). “Attachment trauma and the developing right brain: origins of pathological dissociation,” in Dissociation and the Dissociative Disorders: DSM-V and Beyond, eds P. F. Dell and J. A. O’Neil (New York, NY: Routledge), 107–141.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness. On Depression, Development, and Death. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman.
Sette, G., Coppola, G., and Cassibba, R. (2015). The transmission of attachment across generations: The state of art and new theoretical perspectives. Scand. J. Psychol. 56, 315–326. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12212
Shafran, R., Watkins, E., and Charman, T. (1996). Guilt in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Anxiety Disord. 10, 509–516. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(96)00026-6
Shaver, P. R., and Mikulincer, M. (2002). Attachment-related psychodynamics. Attach. Hum. Dev. 4, 133–161. doi: 10.1080/14616730210154171
Sherman, L. J., Rice, K., and Cassidy, J. (2015). Infant capacities related to building internal working models of attachment figures: a theoretical and empirical review. Dev. Rev. 37, 109–141. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2015.06.001
Shipley, W. U. (1963). The demonstration in the domestic guinea pig of a process resembling classical imprinting. Anim. Behav. 11, 470–474. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(63)90263-x
Silove, D., Harris, M., Morgan, A., Boyce, P., Manicavasagar, V., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., et al. (1995). Is early separation anxiety a specific precursor of panic disorder–agoraphobia? A community study. Psychol. Med. 25, 405–411. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700036291
Simpson, J. A., and Belsky, J. (2016). “Attachment theory within a modern evolutionary framework,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 3rd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 91–116.
Skinner, E., Johnson, S., and Snyder, T. (2005). Six dimensions of parenting: a motivational model. Parent. Sci. Pract. 5, 175–235. doi: 10.1207/s15327922par0502_3
Slavich, G. M., Monroe, S. M., and Gotlib, I. H. (2011). Early parental loss and depression history: associations with recent life stress in major depressive disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 45, 1146–1152. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.03.004
Sroufe, L. A. (1995). Emotional Development: The Organization of Emotional Life in the Early Years. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Sroufe, L. A. (2005). Attachment and development: a prospective, longitudinal study from birth to adulthood. Attach. Hum. Dev. 7, 349–367. doi: 10.1080/14616730500365928
Stern, D. N. (1985). The Interpersonal World of the Infant - A View from Psychoanalysis and Developmental Psychology. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Stevens, G. T., and Zhang, J. (2009). A dynamic systems model of infant attachment. IEEE Trans. Auton. Ment. Dev. 1, 196–207. doi: 10.1109/tamd.2009.2038190
Stievenart, M., Roskam, I., Meunier, J. C., and Vandemoortele, G. (2012). Assessment of preschoolers’ attachment security using the Attachment Q-set and the Attachment Story Completion Task. Int. J. Educ. Psychol. Assess. 12, 62–80.
Stovall-McClough, K. C., and Dozier, M. (2016). “Attachment states of mind and psychopathology in adulthood,” in Handbook of Attachment. Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 3rd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 715–738.
Sullivan, R. M., and Holman, P. J. (2010). Transitions in sensitive period attachment learning in infancy: the role of corticosterone. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 835–844. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.010
Suomi, S. J., Harlow, H. F., and Novak, M. A. (1974). Reversal of social deficits produced by isolation rearing in monkeys. J. Hum. Evol. 3, 527–534. doi: 10.1016/0047-2484(74)90013-x
Szepsenwol, O., and Simpson, J. A. (2019). Attachment within life history theory: an evolutionary perspective on individual differences in attachment. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 25, 65–70. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.03.005
Target, M., Fonagy, P., and Shmueli-Goetz, Y. (2003). Attachment representations in school-age children: the development of the Child Attachment Interview (CAI). J. Child Psychother. 29, 171–186. doi: 10.1080/0075417031000138433
Tasca, G. A., and Balfour, L. (2014). Attachment and eating disorders: a review of current research. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 47, 710–717. doi: 10.1002/eat.22302
Thompson, R. A. (2016). “Early attachment and later development: reframing the questions,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 3rd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 330–348.
Timpano, K. R., Keough, M. E., Mahaffey, B., Schmidt, N. B., and Abramowitz, J. (2010). Parenting and obsessive compulsive symptoms: implications of authoritarian parenting. J. Cogn. Psychother. 24, 151–164. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.24.3.151
Troller-Renfree, S., and Fox, N. A. (2017). “Sensitive periods of development. implications for risk and resilience,” in Handbook of Preschool Mental Health. Development, Disorders, and Treatment, 2nd Edn, ed. J. Luby (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 3–26. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-065302-7.50009-7
Turnbull, O., and Solms, M. (2003). “Memory, amnesia and intuition: a neuro-psychoanalytic perspective,” in Emotional Development in Psychoanalysis, Attachment Theory and Neuroscience, ed. V. Green (New York, NY: Routledge), 63–92. doi: 10.4324/9780203420362-11
Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1995). Adult attachment representations, parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: a meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the Adult Attachment Interview. Psychol. Bull. 117, 387–403. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.387
Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., and Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2019). Bridges across the intergenerational transmission of attachment gap. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 25, 31–36. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.014
Varin, D., Crugnola, C. R., Molina, P., and Ripamonti, C. (1996). Sensitive periods in the development of attachment and the age of entry into day care. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 11, 215–229. doi: 10.1007/bf03172726
Verhage, M. L., Schuengel, C., Madigan, S., Fearon, R. M. P., Oosterman, M., Cassibba, R., et al. (2016). Narrowing the transmission gap: a synthesis of three decades of research on intergenerational transmission of attachment. Psychol. Bull. 142, 337–366. doi: 10.1037/bul0000038
Viddal, K. R., Berg-Nielsen, T. S., Belsky, J., and Wichstrøm, L. (2017). Change in attachment predicts change in emotion regulation particularly among 5-HTTLPR short-allele homozygotes. Dev. Psychol. 53, 1316–1329. doi: 10.1037/dev0000321
Wang, S., Yu, R. J., Tyszka, J. M., Zhen, S. S., Kovach, C., Sun, S., et al. (2017). The human amygdala parametrically encodes the intensity of specific facial emotions and their categorical ambiguity. Nat. Commun. 8:14821. doi: 10.1038/ncomms14821
Waters, E., Merrick, S., Treboux, D., Crowell, J., and Albersheim, L. (2000). Attachment security in infancy and early adulthood: a twenty-year longitudinal study. Child Dev. 71, 684–689. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00176
Westwood, H., Kerr-Gaffney, J., Stahl, D., and Tchanturia, K. (2017). Alexithymia in eating disorders: systematic review and meta-analyses of studies using the Toronto Alexithymia Scale. J. Psychosom. Res. 99, 66–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.06.007
Whalen, D. J., Sylvester, C. M., and Luby, J. L. (2017). Depression and anxiety in preschoolers: a review of the past 7 years. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 26, 503–522. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2017.02.006
Whalen, P. J., Raila, H., Bennett, R., Mattek, A., Brown, A., Taylor, J., et al. (2013). Neuroscience and facial expressions of emotion: the role of amygdala-prefrontal interactions. Emot. Rev. 5, 78–83. doi: 10.1177/1754073912457231
Whipple, N., Bernier, A., and Mageau, G. A. (2011). A dimensional approach to maternal attachment state of mind: relations to maternal sensitivity and maternal autonomy support. Dev. Psychol. 47, 396–403. doi: 10.1037/a0021310
Widiger, T. A., Gore, W. L., Crego, C., Rojas, S. L., and Oltmanns, J. R. (2017). “Five factor model and personality disorder,” in The Oxford Handbook of the Five Factor Model, ed. T. A. Widiger (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 449–478.
Yakeley, J. (2018). Psychoanalysis in modern mental health practice. Lancet Psychiatry 5, 443–450. doi: 10.1016/s2215-0366(18)30052-x
Young, E. S., Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., Huelsnitz, C. O., and Fleck, C. (2019). Childhood attachment and adult personality: a life history perspective. Self Identity 18, 22–38. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2017.1353540
Young, J. E. (2002). “Schema-focused therapy for personality disorders,” in Cognitive Behaviour Therapy: A Guide for the Practising Clinician, ed. G. Simos (London: Routledge), 201–222.
Keywords: attachment, personality, psychopathology, cognitive, representation, dimensions, core beliefs, imprinting
Citation: Gagliardi M (2021) How Our Caregivers Shape Who We Are: The Seven Dimensions of Attachment at the Core of Personality. Front. Psychol. 12:657628. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.657628
Received: 23 January 2021; Accepted: 09 June 2021;
Published: 01 July 2021.
Edited by:
Katrin Simone Heimann, Aarhus University, DenmarkReviewed by:
Paula Thomson, California State University, Northridge, United StatesIris Chi, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, United States
Copyright © 2021 Gagliardi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Marcantonio Gagliardi, mgagliardi1@sheffield.ac.uk; orcid.org/0000-0001-7275-6315