Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 17 March 2021
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic COVID-19 and Existential Positive Psychology (PP2.0): The New Science of Self-Transcendence View all 49 articles

Meaning-Centered Coping in the Era of COVID-19: Direct and Moderating Effects on Depression, Anxiety, and Stress

Updated
  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Almería, Almería, Spain
  • 2Department of Personality, Evaluation and Psychological Treatment, Faculty of Psychology, University of Seville, Seville, Spain
  • 3Chair of History and Philosophy of Mathematical Sciences, Department of Humanities, Social and Political Sciences, ETH Zürich, Zurich, Switzerland

The COVID-19 pandemic has subjected most of the world’s population to unprecedented situations, like national lockdowns, health hazards, social isolation and economic harm. Such a scenario calls for urgent measures not only to palliate it but also, to better cope with it. According to existential positive psychology, well-being does not simply represent a lack of stress and negative emotions but highlights their importance by incorporating an adaptive relationship with them. Thus, suffering can be mitigated (and transformed into growth) by, among other factors, adopting an attitude of positive reframing, maintaining hope, existential courage, life appreciation, engagement in meaningful activities, and prosociality. The conglomerate of these elements has been recently denominated as meaning-centered coping. In this study, we evaluated the protective role of this type of coping on mental health. A sample of 12,243 participants from 30 countries across all continents completed measures of Meaning-Centered Coping Scale (MCCS), depression, stress, anxiety and stressful COVID-19 related conditions they experienced. Results indicated that meaning-centered coping was strongly associated with diminished symptoms of stress, anxiety, and depression. Moreover, it moderated various relationships between vulnerability factors and markers of psychological distress, especially in the case of depression. These findings call for attention to meaning-centered coping approaches in the context of hardship, such as the current COVID-19 health crisis. In these difficult times, decision-makers and health organizations may integrate these approaches into their guidelines.

Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic has drastically affected people’s lives, negatively impacting multiple markers of physical and mental health (Cullen et al., 2020; Luo et al., 2020; Serafini et al., 2020; Sher, 2020). The conditions that produce these effects, such as enforced lockdowns and social isolation, are hardly avoidable or ameliorable. Focusing on the promotion of the already extended view that a very stressful situation like this leads to a proportional decrease in mental health is not sufficient. As such, an investigation of potential coping mechanisms against the specific conditions brought about by the pandemic would benefit millions of people.

In order to understand the significance and possible effects of stressful situations in psychology today, its virtues, its flaws and its exploits, we may look at the evolution of the concept of stress. At first, in psychology, a biomedical conceptualization of stress was adapted (e.g., Basowitz et al., 1955; Bliss et al., 1956; Tan and Yip, 2018), understanding it mainly as an unpleasant state of emotional and physiological arousal, carrying the assumptions that it is mostly harmful, and there is a somewhat linear relationship between the strain and the psychological response. This conceptualization carried inklings of linearity and negative value into psychology from its origins in physics and physiology (see for instance, how Selye’s findings in physiology led to the scientific legitimization of post-traumatic stress disorder in the DSM-III; Yehuda and McFarlane, 1995). General psychological distress can be divided into various facets, and oftentimes there is no terminological agreement among researchers. Although other definitions may be equally valid, this paper will adapt the terminology of Lovibond and Lovibond (1995) and Brown et al. (1997) which is fairly consistent with the tripartite model of anxiety and depression (Clark and Watson, 1991). That is, anxiety is defined by a physiological hyperarousal and subjective experience of negative affect, stress consists of the cognitive representation of said affect (tension, irritability, and getting easily frustrated) and depression is mostly but not exclusively characterized by a general absence of positive affect (dysphoria, hopelessness, etc.). It is indeed true, that, at least to some extent, stressful situations typically lead to increased stress, anxiety, and depression on the population level, as it could be observed during the COVID-19 pandemic (e.g., Salari et al., 2020).

However, we are far from assuming that stressors lead to psychological problems in all cases. It is well researched that protective and vulnerability/risk factors may make a person more or less prone to react with psychological distress to stressors (see for instance risk factors of depression: Dobson and Dozois, 2011). For example, during collective traumas, socially disadvantaged groups generally show worse psychological outcomes than their more advantaged counterparts (e.g., Reid, 2013). Several authors noted that mental health consequences due to the COVID-19 pandemic are more pronounced among vulnerable groups, such as females, younger people, people with low income levels, students, and those who are diagnosed with a mental health disorder or have a preexisting physical health condition (e.g., Alonzi et al., 2020; Asmundson et al., 2020; Fitzpatrick et al., 2020; Iob et al., 2020; Kajdy et al., 2020; Luo et al., 2020; Pieh et al., 2020; Purtle, 2020; Vahedian-Azimi et al., 2020; Xiong et al., 2020).

Beyond vulnerabilities and protective factors, we have a certain capability of transforming our psychological responses even in the most stressful situations, thus disrupting the linearity between stressors and our reactions to them. According to Lazarus (1993), some of the most relevant psychological aspects that define our reactions to stressors are coping (efforts to make a specific action related to the stressor) and appraisal (the person’s evaluation of the significance of the situation related to their well-being). Traditionally, coping mechanisms have been classified as problem or emotion-focused (see Folkman and Lazarus, 1984). Problem-focused coping strategies aim to resolve some aspect of the stressor, while emotion-focused strategies represent the attempt to regulate one’s emotions in the face of adversity. Data showed that during the COVID-19 pandemic, certain types of psychological coping mechanisms, especially problem-focused ones, were related to better mental health outcomes (e.g., Cai et al., 2020; Dawson and Golijani-Moghaddam, 2020; Huang et al., 2020).

Appraisal of stressful situations to optimize affective responses seems to be equally important (for a comprehensive review, see Jamieson et al., 2018). A stressor can be conceived as a challenge or as a threat, and depending on how it is appraised, it may have a very different effect on the individual and their coping strategy. Positive reappraisals and positive meanings influence the emotions the person experiences in the stressful encounter and can activate coping processes that support the positive affect domain (e.g., Folkman and Moskowitz, 2000). For instance, in a recent study, threat, challenge, centrality, and controllability appraisals regarding the COVID-19 health crisis were related to subjective well-being (Zacher and Rudolph, 2020).

Understanding this phenomenon can be useful to develop strategies to approach stressors also as challenges and opportunities for growth, instead of looking at them merely as threats to be ameliorated or avoided. For instance, in Chinese, “stress” is translated as “crisis,” which is written with the characters for “danger” and “opportunity.” This characterization in a way depicts the dialectic between harm and growth, which is in line with the conceptualization of psychological distress offered by existential positive psychology (Wong, 2009, 2011a) also known as PP2.0 (Wong, 2011b). Existential positive psychology not only focuses on the buffering effects of positive emotions and personal strengths in stressful situations (which are relevant contributions of positive psychology, see for instance Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi, 2000; Seligman, 2004), but also on the process of harnessing positive potentials from negative aspects of living, connecting the negative with the positive (e.g., Wong, 2011b; Kashdan and Biswas-Diener, 2014; Ivtzan et al., 2015). Indeed, the acceptance of negative affect seems to be necessary for healthy functioning (e.g., Levin et al., 2014).

The existential positive framework emphasizes meaning in life, virtue, resilience, and well-being as the pillars of personal flourishing. There is a vast literature showing a protective role of these processes. In particular, meaning in life seems to act as a buffer against psychological distress and foster resilience (e.g., Batthyany and Russo-Netzer, 2014; Gloria and Steinhardt, 2016). It also seems to play a transformative and rehabilitative role in disorders related to potentially traumatic events (Calhoun and Tedeschi, 2006; Khanna and Greyson, 2015) and substance abuse (Carreno and Pérez-Escobar, 2019). Especially, meaning in life has been shown to be negatively associated with depression both in cross-sectional studies (e.g., Steger et al., 2006; Carreno et al., 2020) and longitudinal ones (e.g., Disabato et al., 2017). According to systematic reviews and meta-analyses, interventions that foster meaning in life generally seem to reduce psychological distress and improve well-being (e.g., Vos, 2016; Vos and Vitali, 2018). These interventions seem to be especially effective among cancer patients, fostering improvements in spiritual well-being, quality of life, depression, and anxiety, among other areas (e.g., Breitbart et al., 2010; Chochinov et al., 2011). During the COVID-19 health crisis, a number of studies showed that higher levels of meaning in life were associated with lower levels of psychological distress (e.g., Arslan and Yildirim, 2020; Chao et al., 2020; Milman et al., 2020).

Based on Viktor E. Frankl’s work (1969,1985), Wong (1993, 2020a, b) proposed a coping style to face crises in a resilient and transformative way, involving strategies such as courage, responsibility, life appreciation, acceptance without judgment, positive reframing, self-transcendence, faith and hope (see also Wong et al., 2006). A recent study (Eisenbeck et al., unpublished) attempted to operationalize these ideas by creating a short scale that intends to encompass various elements of this approximation, denominated as the Meaning-Centered Coping Scale (MCCS). This instrument measures a set of attitudes and behaviors that include positive reframing of adversity, maintaining hope, existential courage, life appreciation, engagement in meaningful activities, and prosociality (see Eisenbeck et al., unpublished). Elements of this conceptualization of meaning-centered coping are well-studied, show strong relationships with both higher levels of well-being and decreased psychological distress (e.g., Nakamura and Csikzentmihalyi, 2003; Feldman and Snyder, 2005; Schueller and Seligman, 2010; Kleiman et al., 2013; Maddi, 2013; Van Tongeren et al., 2016; Klein, 2017; Jans-Beken and Wong, 2019) and are incorporated in the theory of PP2.0.

This coping style thus involves strategies that may prevent the development of psychopathological symptoms such as psychological stress, anxiety, and depression in situations such as the COVID-19 pandemic. Meaning-centered coping also intends to be “transformative,” as it deals with appraisal and coping processes that may help to maintain the positive affect domain even in the presence of negative affect, such as endowing stressful situations and emotions with a different, more positive meaning that can ultimately lead to growth (e.g., being grateful for parts of the experience) which is one of the central ideas of PP2.0. According to the tripartite model (Clark and Watson, 1991), anxiety, stress, and depression share the element of negative affect to some extent but only depression is characterized by low levels of positive affect. Based on its focus of transforming the negative to positive, it may be feasible to hypothesize that a meaning-centered coping style may be strongly correlated with depression (similarly to the sense of meaning in life which this coping style fosters, as it suggested by previous studies, see Steger et al., 2006). This hypothesized protective role against psychological distress and especially against depression may be extremely relevant during a global crisis like the COVID-19 pandemic, which entails a great deal of mostly unavoidable psychological hardship due to social isolation, confinement and fear of getting infected, among other factors.

In light of the aforementioned theoretical considerations, in the present study we analyzed the relationships between individual vulnerability factors, psychological distress and meaning-centered coping, hypothesizing that meaning-centered coping may play a role in this equation at various levels: directly affecting facets of psychological distress and by moderating the effect of vulnerability factors on psychological distress. We expect that these findings would be independent of the country of origin of the individuals.

Specifically, we postulate the following hypotheses:

H1. Certain demographic characteristics of the participants, such as gender, age, economic status and previous mental disorder diagnosis will be associated with increased levels of stress, anxiety, and depression (demographic vulnerability factors).

H2. Potentially harmful and more or less objectively measurable effects of the pandemic will also be related to markers of psychological distress, such as perceived lifestyle changes, economic and physical health effects of the pandemic, length of confinement and adherence to confinement (COVID-19 related vulnerability factors).

H3. Meaning-centered coping will be a negative predictor of all markers of psychopathology.

H4. The impact of the aforementioned risk factors on depression, anxiety and stress levels will be mitigated by higher levels of meaning-centered coping.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 12,243 people from 30 countries participated in this study. Most of them were female and their average age was 35.44 years (SD = 13.24, range: 18–85). A detailed description of demographic variables can be observed in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics of the sample.

Measures

Depression, Stress, and Anxiety

The Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS-21; Henry and Crawford, 2005), which is the short version of the original, 42-item DASS scale (see Brown et al., 1997) was used in this study. The questionnaire contains 21 items with three subscales measuring depression, anxiety and stress (7 items each). Respondents have to rate each item on a scale of 0 (did not apply to me at all) to 3 (applied to me very much, or most of the time). As raw scores are typically multiplied, total scores are 42 on each subscale and 126 on the total scale (referred to as total psychological distress). Higher scores indicate higher levels of psychological problems. The measure showed good psychometric properties in the past (e.g., Henry and Crawford, 2005). Alphas in the present sample ranged between 0.83 and 0.94.

Meaning-Centered Coping

The MCCS (Eisenbeck et al., unpublished) measures a coping strategy based on the creation of personal meaning, as proposed by existential positive psychology. The measure includes 9 items about life appreciation (e.g., “I am grateful for my life as it is.”) maintaining hope, positive reframing, courage (e.g., “I will get out of this situation stronger than I was before.”) and involvement in meaningful and prosocial activities (e.g., “I help others during this time.”). Items are rated on a 7-point Likert scale from (I do not agree at all) to 7 (I completely agree). Validation of the questionnaire showed a stable one-factor solution, good internal consistency, convergent and divergent validity, and test–retest data in all languages used in the present study (Eisenbeck et al., unpublished). In the present sample, Cronbach’s alphas were between 0.81 and 0.95 in each country (total sample: α = 0.88).

Possible Risk Factors

On one hand, demographic data (age, gender, economic status, education level, and the existence of previous mental health disorder diagnosis) that can pose a risk factor for depression, stress, and anxiety during the COVID-19 health crisis were collected. On the other hand, self-reported data about variables that more or less can be reliably assessed and reported by the participants regarding the intensity of some of the effects of the pandemic were collected. The following variables were measured: (a) economic impact (“This pandemic affects me economically for the worse.”); (b) physical health effects (“This pandemic negatively affects my physical health, although not infected.”); (c) general lifestyle changes (“This pandemic has caused me significant lifestyle changes.”); (d) adherence to confinement rules (“I stay at home except for strictly necessary things (e.g., to go to work, get food or medicine); and (e) duration of confinement (“If you have had to stay at home due to the pandemic, how many days has it been since you started staying home?”). Items were measured on Likert scales from 1 (I do not agree at all) to 7 (I completely agree) with the exception of the last one.

In addition, country-level data that may be related to the psychological distress levels of people during the COVID-19 pandemic were included in the study: (a) objective severity index of the pandemic during data collection in each country: countries with fewer than 100 reported infections per million inhabitants were assigned a severity level of 1, countries with between 100 and 300 infections per million inhabitants a severity level of 2, and countries with more than 2000 cases per million inhabitants a severity level of 3 (data obtained from worldmeters.info); and (b) GDP of each participating country (source: worldbank.org).

Procedure

The present research was part of a larger international project assessing the psychological effects of the COVID-19 pandemic between March and June 2020. Investigators were recruited through ResearchGate announcements. Interested researchers were included in the project if they were willing to adapt the questionnaire package to their languages and to recruit approximately 250 or more participants in their respective countries. All adaptations were realized according to the best practices (Beaton et al., 2000). The researchers recruited the study participants through direct e-mail invitations (i.e., they sent emails to their professional and personal contacts) and social media announcements (Facebook and Instagram). No incentives were given to the participants, with the exception of part of the Canadian sample that was recruited via MTurk. The participation was voluntary and anonymous. Nevertheless, they were given the option to provide their e-mail addresses if they wished to participate in similar studies in the future. This information was handled separately from their other responses in order to maintain anonymity. Respondents filled in the survey (demographic data, risk factors, the DASS-21 and the MCCS) in approximately 10 min. At the end of the participation, they were fully debriefed.

Data Analysis

Data were tested with SPSS (Version 25). Prior to the analysis, data were tested for normality and missing values. Data were removed for respondents who left at least 50% of any given questionnaire unfinished or showed straight-lining. People with COVID-19 diagnosis were less than 1% of the dataset, and they were excluded. Missing data in the formal questionnaires was less than 0.1%, (missing completely at random) and were replaced with the expectation-maximization procedure. Missing data on demographic variables the duration of confinement was 0.21%. It was mostly due to technical error, as age was not measured in 136 cases in the US sample. Because of the low percentage of demographic missing data, they were replaced with the country average. Duration of the confinement was dichotomized to represent values below and above the median of the sample (Mdn = 21 days). Possible outliers were not removed from the database. Data from different countries were compared with analyses of variance (continuous variables) and with Cramer’s V (dichotomous variables).

Four separate analyses of multilevel modeling (MLM) on depression, anxiety, stress, and the total DASS-21 scores were conducted to assess the effects of individual and country-level variables and the moderating role of the MCCS, using the restricted maximum likelihood method. Education status was dichotomized for this analysis to assess possible effects of student status on mental health. First, baseline models with random intercepts were tested to determine whether MLM was warranted. Models were compared to the full, nested models by χ2 difference tests using the maximum likelihood estimation method. The moderator role of the MCCS between the predictor and outcome variables was further assessed with simple slopes at different levels of the moderator variable: at low (−1 SD: 9–35) points), mean (0 SD: 36–58 points), and high levels (+1 SD: 59–63 points) using the PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2017). Simple slopes were computed in all cases, even when the main interaction term was not significant to assess possible trends. For easier interpretation and comparability, the use of standardized variables was maintained throughout the analyses.

Results

As expected, samples from different country of origin significantly differed on almost all demographic variables, such as gender, Cramer’s V = 0.330, p < 0.001, age, F(29) = 89.07, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.175, economic status, F(29) = 19.05, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.043, education level F(29) = 39.04, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.085, mental disorder diagnosis, Cramer’s V = 0.265, p < 0.001, percentage of students, Cramer’s V = 0.325, p < 0.001, and duration of the confinement, Cramer’s V = 0.625, p < 0.001. Differences were also observed on depression, F(29) = 27.07, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.062, anxiety, F(29) = 25.71, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.058, stress, F(29) = 31.05, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.069, the DASS-21 total scores, F(29) = 28.42, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.063, and the MCCS, F(29) = 26.00, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.058, These results warrant the inclusion of the variable country into all analyses of the study. Detailed data of the variables measured in each country can be observed in Tables 1, 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Average data of the measures of the study in each country.

Predictors of Psychopathology

Main Predictors

Effect of country was significant in all baseline models: depression (−2LL = 34092.194; Wald Z = 3.65, p < 0.001, ICC = 0.062), anxiety (−2LL = 34146272; Wald Z = 3.64, p < 0.001, ICC = 0.062), stress (−2LL = 34002.516; Wald Z = 3.66, p < 0.001, ICC = 0.066), and the DASS-21 total scores (−2LL = 34073.175; Wald Z = 3.66, p < 0.001, ICC = 0.064). Although these data suggest that country-level variables explained a small part of the variance, as it was significant, models including both individual and country-level variables were tested. According to the hypotheses, interaction terms were included (see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Four final multilevel models predicting markers of psychological distress.

Results indicated that gender, age, mental disorder diagnosis, meaning-centered coping, perceived economic impact, physical health effect and lifestyle changes due to the pandemic, together with adherence to confinement, had a significant impact on all markers of psychological distress (depression, anxiety, stress, and their combined score). Specifically, females, older participants, participants with mental disorder diagnosis and people who reported having lower levels of meaning-centered coping, higher levels of economic and physical health impact, significant lifestyle changes, and lower levels of adherence to confinement reported higher levels of negative emotional states.

Being a student and having lower economic status were related to increased depression, anxiety and the total DASS-21 scores but not to higher stress levels. More than 21 days in confinement predicted depression levels but not stress or anxiety levels. Country-level variables had no effect on the dependent variables of any of the four models.

The Moderating Role of Meaning Centered Coping

Moderating effects of meaning-centered coping levels, as measured by the MCCS, between individual-level variables and markers of psychological distress were tested in all four models (see Tables 3, 4). Results showed that the meaning-centered coping moderated the effects of all tested variables on depression with the exception of student status: gender, age, economic status, psychological diagnosis, 21 or more days spent at home, economic and physical health impact of the pandemic and perceived lifestyle changes. Meaning-centered coping also moderated the effects of gender, economic status and physical health impact of the pandemic on anxiety. In case of stress and the total DASS-21 scores, it had a moderating role in the effect of gender, economic status, physical health impact and perceived lifestyle changes due to the pandemic. In all significant interactions, the same pattern was observed: higher levels of the MCCS were related to diminished relationships between the aforementioned predictors and markers of psychological distress (see Table 4). That is, meaning-centered coping acted as a buffer between certain vulnerability factors and depression, anxiety, stress, and their total scores.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Moderating role of Meaning-centered coping: results of 36 separate analyses.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to evaluate the relationships between several risk factors and markers of psychological distress during the initial phases of the COVID-19 pandemic, as well as to assess the protective role of meaning-centered-coping in their relations. Four hypotheses were tested in a sample of 12,234 in 30 countries from all continents.

In line with H1, the study confirmed that there are demographic vulnerability factors of psychological distress during the COVID-19 health crisis over which the individual has little to no control, at least in the short term. This is the case of age, gender, economic status, and being a student. These findings are congruent with recent studies showing that females, younger people, students, and those with lower economic status may be the most vulnerable to developing psychological problems during the COVID-19 pandemic (e.g., Alonzi et al., 2020; Asmundson et al., 2020; Fitzpatrick et al., 2020; Iob et al., 2020; Kajdy et al., 2020; Luo et al., 2020; Pieh et al., 2020; Purtle, 2020; Vahedian-Azimi et al., 2020; Xiong et al., 2020). The results also demonstrated that people with mental disorder diagnosis showed higher levels of all aspects of emotional disturbance than their non-diagnosed counterparts. In fact, this condition was the most prominent risk factor among all assessed individual predictors (for similar results, see Asmundson et al., 2020).

Other important vulnerability factors linked to increased distress on an individual basis were perceived economic impact, physical health effects, and lifestyle changes due to the COVID-19 pandemic, as well as lack of adherence to confinement (H2). Among the COVID-19-related factors, noxious effects of the pandemic on physical health (unrelated to the actual illness) was the strongest predictor of all negative emotional states. As previous studies have reported, people with a preexisting physical condition can be extremely vulnerable to the effects of the pandemic (see for instance, Alonzi et al., 2020). Individual duration of confinement was only related to depression in our sample. The severity of the pandemic in each country (percentage of confirmed COVID-19 cases during the data collection) and GDP did not directly predict any facet of psychological distress. These findings indicate that both demographic vulnerability markers and effects of the pandemic at the level of individuals (lifestyle change, economic, and physical problems) were generally related to declined mental health during the COVID-19 crisis, independently from the country of origin of the participants.

However, this alone may lead to an overly simplified picture. As predicted by H3, meaning-centered coping was associated with decreased levels of stress, anxiety and depression, suggesting that this coping style is related to the diminished experience of these negative emotional states, especially depression. H4 was formulated to support its buffer effects, expecting that meaning-centered coping would moderate the relationship between risk factors (both demographic ones and those directly related to the pandemic) and aspects of psychological distress. Results confirmed this hypothesis, showing that meaning-centered coping had a moderating effect of all aforementioned significant demographic and pandemic-related individual vulnerability factors (age, gender, mental disorder diagnosis, confinement duration, economic impact, physical health effects of the pandemic, and adherence to confinement) and levels of depression. These findings suggest that people with higher levels of meaning-centered coping could maintain lower levels of depression, despite finding themselves in a similar situation as those with low levels of meaning-centered coping. A similar protective role of meaning-centered coping was observed in the relationship between some demographic vulnerability factors (age and economic status) and anxiety; and between gender and stress levels; and finally, between some risk factors (gender, economic status, physical health effects and lifestyle changes due to COVID-19) and general psychological distress (combined score of depression, stress, and anxiety).

The moderation effects of meaning-centered coping were most evident with regards to the depression subscale which is in line with previous studies reporting a close relationship between meaning in life and depression (e.g., Steger et al., 2006; Disabato et al., 2017; Carreno et al., 2020). Depression, as measured by the DASS-21, is characterized by meaninglessness in life, hopelessness, and lacking interest and initiative (i.e., low levels of positive affect; Lovibond and Lovibond, 1995). The MCCS, on the other hand, is defined by quite the opposite: meaning in life, maintaining hope, prosociality, and proactivity. One potential explanation of these findings is that meaning-centered coping is strongly related to the maintenance of positive affect in the face of adversity, and has both a diminishing and transformative power over negative affect. Overall, these findings suggest that while the intensity of the COVID-19-related conditions correlates with the degree of presence of psychological distress, this relationship is highly modulated by meaning-centered coping, diminishing their harmful effects, especially in the case of depressive symptoms. They provide additional evidence of the role of meaning in life in promoting resilience, as has been previously suggested (for a review see Wong, 2012; Hicks and Routledge, 2013; Batthyany and Russo-Netzer, 2014).

The above-mentioned findings may offer empirical support to existential positive psychology (e.g., Wong, 2009, 2011a) and highlight the importance of strategies and interventions targeting not only the reduction of negative affect but also the transformation of adversities into growth and increasing positive affect. These strategies are integrated in positive psychology (for interventions, see for instance, Craske et al., 2019; Carr et al., 2020; Hendriks et al., 2020) and especially in existential positive psychology (e.g., Wong, 2010, 2012, 2020a). Interventions based on these considerations may be extremely relevant during the COVID-19 crisis, as the experience of negative affect is unavoidable for a large part of the population.

Although the results show a potentially relevant role of meaning-centered coping in the COVID-19 era, there are some limitations we ought to discuss. First and foremost, since it is a cross-sectional study, causal directionality of the relationships here investigated cannot be confirmed. The relationship may even be the other way around, that is, lower levels of psychological distress may lead to more meaning-centered coping. Although the moderating effects of meaning-centered coping between vulnerability factors and psychological distress make our interpretation feasible, it is possible that the relationship may be circular: this type of coping leads to lower levels of psychological distress (i.e., more positive feelings) which in turn leads to more frequent use of adaptive coping strategies (for the role of positive emotions in broadening the individual’s thought-action repertoire, see Fredrickson, 2004). Future longitudinal studies should address this issue. Second, we did not use a representative sample. For instance, females were overrepresented which limits the generalizability of the findings. Third, we focused on general, cross-cultural trends although controlling for the effect of country. Future studies may look at similar phenomena independently in each country. Fourth, the cutoff point of the duration of confinement for a statistically significant effect was not assessed. This variable also reflects on the data collection period of each country during the rapidly changing situation (for instance, data in Spain was collected first, and data in the United States was collected last, hence the differences). These differences based on the data collection period were partially mitigated by the inclusion of the variable “severity index” that intended to add an objectively comparable score of the situation of the pandemic in each country during their respective data collection periods. Fifth, we used self-reported measures that can be biased. Future studies using behavioral and physiological measures could yield more robust results on the protective role of meaning-centered coping.

To conclude, the results of the present study point to a critical role of meaning-centered coping in attenuating the detrimental effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on psychological distress, especially on depressive symptoms. These effects were observed in a global sample, including more than 12,243 participants from all continents. This protective effect seems to be exerted via two different mechanisms: by diminishing the emergence of all types of psychological distress and acting as a moderator between hardship and such symptoms. The findings suggest that future interventions from an existential positive psychology point of view that foster meaning-centered coping can be beneficial, especially for members of vulnerable groups.

We urge decision-makers not to place the focus only on risk factors and strategies to ameliorate the magnitude of the COVID-19-related situations and make more emphasis on fostering psychological resilience and promoting a proactive attitude in individuals so that they do not regard themselves as passive sufferers of threats. Promoting effective coping mechanisms such as meaning-centered strategies may prove to be adequate in the current situation and prescriptions issued by institutions like the World Health Organization could benefit millions of people by integrating these considerations.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by – Comisión de Bioética, Universidad de Almería – Ethics Commission for Research within the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Universitatea “Alexandru Ioan Cuza” Din Iaşi – Comité de Ética en Investigación, Universidad de Monterrey – Ethics Commission Batna 1 University Rectorat – Department of Applied Psychology, Guru Jambheshwar University of Science and Technology – Comissão Nacional de Ética em Pesquisa – Medizinische Fakultät Ethikkommission, Heinrich-Heine-Universität Düsseldorf – Kutatásetikai Bizottság, Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem, Pedagógiai és Pszichológiai Kar – Ethics Commission, International University of Business Agriculture and Technology (IUBAT) – Comitato Etico Della Ricerca Psicologica (AREA 17) Dipartimenti/Sezione di Psicologia – Università di Padova – Institutional Review Board, Lebanese American University – Ethical Review Board COMSATS University Islamabad, Lahore Campus – Ethics Commission, Komisija za Etiko Filozofske Fakultete, Filozofska Fakulteta, Univerze v Ljubljani – Ethics Commission, Pathumwan Institute of Technology – Ethics Commission, Örebro Universitet – Institutional Review Board, Rutgers University – Faculty Ethics Committee, University of South Wales – Institutional Review Board, Ağri İbrahi̇m Çeçen Üni̇versi̇tesi̇ Rektörlüğü. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

NE contributed to the manuscript by coordinating the data collection, theoretical framing, text writing, and data analysis. JAP-E contributed to the manuscript by theoretical framing, text writing, and offering a critical revision of the manuscript. DC contributed to the manuscript by coordinating the data collection, theoretical framing, text writing, and revision of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We truly appreciate the participation of all international collaborators and participants in this project.

References

Alonzi, S., La Torre, A., and Silverstein, M. W. (2020). The psychological impact of preexisting mental and physical health conditions during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol. Trauma 12, S236–S238. doi: 10.1037/tra0000840

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arslan, G., and Yildirim, M. (2020). Coronavirus stress, meaningful living, optimism, and depressive symptoms: a study of moderated mediation model. Psyarxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/ykvzn

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asmundson, G. J., Paluszek, M. M., Landry, C. A., Rachor, G. S., McKay, D., and Taylor, S. (2020). Do pre-existing anxiety-related and mood disorders differentially impact COVID-19 stress responses and coping? J. Anxiety Disord. 74:102271. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basowitz, H., Persky, H., Korchin, S. J., and Grinker, R. R. (1955). Anxiety and Stress. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Batthyany, A., and Russo-Netzer, P. (2014). Meaning in Positive and Existential Psychology. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Beaton, D. E., Bombardier, C., Guillemin, F., and Ferraz, M. B. (2000). Guidelines for the process of cross-cultural adaptation of self-report measures. Spine 25, 3186–3191. doi: 10.1097/00007632-200012150-00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bliss, E. L., Migeon, C. J., Branch, C. H., and Samuels, L. T. (1956). Reaction of the adrenal cortex to emotional stress. Psychosom. Med. 18, 56–76. doi: 10.1097/00006842-195601000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breitbart, W., Rosenfeld, B., Gibson, C., Pessin, H., Poppito, S., Nelson, C., et al. (2010). Meaning-centered group psychotherapy for patients with advanced cancer: a pilot randomized controlled trial. Psycho Oncol. 19, 21–28. doi: 10.1002/pon.1556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. A., Chorpita, B. F., Korotitsch, W., and Barlow, D. H. (1997). Psychometric properties of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) in clinical samples. Behav. Res. Ther. 35, 79–89. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(96)00068-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cai, H., Tu, B., Ma, J., Chen, L., Fu, L., Jiang, Y., et al. (2020). Psychological impact and coping strategies of frontline medical staff in hunan between January and March 2020 during the outbreak of Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) in Hubei, China. Med. Sci. Monit. 26:e924171.

Google Scholar

Calhoun, L. G., and Tedeschi, R. G. (2006). “The foundations of posttraumatic growth: an expanded framework,” in Handbook of Posttraumatic Growth: Research and Practice, eds L. G. Calhoun and R. G. Tedeschi (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 3–23.

Google Scholar

Carr, A., Cullen, K., Keeney, C., Canning, C., Mooney, O., Chinseallaigh, E., et al. (2020). Effectiveness of positive psychology interventions: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Posit. Psychol. 1–21. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2020.1818807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carreno, D. F., and Pérez-Escobar, J. A. (2019). Addiction in existential positive psychology (EPP, PP2.0): from a critique of the brain disease model towards a meaning-centered approach. Couns. Psychol. Q. 32, 415–435. doi: 10.1080/09515070.2019.1604494

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carreno, D. F., Eisenbeck, N., Cangas, A. J., García-Montes, J. M., Del Vas, L. G., and María, A. T. (2020). Spanish adaptation of the personal meaning profile-brief: meaning in life, psychological well-being, and distress. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 20, 151–162. doi: 10.1016/j.ijchp.2020.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chao, M., Chen, X., Liu, T., Yang, H., and Hall, B. J. (2020). Psychological distress and state boredom during the COVID-19 outbreak in China: the role of meaning in life and media use. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 11:1769379. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2020.1769379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chochinov, H. M., Kristjanson, L. J., Breitbart, W., McClement, S., Hack, T. F., Hassard, T., et al. (2011). Effect of dignity therapy on distress and end-of-life experience in terminally ill patients: a randomised controlled trial. Lancet Oncol. 12, 753–762. doi: 10.1016/s1470-2045(11)70153-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, L. A., and Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 100:316. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.100.3.316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craske, M. G., Meuret, A. E., Ritz, T., Treanor, M., Dour, H., and Rosenfield, D. (2019). Positive affect treatment for depression and anxiety: a randomized clinical trial for a core feature of anhedonia. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 87:457. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cullen, W., Gulati, G., and Kelly, B. (2020). Mental health in the Covid-19 pandemic. QJM 113, 311–312.

Google Scholar

Dawson, D. L., and Golijani-Moghaddam, N. (2020). COVID-19: psychological flexibility, coping, mental health, and wellbeing in the UK during the pandemic. J. Contextual Behav. Sci. 17, 126–134. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2020.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Disabato, D. J., Kashdan, T. B., Short, J. L., and Jarden, A. (2017). What predicts positive life events that influence the course of depression? A longitudinal examination of gratitude and meaning in life. Cogn. Ther. Res. 41, 444–458. doi: 10.1007/s10608-016-9785-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobson, K. S., and Dozois, D. J. (2011). Risk Factors in Depression. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Feldman, D. B., and Snyder, C. R. (2005). Hope and the meaningful life: theoretical and empirical associations between goal–directed thinking and life meaning. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 24, 401–421.

Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, K. M., Harris, C., and Drawve, G. (2020). Living in the midst of fear: depressive symptomatology among US adults during the COVID-19 pandemic. Depress. Anxiety 37, 957–964. doi: 10.1002/da.23080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folkman, S., and Lazarus, R. S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company.

Google Scholar

Folkman, S., and Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Positive affect and the other side of coping. Am. Psychol. 55:647. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.55.6.647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frankl, V. E. (1969). The Will to Meaning: Foundations and Applications of Logotherapy. Washington, DC: Meridian.

Google Scholar

Frankl, V. E. (1985). Man’s Search for Meaning. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.

Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The broaden–and–build theory of positive emotions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 359, 1367–1377.

Google Scholar

Gloria, C. T., and Steinhardt, M. A. (2016). Relationships among positive emotions, coping, resilience and mental health. Stress Health 32, 145–156. doi: 10.1002/smi.2589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: Guilford publications.

Google Scholar

Hendriks, T., Schotanus-Dijkstra, M., Hassankhan, A., De Jong, J., and Bohlmeijer, E. (2020). The efficacy of multi-component positive psychology interventions: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J. Happiness Stud. 21, 357–390. doi: 10.1007/s10902-019-00082-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, J. D., and Crawford, J. R. (2005). The short-form version of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 44, 227–239. doi: 10.1348/014466505x29657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hicks, I. J., and Routledge, C. (2013). “The experience of meaning in life,” in Classical Perspectives, Emerging Themes, and Controversies, eds J. A. Hicks and C. Routledge (Dordrecht: Springer).

Google Scholar

Huang, L., Lei, W., Xu, F., Liu, H., and Yu, L. (2020). Emotional responses and coping strategies in nurses and nursing students during Covid-19 outbreak: a comparative study. PLoS One 15:e0237303. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0237303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iob, E., Frank, P., Steptoe, A., and Fancourt, D. (2020). Levels of severity of depressive symptoms among at-risk groups in the UK during the COVID-19 pandemic. JAMA Netw. Open 3:e2026064. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.26064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivtzan, I., Lomas, T., Hefferon, K., and Worth, P. (2015). Second Wave Positive Psychology: Embracing the Dark Side of Life. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Jamieson, J. P., Crum, A. J., Goyer, J. P., Marotta, M. E., and Akinola, M. (2018). Optimizing stress responses with reappraisal and mindset interventions: an integrated model. Anxiety Stress Coping 31, 245–261. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2018.1442615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jans-Beken, L., and Wong, P. T. (2019). Development and preliminary validation of the existential gratitude scale (EGS). Couns. Psychol. Q. 34, 1–15. doi: 10.15703/kjc.11.1.201003.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kajdy, A., Feduniw, S., Ajdacka, U., Modzelewski, J., Baranowska, B., Sys, D., et al. (2020). Risk factors for anxiety and depression among pregnant women during the COVID-19 pandemic: a web-based cross-sectional survey. Medicine 99:e21279. doi: 10.1097/md.0000000000021279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kashdan, T., and Biswas-Diener, R. (2014). The Upside of Your Dark Side: Why Being Your Whole Self–Not Just Your “Good” Self–Drives Success and Fulfillment. New York, NY: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Khanna, S., and Greyson, B. (2015). Near-death experiences and posttraumatic growth. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 203, 749–755. doi: 10.1097/nmd.0000000000000362

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleiman, E. M., Adams, L. M., Kashdan, T. B., and Riskind, J. H. (2013). Gratitude and grit indirectly reduce risk of suicidal ideations by enhancing meaning in life: evidence for a mediated moderation model. J. Res. Pers. 47, 539–546. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2013.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, N. (2017). Prosocial behavior increases perceptions of meaning in life. J. Posit. Psychol. 12, 354–361. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2016.1209541

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S. (1993). From psychological stress to the emotions: a history of changing outlooks. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 44, 1–22. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.000245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, M. E., MacLane, C., Daflos, S., Seeley, J. R., Hayes, S. C., Biglan, A., et al. (2014). Examining psychological inflexibility as a transdiagnostic process across psychological disorders. J. Contextual Behav. Sci. 3, 155–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2014.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovibond, P. F., and Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav. Res. Ther. 33, 335–343. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-u

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, M., Guo, L., Yu, M., and Wang, H. (2020). The psychological and mental impact of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) on medical staff and general public-A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 291:113190. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maddi, S. R. (2013). “Hardiness as the existential courage to grow through searching for meaning,” in The Experience of Meaning in Life, eds J. Hicks and C. Routledge (Dordrecht: Springer), 227–239. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-6527-6_18

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milman, E., Lee, S. A., and Neimeyer, R. A. (2020). Social isolation and the mitigation of coronavirus anxiety: the mediating role of meaning. Death Stud. 1–13. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2020.1775362 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Nakamura, J., and Csikzentmihalyi, M. (2003). “The construction of meaning through vital engagement,” in Flourishing: Positive Psychology and the Life Well-Lived, eds C. L. M. Keyes and J. Haidt (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 83–104. doi: 10.1037/10594-004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pieh, C., Budimir, S., and Probst, T. (2020). The effect of age, gender, income, work, and physical activity on mental health during coronavirus disease (COVID-19) lockdown in Austria. J. Psychosom. Res. 136:110186. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2020.110186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purtle, J. (2020). COVID-19 and mental health equity in the United States. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 55, 969–971. doi: 10.1007/s00127-020-01896-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reid, M. (2013). Disasters and social inequalities. Sociol. Compass 7, 984–997. doi: 10.1111/soc4.12080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salari, N., Hosseinian-Far, A., Jalali, R., Vaisi-Raygani, A., Rasoulpoor, S., Mohammadi, M., et al. (2020). Prevalence of stress, anxiety, depression among the general population during the COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Global. Health 16, 1–11.

Google Scholar

Schueller, S. M., and Seligman, M. E. (2010). Pursuit of pleasure, engagement, and meaning: relationships to subjective and objective measures of well-being. J. Posit. Psychol. 5, 253–263. doi: 10.1080/17439761003794130

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E. (2004). Authentic Happiness: Using the New Positive Psychology to Realize Your Potential for Lasting Fulfillment. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.

Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E. P., and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: an introduction. Am. Psychol. 55, 5–14. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serafini, G., Parmigiani, B., Amerio, A., Aguglia, A., Sher, L., and Amore, M. (2020). The psychological impact of COVID-19 on the mental health in the general population. QJM 113, 531–537.

Google Scholar

Sher, L. (2020). The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on suicide rates. QJM 113, 707–712. doi: 10.1093/qjmed/hcaa202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S., and Kaler, M. (2006). The meaning in life questionnaire: assessing the presence of and search for meaning in life. J. Couns. Psychol. 53:80. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.53.1.80

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, S. Y., and Yip, A. (2018). Hans Selye (1907–1982): founder of the stress theory. Singapore Med. J. 59:170. doi: 10.11622/smedj.2018043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vahedian-Azimi, A., Moayed, M. S., Rahimibashar, F., Shojaei, S., Ashtari, S., and Pourhoseingholi, M. A. (2020). Comparison of the severity of psychological distress among four groups of an Iranian population regarding COVID-19 pandemic. BMC Psychiatry 20:402. doi: 10.1186/s12888-020-02804-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Tongeren, D. R., Green, J. D., Davis, D. E., Hook, J. N., and Hulsey, T. L. (2016). Prosociality enhances meaning in life. J. Posit. Psychol. 11, 225–236. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2015.1048814

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vos, J. (2016). “Working with meaning in life in mental health care: a systematic literature review of the practices and effectiveness of meaning-centred therapies,” in Clinical Perspectives on Meaning, eds P. Russo-Netzer, S. Schulenberg, and A. Batthyany (Cham: Springer). doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-41397-6_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vos, J., and Vitali, D. (2018). The effects of psychological meaning-centered therapies on quality of life and psychological stress: a metaanalysis. Palliat. Support. Care 16, 608–632. doi: 10.1017/S1478951517000931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, P. T. P (1993). Effective management of life stress: the resource–congruence model. Stress Med. 9, 51–60. doi: 10.1002/smi.2460090110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, P. T. P. (2009). “Positive existential psychology,” in Encyclopedia, ed. S. Lopez (Oxford: Blackwell).

Google Scholar

Wong, P. T. P. (2010). Meaning therapy: an integrative and positive existential psychotherapy. J. Contemp. Psychother. 40, 85–93. doi: 10.1007/s10879-009-9132-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, P. T. P. (2011a). What is existential positive psychology? Int. J. Existent. Posit. Psychol. 3, 1–10. doi: 10.1002/9780470773222.ch1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, P. T. P. (2011b). Positive psychology 2.0: towards a balanced interactive model of the good life. Can. Psychol. 52:69. doi: 10.1037/a0022511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, P. T. P. (2012). The Human Quest for Meaning: Theories, Research, and Applications. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Wong, P. T. P. (2020a). Existential positive psychology and integrative meaning therapy. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 32, 565–578. doi: 10.1080/09540261.2020.1814703

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, P. T. P. (2020b). Made for Resilience and Happiness: Effective coping with Covid-19 According to Viktor E. Frankly and Paul TP Wong. Oakland, CA: INPM Press.

Google Scholar

Wong, P. T. P., Reker, G. T., and Peacock, E. J. (2006). “A resource-congruence model of coping and the development of the coping schema inventory,” in International and Cultural Psychology Series. Handbook of Multicultural Perspectives on Stress and Coping, eds P. T. P. Wong and L. C. J. Wong (Boston, MA: Spring Publications), 223–283. doi: 10.1007/0-387-26238-5_11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiong, J., Lipsitz, O., Nasri, F., Lui, L. M., Gill, H., Phan, L., et al. (2020). Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on mental health in the general population: a systematic review. J. Affect. Disord. 277, 55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yehuda, R., and McFarlane, A. C. (1995). Conflict between current knowledge about posttraumatic stress disorder and its original conceptual basis. Am. J. Psychiatry 152, 1705–1713. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.12.1705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zacher, H., and Rudolph, C. W. (2020). Individual differences and changes in subjective wellbeing during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. Am. Psychol. 76, 50–62. doi: 10.1037/amp0000702

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: COVID-19, meaning-centered coping, stress appraisal, psychological distress, depression, anxiety, existential positive psychology, positive psychology (PP1.0 and PP2.0)

Citation: Eisenbeck N, Carreno DF and Pérez-Escobar JA (2021) Meaning-Centered Coping in the Era of COVID-19: Direct and Moderating Effects on Depression, Anxiety, and Stress. Front. Psychol. 12:648383. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.648383

Received: 31 December 2020; Accepted: 16 February 2021;
Published: 17 March 2021.

Edited by:

Claude-Hélène Mayer, University of Johannesburg, South Africa

Reviewed by:

Lilian Jans-Beken, The Thriving Human Science Center, Netherlands
Frank Martela, University of Helsinki, Finland

Copyright © 2021 Eisenbeck, Carreno and Pérez-Escobar. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nikolett Eisenbeck, ZWlzZW5iZWNrbmlraUBnbWFpbC5jb20=; bmVpc2VuYmVja0B1cy5lcw==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.