- 1Doctoral School of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
- 2Neuropsychology and Child Development Research Group, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
- 3Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
- 4Institute of Education, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
The present study investigated the cross-cultural variations in parents' views on the role of play in child development and the primary purpose of preschool education from Ethiopia and Hungary. It also examined the cross-cultural variations in preschoolers' executive functions (EF), the frequency of their engagement in home activities, and the role of these activities in the development of EF skills. Participants included 266 preschoolers (Ethiopia: 139 of which 44.6% boys; Mage = 63.83 months; SD = 7.68 months; Hungary: 127 of which 48% boys; Mage = 62.06 months; SD = 9.37 months) with their parents (Ethiopia: 45.32% male; Mage = 36.66 years; SD = 7.14 years; Hungary: 13.18% male; Mage = 37.71 years; SD = 5.97 years). The independent samples t-test showed that Ethiopian parents view fostering academic skills for preschooler significantly more important than their Hungarian counterparts do. We also found that while Ethiopian parents hold the belief that academic and cognitive development is the major purpose of preschool education, Hungarian prioritize social-emotional development and entertainment. Additionally, preschoolers in Ethiopia were reported to engage in academic and arts and crafts activities after preschool significantly more frequently than their Hungarian counterparts. On the contrary, preschoolers in Hungary were found to engage in fine-motor activities, solitary play, sports and other physical activities significantly more frequently than their Ethiopian counterparts. No significant differences were found in EF skills between preschoolers from Ethiopia and Hungary. Results from hierarchical regression analyses showed that, after accounting for age and SES, preschoolers' frequency of pretend play and their parents' play support beliefs were found to be small to medium-sized predictors of inhibitory control skills in both samples. However, children's frequency of having breakfast at home was another significant predictor in the Ethiopian sample only. The frequency of participation in arts and crafts and other fine-motor activities were found to be important predictors of preschoolers' visual-spatial working memory skills in the Ethiopian and the Hungarian samples, respectively. We also found that, after controlling for SES, parental play support was an important factor associated with preschoolers' shifting skills only in the Hungarian sample. Based on the findings, we made important conclusions.
Introduction
The preschool years have been recognized as an important period when the development of executive function (EF) skills undergoes marked improvements (Diamond et al., 1997; Carlson, 2005; Garon et al., 2008; Best and Miller, 2010; Welsh et al., 2010; Anderson and Reidy, 2012; Howard et al., 2015; Nieto et al., 2016). The successful development of preschoolers' EF has been linked with better school adjustment and academic success in the beginning of primary education (Blair and Raver, 2015; Moriguchi et al., 2016). On the other hand, inadequate development of EF skills is linked with different physical, psychological, and academic-related problems such as physical aggression (Séguin and Zelazo, 2005), developmental psychopathology (Pennington and Ozonoff, 1996), and school readiness and difficulties in academic progress (Blair, 2002; Diamond, 2007). The present study investigated the cross-cultural variation in these important skills in preschoolers and the contribution of preschoolers' home experience and parental play beliefs in the development of EF skills.
EF refers to a set of cognitive skills responsible for the conscious control and regulation of one's thoughts and actions to guide goal-directed thinking and action (Diamond, 2013; Zelazo et al., 2016). It is generally considered to consist of three independent but interrelated components. Whether the same applies in childhood remains a question. For instance, studies by Fuhs and Day (2011) and Wiebe et al. (2011) with preschoolers support the unidimensional nature of EF in childhood. On the contrary, there are studies evidencing the different components of EF in the early period of life (see Usai et al., 2014; Howard et al., 2015; Skogan et al., 2015). In this line, previous studies (such as Miyake et al., 2000; Diamond, 2013; Howard et al., 2015; Zelazo et al., 2016) found that in the preschool years EF skills are composed of three core competencies: working memory, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility.
Working memory refers to the ability to concurrently hold information in mind and operate with it for a brief period of time (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974; Baddeley, 1992). According to Lan et al. (2011), working memory is regarded as an important element of EF since children must conceive and temporarily store information when they successfully perform a task. Based on its content, working memory can be categorized as verbal/semantic and non-verbal/visuospatial working memory (Miyake et al., 2000; Diamond, 2013).
Inhibition is defined as the cognitive ability to regulate one's thoughts and actions in order to dominate a strong internal tendency and/or external temptation (Diamond, 2013). It helps a person to get all the facts before reaching a conclusion (Montoya et al., 2018).
Cognitive flexibility is the capacity to flexibly switch from one mindset to another in order to adapt one's course of thoughts and action to a changing situation (Davidson et al., 2006; Garon et al., 2008). For instance, cognitive flexibility is needed when we shift our attention from one dimension/feature of a task to another (Miyake et al., 2000; Diamond, 2013).
The association between EF and early literacy skills is well-documented (Bierman et al., 2008; Foy and Mann, 2013; Weiland et al., 2014; Davidse et al., 2015; Lonigan et al., 2016). There have also been many compelling studies that prove the relationship between EF and different elements of school readiness that include competencies in language, math, science in addition to social-emotion skills (Carlson and Wang, 2007; Bull et al., 2008; Ponitz et al., 2009; Nayfeld et al., 2013). There is also extensive evidence indicating the predictive (both concurrent and longitudinal) power of EF in literacy and reading (Adams and Snowling, 2001; Welsh et al., 2010), mathematics (Bull and Scerif, 2001; St Clair-Thompson and Gathercole, 2006; Brock et al., 2009; Lee et al., 2012) and short- and long-term academic success (Blair and Razza, 2007; McClelland et al., 2007; Purpura et al., 2017). Moreover, EF is recognized as the strongest predictor of school readiness in the context of the development of pre-academic skills (Blair and Razza, 2007).
Culture and Executive Function
Due to variations in social systems, beliefs, attitudes, and values across cultures, there has been an increased interest to explore the influence of culture on human developmental outcomes (Burrage et al., 2008; Suchodoletz et al., 2013; Wanless et al., 2013). Considering the importance of EF skills, the influence of culture on children's development of EF skills deserves attention (Ellefson et al., 2017). Findings from cross-cultural investigations so far suggest that, for instance, children from Eastern culture typically perform better on EF tasks than their Western counterparts (Sabbagh et al., 2006; Oh and Lewis, 2008; Lewis et al., 2009; Lan et al., 2011; Grabell et al., 2015). Different explanations have been proposed. For instance, Chen et al. (1998) mention early parental expectation of children' mastery of impulse control. While children from Eastern cultures (e.g., Chinese) are expected to have mastery of impulse control around 2 years of age, children from Western cultures are not expected the same until the preschool years (Chen et al., 1998). This could create opportunities for Chinese children to be taught the importance of self-control (Ellefson et al., 2017) and start practicing EF skills at an earlier age than their US counterparts (Sabbagh et al., 2006). There is also another explanation which is linked to the difference in parenting practices in the context of individualist vs. collectivist cultures (Schmitt et al., 2019). In a collectivist culture like China, children, for instance, are supposed to practice and learn how to behave in the society through adults modeling of the “acceptable” behaviors (Jian, 2009). On the other hand, individual choices and negotiation are valued elements in child rearing practices in individualistic cultures (Greenfield et al., 2006).
The other explanation comes from the difference in educational styles and schooling environments in the two cultural groups in general. According to Lan and colleages (2011), the difference of educational styles between countries (e.g., Chines and US) is the major reason for the variation in EF skills. While being able to control one's behavior, attention and concentration are a central element of Chinese education, self-expression and decision-making freedom are more in focus in US education (Song and Jinyu, 2016). On top of that, compared to the preschool settings in US, impulse control is exceedingly valued in Chinese preschools (Tobin et al., 1989). This might mean that the schooling environment in China is more supportive of the development of EF skills than the schooling environment in the US.
Home Experience in Child Development
Home experience is proposed to be among the most important factors contributing to individual differences in early brain development. In this regard, there is a growing body of evidence demonstrating that children's home experiences such as family socioeconomic status (Mezzacappa, 2004; Noble et al., 2005; Sarsour et al., 2011), years of parental education (Ardila et al., 2005), parent-child interactions (Bernier et al., 2010, 2012; Rhoades et al., 2011; Blair et al., 2014; Linebarger et al., 2014; Lucassen et al., 2015; Meuwissen and Carlson, 2015), parental scaffolding/support (Hughes and Ensor, 2009; Bernier et al., 2010; Hammond et al., 2012; Hughes and Devine, 2017), social interactions (Moriguchi, 2014), and children's engagement in physical activity (Best, 2010) have all been implicated in the development of EF skills. Contrarily, children's experience of abuse and maltreatment (Belsky and de Haan, 2011; Pechtel and Pizzagalli, 2011), adverse parenting (Hughes and Devine, 2017), negative caregiving behaviors (Ardila et al., 2005), chaotic home environment (Hughes and Ensor, 2009; Brown et al., 2013; Vernon-feagans et al., 2016), and exposure to maternal depression (Hughes et al., 2013) have adverse effects on early development of EF.
Play and Child Development
One of common home experiences of children that have valuable contribution to child learning and development is play (Ginsburg, 2007; Lin and Li, 2018). Beyond entertainment, play is important for cognitive, physical, social, and emotional development (Parmar et al., 2004; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2004; Burdette and Whitaker, 2005; Johnson et al., 2005; Ginsburg, 2007; Göncü and Gaskins, 2011). Moreover, play gives children opportunities to develop their skills of working in groups and sharing, negotiating, and resolving conflicts with others (Pellegrini and Smith, 1998; Mcelwain and Volling, 2005).
Parental views about the importance of play for child development and the frequency at which children engage in different types and forms of play show substantial variation (Roopnarine, 2012). Roopnarine (2011) argued that the parents' views of the importance of play for child development fall along a continuum. Views that acknowledge the significant contribution of play in child development are at one end of the continuum. The other end of the continuum consists of views that considers play as something children do naturally just for amusement. The middle of the continuum represents views of parents that appreciate the specific benefits of play but favor academic activities for children.
Children's experience of play at home (see Farver and Wimbarti, 1995; Haight et al., 1997; Avornyo and Baker, 2018; Ihmeideh, 2019), and ability to gain the developmental benefit of play from playing with peers and adults are related to the ideas parents hold about play (Bulotsky-Shearer et al., 2016). Moreover, the beliefs parents hold about the importance of play for child development guide their decision-making processes to get involved in their children's play (Holmes, 2011). Parents who value play for its educational and cognitive benefits are more likely to encourage and assist their children's play through different means such us supplying numerous play materials to promote play opportunities, to join their children's play activities (e.g., parent-child play), and encourage and facilitate early peer contacts (see Parmar et al., 2004; Johnson et al., 2005). These opportunities in turn seem to create better developmental opportunities for children (Thorp et al., 1995; Carson and Parke, 1996; O'brien and Md-Yunus, 2007; Lin and Yawkey, 2014). Thus, investigating parental play beliefs, the level of involvement in their children's play and children's play experiences at home are important issues to be examined in relation to children's developmental outcomes (LaForett and Mendez, 2016).
Culture and Play Beliefs
Even though child play is a universal activity, how it looks like is not necessarily the same across cultures (Farver, 1999; Gaskins, 1999). The description of play, the selection of play partners, the context under which play happens, and the time allocated to play are extensively affected by the cultural beliefs and experiences (Gaskins, 2006; Gaskins et al., 2006). Thus, there is no assurance to think that all communities hold similar views about the role of play in children's development and give equivalent play opportunities for their children (see Göncü et al., 2000; Parmar et al., 2004; Holmes, 2011; Roopnarine, 2011; Roopnarine and Jin, 2012; Roopnarine and Davidson, 2015). For instance, there are findings indicating that while adults with European cultural origin hold a strong play support belief that acknowledges the important role of play in child development (Haight et al., 1997; Parmar et al., 2004; Yahya and Wood, 2017), parents in some non-Western cultures or in low-income communities hold weak play support beliefs and appreciate the valuable contribution of academic training for children instead of play (Holmes, 2011; Yahya and Wood, 2017). In Mexican culture, in contrast to play activities, children's participation in work-related activities is thought to be far more important (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2002). Similarly, the Caribbean teachers and parents emphasize the importance of academic activities for preschoolers instead of play activities (Leo-Rhynie et al., 2009; Logie, 2013). Likewise, Mayan mothers do not value the importance of play for their children's development (Rogoff et al., 1993). In their investigation, Farver et al. (1995) reported that Korean-American parents, compared to their Anglo-American counterparts, hold beliefs that academics is more valuable than play for their children, and they participate less in their children's pretend play. Children with parents holding less favorable attitudes toward play may be given less opportunities to play (Göncü et al., 2000).
On the whole, parental and cultural belief systems guide the play support beliefs parents hold and the types of play culture values (Roopnarine et al., 2003). The finding of a study examining cultural variations in the value, occurrence, frequency, and partners of play among toddlers/children, for instance, showed cultural disparities (Göncü et al., 2000). Mexican and Indonesian mothers who do not value the importance of play were found to give few chances to their children to play with them (Farver and Howes, 1993; Farver and Wimbarti, 1995). Besides, given the same form of play (e.g., parent-child play) in different cultures, the contents of play may demonstrate cross-cultural variations. For instance, the parent–child play activities in the Western technologically developed societies comprise of more language and gesture-oriented routines (Turkheimer et al., 1989) that may facilitate language and cognitive development in unique ways (Roopnarine, 2011). Thus, in order to fully comprehend the meaning of different play activities for children's development in different socio-cultural contexts, the cultural foundation of play should be taken into account (see Roopnarine, 2011).
Socio-Cultural Contexts in Ethiopia and Hungary
Ethiopia
Ethiopia is a multi-ethnic and ecologically diverse East African country with a population of about 109 million (World Bank, 2019b) of which around eighty percent live in rural areas (World Bank, 2019a). The World Bank's 2018 report showed that Ethiopia's gross domestic product (GDP) was about 84.36 billion USD (World Bank, 2019b). In Ethiopia, while 71% of children (age 0–17) live with both biological parents, 17% of children live with either their biological mother or father only (Better Care Network, 2015). Most of the preschoolers are monolingual.
Kindergarten, which is mainly an urban phenomenon and left for the private sector, is a 3 year program for 4 to 6 year olds (Woldehanna, 2011). Accordingly, the government has very limited involvement in early childhood education such as providing some technical support and quality monitoring (Woldehanna, 2011). In order to improve the enrollment of young children in early childhood education in the country, “O” class and child-to-child programs, as part of early childhood education, have been launched very recently beyond the kindergarten program (MoE (Ministry of Education), 2016). Even though the Education and Training Policy (MoE, 1994) of the country underlines the importance of children's overall development during the preschool years, the preschools in the country run a very structured programs that focus more on academic activities and far less on play (see Tigistu, 2013; Admas, 2016). Interestingly, parents of preschoolers also hold academic focused beliefs that could drive preschools to focus on academic development instead of other aspects of development such as social-emotional development (Tigistu, 2013; Admas, 2016).
Hungary
Hungary is a country in East-Central Europe with a population of about 9.8 million of which 28.36% is reported as rural (World Bank, 2019c). According to World Bank's 2019 report, the country's GDP was 160.97 billion USD. Regarding ethnicity, the country is relatively homogenous (Józsa et al., 2018) and Hungarian is the official language. The majority of the Hungarian children are monolingual. It is estimated that about half a million children in Hungary grow up in single parent families (Coface Families Europe, 2018).
According to the Hungarian National Public Education law, children shall attend preschool education from the age of 3 until they reach compulsory school age (Act CXC of 2011 on National Public Education). As indicated in the Govn decree 363/2012 (XII. 17) on the Core Program for Kindergartens, the goal of preschool education in Hungary is “. promoting a harmonious physical and social development of the child's personality.” [363/2012 (XII. 17)]. The national core curriculum for kindergarten education regulates the pedagogical work in the kindergartens. The core program indicates the types of activities in the program as:
Play (as the fundament of preschool education); Rhyming and Storytelling; Drawing, Painting, Making Patterns, Handwork; Singing, Music, Singing Games, Children's Dances; Becoming Actively Acquainted with the Outer World (content in environmental and content in mathematics); Movement (physical activity); activities of a work nature (e.g., taking care of animals, plants); activity based learning (as cited in Böddi and Serfozo, 2019, p. 189).
Present Study
Variations in social systems and values across cultures make the generalizability of the findings from child development studies across different populations and cultural groups difficult. Taking this point into account, many scholars have been investigating cross-cultural variations in human development including cognitive development. In this regard, findings have been indicating cultural consistencies and variabilities in child development. For instance, the cross-cultural comparison of children's performance on EF tasks show that children from Asian countries outperform their Western counterparts (e.g., Sabbagh et al., 2006; Lewis et al., 2009; Imada et al., 2013). However, such comparisons of EF skills have mostly been limited to children from Eastern and Western cultures. The present study, therefore, is believed to be one of the first efforts to enrich the literature on the areas by comparing samples from Ethiopia and Hungary.
Cross-cultural variations in children's home (such as parenting practices and parental expectation) and school experiences could take important weight in explaining the cross-cultural variations in children's EFs development (see Chen et al., 1998; Ellefson et al., 2017; Schmitt et al., 2019). However, very little is known about the importance of other home experiences of children such as play, sport and physical activities, arts and crafts activities, and fine motor activities, for example, in explaining the cross-cultural differences in children's EF skills. In line of this point in their investigation of the contribution of preschoolers' home experiences in the development of EF, Metaferia et al. (2020a) strongly recommended a follow-up study with a cross-cultural design taking the potential variations in preschoolers' home experience as a function of different socio-cultural contexts into account. We selected Ethiopia and Hungary for comparison taking their very different socio-cultural backgrounds into account, including the far reaching differences between the aims of and the parental expectations from early childhood education. Previous studies (Tigistu, 2013; Admas, 2016) suggested that children's academic development was very important for Ethiopian parents, even during the preschool years. As opposed to this, the focus of early childhood education in Hungary is on the physical and socio-emotional development of children (Govn decree 363/2012 XII. 17).
Therefore, the present investigation aimed at examining parents' beliefs about play and the purpose of preschool education, and preschoolers' home activities cross-culturally with preschoolers from Ethiopia and Hungary. The other purpose of the current investigation was to explore cross-cultural variations in the development of EF skills including inhibitory control, shifting and visual-spatial working memory. Finally, exploring cross-cultural variations in the links between preschoolers' home experiences and EF skills was another purpose of the study. To realize these purposes of the study, we formulated the research questions below:
• Do Ethiopian and Hungarian parents hold different views about the role of play in child development and the purpose of preschool education?
• Is there cross-cultural variation in the frequency of preschoolers' activities at home?
• Are there cross-cultural differences in preschoolers' EF skills including inhibition, switching, and visual-spatial working memory?
• Is there a cross-cultural difference in the relationships between the frequency of preschoolers' activities at home and their performance on the battery of EF tasks?
We hypothesized that there would be clear cross-cultural variations in parental attitudes about the purpose of preschool education between the two sample groups. More specifically, we speculated that Ethiopian parents' educational beliefs tend toward academic skills while Hungarian parents would tend to hold the belief that social-emotional development is the main purpose of preschool education. We also hypothesized that Hungarian parents would hold significantly stronger play support beliefs than their Ethiopian counterparts.
Based on previous studies (e.g., Metaferia et al., 2020a,b), we hypothesized that there might be cross-cultural variations in the relationship the frequency of children's home activities and their parents' play support would have with children's performance on the battery of EF task. To be more specific, it was found in a previous study that parental play support beliefs and preschoolers' frequency of breakfast at home were positively linked to children's inhibitory control skills in an Ethiopian sample (Metaferia et al., 2020b). On the other hand, in addition to parental play support beliefs, preschoolers' frequency of participation in pretend play was an important predictor of inhibitory control skills in a Hungarian sample (Metaferia et al., 2020a). We also speculated that children's frequency of participation in arts and crafts and in fine motor activities were important predictor of children's performance in VSWM task for Ethiopian and Hungarian samples, respectively. Regarding the second and third research questions, we made no hypothesis, thus, these parts of the study were exploratory.
Materials and Methods
Participants
The participants of the study were children attending preschools during the time of data collection and their parents. Preschoolers with special needs (recognized by the preschools), and multilinguals were excluded from the study. Preschools from which the participant children were selected represent lower to middle-class areas in their respective countries. The education level of parents and family household annual income are presented in Table 1.
Ethiopia
One hundred and thirty-nine preschoolers (62 boys, and 77 girls; Mage = 63.83 months; SD = 7.68 months; age range = 44–84 months) and their parents (63 male and 76 females; Mage = 36.66 years; SD = 7.14 years; age range = 20–60 years) recruited from nine preschools in the capital, Addis Ababa, participated in the study. All participating preschoolers were Amharic monolinguals. Family members of the participant preschoolers who supplied the requested information for the study were preschoolers' father (43.2%), mother (53.2%), grandfather (0.7%), and grandmother (1.4%).
Hungary
One hundred and twenty-seven preschoolers (61 boys, 66 girls; Mage = 62.06 months; SD = 9.37 months; age range = 45–84 months) and their parents (17 male and 110 females; Mage =37.71 years; SD = 5.97 years; age range = 22–63 years) from seven preschools in the capital, Budapest, took part in the study. Seventy-six of the total Hungarian participants in the current study also participated in a previous study (Metaferia et al., 2020a) of ours. All participating preschoolers were Hungarian monolinguals. Preschoolers' caregivers who provided the required information about the participant child were their father (7.9%), mother (90.6%), grandfather (0.8%), and grandmother (0.8%).
Independent sample t-tests depicted that there were no significant differences between the two samples on children's age [(MEthiopia = 63.83, SDEthiopia = 7.68; MHungary = 62.06, SDHungary = 9.37); t (241.98) = 1.67, p = 0.097] or average parental education level [(MEthiopia = 0.3.13, SDEthiopia = 1.05; MHungary = 2.98, SDHungary = 0.95); t (260) = 1.17, p = 0.245].
Procedure
Preschools from Addis Ababa and Budapest were contacted to recruit participants for the present study. After obtaining written consent of preschool directors, information sheet and consent paper were sent to parents of preschoolers to request for their participation including their preschoolers in the present study. When parents agreed to participate by signing the consent form, the questionnaires were sent to them. When parents returned the filled in questionnaire to preschools, neuropsychological tests measuring EF skills were administered to their children by trained research assistants. The tests were administered individually at a separate quiet room in their respective preschools. The sequence of the tasks was kept the same for all participants: the go/no-go task, the switching task, and the visual-spatial working memory task. The test session lasted about 20–25 min for each child. The procedure applied in the present investigation was permitted by the Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Education and Psychology at ELTE Eötvös Loránd University's (issue number: 2017/209).
Measures
A questionnaire with four parts (demographic information, parental views on the goal of preschool education, parental play beliefs, and the child's home activities) was used in order to collect data from parents. Originally, the questionnaire was prepared in English and translated into the target languages (Amharic and Hungarian). As an average, the questionnaire took 30 min to be filled in.
Demographic Information
The demographic information consisted of the child's gender, age, year of enrolment in preschool, and relationship with the respondent, parents' highest level of education, and the family size and gross annual income was collected. A five-point scale ranging from elementary school completed (1) to graduate degree/Master's or above (5) was applied to measure the educational level of both parents. A seven- and a nine-point categorical scales were applied in measuring the family's gross annual income for Ethiopian and Hungarian samples, respectively. The z-score of the average education of parents and the z-score of annual income per family size were averaged to create SES variable.
Parental Play Beliefs Scale
Parental play beliefs were measured using the Parental Play Beliefs Scale (PPBS) developed by Fogle and Mendez with sample of low-income mothers with children enrolled in Head Start program (Fogle and Mendez, 2006). The PPBS consists of 25 items with a five-point Likert-scale ranging from 1 (disagree) to 5 (very much agree). In the scale validation study, a principal component analysis yielded two subscales, namely Play Support and Academic Focused beliefs (Fogle and Mendez, 2006). The Play Support scale includes 17 items (e.g., “I can help my child learn to control his or her emotions during play,” and “through play my child develops new skills and abilities”) and the Academic Focused scale consists of 8 items (e.g., “I would rather read to my child than play together,” and “I do not think my child learns important skills by playing”).
In the present investigation, item differential function (DIF) analysis was carried out on the scale using ordinal logistic regression technique (see Zumbo, 1999) in order to examine whether the items function similarly in the two sample groups. Accordingly, an item (item 2) from Academic Focused and four items (item 21, 22, 23, and 24) from Play Support scales were excluded from the scale as they functioned differently in the two sample groups. In the scale validation study, the subscales depicted internal consistency of α = 0.90 and 0.73 for Play Support and Academic Focused scales, respectively (Fogle and Mendez, 2006). The subscales also showed a good level of internal consistency in the current study (Ethiopian sample: Play Support = 0.89, Academic Focused = 0.70; Hungarian sample: Play Support = 0.80, and Academic Focused = 0.63) after removing the items functioning differently. The bivariate correlation between the two subscales depicted similar sized, significant negative correlations in both samples (rEthiopia = −0.41, p < 0.001, and rHungary = −0.39, p < 0.001).
Parents' Beliefs About the Purpose of Preschool Education Scale
A ranking-order scale with 7 items, developed by Metaferia et al. (2020a), was used to measure parents' views on the purpose of preschool education. The scale consists of a list of potential purposes of preschool education (general knowledge, cognitive skills, language development, social skills, enjoy themselves, emotional well-being, and academic skills). Parents are asked to rank the purposes of preschool education listed based on their importance (1 as the most important to 7 as the least important) based on their personal beliefs. Average rankings for each item were calculated in the two samples.
Child's Home Activities Scale
The Child's Home Activities Scale (CHAS), developed by Metaferia et al. (2020b), was used to measure preschoolers' frequency of participation in the selected home activities. The activities included in the scale were planned as individual indicators of preschoolers' activities at their home. The scale was designed to gather information about different aspects of children's everyday lives such as frequency of engagement in different type/form of play (e.g., pretend play, motor play, solitary play, and peer play), academic-related activities, art and craft activities and sports and physical activities. Furthermore, frequency of having breakfast at home, and spending mealtime together with family were addressed in the scale intending to gather information about whether or not children get nutrition before preschool as opposed to “mealtime together” which was designed to examine its social implications. The scale consists of ten Likert-scaled items ranging from “very rarely /less than once a week” (1) to “very frequently /most of the time during the day” (5). In the scale, parents were requested to rate the frequency at which their children participate in the home activities included in the scale: breakfast at home, spending meal time with family, academic-related activities, pretend play, motor play, fine motor activities, arts and crafts, solitary play, peer play, and sports and physical activity.
Translation of the Survey
The English version of the questionnaire was carefully translated to Amharic and Hungarian. Three independent translators of which two were educational psychologists with a master's degree and one applied developmental psychologist with a PhD level of education were involved in the translation of the Amharic version of the questionnaire (see Metaferia et al., 2020b). The translators were native speakers of Amharic and fluent in English. Upon completion, the three translations were collected and checked for the discrepancies by the first author and made important amendments. The translation of the Hungarian version of the questionnaire involved two independent native speakers of Hungarian and fluent in English. The collected translations from the translators were cross checked for their discrepancies and made important revisions by the second and third authors (see Metaferia et al., 2020a).
Executive Functions Measures
Three computerized neuropsychological tests, a go/no-go task, a switching task and a Mr. Peanut task, were administered to children to measure their inhibitory control, switching, and visual-spatial working memory, respectively. The first two tests were run using 1.85.1 version of PsychoPy (Psychological Software Tools; Peirce, 2008).
Go/No-Go Task
A fish and shark go/no-go task was applied to measure the inhibitory control skills of the participant preschoolers (see Wiebe et al., 2012). The task consisted of a practice and a test session. The practice session consisted of 4-go and 2-no-go trials demanding children to respond to the target and withhold for the non-target stimuli. The testing session consisted of 40-go and 20-no-go trials where a stimulus appears on a laptop screen for 1,500 ms unless a child responded earlier. Children were expected to respond to the target (i.e., when the target/fish stimulus appears on the screen) as quickly as possible by pressing the space bar in a laptop keyboard and withhold for the non-target (i.e., when the non-target/shark stimulus appears on the screen). Using frequency of correctly responding to go trials and no-go trials, sensitivity (d') was calculated for each child following the signal detection theory (see Macmillan and Creelman, 2005).
Switching Task
A cat and tiger go/no-go task with four blocks was used to measure shifting skill of the participant children (Metaferia et al., 2020a,b). The go and no-go stimuli were counterbalanced by switching the target and non-target stimuli as the task moves from one block to the next block. Correspondingly, participants were informed the target and non-target stimuli before the beginning of every block. Each block consisted of 16 go and 8 no-go trials where each stimulus appeared on the screen for 1,200 ms if the child did not respond to it earlier. Before the beginning of the actual test, a practice session involving 4-go and 2-no-go trials, was carried out. In order to estimate the switching task score of each child, sensitivity (d') was calculated for each block based on signal detection theory. Based on the sensitivity scores of each child for every block, the sensitivity score differences between switching blocks was calculated (i.e., sensitivity of block 3 minus block 1 and sensitivity of block 4 minus block 2). Next, the average of the two differences was calculated as switching score of every child (see Metaferia et al., 2020b).
Mr. Peanut Visual Spatial Working Memory Task
The children's visual spatial working memory (VSWM) was measured using Mr. Peanut visual special working memory task designed based on work of Morra (1994). Mr. Peanut is a “crown-like” picture that appears on a computer screen with stickers on his body and disappears for a while and reappears on the screen again without stickers. The stickers can appear 14 different possible locations on Mr. Peanut body part. The task for the children is to put sticker on Mr. Peanut's body using their memory after he removed them. The task begins with a practice session consists of 3 trials where children learn how to put stickers on Mr. Peanut's body by memorizing their locations before they are removed. The task progresses step-by-step starting from one sticker to seven stickers at a time where in every stage three attempts were given. In order to move to the next level of the task the child should at least correctly respond to one of the three trials at the present stage of the task. At each level of the task, correctly responding to one or at least two of the three trials results scores of 0.33 and 1, respectively. The total score was calculated by adding an individual score the child earned at each of the stages he/she gone through.
Data Analyses
SPSS v20.0 for Windows (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA) was used in conducting the statistical analyses. All variables used in the analyses were found to be normally distributed. Independent samples t-tests were applied to compare preschoolers' activities at home and their performance in EF tasks, and parental play beliefs between the two samples. Bivariate correlation analyses were used to examine the relationship preschoolers' EFs could have with parental play beliefs and preschoolers' home activities. We also built two-step hierarchical regression models explaining preschoolers' EF skills using parental play beliefs and preschoolers' home activities as predictors for each sample separately. Accordingly, children's age and SES were entered in the first step as control variables. Next, the significant correlates of the corresponding EF component from home activity and parental play belief variables were entered together into the regression equations. Moreover, Fisher z-transformation was carried out to make comparison of the correlation between preschoolers' home experiences (preschoolers' home activity and their parents' play beliefs) and their performance on the battery of EF tasks between the two sample groups. Furthermore, Friedman-tests were used to analyze parents' priorities regarding the purpose of preschool education in the two groups separately. Post-hoc test using a Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were also carried out to examine the significance of the differences in their beliefs in the two samples independently.
Results
Table 2 presents descriptive statistics for EF and home experience measures in the two samples.
Research Question 1: Do Ethiopian and Hungarian parents hold different views about the role of play in child development and the purpose of preschool education?
As can be seen in Table 3, the independent-samples t-test showed that there was no significant difference between the play support beliefs that the Ethiopian and the Hungarian parents held. However, Ethiopian parents held significantly stronger academic focused beliefs than their Hungarian counterparts.
A Friedman ANOVA and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were carried out to assess priorities in parental educational beliefs in both samples, separately. Table 4 presents the summary of the median and interquartile range for parental educational beliefs in the two samples. The result showed that parents hold different prioirities for the different educational goals in both samples (Ethiopia: χ2(6) = 83.93, p < 0.001; and Hungary: χ2(6) = 253.43, p < 0.001). Post-hoc test using a Wilcoxon signed-rank tests with Bonferroni-adjusted significance level of p < 0.0024, was carried out. The summary of the Wilcoxon signed-rank test using the z-statistics and significance values are presented in Table 5. As Table 4 shows that while Ethiopian parents held an educational belief that prioritizes academic and cognitive development, Hungarian parents held the educational beliefs that prioritize social-emotional development and enjoyment for their children.
Table 4. The median and interquartile range of parental beliefs about the purpose of preschool education for Ethiopian and Hungarian samples.
Research Question 2: Is there cross-cultural variation in the frequency of preschoolers' activities at home?
As shown in Table 3, the result of independent samples t-tests showed that Ethiopian preschoolers practiced academic skills and participated in arts and crafts activities after preschool significantly more often than their Hungarian peers. On the other hand, Hungarian preschoolers engage in fine-motor activities, solitary play, and sports and other physical activities significantly more often than their Ethiopian counterparts. However, there were no significant differences regarding frequency of mealtime together with family, breakfast at home, pretend play, motor play or peer play.
Research Question 3: Are there cross-cultural differences in preschoolers' EF skills including inhibition, switching, and visual-spatial working memory?
Independent-samples t-tests were carried out in order to examine if there were cross-cultural variations in children's performance on the battery of EF tasks between the two samples. The results indicated that there were no statistically significant difference in inhibitory control [(MEthiopia = 2.73, SDEthiopia = 0.85; MHungary = 2.92, SDHungary = 0.90); t (251.61) = −1.70, p = 0.091], switching [(MEthiopia = −0.10, SDEthiopia = 0.64; MHungary = −0.18, SDHungary = 0.67); t (241.86) = 0.94, p = 0.348] or VSWM [(MEthiopia = 2.62, SDEthiopia = 0.77; MHungary = 2.54, SDHungary = 0.92); t (228.00) = 0.78, p = 0.435] between the two samples.
Research Question 4: Is there cross-cultural variation in the relationships between the frequency of preschoolers' activities at home and their performance on the battery of EF tasks?
As shown in Table 6, the bivariate correlation analyses indicated that the sociodemographic variables (age of preschoolers and SES) showed significant, positive correlations with preschoolers' performance in all the three EF tasks in both countries. Frequency of participation in pretend play, fine motor activities and arts and crafts activities showed significant positive correlation with preschoolers' performance on Mr. Peanut VSWM task in both countries. On the other hand, frequency of breakfast at home and peer play were also significant correlates of children's score on VSWM task in the Ethiopian sample. Frequency of breakfast at home, pretend play, peer play and parental play support were significant correlates of inhibitory control in both samples. On the other hand, while preschoolers' frequency of participation in motor play showed significant, positive association with inhibitory control in the Hungarian sample, frequency of mealtime together with family, participation in fine motor activities, and in sports and physical activities showed significant, positive correlations with inhibitory control in Ethiopia. We also found that parental play support and preschoolers' frequency of pretend play and peer play displayed significant, positive correlations with preschoolers' performance in the switching task in both countries. Additionally, frequency of breakfast at home and participation in sport and physical activities showed significant, positive correlations with shifting skills in Ethiopia and Hungary, respectively.
Table 6. Bivariate correlations among demographics, home activities, parental play beliefs, and executive functions variables for Ethiopian and Hungarian samples.
Comparisons of significant correlations of preschoolers' home experiences and their performance on the battery of EF tasks between the two sample groups were carried out using Fisher z-transformation (single sided test). The correlations between frequency of pretend play and inhibitory control, peer play and inhibitory control, and parental play support and inhibitory control were comparable in the two countries (zs < .1.40, ns). However, correlation between breakfast and inhibitory control were significantly higher in case of the Ethiopian sample than their Hungarian counterparts (z = 1.93, p = 0.027). The correlation between frequency of pretend play and VSWM, frequency of fine motor and VSWM, and frequency of arts and crafts and VSWM were also comparable in the two samples (zs <0.50, ns). Moreover, the correlations between pretend play and switching, peer play and switching, and parental play support and switching were also equivalent (zs <0.32, ns) (See the Supplementary Material).
Next, the contribution of parental play beliefs and preschoolers' home activities to the EFs were examined in separate regression models for each country. Based on the results of the correlational analyses, all the variables that had significant correlations with the corresponding component of EF were entered simultaneously into a regression model, after controlling for preschoolers' age and SES.
In Ethiopia, preschoolers' age and SES together accounted for a significant proportion of the variance (24%) in the inhibitory control [F(2, 108) = 16.56, p < 0.001]. Next, frequency of mealtime together with family, breakfast at home, pretend play, fine motor activities, peer play, sport and physical activities, and parental play support were entered into the model predicting inhibitory control. Only frequency of breakfast at home, pretend play, and parental play support were found significant predictors in the model that accounted for an additional 20% variance in inhibitory control. After removing non-significant predictors that included mealtime, fine motor activities, peer play, and sport and physical activities from the equation, the model was significant [F(5, 105) = 15.97, p < 0.001] and explained 43% of the variance in preschoolers' performance in inhibitory control task. In the model, while the parental play support variable was a medium-sized predictor (β = 0.32, p < 0.001), frequency of breakfast (β = 0.17, p = 0.035) and pretend play (β = 0.17, p = 0.041) variables were small-sized predictors.
Another regression equation was built for the Ethiopian sample to examine the contribution of preschoolers' home activities to their VSWM task performance, after putting age and SES in control. Preschoolers' age and the SES variables together accounted 21% of the variance in VSWM [F(2, 118) = 15.98, p < 0.001]. Based on the results of correlational analyses, frequency of breakfast at home, pretend play, fine motor activities, arts and crafts activities, and peer play were simultaneously entered into the equation predicting VSWM. The result showed that only the frequency of participation in arts and crafts activities was a significant predictor next to sociodemographic variables that improved the model by 3%. After removing the variables not significantly contributing (breakfast at home, pretend play, fine motor activities, and peer play) to the model, the final model explained 24% [F(3, 117) = 12.42, p < 0.001] of the variance in VSWM. Finally, in order to examine the contribution of preschoolers' home experience in their performance on switching task, another regression model was built. Preschoolers' age and SES were included to the equation as the first step. However, the model was not significant [F(2, 97) = 2.61, p = 0.079]. Table 7 depicts the summary of the final model.
Table 7. Summary of hierarchical multiple regression models predicting executive function skills in Ethiopian sample.
In Hungary, we also performed hierarchical regression analyses to determine the extent to which parental play beliefs and preschoolers home experiences predict children's EF skills. Since sociodemographic variables (preschoolers' age and SES) were significantly related to EF skills that includes inhibitory control, shifting, and VSWM in the present study, these variables were entered as a control into the regression equation. Based on the results from bivariate correlation, the additional contribution of parental play belief and preschoolers' home activity variables was then examined by simultaneously entering them into the regression equation.
When age and SES were entered simultaneously as predictors of preschoolers' inhibitory control skill, they accounted 18% of the variance [F(2, 103) = 11.55, p < 0.001]. Then, preschoolers' frequency of breakfast at home, pretend play, peer play, motor play, and parental play support were simultaneously entered into the model. However, frequency of pretend play and parental play support were significant predictors in the model that explained an additional 16% of the variance in preschoolers' inhibitory control score. The final model was significant [F(4, 101) = 13.12, p < 0.001]. As can be seen in Table 8, the positive beta values of pretend play (β = 0.23, p = 0.007) and parental play support (β = 0.31, p = 0.001) suggest that preschoolers with frequent engagement in pretend play and parents holding better play support belief predicted better inhibitory control skills of children.
Table 8. Summary of hierarchical multiple regression models predicting executive function skills in Hungarian sample.
Again, in the Hungarian sample, age and SES together explained 17% of the variance in VSWM [F(2, 100) = 9.93, p < 0.001]. Next, preschoolers' frequency of participation in pretend play, fine motor activities, and arts and crafts were simultaneously entered to the equation predicting preschoolers' performance in VSWM task. However, frequency of participation in fine motor activities was the only significant predictor improving the model by 6% and thus, after removing the insignificant predictors from the model the final model explained the total of 23% of the variance in VSWM score [F(3, 99) = 9.66, p < 0.001]. Lastly, age and SES together entered to the model predicting preschoolers' performance in switching task. However, only SES variable was contributed significantly in model. After removing age, SES explained 5% of the variance in switching score [F(1, 103) = 5.27, p = 0.024]. Next, based on the bivariate correlation result, pretend play, peer play, sport and physical activities, and parental play support were simultaneously entered into the model. However, only parental play support was found to be significant predictor of switching score that improved the model by 4% [F(2, 102) = 5.21, p = 0.007].
Discussion
The study aimed at exploring the cross-cultural variation in the frequency of preschoolers' engagement in home activities and parental beliefs about the role of play in child development and the purpose of preschool education in Ethiopia and Hungary. Further aims of the present investigation were to examine the cross-cultural variation in the development of preschoolers' EF skills including inhibitory control, shifting, and visual-spatial working memory; and to examine the correlations between preschoolers' home experiences (preschoolers' home activities and their parents' play beliefs) and EF cross-culturally. It is important to note that we intended to recruit two samples that did not differ in terms of SES so that any differences between the samples truly reflect cultural variations. In fact, this was confirmed by the non-significant difference between parental educational levels.
We found that Ethiopian parents hold significantly stronger academic focused beliefs than their Hungarian counterparts. This coincided with our finding indicating that Ethiopian preschoolers practiced academic skills after preschool significantly more often than their Hungarian counterparts. However, no significant cross-cultural variation was found on the play support beliefs that parents hold. Ethiopian parents holding stronger academic focused beliefs while holding comparable play support beliefs with their Hungarian counterparts may indicate that Ethiopian parents recognize that play may have some benefits to child development but prefer academic activities over play for their children.
We also found support for our hypothesis regarding the cross-cultural variation of parental beliefs of the primary purpose of preschool education between the two samples. While academic and cognitive development were reported as the primary purposes of preschool education for Ethiopian parents, social-emotional development and enjoyment for children are the principal purposes of preschool education for Hungarian parents. This is in line with the finding reported by Tigistu (2013) indicating that academic practice is the focus of preschool education in Ethiopia to meet the parents' expectations and demands. Also, Metaferia et al. (2020a) reported that Hungarian parents hold the belief that social-emotional development is the primary purposes of preschool education.
Our finding also demonstrated that Ethiopian preschoolers practiced academic skills and arts and crafts activities at their home more frequently than their Hungarian counterparts. In contrast, our finding depicted that Hungarian preschoolers more frequently engage in fine-motor activities, solitary play, and sports and physical activities at their home compared to their Ethiopian counterparts. We found comparable frequency of spending mealtime together with family, having breakfast at home, and engage in pretend play, motor play, and peer play between preschoolers in Ethiopia and Hungary.
Our finding indicated that there was no significant cross-cultural variation in preschoolers' performance on any of the EF tasks including the Go/no-go task, the switching task and the Mr. Peanut visual spatial working memory task. Our finding also depicts that, after controlling for age and SES, preschoolers' frequency of participation in pretend play and their parents' play support are important variables predicting their inhibitory control skills in both the Ethiopian and the Hungarian samples. These results indicate that preschoolers with parents acknowledging the importance of play for the overall development of children and frequently engage in pretend play have better inhibitory control skills than their counterparts. This finding is consistent with the findings reporting that parental play support (Metaferia et al., 2020a,b) and children's engagement in pretend play (Kelly and Hammond, 2011; Metaferia et al., 2020a) are important factors associated with the development of inhibitory control.
The importance of parental play support for the development of inhibitory control in preschoolers could be explained by different possible means. For instance, parents with strong play support beliefs could better facilitate children's play by different means ranging from supplying play resources to their children to actively engaging in their children's play activities (Johnson et al., 2005) that could in turn improve the parent-child interaction, parental scaffolding, and intimacy. Several studies (see Bernier et al., 2010, 2012; Hammond et al., 2012; Fay-Stammbach et al., 2014) underline the positive contributions of these variables in children's inhibitory control skills.
Our result also demonstrated a cross-cultural variation in the role of breakfast in preschoolers' inhibitory control; on top of parental play support and frequency of pretend play, frequency of breakfast at home proved to be an important factor predicting the development of inhibitory control skills in the Ethiopian sample only. This result replicated the finding reported by Metaferia et al. (2020b) indicating that frequency of breakfast at home is an important factor associated with preschoolers' performance in inhibitory control task. On top of that, the correlation between preschoolers' experience of breakfast at home and their inhibitory control skill is significantly higher in the Ethiopian sample than in the Hungarian.
As expected, we found a cross-cultural variation in preschoolers' home experiences associated with the development of VSWM and shifting skills. While frequency of participation in arts and crafts activities was the important factor predicting VSWM in Ethiopian preschoolers after accounting for preschoolers' age and family SES, frequency of participation in fine motor activities was important predictor of preschoolers' performance in Mr. Peanut's visual-spatial working memory task for Hungarian preschoolers. We also found an interesting result that parental play support is important predictor of the shifting skills in the Hungarian but not in the Ethiopian sample. One of the possible explanations for this cross-cultural variation could be linked to the nature and structure of the play opportunities and activities parents recurrently facilitate for their children at home. Concerning this point, based on their interpretation of the findings from parental belief studies, LaForett and Mendez (2016) underlined that parents' play beliefs could affect the nature of play activities their children frequently engage in. For instance, many of the play activities that Hungarian parents facilitate for their children in their everyday lives could involve the use of shifting skills on top of inhibitory control skills compared to their Ethiopian counterparts.
We believe that the present study makes important contributions to the current literature on the cross-cultural investigation in the development of preschoolers EFs, the role of children's home experience in the development of EF, and the beliefs parents hold about the importance of play in child development and the primary purposes of preschool programs. We focused on samples from two countries that are rarely researched in this field. Yet, it has some limitations. First, while aiming to recruit two samples that are comparable in terms of demographics, both samples were recruited using convenience sampling technique which is not a representative sampling method. Thus, generalization of the findings could be difficult. Besides, it would have been better to use substantially larger samples for a more precise estimation of the results in our cross-cultural investigation. Second, as self-report technique was employed in collecting data from parents about their beliefs of play for child development and the purpose of preschool education and preschoolers' activities at home, the technique might be susceptible to social desirability bias. Third, we have no information regarding the reliability of the measures used to collect preschoolers' home activities as we assessed these variables using individual items. Finally, IQ and language skills of preschoolers, and the effect of other variables within the family and community (such as parenting style, scaffolding, availability of play opportunities including playgrounds) that may be important for the outcome measures were not assessed.
Conclusion
All in all, from the finding of the present study, we made the following conclusions. While building socio-emotional competence seems to be the primary purpose of preschool education for Hungarian parents of preschoolers, academic and cognitive skills development are the priority for their Ethiopian counterparts. In addition, Ethiopian parents were shown to hold significantly stronger academic focused beliefs than their Hungarian counterparts. However, no significant cross-cultural variation was found between Ethiopian and Hungarian parents on play support beliefs. Ethiopian preschoolers seem to practice academic skills and arts and crafts activities at their home more frequently compared to their Hungarian counterparts. On the other hand, Hungarian preschoolers were reported to engage in fine-motor activities, solitary play, and sports and physical activities more frequently at their home compared to their Ethiopian counterparts. We found no cross-cultural variation in the development of the children's EF skills. Parental play support and preschoolers' participation in pretend play were found to be important variables associated with the development of inhibitory control regardless of the socio-cultural variations between the two nations. However, there were cross-cultural variations in terms of predictive factors associated with the development of both VSWM and shifting skills in preschoolers; while frequent participation in arts and crafts was shown an important home experience for the development of VSWM skills in Ethiopian preschoolers, frequent participation in fine motor activities was found an important factor associated with the development of the same skill in the Hungarian sample. Finally, parental play support seems to be an important element of preschoolers' home experience associated with the development of their shifting skills in Hungarian, but not in Ethiopian preschoolers.
Data Acquisition Statement
The data collection process was started by establishing a rapport with preschool directors. Consent papers were sent to the parents of the preschoolers to request for their participation including their children. When parents return the signed papers to the preschools, they were provided with questionnaire to fill it out and returned it back to the preschools. Trained data collectors administered individual EF tests for the participant children at the preschool centers in a separate and silent rooms. The data collection period ranges from November 2018 to October 2019.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the corresponding author upon request.
Ethics Statement
The procedure implemented in the current study was approved by the Eötvös Loránd University's Research Ethics Committee (issue number: 2017/209). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.
Author Contributions
BM, JF, and ZT made valuable contributions to conception and design of the study and data analysis and interpretation. BM prepared the manuscript. JF and ZT read the manuscript and provided crucial feedback. All authors approved the submitted version of the manuscript.
Funding
ZT was funded by the Hungarian National Research, Development and Innovation Office (grant number PD121297).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank all participants, preschool directors, and data collectors.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.646074/full#supplementary-material
References
Act CXC of 2011 on National Public Education. Retrieved from: http://njt.hu/cgi_bin/njt_doc.cgi?docid=139880.329881 (accessed August 10 2020).
Adams, J. W., and Snowling, M. J. (2001). Executive function and reading impairments in children reported by their teachers as ‘hyperactive.' Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 19, 293–306. doi: 10.1348/026151001166083
Admas, F. (2016). Early childhood education in ethiopia: present practices and future directions. Ethiopian J. Educ. 36, 41–72.
Anderson, P. J., and Reidy, N. (2012). Assessing executive function in preschoolers. Neuropsychol. Rev. 22, 345–360. doi: 10.1007/s11065-012-9220-3
Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Matute, E., and Guajardo, S. (2005). The influence of the parents' educational level on the development of executive functions. Dev. Neuropsychol. 28, 539–560. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2801_5
Avornyo, E. A., and Baker, S. (2018). The role of play in children's learning: the perspective of Ghanaian early years stakeholders. Early Years 5146, 1–16. doi: 10.1080/09575146.2018.1473344
Baddeley, A. D., and Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychol. Learn. Motivat. 8, 47–89. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1
Belsky, J., and de Haan, M.. (2011). Annual research review: parenting and children' s brain development: the end of the beginning. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 52, 409–428. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02281.x
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., Deschênes, M., and Matte-Gagné, C. (2012). Social factors in the development of early executive functioning: a closer look at the caregiving environment. Dev. Sci. 15, 12–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01093.x
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., Whipple, N., Annie, B., Stephanie, M., Carlson, N. W., et al. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: early parenting precursors. Child Dev. 81, 326–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x
Best, J. R. (2010). Effects of physical activity on children' s executive function: contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Dev. Rev. 30, 331–351. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2010.08.001
Best, J. R., and Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Dev. 81, 1641–1660. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x
Better Care Network (2015). Ethiopia DHS 2011: Children's Care and Living Arrangements. Retrieved from: https://bettercarenetwork.org/sites/default/files/Ethiopia DHS 2011- Children's Care and Living Arrangements.pdf
Bierman, K. L., Nix, R. L., Greenberg, M. T., Blair, C., and Domitrovich, C. E. (2008). Executive functions and school readiness intervention: impact, moderation, and mediation in the Head Start REDI program. Dev. Psychopathol. 20, 821–843. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000394
Blair, C. (2002). School readiness. Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry. Am. Psychol. 57, 111–127. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.57.2.111
Blair, C., Cybele Raver, C., and Berry, D. J. (2014). Two approaches to estimating the effect of parenting on the development of executive function in early childhood. Dev. Psychol. 50, 554–565. doi: 10.1037/a0033647
Blair, C., and Raver, C. C. (2015). School readiness and self-regulation: a developmental psychobiological approach. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 711–731. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015221
Blair, C., and Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Dev. 78, 647–663. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x
Böddi, Z., and Serfozo, M. (2019). Brief introduction of preschools and preschool teacher training in Hungary. GYERMEKNEVELÉS 7, 187–194. doi: 10.31074/201923187194
Brock, L. L., Rimm-kaufman, S. E., Nathanson, L., and Grimm, K. J. (2009). The contributions of ‘hot' and ‘cool' executive function to children' s academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Child. Res. Q. 24, 337–349. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.06.001
Brown, E. D., Ackerman, B. P., and Moore, C. A. (2013). Family adversity and inhibitory control for economically disadvantaged children: preschool relations and associations with school readiness. J. Fam. Psychol. 27, 443–452. doi: 10.1037/a0032886
Bull, R., Espy, K. A., and Wiebe, S. A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Dev. Neuropsychol. 33, 205–228. doi: 10.1080/87565640801982312
Bull, R., and Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children' s mathematics ability: inhibition, switching, and working memory. Dev. Neuropsychol. 19, 273–293. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN1903_3
Bulotsky-Shearer, R. J., McWayne, C. M., Mendez, J. L., and Manz, P. H. (2016). “Preschool peer play interactions – a develop- mental context for learning for ALL children: rethinking issues of equity and opportunity,” in The Culture of Child Care: Attachment, Peers, and Quality in Diverse Communities, eds K. Sanders and A. W. Guerra (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 179–202.
Burdette, H. L., and Whitaker, R. C. (2005). Resurrecting free play in young children: looking beyond fitness and fatness to attention, affiliation, and affect. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 159, 46–50. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.1.46
Burrage, M. S., Ponitz, C. C., Mccready, E. A., Sims, B. C., Jewkes, A. M., Morrison, F. J., et al. (2008). Age- and schooling-related effects on executive functions in young children: a natural experiment. Child Neuropsychol. 14, 510–524. doi: 10.1080/09297040701756917
Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Dev. Neuropsychol. 28, 617–644. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_3
Carlson, S. M., and Wang, T. S. (2007). Inhibitory control and emotion regulation in preschool children. Cogn. Dev. 22, 489–510. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.08.002
Carson, J. L., and Parke, R. D. (1996). Reciprocal negative affect in parent-child interactions and children' s peer competency. Child Dev. 67, 2217–2226. doi: 10.2307/1131619
Chen, X., Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Cen, G., and Stewart, S. L. (1998). Child-rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: a cross-cultural study. Dev. Psychol. 34, 677–686. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.4.677
Coface Families Europe (2018). Single Parent Centre has Opened in Budapest, Hungary. Retrieved from: http://www.coface-eu.org/consumers/single-parent-centre-has-opened-in-budapest-hungary/#:~:text=More%20than%20300.000%20families%20and,support%20centre%20for%20these%20families (accessed August 02, 20202).
Davidse, N. J., Jong, M. T., and de Bus, A. G. (2015). Causal relations among executive functions and academic skills from causal relations among executive functions and academic skills from preschool to end of first grade. English Linguist. Res. 4, 49–60. doi: 10.5430/elr.v4n1p49
Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., and Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia 44, 2037–2078. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006
Diamond, A. (2007). Consequences of variations in genes that affect dopamine in prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex 17, 161–170. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm082
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
Diamond, A., Prevor, M. B., Callender, G., and Druin, D. P. (1997). Prefrontal cortex cognitive deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 62, 1–206. doi: 10.2307/1166208
Ellefson, M. R., Ng, F. F. Y., Wang, Q., and Hughes, C. (2017). Efficiency of executive function: a two-generation cross-cultural comparison of samples from Hong Kong and the United Kingdom. Psychol. Sci. 28, 555–566. doi: 10.1177/0956797616687812
Farver, J., Kim, Y. K., and Lee, Y. (1995). Cultural differences in Korean- and Anglo-American preschoolers' social interaction and play behaviors. Child Dev. 66, 1088–1099. doi: 10.2307/1131800
Farver, J., and Wimbarti, S. (1995). Indonesian children' s play with their mothers and older siblings. Child Dev. 66, 1493–1503. doi: 10.2307/1131659
Farver, J. A. M. (1999). “Activity setting analysis: A model for examining the role of culture in development,” in Children's Engagement in the World: Sociocultural Perspectives, ed A. Göncü (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 99–127.
Farver, J. M., and Howes, C. (1993). Cultural differences in american and mexican mother-child pretend play. Merrill Palmer Q. 39, 344–358.
Fay-Stammbach, T., Hawes, D. J., and Meredith, P. (2014). Parenting influences on executive function in early childhood: a review. Child Dev. Perspect. 8, 258–264. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12095
Fogle, L. M., and Mendez, J. L. (2006). Assessing the play beliefs of African American mothers with preschool children. Early Child. Res. Q. 21, 507–518. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2006.08.002
Foy, J. G., and Mann, V. A. (2013). Executive function and early reading skills. Read. Writ. 26, 453–472. doi: 10.1007/s11145-012-9376-5
Fuhs, M. W., and Day, J. D. (2011). Verbal ability and executive functioning development in preschoolers at head start. Dev. Psychol. 47, 404–416. doi: 10.1037/a0021065
Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., and Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychol. Bull. 134, 31–60. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31
Gaskins, S. (1999). “Children's daily lives in a Mayan village: a case study of culturally constructed roles and activities,” in Children's Engagement in the World: Sociocultural Perspectives, ed A. Goncu (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 25–60.
Gaskins, S. (2006). “Cultural perspectives on infant-caregiver interaction,” in Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition, and Interaction, eds J. Enfeld and S. C. Levinson (England: Oxford), 279–298.
Gaskins, S., Haight, W., and Lancy, D. F. (2006). “The cultural construction of play,” in Play and Development: Evolutionary, Sociocultural, and Functional Perspectives, eds A. Goncu and S. Gaskins (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Publishers), 179–202.
Ginsburg, K. R. (2007). The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent-child bond. Pediatrics 147:e2020011585. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-2697
Göncü, A., and Gaskins, S. (2011). “Comparing and extending Piaget's and Vygotsky's understandings of play: Symbolic play as individual, sociocultural, and educational interpretation,” in The Oxford Handbook of the Development of Play, ed A. Pellegrini (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 48–57.
Göncü, A., Mistry, J., and Mosier, C. (2000). Cultural variations in the play of toddlers. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 24, 321–329. doi: 10.1080/01650250050118303
Grabell, A. S., Olson, S. L., Miller, A. L., Kessler, D. A., Felt, B., Kaciroti, N., et al. (2015). The impact of culture on physiological processes of emotion regulation: a comparison of US and Chinese preschoolers. Dev. Sci. 18, 420–435. doi: 10.1111/desc.12227
Greenfield, P. M., Trumball, E., Keller, H., Rothstein-Fisch, C., Suzuki, L. K., and Quiroz, B. (2006). “Cultural conceptions of learning and development,” in Handbook of Educational Psychology, eds P. A. Alexander and P. H. Winne (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 675–692.
Haight, W. L., Parke, R. D., and Black, J. E. (1997). Mothers' and fathers' beliefs about and spontaneous participation in their toddlers' pretend play. Merrill-Palmer Q. 43, 271–290.
Hammond, S. I., Müller, U., Carpendale, J. I. M., Bibok, M. B., and Liebermann-Finestone, D. P. (2012). The effects of parental scaffolding on preschoolers' executive function. Dev. Psychol. 48, 271–281. doi: 10.1037/a0025519
Howard, S. J., Okely, A. D., and Ellis, Y. G. (2015). Evaluation of a differentiation model of preschoolers' executive functions. Front. Psychol. 6:285. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00285
Hughes, C., and Devine, R. T. (2017). For better or for worse? Positive and negative parental influences on young children's executive function. Child Dev. 90, 593–609. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12915
Hughes, C., and Ensor, R. (2009). How do families help or hinder the emergence of early executive function. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 123, 35–50.
Hughes, C., Roman, G., Hart, M. J., and Ensor, R. (2013). Does maternal depression predict young children' s executive function? – a 4-year longitudinal study. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 2, 169–177. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12014
Ihmeideh, F. (2019). Getting parents involved in children's play?: Qatari parents' perceptions of and engagement with their children's play. Int. J. Prim. Elementary Early Years Educ. 47, 42–63. doi: 10.1080/03004279.2017.1399152
Imada, T., Carlson, S. M., and Itakura, S. (2013). East – West cultural differences in context-sensitivity are evident in early childhood. Dev. Sci. 2, 198–208. doi: 10.1111/desc.12016
Jian, H. (2009). A contrastive study of cultural diversity of learning styles between China and the United States. Int. Educ. Stud. 2:p163. doi: 10.5539/ies.v2n1p163
Johnson, J. E., Christie, J., and Wardle, F. (2005). Play Development and Early Education. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Józsa, K., Török, B., and Stevenson, C. (2018). Preschool and kindergarten in Hungary and the United States: a comparison within transnational development policy. Int. J. Educ. Dev. 62, 88–95. doi: 10.1016/j.ijedudev.2018.03.001
Kelly, R., and Hammond, S. (2011). The relationship between symbolic play and executive function in young children. Australas. J. Early Child. 36, 21–27. doi: 10.1177/183693911103600204
LaForett, D. R., and Mendez, J. L. (2016). Children's engagement in play at home: a parent's role in supporting play opportunities during early childhood. Early Child Dev. Care 187, 910–923. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2016.1223061
Lan, X., Legare, C. H., Ponitz, C. C., Li, S., and Morrison, F. J. (2011). Investigating the links between the subcomponents of executive function and academic achievement: a cross-cultural analysis of Chinese and American preschoolers. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108, 677–692. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.11.001
Lee, K., Ng, S. F., Pe, M. L., Ang, S. Y., Nabil, M., Mohd, A., et al. (2012). The cognitive underpinnings of emerging mathematical skills: executive functioning, patterns, numeracy, and arithmetic. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 82, 82–99. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.2010.02016.x
Leo-Rhynie, E., Minott, C., Gift, S., McBean, M., Scott, A.-K., and Wilson, K. (2009). Competency of Children in Guyana, Rural Jamaica, and St. Vincent and the Grenadines Making the Transition From Pre-primary School With Special Emphasis on Gender Differences. Jamaica: Dudley Grant Memorial Trust Kingston.
Lewis, C., Koyasu, M., Oh, S., Ogawa, A., Short, B., and Huang, Z. (2009). Culture, executive function, and social understanding. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 123, 69–85.
Lin, X., and Li, H. (2018). Parents' play beliefs and engagement in young children' s play at home at home. Eur. Early Child. Educ. Res. J. 26, 161–176. doi: 10.1080/1350293X.2018.1441979
Lin, Y.-C., and Yawkey, T. (2014). Parents' play beliefs and the relationship to children's social competence. Education 135, 107–114.
Linebarger, D. L., Barr, R., Lapierre, M. A., and Piotrowski, J. T. (2014). Associations between parenting, media use, cumulative risk, and children' s executive functioning. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 35, 367–377. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000069
Logie, C. (2013). “Pedagogical dilemmas and issues affecting early childhood teaching and learning: Reflections and experiences from Trinidad and Tobago,” in Issues and Perspectives in Early Childhood Education in the Caribbean, eds C. Logie and J. L. Roopnarine (La Romaine: Caribbean Publishers), 114–142.
Lonigan, C. J., Lerner, M. D., Goodrich, J. M., Farrington, A. L., and Allan, D. M. (2016). Executive function of Spanish-speaking language- minority preschoolers: structure and relations with early literacy skills and behavioral outcomes. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 144, 46–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.003
Lucassen, N., Kok, R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Jaddoe, V. W. V., Hofman, A., et al. (2015). Executive functions in early childhood: the role of maternal and paternal parenting practices. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 33, 489–505. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12112
Macmillan, N. A., and Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection Theory: A User's Guide (2nd edn.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Connor, C. M., Farris, C. L., Jewkes, A. M., and Morrison, F. J. (2007). Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers' literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Dev. Psychol. 43, 947–959. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.947
Mcelwain, N. L., and Volling, B. L. (2005). Preschool children' s interactions with friends and older siblings: relationship specificity and joint contributions to problem Behavior 19, 486–496. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.486
Metaferia, B. K., Futo, J., Drew, R., and Takacs, Z. K. (2020a). Parents' beliefs about play and the purpose of preschool education, preschoolers' home activity and executive functions. Front. Psychol. 11:1104. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01104
Metaferia, B. K., Takacs, Z. K., and Futo, J. (2020b). The relationship between parental play beliefs, preschoolers' home experience, and executive functions: an exploratory study in Ethiopia. Front. Psychol. 11:624. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00624
Meuwissen, A. S., and Carlson, S. M. (2015). Fathers matter: the role of father parenting in preschoolers' executive function development. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 140, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.010
Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Dev. 75, 1373–1386. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00746.x
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
MoE (Ministry of Education) (2016). Education Statistics Annual Abstract 2015/16. Addis Ababa: Ministry of Education.
Montoya, M. F., Susperreguy, M. I., Dinarte, L., Morrison, F. J., San Martín, E., Rojas-Barahona, C. A., et al. (2018). Executive function in Chilean preschool children: do short-term memory, working memory, and response inhibition contribute differentially to early academic skills? Early Child. Res. Q. 46, 187–200. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.02.009
Moriguchi, Y. (2014). The early development of executive function and its relation to social interaction: a brief review. Front. Psychol. 5:388. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00388
Moriguchi, Y., Chevalier, N., and Zelazo, P. D. (2016). Editorial: development of executive function during childhood cognitive processes of EF during childhood. Front Psychol. 7:6. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00006
Morra, S. (1994). Issues in working memory measurement: testing for M capacity. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 17, 143–159.
Nayfeld, I., Fuccillo, J., and Greenfield, D. B. (2013). Executive functions in early learning: extending the relationship between executive functions and school readiness to science. Learn. Individ. Differ. 26, 81–88. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.04.011
Nieto, M., Ros, L., Medina, G., Ricarte, J. J., and Latorre, J. M. (2016). Assessing executive functions in preschoolers using shape school task. Front. Psychol. 7:1489. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01489
Noble, K., Norman, F., and Farah, M. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of SES in Kindergarten children. Dev. Sci. 8, 74–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.x
O'brien, L. M., and Md-Yunus, S. (2007). For parents particularly: how parents can encourage creativity in children. Child. Educ. 83, 236–237. doi: 10.1080/00094056.2007.10522921
Oh, S., and Lewis, C. (2008). Korean preschoolers' advanced inhibitory control and its relation to other executive skills and mental state understanding. Child Dev. 79, 80–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01112.x
Parmar, P., Harkness, S., and Super, C. M. (2004). Asian and Euro-American parents' ethnotheories of play and learning: effects on preschool children' s home routines and school behaviour. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 28, 97–104. doi: 10.1080/01650250344000307
Pechtel, P., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2011). Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective function: an integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology 214, 55–70. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2009-2
Peirce, J. W. (2008). Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Front. Neuroinform. 2:10. doi: 10.3389/neuro.11.010.2008
Pellegrini, A. D., and Smith, P. K. (1998). The development of play during childhood: forms and possible functions. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Rev. 3, 51–57. doi: 10.1017/S1360641798001476
Pennington, B. F., and Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 37, 51–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01380.x
Ponitz, C. C., McClelland, M. M., Matthews, J. S., and Morrison, F. J. (2009). A structured observation of behavioral self-regulation and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. Dev. Psychol. 45, 605–619. doi: 10.1037/a0015365
Purpura, D. J., Schmitt, S. A., and Ganley, C. M. (2017). ations of mathematics and literacy: the role of executive functioning components. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 153, 15–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.010
Rhoades, B. L., Greenberg, M. T., Lanza, S. T., and Blair, C. (2011). Demographic and familial predictors of early executive function development: contribution of a person-centered perspective. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108, 638–662. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.004
Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Göncü, A., and Mosier, C. (1993). Guided in participation cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 58:183.
Roopnarine, J. L. (2011). “Cultural variations in beliefs about play, parent?child play, and children's play: meaning for childhood development,” in The Oxford Handbook of the Development of Play, ed A. Pellegrini (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 19–37.
Roopnarine, J. L. (2012). What is the state of play? Int. J. Play 1, 228–230. doi: 10.1080/21594937.2012.735452
Roopnarine, J. L., and Davidson, K. L. (2015). Parent-child play across cultures: advancing play research. Am. J. Play 7, 228–252.
Roopnarine, J. L., and Jin, B. (2012). Indo Caribbean immigrant beliefs about play and its impact on early academic performances. Am. J. Play 4, 441–463.
Roopnarine, J. L., Shin, M., Jung, K., and Hossain, Z. (2003). “Play and early development and education: the instantiation of parental belief systems,” in Contemporary Perspectives on Play in Early Childhood Education, eds O. N. Saracho and B. Spodek (Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing), 115–132.
Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., and Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: a comparison of Chinese and U.S. Presch. Psychol. Sci. 17, 74–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01667.x
Sarsour, K., Sheridan, M., Jutte, D., Nuru-Jeter, A., Hinshaw, S., and Boyce, W. T. (2011). Family socioeconomic status and child executive functions: the roles of language, home environment, and single parenthood. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 120–132. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710001335
Schmitt, S. A., Korucu, I., Purpura, D. J., Whiteman, S., Zhang, C., and Yang, F. (2019). Exploring cross-cultural variations in the development of executive function for preschoolers from low and high socioeconomic families. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 43, 212–220. doi: 10.1177/0165025418785469
Séguin, J. R., and Zelazo, P. D. (2005). “Executive function in early physical aggression,” in Developmental Origins of Aggression, eds R. E. Tremblay, W. W. Hartup, and J. Archer (New York, NY: Guilford), 307–329.
Skogan, A. H., Egeland, J., Zeiner, P., Øvergaard, K. R., Oerbeck, B., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., et al. (2015). Factor structure of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functions (BRIEF-P) at age three years. Child Neuropsychol. 22, 472–492. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2014.992401
Song, H., and Jinyu, A. (2016). The relationship between executive functions and IQ in Korean children and the comparison with Chinese children. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child. 6, 158–165. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2015.1136631
St Clair-Thompson, H. L., and Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 59, 745–759. doi: 10.1080/17470210500162854
Suchodoletz, A., Von Gestsdottir, S., Wanless, S. B., Mcclelland, M. M., Birgisdottir, F., Gunzenhauser, C., et al. (2013). Behavioral self-regulation and relations to emergent academic skills among children in Germany and Iceland. Early Child. Res. Q. 28, 62–73. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.05.003
Tamis-LeMonda, C., Shannon, J. D., Cabrera, N. J., and Lamb, M. E. (2004). Fathers and mothers at play with their 2- and 3-year-olds contributions to Language and Cognitive Development. Child Dev. 75, 1806–1820. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00818.x
Tamis-LeMonda, C., Uzgiris, I., and Bornstein, M. (2002). “Play in parent-child interactions,” in Handbook of Parenting, 2nd edn. Vol. 5, ed M. Bornstein (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 221–241.
Thorp, D. M., Stahmer, A. C., and Schreibman, L. (1995). Effects of sociodramatic play training on children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 25, 265–282. doi: 10.1007/BF02179288
Tigistu, K. (2013). Professionalism in early childhood education and care in Ethiopia: what are we talking about? Child. Educ. 89, 152–158. doi: 10.1080/00094056.2013.792641
Tobin, J. J., Wu, D. Y. H., and Davidson, D. H. (1989). Preschool in Three Cultures: Japan, China and the United States. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Turkheimer, M., Bakeman, R., and Adamson, L. B. (1989). Do mothers support and peers inhibit skilled object play in infancy? Infant Behav. Dev. 12, 37–44. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(89)90051-9
Usai, M. C., Viterbori, P., Traverso, L., and De Franchis, V.. (2014). Latent structure of executive function in five- and six-year-old children: a longitudinal study. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 11, 447–462. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2013.840578
Vernon-feagans, L., Willoughby, M., and Garrett-peters, P. (2016). Predictors of behavioral regulation in kindergarten: household chaos, parenting, and early executive functions. Dev. Psychol. 52, 430–441. doi: 10.1037/dev0000087
Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Lan, X., Son, S. H., Cameron, C. E., Morrison, F. J., et al. (2013). Gender differences in behavioral regulation in four societies: the United States, Taiwan, South Korea, and China. Early Child. Res. Q. 28, 621–633. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.04.002
Weiland, C., Barata, M. C., and Yoshikawa, H. (2014). The co-occurring development of executive function skills and receptive vocabulary in preschool- aged children: a look at the direction of the developmental pathways. Infant Child Dev. 23, 4–21. doi: 10.1002/icd.1829
Welsh, J. A., Nix, R. L., Blair, C., Bierman, K. L., and Nelson, K. E. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. J. Educ. Psychol. 102, 43–53. doi: 10.1037/a0016738
Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T., Mize, J., Clark, C. A. C., Chevalier, N., and Andrews, K. (2011). The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108, 436–452. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.008
Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T. D., and Espy, K. A. (2012). Separating the fish from the sharks: a longitudinal study of preschool response inhibition. Child Dev. 83, 1245–1261. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01765.x
Woldehanna, T. (2011). The effects of early childhood education attendance on cognitive development: evidence from urban. Ethiopian J. Econ. XX, 113–164.
World Bank (2019a). Africa Can End Poverty: Exploring Africa's Economic Challenges and Opportunities. Washington, DC: World Bank.
World Bank (2019c). The World Bank Data. Washington, DC: World Bank. Retrieved from: https://data.worldbank.org/country/hungary?view=chart (accessed June 20, 2020).
Yahya, R., and Wood, E. A. (2017). Play as third space between home and school?: Bridging cultural discourses. J. Early Child. Res. 15, 305–322. doi: 10.1177/1476718X15616833
Zelazo, P. D., Blair, C. B., and Willoughby, M. T. (2016). Executive Function: Implications for Education. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.
Keywords: executive function, home experience, parental view, preschool children, play
Citation: Metaferia BK, Futo J and Takacs ZK (2021) Parents' Views on Play and the Goal of Early Childhood Education in Relation to Children's Home Activity and Executive Functions: A Cross-Cultural Investigation. Front. Psychol. 12:646074. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.646074
Received: 08 February 2021; Accepted: 29 March 2021;
Published: 26 April 2021.
Edited by:
Dieter Baeyens, KU Leuven, BelgiumReviewed by:
Isabel Piñeiro, University of A Coruña, SpainSergey Malykh, Psychological Institute of Russian Academy of Education (RAE), Russia
Copyright © 2021 Metaferia, Futo and Takacs. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Biruk K. Metaferia, Ymlya2ViZTIwMDQmI3gwMDA0MDtnbWFpbC5jb20=