Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 28 May 2021
Sec. Environmental Psychology

Does the Spatial Layout of a Playground Affect the Play Activities in Young Children? A Pilot Study

  • 1Department of Human Studies, Seisen University, Hikone, Japan
  • 2Graduate School of Human Development and Environment, Kobe University, Kobe, Japan

Background: The objective of this study was to describe, through measurement of physical activity and observation of free outdoor play, the relation between children’s free play and the spatial layout of the playground. To accomplish this, we altered the spatial layout of the same playground to see how the layout affects the play activity and the physical activity levels in the same children.

Methods: Participants were six young children (four girls and two boys; mean age = 5 years and 1 month, SD = 2.59 months). Participants’ physical activity level and the duration of different types of action that occurred in each area and their transitions were compared before and after the alteration of the play-equipment layout using the data from accelerometers and video recordings.

Results: A significant increase in physical activity occurred after the spatial layout alteration, which was related to action differences. Before the alteration, children tended to play in a similar manner for a given play area; however, after the alteration, pronounced interindividual variation in play activity across children was observed.

Conclusion: The present pilot study found that in free play situations in the outdoor playground, the spatial layout of playground affects the pattern of play activity and the physical activity levels of young children.

Introduction

Over the past 50 years, play opportunities for children, especially free play in outside play spaces, have continually declined in some countries (Burdette and Whitaker, 2005; Malone, 2007). This trend contrasts the research-grounded understanding of the importance of play for children’s healthy development (Ginsburg, 2007; Pellegrini et al., 2007). The decline in the opportunities for free play makes preschool playgrounds all the more important, where young children spend an ample amount of time and can choose what to do freely. Free play is an activity that is freely chosen and is performed for individuals’ own sake. Adult-directed structured play, such as games and sports, do not fall into the category of free play (Burdette and Whitaker, 2005; Gray, 2011). Through play, children can experiment, solve problems, think creatively, corporate with others, and gain a deeper knowledge about themselves and the world. Today, it is crucial that we better understand the nature of children’s free play behaviors and how we can promote them.

Play activities have been quantitatively described in terms of physical activity levels as measured by step counts or accelerometers. Previous studies found that physical activity levels are influenced by individual or environmental factors. The individual factors included sex (Hinkley et al., 2008), ethnicity (Sallis et al., 1993), and fundamental movement skills (Fisher et al., 2005; Williams et al., 2008; Cohen et al., 2014), while the environmental factors included parents’ obesity (Danner et al., 1991), parents’ physical activity level (Xu et al., 2018), active or outdoor playtime (Burdette et al., 2004; Raustorp et al., 2012), the amount of play equipment (Herrington and Lesmeister, 2006; Sando and Sandseter, 2020), the number of children per m2 in the playground (Cardon et al., 2008; Van Cauwenberghe et al., 2012), playground esthetics or safety (Farley et al., 2007; Gray, 2011), natural play area (Fjørtoft and Sageie, 2000; Dowdell et al., 2011; Herrington and Brussoni, 2015), spatial layout of equipment (Liempd et al., 2020), institutional policy (Dowda et al., 2009; Trost et al., 2010), physical activity training and education for teachers and/or parents (Bower et al., 2008), and sociocultural differences (Leeger-Aschmann et al., 2016).

In preschools, various intervention methods to promote spontaneous free play have been proposed, which include decreasing playground density (Van Cauwenberghe et al., 2012), the provision of play equipment (Hannon and Brown, 2008), the provision of playground markings (Cardon et al., 2009), teachers’ encouragement (Brown et al., 2009), and increased recess duration (Holmes et al., 2006). A couple of methods, such as decreasing playground density and provision of portable play equipment, resulted in partially increased physical activity (Hannon and Brown, 2008; Van Cauwenberghe et al., 2012), and another study reported positive cognitive effects (Holmes et al., 2006). However, there are also a number of other studies that have reported inconclusive evidence concerning the causal relationship between playground features and children’s play activity pattern or physical activity level (Mota et al., 2005; Ridgers and Stratton, 2005; Stratton and Mullan, 2005; Ridgers et al., 2007; Bohn-Goldbaum et al., 2013; Broekhuizen et al., 2014).

In the present study, we altered the spatial layout of the same playground to see how the layout affects the play activity and the physical activity levels in the same children. The central concept guiding this alteration of spatial layout is that of affordances. Gibson (1979) developed this concept to account for the fact that humans and animals including young children primarily pay attention to and perceive what they can do in a given environment, instead of perceiving the abstract qualities of objects in the environment such as position, shape, and color as such (Gibson, 1979). Young children’s play is always situated or embedded (Adolph et al., 2019), and the possibilities offered by the environment are the basic conditions for spontaneous play. In order to promote children’s free play in the playground, it is desirable that the playground has a rich set of affordances that afford a wide variety of actions by young children.

Based on the concept of affordances, in the present study, we aimed to enrich the variety of play activities of children by altering the spatial layout of the playground in a daycare center in Japan. After the discussion with caregivers in the daycare center, we altered the playground based on the following three basic plans: (1) to make available the affordance for climbing (e.g., a climbable mound), (2) to afford easy access to toys and objects used in each area, and (3) to clearly separate play areas with a different set of affordances. Since the alteration of the playground was conducted in an exploratory manner, in the present study, we focused on general questions such as the following: Does the alteration of the spatial layout of playground affects the pattern of play activity of young children playing in the playground? Does the alteration of the spatial layout of playground affects the physical activity levels of young children playing in the playground?

Materials and Methods

Participants

Six children (four girls and two boys; M = 5 years and 1 month, SD = 2.59 months) from 4-year-old classes at a private certified daycare center in Osaka, Japan participated. All participants voluntarily participated in this study. During the observations, all children from other classes were also playing in the playground. The daycare center was in its third year and had a capacity of 90 children, aged 0–6 years, when the present observations took place. Informed consent forms were obtained from all children’s parents and the principal of the daycare center.

Playground

The original spatial layout of the playground of the daycare center is illustrated in Figure 1A. The playground was segmented into 11 areas indicated in Figure 1C: 10 areas surrounding each play equipment and an area of open space. The detailed features of play equipment are indicated in Table 1. The total ground area was about 600 m2. The sandbox and slide were fixed on the opposite side of the building near a fence (illustrated in the lower right part of Figure 1A). Utensils used for sand play were placed on tool shelves near the slide area by the fence. At the center of the playground, between the open and tires areas, there was play equipment made of wood with steps that afforded climbing (referred to as “woodstep,” see Table 1). Ten tires were unevenly placed in the part of the open space (referred to as “tires,” see Table 1). There was a slope made of wood (referred to as “woodslope,” see Table 1) and a tower-shaped climbing frame (referred to as “tower,” see Table 1) next to the sandbox along the fence.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of the spatial layout of the playground and the area separations for observation. (A,B) illustrate the spatial layout before and after the alteration, respectively. (C,D) indicate area separations before and after the alteration, respectively. The numbers indicated below the area name are the square meters (m2) of the area.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The detailed features of the play equipment and area.

After the video observation of children’s play at the aforementioned original playground, we changed the spatial layout of the play equipment based on the discussion with the caregivers of the daycare center. By changing the spatial layout of the play equipment, we aimed to enrich the variety of play activities of children. To achieve this aim, although our approach was admittedly exploratory, we chose to alter the playground layout according to the following three plans: (1) to make available the affordance for climbing (e.g., a climbable mound), (2) to afford easy access to toys and objects used in each area, and (3) to clearly separate play areas with a different set of affordances. First point was considered because the original playground was relatively level, and there was little chance for children to explore convexities or concavities. Second point reflects the problem the caregivers were aware of in the original playground, especially in the sandbox area, in which a shelf for toys and utensils cannot be easily accessed by children while playing in the area. The shelf for utensils was located far from the sandbox, and children needed to move back and forth to use the utensils. Third point concerns safety. In the original layout, the woodstep was in the middle of the open area, and the woodslope was facing the open area. Since children played with balls mainly in the open area, balls often got in the way of play activity at those contiguous areas; thus, children playing with balls were somewhat restricted, while other children felt uneasy that they might be hit by balls.

The spatial layout was altered following the above basic plans (see Figures 1B,D). First, a mound was created in an area on the side of the building where tires had been scattered before the alteration, and 10 tires were embedded in the ground vertically in the remaining area of the original tires area (see Table 1). The tower was placed at the center of the playground. The woodstep was placed near the building, and a track was created around it by marking a line (see Table 1). Children could climb on and sit or stand on the tower or the woodstep and observe other children from the height. Second, we placed play equipment between the sandbox and tires areas to provide various play opportunities in the sandbox area. Some of the utensils were placed on the play equipment. Third, for ball play to be confined within the open area without disturbing the children playing in the other areas, the open area was segmented with play equipment or lines, which were marked on the ground to create the aforementioned track. There was no play equipment in the open area. With the segmentation, the open area became a separated, rectangular area.

Measurement and Observation Procedures

Measurement of Physical Activity

Children’s physical activity level was measured with an Actigraph wGT3X accelerometer (Actigraph Corp., Pensacola, FL, United States). Actigraph wGT3X is a lightweight (19 g; 4.6 cm × 3.3 cm × 1.5 cm), triaxial accelerometer, which collects motion data on three orthogonal axes—vertical (Y), horizontal right–left (X), and horizontal front–back axis (Z)—between 0.05 and 2.0 Gs. This accelerometer has been validated for use with children in laboratory and field settings (Santos-Lozano et al., 2012; Delisle et al., 2015; Carson et al., 2019). Participants wore this equipment using an elastic belt at the waist. The analog acceleration data were converted to a digital signal, and the value (count) was stored in specified time interval (epoch). Ten-second epochs were used for this study. After data collection, the monitor was downloaded to a computer for subsequent data reduction and analysis.

Video Recording

The free play in the playground was recorded on November 27, 2018 (referred to as “before the alteration”) and on January 23, 2019 (referred to as “after the alteration”). The alteration of the spatial layout of the playground was implemented on December 16, 2018. Video recording was performed with three cameras (Sony HDR-PJ675), which were set to record most of the playground. The climate of the area was warm, and the temperature on the day of observation was 19°C for before the alteration and 16°C after the alteration. All participants wore long sleeve T-shirts and sometimes rolled up their sleeves. At the center that we observed, children usually have free, unstructured outdoor play time from around 10:30 A.M. to 12:00 P.M., without any instructions from caregivers. The observation time was 97 min for before the alteration and 87 min for after the alteration.

Action Coding

The actions that appeared during play were coded by observing the video recordings (Adolph, 2016). Table 2 summarizes the code names and definitions. Ten categories, which included (1) locomotion, (2) climbing, (3) manipulation, (4) sedentary, (5) cycling, (6) sloping, (7) sand play, (8) water play, (9) sporting, and (10) horseplay, were created during observation. Categories were defined so that multiple codes would not overlap and so children’s actions could be described without leaving anything out. The coding was performed using video analysis software (Datavyu1). We recorded the times when participants (1) started and ended the actions and (2) entered and exited the areas. The first coder coded 100% of children’s actions; then, a second coder coded 25% of the total time. The coding agreement rate for the behavior categories was 0.91.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Children’s actions observed in play were coded into these 10 categories.

Data Analysis

Data analyses were conducted as follows. Participants’ physical activity level was modeled using a linear mixed model with fixed-effects for layout (original vs. altered) and area (nine areas) factors and random intercept effects for individual children. To investigate the duration of actions in each area, the proportion of duration of observed actions in each area (referred to as “assemblage of actions”) were calculated for the original playground and after the alteration of layout. A multivariate non-parametric permutational analysis of variance was performed to test the presence of significant variations in assemblage of actions among areas and among individuals using the adonis function in R package vegan (Oksanen et al., 2016). We constructed dendrograms to visualize the similarities among the assemblage of actions of each individual play episode according to the unweighted pair-group mean arithmetic (UPGMA) method using R function hclust. To examine the visiting sequences of transitions among different areas in the playground, we summarized the sequential relationships between areas in the playground visited by children in a transition matrix—separately for before and after the alteration of spatial layout. A Chi-square test of quasi-independence was used to test whether the overall sequential pattern differed from a random ordering of events. Binomial test z-scores (adjusted residuals) were computed from frequency counts of transitions between each consecutive behavior corresponding significance levels (Bakeman and Quera, 2011). Significance level for statistical analysis was set at 0.05.

Results

Children’s playing time was 83 min 24 s before the alteration and 62 min 18 s after the alteration (see Table 3). In particular, for the play times, after the alteration, three participants—P2, P4, and P6—were under 60 min because of class activities. In the original playground, four children spent most of their time at the sandbox area, and two were most often at the woodslope area and playing with sand. In the altered playground, children spent most of their time at various places. P1 was mostly in the open area, P3 and P4 were at the sandbox area for more than 20 min, and P5 and P6 were at the tires area for around 40 min.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Participants’ playing time: total and in each area.

Assemblage of Actions in Each Area

Figure 2 presents the proportion of duration of 10 categories of action (Table 2) observed in each area of the playground before and after the alteration of the layout. The proportion of actions observed during play in each area differed between the original and altered spatial layout. For example, the relative duration of manipulation increased in the sandbox area and that of locomotion, sloping, and climbing increased in the slide area, respectively. The proportion of sporting increased in the open area and that of sloping and climbing increased in the woodslope area, respectively (Figure 2). On the other hand, the relative duration of sand play generally decreased. In the newly constructed mound area, sedentary occupied the highest percentage at 37%, and locomotion and cycling were 23 and 20%, respectively. In the track area, cycling was the most popular action (53%), and locomotion was the second (35%) (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Comparison of assemblage of actions before and after the spatial alteration. The data for mound and track areas were not available before the alteration.

We constructed dendrograms to visualize the similarities among the assemblage of actions of each individual play episode. The dendrograms are shown in Figure 3A for before the alteration and Figure 3B for after the alteration. In the dendrogram for before the alteration (Figure 3A), small clusters of H (tower), F (woodstep), and A (sandbox) are observed. By contrast, in the dendrogram for after the alteration of the layout (Figure 3B), except J (track), clustering based on areas disappeared. Instead, clustering of the same participant (notably P5, P4, and P6) emerged. To statistically examine the effect of area and participant on the proportion of actions of play episode, non-parametric permutational multivariate analyses of variance showed that, before the alteration, the Area had a significant effect on the assemblage of play action. By contrast, after the alteration, a significant effect from the Area was not detected; however, the Individual did have a significant effect (see Table 4).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The hierarchical clusters of duration of each action in each area before and after the alteration [(A) dendrogram of before the alteration, (B) dendrogram of after the alteration]. The height, shown on the y-axis, means the similarity between action durations in the given area. The letters in dendrograms indicate each area (A, sandbox; B, slide; C, tires; D, open; F, woodstep; G, woodslope; H, tower; I, mound; and J, track).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Permutational multivariate analysis of variance results for the assemblage of the 10 action categories observed during play [R2 (coefficient of determination) indicates how well each factor explains the dependent variable].

Physical Activity Measurement

The average counts of all participants per epoch that reflect the children’s physical activity levels are shown in Figure 4. Linear mixed model analyses on the children’s physical activity levels with fixed-effects for area and layout and random intercept effects for individual children revealed significant effects of both Layout, F(1,69) = 16.88, p < 0.001, and Area, F(8,66) = 2.91, p = 0.008, but no significant interaction was found between the two factors. Bonferroni-corrected multiple comparisons revealed that the children’s physical activity levels were significantly greater in the tires, open, and track areas as compared to the tower area (ps < 0.05). Children’s physical activity levels after the alteration were significantly greater in the sandbox, open, woodstep, and woodslope areas.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The averaged accelerometer counts per epoch before and after the alteration. The data for mound and track areas were not available before the alteration.

Transitions Between Places

To investigate the visiting sequences of transitions among different places in the playground, we summarized the sequential relationships between places in the playground visited by children in a transition matrix, separately for before and after the alteration (see Table 5). In a chi-square test of quasi-independence, before the alteration, the overall sequential pattern differed significantly from a random ordering of events, χ2(29) = 124.43, p < 0.001. However, after the alteration, the overall sequential pattern was not significantly different from a random ordering of events, χ2(55) = 64.63, p = 0.18), indicating that there were no stereotyped patterns of sequential transitions between places in the playground. We further calculated binomial test z-scores (adjusted residuals) from frequency counts of transitions between each consecutive behavior corresponding significance levels. In the original playground, the transition between the sandbox and tower and the transition between the tires and open areas occurred much more frequently than expected in both directions, p < 0.001. In contrast, after the alteration, no such clear tendency of sequential transition patterns was observed except that unidirectional transitions were more likely to occur than chance from open to sandbox and, to a lesser degree, from sandbox to slide.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Frequencies of sequential transition between consecutive places in playground before and after the alteration.

Discussion

The present study explored the influence of the spatial arrangement of play equipment in a playground on children’s play activity patterns and physical activity levels. We aimed to enrich the variety of play activities of children by making the following changes to the spatial layout of playground: (1) to make available the affordance for climbing (e.g., a climbable mound), (2) to afford easy access to toys and objects used in each area, and (3) to clearly separate play areas with a different set of affordances. Before the alteration, children tended to play in a similar manner for a given play area; however, after the alteration, pronounced interindividual variation in play activity across children was observed. We also observed that the alteration of play equipment layout promoted new types of play such as organized, sporting play, locomotive play (e.g., walking across tires), and imitative play (e.g., using sandbox, kitchen-like play equipment, and utensils). The frequencies of sequential transition between consecutive places in the playground were also altered, indicating the reduction in stereotypical patterns of sequential transitions between places after the alteration of the playground. These changes in the play activities of children lead them to display increased level of physical activity after the alteration of the spatial layout compared to that in the original playground.

The Relation Between Play Patterns and Physical Activity Levels

When we changed the spatial arrangement in the playground, we created the area with vertically embedded tires and the area with a climbable mound so that children can climb onto them and experience the height. The increase in the duration of free play was observed in three children in the tires area, and the duration of locomotion was increased as they played by walking on the embedded tires. The marked increase in the duration of play was also observed in two children in the mound area. We also improved the accessibility to utensils and toys in the sandbox area so that children could play with utensils, for example, to imitate cooking. As a consequence, the children moved back and forth to carry sand or to manipulate utensils rather than to sit and to mold sand as they did in the original playground. The open area was demarcated by placing other play equipment. The demarcation of the open area afforded the organized soccer play, which in turn increased physical activity levels in children played in this area.

Implications for Play Ground Design

Although it may be difficult to install a new set of play equipment or renovate the whole playground at daycare centers or in public parks, as demonstrated in the present study, it is possible to alter the spatial layout of play equipment in such a way to influence children’s free play behavior. Previous studies reported that the altered mode of play, the reported level of fun, or the assessment of playground would be heightened through non-standardizing the design of stepping stones in older children (Prieske et al., 2015; Sporrel et al., 2017). The findings of the present study add to these evidence by showing that even young preschool-aged children spontaneously changed their play patterns in such a way to use different opportunities provided by the playground. This finding may have practical implications: although it would be difficult to have preschool-aged children verbally assess the playground (c.f., Sporrel et al., 2017), it is possible for designers to assess the play activities of young children after rearranging the equipment in a manner conducted in the present study and to further examine what kind of environment affords children’s free play.

Limitations: Other Possible Explanation of Increased Physical Activity

The alteration of the spatial layout of the playground was an irreversible process. Once the spatial layout had been changed, it was not possible to restore the original layout of the playground. Because of this, we could not have a control group who play in the original playground twice in addition to the children who played in the altered playground. Another limitation of this study was the small sample size, which means that it may be difficult to generalize the results of this study. For the future suggestion, the larger sample size from various developmental stages or multiple single-subjects study design will be necessary. Another limitation of the present study might be the short period between the alteration of the playground and the measurement of play. We might have assessed a novelty effect, even though a month had passed since the alteration. We cannot articulate the optimal interval, but we also cannot rule it out as the reason behind the increased physical activity.

Conclusion

The present study reported that young children’s mode of play in a playground could be altered by the spatial rearrangement of the play equipment. Young children may have perceived unique possibilities or constraints related to this alteration. Children’s mode of play is thus affected by the arrangement of the playground, and this should be explored further in intervention studies.

Data Availability Statement

All data are available on reasonable request from the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

MS and TN conceptualized and designed the study. MS acquired the data, performed the initial analysis, interpreted the data, drafted the initial manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. TN contributed to statistical analyses, interpreted the data, and critically revised the manuscript. Both authors agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work were appropriately integrated and resolved.

Funding

MS was supported by JSPS KAKENHI grant number JP19K02631. TN was supported by JSPS KAKENHI grant number JP18KT0079.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We give special thanks to all the participating teachers from the Momonokinomori certified center for early child education and care, in Osaka, Japan, for their precious contributions.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.datavyu.org

References

Adolph, K., Hoch, J. E., and Ossmy, O. (2019). “James Gibson’s ecological approach to locomotion and manipulation: development and changing affordances,” in Perception Information Detection: Reflections on Gibson’s Ecological Approach to Visual Perception, eds J. B. Wagman and J. J. Blau (New York, NY: Routledge).

Google Scholar

Adolph, K. E. (2016). Video as data: from transient behavior to tangible recording. APS Obs. 29, 23–25.

Google Scholar

Bakeman, R., and Quera, V. (2011). Sequential Analysis and Observational Methods for the Behavioral Sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Bohn-Goldbaum, E. E., Phongsavan, P., Merom, D., Rogers, K., Kamalesh, V., and Bauman, A. E. (2013). Does playground improvement increase physical activity among children? A quasi-experimental study of a natural experiment. J. Environ. Public Health 2013:109841. doi: 10.1155/2013/109841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bower, J. K., Derek, D. P., Tate, D. F., Rubin, D. A., Benjamin, S. E., and Ward, D. S. (2008). The childcare environment and children’s physical activity. Am. J. Prev. Med. 34, 23–29. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2007.09.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broekhuizen, K., Scholten, A. M., and Vries, S. I. (2014). The value of (pre)school playgrounds for children’s physical activity level: a systematic review. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 11:59. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-11-59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, W. H., Googe, H. S., Mclver, K. L., and Rathel, J. M. (2009). Effects of teacher-encouraged physical activity on preschool playgrounds. J. Early Interv. 31, 126–145.

Google Scholar

Burdette, H. L., and Whitaker, R. C. (2005). Resurrecting free play in young children: looking beyond fitness and fatness to attention, affiliation, and affect. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 158, 353–357. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.1.46

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burdette, H. L., Whitaker, R. C., and Danniels, S. R. (2004). Parental report of outdoor playtime as a measure of physical activity in preschool-aged children. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 158, 353–357. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.158.4.353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardon, G., Cauwenberghe, E. V., Labarque, V., Haerens, L., and Bourdeaudhuij, I. D. (2008). The contribution of preschool playground factors in explaining children’s physical activity during recess. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 26, 5–11. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-5-11

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardon, G., Labarque, V., Smits, D., and Bourdeaudhuij, I. D. (2009). Promoting physical activity at the pre-school playground: the effects of providing markings and play equipment. Prev. Med. 48, 335–340. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2009.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carson, V., Lee, E. Y., Hesketh, K. D., Hunter, S., Kuzik, N., Predy, M., et al. (2019). Physical activity and sedentary behavior across three time-points and associations with social skills in early childhood. BMC Public Health 19:27. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-6381-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, K. E., Morgan, P. J., Plontnikoff, R. C., Callister, R., and Lubans, D. (2014). Fundamental movement skills and physical activity among children living in low-income communities: a cross-sectional study. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 11:49.

Google Scholar

Danner, F., Noland, M., McFadden, M., Dewalt, K., and Kotchen, J. M. (1991). Description of the physical activity of young children using movement sensor and observation methods. Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 3, 11–20. doi: 10.1123/pes.3.1.11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delisle, C., Sandin, S., Forsum, E., Henriksson, H., Trolle-Lagerros, Y., Larsson, C., et al. (2015). A web- and mobile phone-based intervention to prevent obesity in 4-year-olds (MINISTOP): a popular-based randomized controlled trial. BMC Public Health 15:95. doi: 10.1186/s12889-015-1444-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dowda, M., Brown, W. H., Mclver, K. L., Pfeiffer, K. A., O’Neill, J. R., Addy, C. L., et al. (2009). Policies and characteristics of the preschool environment and physical activity of young children. Pediatrics 123:e261–e266. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dowdell, K., Gray, T., and Malone, K. A. (2011). Nature and its influence on children’s outdoor play. J. Outdoor. Environ. Educ. 15, 24–35. doi: 10.1007/BF03400925

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farley, T. A., Meriwether, R. A., Baker, E. T., Watkins, L. T., Johnson, C. C., and Webber, L. S. (2007). Safe play space to promote physical activity in inner-city children: results from a pilot study of an environmental intervention. Am. J. Public Health 97, 1625–1631. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.092692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, A., Reiley, J. J., Kelly, L. A., Montgomery, C., Williamson, A., Paton, J. Y., et al. (2005). Fundamental movement skills and habitual physical activity in young children. Med. Sci. Sports. Exerc. 37, 684–688.

Google Scholar

Fjørtoft, I., and Sageie, J. (2000). The natural environment as a playground for children landscape description and analyses of a natural playscape. Landsc. Urban Plan. 48, 83–97. doi: 10.1016/S0169-2046(00)00045-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, J. J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Ginsburg, K. R. (2007). The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent-child bonds. Pediatrics 119, 182–191.

Google Scholar

Gray, P. (2011). The decline of play and the rise of psychopathology in children and adolescent. Am. J. Play. 3, 443–463.

Google Scholar

Hannon, J. C., and Brown, B. B. (2008). Increasing preschoolers’ physical activity intensities: an activity-friendly preschool playground intervention. Prev. Med. 46, 532–536. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2008.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrington, S., and Brussoni, M. (2015). Beyond physical activity: the importance of play and nature-based play spaces for children’s health and development. Curr. Obes. Rep. 4, 477–483. doi: 10.1007/s13679-015-0179-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrington, S., and Lesmeister, C. (2006). The design of landscapes at child-care centers: seven cs. Landsc. Res. 31, 63–82. doi: 10.1080/01426390500448575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinkley, T., Crawford, D., Salmon, J., Okely, A. D., and Hesketh, K. (2008). Preschool children and physical activity: a review of correlates. Am. J. Prev. Med. 34, 435–441. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2008.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, R. M., Pellegrini, A. D., and Schmidt, S. L. (2006). The effects of different recess timing regimens on preschoolers’ classroom attention. Early Child Dev. Care 176, 735–743. doi: 10.1080/03004430500207179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leeger-Aschmann, C. S., Schmutz, E. A., Radtke, T., Kakebeeke, T. H., Zysset, A. E., Messerli-Burgy, N., et al. (2016). Regional sociocultural differences as important correlate of physical activity and sedentary behaviour in Swiss preschool children. Swiss Med. Wkly. 146:w14377. doi: 10.4414/smw.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liempd, I. H., Oudgenoeg-Paz, O., and Leseman, P. P. M. (2020). Do spatial characteristics influences behavior and development in early childhood education and care? J. Environ. Psychol. 67:101385. doi: 10.1016/j/jenvp.2019.101385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malone, K. (2007). The bubble-wrap generation: children growing up in walled gardens. Environ. Educ. Res. 13, 513–527.

Google Scholar

Mota, J., Silva, P., Santos, M. P., Ribeiro, J. C., Olivera, J., and Duarte, J. A. (2005). Physical activity and school recess time: differences between the sexes and the relationship between children’s playground physical activity and habitual physical activity. J. Sports. Sci. 23, 269–275. doi: 10.1080/02640410410001730124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oksanen, J., Blanchet, F. G., Friendly, M., Kindt, R., Legendre, P., McGlinn, D., et al. (2016). vegan: Community Ecology Package, Version 2.5.

Google Scholar

Pellegrini, A. D., Dupuis, D., and Smith, P. K. (2007). Play in evolution and development. Dev. Rev. 27, 261–276. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2006.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prieske, B., Withagen, R., Smith, J., and Zaal, T. J. M. (2015). Affordance in a simple playscape: are children attracted to challenging affordance? J. Environ. Psychol. 41, 101–111. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.11.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raustorp, A., Pagels, P., Boldmann, C., Cosco, N., Soderstrom, M., and Martensson, F. (2012). Accelerometer measured level of physical activity indoors and outdoors during preschool time in Sweden and the United States. J. Phys. Act. Health 9, 801–808. doi: 10.1123/jpah.9.6.801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridgers, N. D., and Stratton, G. (2005). Physical activity during school recess: the liverpool sporting playgrounds project. Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 17, 281–290. doi: 10.1123/pes.17.3.281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridgers, N. D., Stratton, G., Fairclough, S. J., and Twisk, J. W. R. (2007). Long-term effects of a playground markings and physical structures on children’s recess physical activity levels. Prev. Med. 44, 393–397. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2007.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sallis, J. F., Nader, P. R., Broyles, C. C., Berry, C. C., Elder, J. P., McKenzie, T. L., et al. (1993). Correlates of physical activity at home in Mexican-American and Anglo-American preschool children. Health Psychol. 12, 390–398. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.12.5.390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sando, O. J., and Sandseter, E. B. H. (2020). Affordances for physical activity and well-being in the ECEC outdoor environment. J. Environ. Psychol. 69:101430. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101430

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santos-Lozano, A., Torres-Luque, G., Marin, P. J., Ruiz, J. R., Lucia, A., and Garatachea, N. (2012). Intermonitor variability of GT3X accelerometer. Int. J. Sports Med. 33, 994–999. doi: 10.1055/s-0032-1312580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sporrel, K., Calijouw, S., and Whitagen, R. (2017). Children prefer a nonstandardized to a standardized jumping stone configuration: playing time and judgement. J. Environ. Psychol. 53, 131–137. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stratton, G., and Mullan, E. (2005). The effect of multicolor playground markings on children’s physical activity level during recess. Prev. Med. 41, 828–833. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2005.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trost, S. G., Ward, D. S., and Senso, M. (2010). Effects of child care policy and environment on physical activity. Med. Sci. Sports. Exerc. 42, 520–525. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181cea3ef

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Cauwenberghe, E., De Bourdeaughuij, I., Maes, L., and Cardon, G. (2012). Efficacy and feasibility of lowering playground density to promote physical activity and to discourage sedentary time during recess at preschool: a polot study. Prev. Med. > 55, 319–321. doi: 10.1026/j.ypmed.2012.07.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, H. G., Pfeiffer, K. A., O’Neill, J. R., Dowda, M., Mclever, K. L., Brown, W. H., et al. (2008). Motor skill performance and physical activity in preschool children. Obesity 16, 1421–1426. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, C., Quan, M., Zhang, H., Zhou, C., and Chen, P. (2018). Impact of parent’s physical activity on preschool children’s physical activity: a cross-sectional study. PeerJ 6:e4405.

Google Scholar

Keywords: playground, spatial layout, free play, young children, accelerometer, physical activity, play equipment, outdoor play

Citation: Sumiya M and Nonaka T (2021) Does the Spatial Layout of a Playground Affect the Play Activities in Young Children? A Pilot Study. Front. Psychol. 12:627052. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.627052

Received: 08 November 2020; Accepted: 14 April 2021;
Published: 28 May 2021.

Edited by:

Edward Edgerton, University of the West of Scotland, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Jane Jie Yu, Zhejiang University, China
Andrea Bosco, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Sumiya and Nonaka. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Masashi Sumiya, c3VtaXlhLW1Ac2Vpc2VuLmFjLmpw

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.