Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 21 July 2021
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Initial Validation of the Mindful Presence Scale: The Issue of the Construal Level of Scale Items

\nAttila Lengyel
Attila Lengyel1*Danica KeczeliDanica Keczeli2Rbert OroszRóbert Orosz3Zoltn BcsZoltán Bács4Anetta MüllerAnetta Müller2Szilvia Sz&#x;keSzilvia Szőke5va Bcsn BbaÉva Bácsné Bába2
  • 1Department of Tourism Management and Catering, Institute of Rural Development Tourism and Sports Management, Faculty of Economics and Business, University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary
  • 2Department of Sport Economics and Management, Institute of Rural Development, Tourism and Sports Management, Faculty of Economics and Business, University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary
  • 3Institute of Psychology, Faculty of Humanities, University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary
  • 4Institute of Accounting and Finance, Faculty of Economics and Business, University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary
  • 5Department of Research Methodology and Statistics, Institute of Sectorial Economics and Methodology, Faculty of Economics and Business, University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary

Our research has two main aims. It undertakes the validation of a six-item trait mindfulness scale called Mindful Presence Scale (MPS), which measures central aspects of mindfulness. For the first time in mindfulness literature, the construal level of scale items is also examined. Four questionnaire-based online studies were conducted. Study 1 drew three samples (nn = 391, np = 215, and nb = 235) from the students at the University of Debrecen. It examined the factor structure, reliability, and internal consistency of the three differently worded scale versions. The positively worded scale version (MPSp) yielded a stable two-factor structure and demonstrated the best psychometric properties. Study 2 performed a confirmatory factor analysis on a sample drawn from public employees across the country (ncfa = 420). The two-factor solution in Study 1 was confirmed. χ2 tests were not significant, and fit indices were excellent. There was no significant difference between the high-level construal subscale (Fhlc) and the low-level construal subscale (Fllc) in terms of factorial stability. Participant of Study 3 were students who did not take part in Study 1. The sample (ninv = 250) was tested for measurement invariance across gender. The scaled results supported strong/threshold invariance. Study 4 tested concurrent validity with 10 concurrent instruments. A sample of secondary school teachers (ncon = 128) was tested by examining Spearman's rank order correlations. There was a significant difference in how the Fhlc and Fllc subscales predicted scores of some of the concurrent instruments. Further research is warranted into how the construal level of mindfulness scale items affects the recollection of the mindful experience. Overall, MPSp proved to be a valid short mindfulness measure.

Introduction

Mindfulness is a multifaceted construct with many contested definitions that can demonstrate both considerable overlaps and contradictions (Van Dam et al., 2018; Anālayo, 2019). Instead of embracing any of the definitions of the past three decades, the present study attempts to identify a construct which is proposed to constitute the basis for the evolution of all other facets of mindfulness. Based on this core conceptualization of mindfulness, a short self-report instrument is developed as the operationalization of the construct and an initial validation procedure of the scale is carried out. Although numerous self-report measures of trait mindfulness have been developed and validated (Baer et al., 2004, 2008; Walach et al., 2006; Feldman et al., 2007; Cardaciotto et al., 2008; Chadwick et al., 2008; Davis et al., 2009; Bergomi et al., 2013; Frank et al., 2016; Pratscher et al., 2019) since the Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale (hereafter MAAS) was first tested (Brown and Ryan, 2003), apart from the five-item MAAS (Van Dam et al., 2010; Osman et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2017) and six-item MAAS (Black et al., 2012) no other really short (under 10 items) trait mindfulness scale has been validated before. As a second goal, our study attempts to investigate whether the construal level of the scale items has any significant effect on the recollection of mindful episodes by respondents of the self-report questionnaire.

Mindfulness is a metacognition-based dynamic process involving the ability to intentionally regulate and control attention (Wells, 2000, 2002; Teasdale et al., 2002; Sugiura, 2004; Norman, 2017; Bernstein et al., 2019). This dynamism also entails, with the exclusion of very experienced meditators, a constant fluctuation between mindful and mindless states. Based on this fact, the question arises: How does mindfulness begin? According to Bishop et al., “Mindfulness begins by bringing awareness to current experience…” (Bishop et al., 2004, p. 232). In Bishop et al.'s (2004) operational definition, mindfulness is a two-component hierarchical process where the first step is a samatha-type attention control employed to bring back and anchor attention in the present moment. The special importance of being able to bring attention back to the present moment and anchor it there has been supported by theoretical arguments and evidenced by numerous empirical studies (Baer et al., 2008; Dreyfus, 2011; Gethin, 2011; Christopher et al., 2012; Lilja et al., 2013; Siegling and Petrides, 2016; Watson-Singleton et al., 2018). Brown and Ryan states, “This present-centered attention–awareness is, in our view, foundational to mindfulness…” (Brown and Ryan, 2003, p. 824). Cardaciotto et al. (2008) consider awareness a “central component” of mindfulness (Cardaciotto et al., 2008, p. 220). Based on what has been said so far, we propose that scale items that measure the ability to bring attention to and anchor it in the present moment and direct it intentionally to anything that arises in the space of consciousness, should be categorized under the term Mindful Presence (MP). Once MP is established and maintained, it can serve as a springboard to the second step in the mindfulness meditation process, which involves the open, curious, non-reacting, non-judgmental witnessing of anything that is arising and unfolding (vipassana-type meditation) in the space of consciousness, that is, experiencing non-identification with the sensual information, thoughts, feelings, images, or desires. Without stabilizing attention and establishing an MP, no insight is possible. You cannot let things go without even becoming aware that you are holding on to them. Bishop et al. (2004) emphasize that “skills” such as being able to let things arise (openness, curiosity, and non-judgment) and letting them pass (non-reaction) as well as the ability to exercise virtues such as compassion, wisdom, or moderation, to mention a few considered necessary in Buddhist practice, are “correlates” or “benefits” of the MP established by anchoring attention in the present moment.

Trait mindfulness instruments developed so far aim to tap into various dimensions or facets of mindfulness, including observing, non-judging, non-reacting, acceptance, acting with awareness, describing, attention, present focus, and awareness. Dimensions such as observing, acting with awareness, attention, present focus, and awareness overlap with MP. Instead of using various terms for these overlapping facets, we suggest that their items are better categorized according to their level of abstraction or construal level (Liberman and Trope, 1998; Trope and Liberman, 2010). All the items in Table 1, whether worded in a general (abstract) or a concrete (action-specific) way, tap into the construct of MP.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Relevant sample items from scales used in the validation process.

While all items in Table 1 can be considered operationalizations of MP, they represent different levels of construal. For example, PHLMS Awareness subscale item 5 is considered to be low-level construal since it describes the concrete daily activity of having a shower. In comparison, CAMS-R Attention subscale item 11 is a statement about a general ability; thus, it is categorized as a high-level construal item. It has to be noted that construal level is a continuum from the local (specific, concrete) to the global (abstract, general) with no clear-cut boundaries (Alter and Oppenheimer, 2008). Hence, the terms “low-level construal” and “high-level construal” in the present paper are relative terms and in fact denote higher and lower levels of construal.

The level of construal might affect psychological constructs (Vess et al., 2011; Gong and Medin, 2012; Mantzios and Wilson, 2014), and mindfulness may also have an effect on the level of construal (Heeren et al., 2009; Chan and Wang, 2019). The question of how the construal level of self-report items might affect the recollection of mindful moments by respondents is practically unresearched to date. High-level and low-level construal items of the MP construct might influence how mindful behavior is recollected by respondents. The conceptual framework of investigating the possible effects of construal level of scale items on recollection of mindful episodes is shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Hypothesized effect of construal level on the recollection of mindful experience.

We hypothesized that the high- and low-level construal items would perform differently in the statistical procedures. We also expected that because both high- and low-level construal items are operationalizations of MP as a single construct, there would be signs of unidimensionality.

Materials and Methods

Item Generation and Development of the Measuring Instrument

This initial phase consisted of four steps:

1. Item generation. Deductive approach was used to create an initial pool of 58 MP-related items from existing mindfulness scales and MP-related items created by the authors based on the operational definition of MP given in the Introduction. All items were categorized into either the high-level or the low-level construal group depending on whether they described a specific action (e.g., having a shower) or a general skill (e.g., ability to focus on what one is doing).

2. Development of the measurement instrument. Content validity of scale items was determined by an expert panel of five judges, two psychiatrists using mindfulness-based therapies in their clinical practice, and three mindfulness meditation experts. The two psychiatrists are certified mindfulness trainers who have been running Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT) groups for over 5 years. They both graduated from a mindfulness trainer course certified by the Oxford Mindfulness Center. Two of the meditation experts are senior leaders of Buddhist meditation communities with decades of meditation experience in both vipassana- and samatha-type techniques. The third expert is a doctor and certified Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) trainer running courses mainly for people struggling with addiction problems.

Similar to the procedure used by Cardaciotto et al. (2008), judges rated high-level and low-level construal items separately on a 5-point Likert-type rating scale (1= very poor, 2 = poor, 3 = fair, 4 = good, and 5 = very good) based on how well, in their opinion, the item measured MP. Interclass correlation (ICC) coefficient was calculated using SPSS version 25. A high degree of interrater agreement was found: average measure = 0.842, lower bound = 0,801, upper bound = 0.877 with a 95% confidence interval [F(199,398) = 6.349, p < 0.001]. Based on these results, three items from both pools with the highest mean ratings were chosen to form the basis of the Mindful Presence Scale (MPS). Five of the six items in MPS are differently worded variations of the already-existing mindfulness scale items. Although all items are operationalizations of the theoretical construct of MP described in the Introduction, we hypothesized that the construal level of the items is an aspect that will manifest in the factor analytical procedure. The scale consists of six items because we hypothesized that the high- and low-level construal scale items would gravitate toward two hidden variables (factors) and the minimum acceptable number of indicators per factor for relatively small samples with adequately high factor loadings is three (Wolf et al., 2013; Koran, 2020).

3. Handling open methodological issues. Some open methodological issues were dealt with in this phase. Ambiguity of scale items may seriously affect the validity (Carmody et al., 2009; Park et al., 2013). Many of the scale items of the frequently used mindfulness scales are abstract without any context. As an example, MAAS item 11 is a general statement “I find myself listening to someone with one ear, doing something else at the same time.” We can interpret this statement in two typical everyday contexts, leisure, and work. The distinction seems rather important as the level of agreement or disagreement with the item might change radically. The questionnaire used in this study avoids this pitfall by giving clear instructions to the respondents to think about the scale items imagining that: they are at home, alone, spending their free time.

The other open issue to be resolved was the question of positively and negatively worded items. There are authors who argue for negatively worded items (Brown and Ryan, 2003), or positively worded items (Hartley, 2014), or some mixture of them (Baer et al., 2008). The presently used mindfulness scales show considerable differences in their approach to item wording. The authors decided on a strategy to initially screen all the three above-discussed wording approaches in connection with the MPS items using factor analytic and reliability checking procedures and move on to the validation process with the scale version that has shown the best psychometric properties. Similar to MAAS, a six-point rating scale was used in the questionnaire with scale points ranging from 1 (almost never) to 6 (almost always).

4. Pilot test to check face validity. A sample of convenience (n1 = 176) from Facebook was used to test the face validity of the positively and negatively worded scale items. Of the sample, 72.6% had college or university degrees or both. Based on the feedback received from the respondents, the wording of test items was modified and finalized.

Overview of Validation Procedure

The validation process consists of four studies involving samples of various base populations. In Study 1, samples were drawn from among undergraduate students at the University of Debrecen via convenience sampling. Study 1 used three samples (n1 = 391 negatively worded version, n2 = 215 positively worded version, and n3 = 235 balanced version). The three sets of data were tested with exploratory factor analysis (EFA) as well as reliability and internal consistency indices. In Study 2, a sample was drawn from a pool of local government employees across the country (n4 = 420) to validate the findings of Study 1 through confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). In Study 3, a sample of students who have not taken part in Study 1 (n5 = 250) studying at the University of Debrecen was tested to examine measurement invariance across gender. In Study 4, the MPS version with the best psychometric properties was included in the validation questionnaire with 10 concurrent instruments. This validation questionnaire was sent out to teachers of secondary schools. The sample (n6 = 128) served as a basis for testing concurrent validity. Figure 2 shows the study flow.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Study flow.

As EFA and CFA can be viewed as two ends of a continuum in terms of imposing or not imposing restrictions on the solutions, they are rather complimentary than mutually exclusive (Hoyle, 2000); thus, both methods are applied in Study 1 and Study 2, respectively. In order to support construct validity, measurement invariance (special application of CFA) is tested in Study 3. Finally in Study 4, convergent and discriminant validity of the scale is tested using concurrent instruments.

The data were collected via self-reported online questionnaires that contained the scale items and questions about demographic information. Before completing the questionnaire, participants were informed about the goal of the study, the anonymity of the questionnaire, and the ethical approval that had been obtained from the Committee of Scientific Ethics at the University of Debrecen (GTKDH/52/2021:15.13.). They were informed that data management was in accordance with European Union directives and Hungarian laws regulating the handling of personal data. Before they were able to proceed to completion, they were asked to give their informed consent that the data they provided could be used for scientific analysis and the results published in scientific journals. For the statistical analysis, both commercial (SPSS version 25) and noncommercial software (R statistical packages and FACTOR software) were used.

Study 1: Factor Structure, Reliability, and Internal Consistency of the Three Scale Versions

Participants

Undergraduate students studying at the University of Debrecen were recruited to complete the online self-reported questionnaires containing the scales MPSp, MPSn, and MPSb. For each scale version, 1,000 e-mails containing the link to the questionnaire and the brief introduction of the research were sent out. Out of the 3,000 questionnaires, 902 were sent back. Forty-two were excluded from the analysis as they were either empty or missing too much information. Sixteen respondents marked the “I do not approve” option for the future scientific use of the data and three questionnaires were judged to be inadequate as the respondents marked the same scale point all through the questionnaire. As the samples were drawn from the same base population, they (n1 = 391, n2 = 215, n3 = 235) showed very similar demographic characteristics concerning age, gender, and training program (2-year vocational, bachelor, masters). In n1, the mean age for respondents was 21.74 years (SD = 10.84), and the proportion of women was 54.80%. In n1, the three training programs were represented as follows: 2-year vocational = 34.3%, bachelor = 41.7%, and masters = 24%. In n2, the mean age for respondents was 20.81 years (SD = 4.85), and the proportion of women was 60.10%. In n2, the three training programs were represented as follows: 2-year vocational = 27.3%, bachelor = 49.2%, and masters = 23.5%. In n3, the mean age for respondents was 21.08 years (SD = 6.61), and the proportion of women was 67.8%. In n3, the three training programs were represented as follows: 2-year vocational = 37.9%, bachelor = 38.2%, and masters = 21.9%. The sample sizes were all above the typically recommended minimum of n = 200 in the EFA literature (Fabrigar and Wegener, 2011; Jung and Lee, 2011), and the subject-to-variable ratios were well above the conservative recommendation of 20:1 (Osborne et al., 2014) even in the smallest (n2) sample (36:1).

Data Analysis

For factor extraction, robust diagonally weighted least squares (RDWLS) (Mîndrila, 2010) was employed as there are only three indicators per factor, and a relatively small sample size (Forero et al., 2009). Dimensionality tests were run using FACTOR software, random.polychor.pa R package (Jordan et al., 2019), nFactors R package (Raiche, 2010), psych R package (Revelle, 2016), and RgenData R package (Ruscio and Roche, 2012). As the factors were expected to be correlated, oblique rotation method (direct oblimin) was employed (Verhaeghen and Aikman, 2020). For the minimum percentage of variance accounted for by latent variables, a 60% cutoff value was used (Hair et al., 2009). Special EFA indices were computed to provide further information about various aspects of the EFA solutions (Bentler, 1977; Lorenzo-Seva, 2003; Finch et al., 2008; Rodriguez et al., 2016; Bado et al., 2018; Ferrando and Lorenzo-Seva, 2018).

Alpha's excessive use in psychometric literature has been criticized by many studies (Morera and Stokes, 2016; McNeish, 2018). There are several alternatives to alpha recommended by the literature such as GLB and Omega (Sijtsma, 2009; Watkins, 2017), ordinal alpha (Basto and Pereira, 2012; Morera and Stokes, 2016), or coefficient H (McNeish, 2018). The authors adopted an approach of reporting all above-mentioned indicators of consistency and reliability.

Results and Discussion

As the data proved to be non-normal by both the Kolmogorov–Smirnov and the Shapiro–Wilk tests (p > 0.001), the polychoric correlation matrices were computed for the three scale versions in FACTOR software using Bayesian estimation. Factorability tests based on the polychoric matrices resulted: MPSp (determinant of the matrix = 0.09; Bartlett's statistic = 499.90, df =15; p = 0.00; Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin test = 0.74), MPSn (determinant of the matrix = 0.49; Bartlett's statistic = 272.80, df = 15; p = 0.00; Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin test = 0.72), MPSb (determinant of the matrix = 0.01; Bartlett's statistic = 1,081.70, df = 66; p = 0.00; Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin test = 0.79). For comparison, most tests were run on both the Pearson and the polychoric matrices. For MPSp and MPSn, there is a strong agreement between the employed dimension testing methods suggesting two dimensions. For MPSb, most methods suggested four dimensions. In the subsequent EFA, only these factor models were tested. Factor analysis was done in FACTOR software. Table 2 shows the pattern matrices of the oblique solution for the three scale versions with factor loadings and cross-loadings ≥0.30.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Result of EFA.

Fhlc denotes the factor with high-level construal scale items loading onto it, and Fllc denotes the factor with low-level construal scale items loading onto it. What is immediately apparent from Table 2 is that all three scale versions, with the exception of the first high-level construal scale item of the negatively worded subscale of MPSb, demonstrate a clear factor structure. In the case of MPSp and the MPSp part of MPSb, both the high-level and low-level construal variables load strongly on the two factors. The MPSn and the MPSn part of MPSb yielded weaker factor loadings. Table 3 contains indices that provide further information about various aspects of the EFA solutions. At first glance, it seems obvious that the MPSp two-factor solution and the MPSp part of MPSb come closest to the cutoff criteria, while MPSn and the MPSn part of MPSb do rather poorly.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Indices to assess the quality of EFA factor solutions.

It must be noted that, in spite of the two-factor structure, based on the indices measuring the scale's closeness to unidimensionality (UniCo, ECV, MIREAL), MPSp seems to demonstrate that there is a common underlying factor. Its unidimensional character is supported by fact that the first eigenvalue accounts for 53.15% of the 72.47% total explained variance. Factor scores of the two MPSp factors are strongly correlated with the MPSp composite scores (Fhlc r = 0.81, p < 0.05, Fllc r = 0.83, p < 0.05) and the interfactor correlation is of medium strength (r = 0.46, p < 0.05). It has to be noted that in the subsequent studies, the interfactor correlation between the high-level and low-level construal factors ranged from 0.43 to 0.62, p < 0.05.

Although there seems to be a difference between the high-level construal (Fhlc) and the low-level construal (Fllc) variables according to data in Table 3, in the other three samples examined in subsequent studies (n4, n5, n6), there was no significant difference in EFA indices of the two subscales.

Table 4 shows the scale reliability and internal consistency values. In the case of a multidimensional model sum scores for the total scale are not computed only subscale scores.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Reliabilities of the three scale versions.

Scale reliability values depicted in Table 4 seem to follow a consistent pattern. In the case of MPSp, all reliability indices are higher for the high-level construal subscale (Fhlc). However, in MPSn and MPSb, the exact opposite holds true as values for the low-level construal subscale (Fllc) are consistently higher than values for the high-level construal subscale (Fhlc). In subsequent studies, although to a lesser extent, the same pattern emerged. In n6 and n4, however, in n5 it was the low-level construal subscale that demonstrated significantly better reliability values.

Based on the EFA loading values, EFA indices, and the reliability and consistency values, MPSp was chosen to be used in the next phase of validation. The scale can be viewed in the Appendix section.

Study 2

Participants

In this study, the positively worded scale version demonstrating the best psychometric properties in Study 1 was tested on a sample of local government employees. The questionnaires were sent out to 2,650 recipients across the country. Four hundred and forty-one questionnaires were returned. Of them, nine were empty and 12 had so much missing data that they were excluded from statistical analysis. In the remaining sample (n4 = 420), the mean age for respondents was 45.74 years (SD = 10.84) and the proportion of women was 54.88 %. In n4, the educational level was represented as follows: PhD or higher = 0.03%, bachelor = 40.5%, master = 38.2%, and other = 21.3%. As the loading values in Study 1 were homogeneous, the loading average was above 0.6, and there are three variables per factor, the sample size is perfectly sufficient for a CFA. The minimally acceptable sample size in this scenario is n ≥ 400 (Gagne and Hancock, 2006).

Data Analysis

As the EFA revealed a strong tendency toward unidimensionality in MPSp, the CFA model was fitted to both the two- and the one-factor solution. Tests were run in FACTOR using both MLR (robust maximum likelihood) and robust RDWLS to test model fit (Mîndrila, 2010; Finney and DiStefano, 2013; Li, 2016a,b). Cutoff values were based on conservative recommendations (Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003; Brown, 2014). Significant χ2 test results have been ignored in many psychometric studies (Heene et al., 2011). However, Ropovik (2015) rightly points out that the χ2 test is the only statistical test of model-data fit. Since in our study the sample size is not big, the model is simple, and the factor loadings are adequately high, the χ2 test results provide valuable information. χ2 test takes on even bigger significance if we consider that earlier studies found that fit indices such as TLI, CFI, or RMSEA might not be trusted with small sample sizes (Ainur et al., 2017). Ts-RMSEA and Ts-CFI are values gained through equivalence testing. This method typically avoids Type I and Type II errors that commonly occur during conventional hypothesis testing (Yuan et al., 2016). Normed/relative χ22/df) values below 2 are accepted as a conservative cutoff (Bigras et al., 2017).

Results and Discussion

Table 5 shows the fit indices for the one- and two-factor model of MPSp.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. CFA fit indices.

None of the fit indices for the one-factor solution meet generally adopted cutoff criteria. Normed/relative χ22/df) is not part of Table 5, but it is apparent that for the two-factor model all relative χ2 values fall below 2. It can be stated that the two-factor model of MPSp suggested by Study 1 has been validated by the CFA in Study 2. Figure 3 shows the CFA model for MPSp with factor loadings and interfactor correlation.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. CFA model for MPSp.

Study 3: Measurement Invariance Across Gender

Participants

In Study 3, students who have not taken part in Study 1 studying at the University of Debrecen were tested to examine measurement invariance across gender. The potential respondents received the link to the online questionnaire in an e-mail. Seven hundred and sixty-seven e-mails were sent out, and 271 students completed the questionnaire. Eight questionnaires were empty, six questionnaires did not contain information for gender, and seven questionnaires contained so much missing data that they were excluded from the analysis. In n5, the mean age for respondents was 20.92 years (SD = 4.74), and the proportion of women was 61.6%. There was no significant difference in demographics between those who did not send back the questionnaires and those who were included in the study. In n5, the three training programs were represented as follows: 2-year vocational = 31.2%, bachelor = 39.6%, and masters = 29.2%. AFIs (approximate fit indices) such as CFI are not very sensitive to sample size. χ2 tends to over-reject models as sample size increases; thus, n5 = 250 can be considered to be suitable for the measurement invariance analysis (Putnick and Bornstein, 2016).

Data Analysis

Data analysis was carried out using the Lavaan R package. The Lavaan package has been used extensively by numerous psychometric validation studies before (Liu et al., 2017; Sinval et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2019). WLSMV and MLR estimators are suggested for measurement invariance tests on ordered polytomous data, with WLSMV being a better estimator of overall model fit and MLR a better estimator of change in fit (Sass et al., 2014; Li, 2016b; Koziol and Bovaird, 2018). The MPSp scale and its two subscales Fhlc and Fllc were tested for invariance across gender. The four steps of testing measurement invariance with MLR, treating variables as continuous, are configural, weak, strong, and strict. With WLSMV, treating variables as ordered categorical, the four models are termed baseline, loading, threshold, and unique factor models, respectively (Liu et al., 2017). It has to be noted that in psychometric testing, strict/unique invariance is rarely reached or needed (Eriksson and Boman, 2018). Although values of RMSEA and SRMR were reported, CFI is considered to be the most reliable change-in-fit indicator (Cheung and Rensvold, 2002); thus besides the chi-square tests (Satorra and Bentler, 2001), it was used as the main determinant of invariance (Meade et al., 2008). Cutoff values suggested by Schermelleh-Engel et al. (2003) were used. The nested models were tested using the “measurementInvariance” function of the “semTools package” in R statistical software (Jorgensen et al., 2019).

Results and Discussion

Fit indices of the configural/baseline model were good. Evaluation was based on cutoff values (CFI ≥ 0.97, TLI ≥ 0.97, RMSEA ≤ 0.05, SRMR ≤ 0.05). The results with the MLR estimator were CFI = 1.00, TLI = 1.00, RMSEA = 0.00, and SRMR = 0.02 and with the WLSMV estimator CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.02, and SRMR = 0.03.

Table 6 shows the results of the scaled chi-square difference test. Evaluation was based on ΔCFI cutoff values (ΔCFI ≤ 0.01). For the nested models with the MLR estimator, CFI values ranged 0.99–1, ΔCFI values −0.002–0.000, with the WLSMV estimator CFI values ranged 0.97–0.99, ΔCFI values −0.01–−0.02. With the MLR estimator, RMSEA values ranged 0.000–0.026 and with the WLSMV estimator 0.023–0.055. SRMR values with the MLR estimator were 0.037–0.067, while with the WLSMV estimator, they ranged 0.032–0.057. According to the cited cutoff recommendations and ΔCFI values by the WLSMV estimator, strict/unique factor invariance does not hold. Based on values by the MLR estimator, however, strict/unique invariance seems to hold.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Scaled chi-square difference test of MPSp (method = “satorra.bentler.2001”).

Table 7 shows the results of the scaled chi-square difference test for MPSp subscales. Evaluation was based on ΔCFI cutoff values (ΔCFI ≤ 0.01).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Scaled chi-square difference test of Fhlc and Fllc subscales (method = “satorra.bentler.2001”).

Testing the Fhlc with the MLR estimator, CFI was 1 at all levels of the nested model, while with the WLSMV estimator, CFI values ranged 0.96–1.00, ΔCFI values ranged −0.035–0.000. With the MLR estimator, RMSEA values ranged 0.00–0.03, and with the WLSMV estimator, RMSEA values were 0.00 at all levels of the nested model. SRMR values with the WLSMV estimator ranged 0.00–0.05, and with the MLR estimator 0.00–0.06. Based on ΔCFI values by the WLSMV estimator, strict/unique factor invariance does not hold. Based on the values by the MLR estimator, however, strict/unique invariance seems to hold.

In the case of the Fllc, the MLR estimator CFI values ranged 0.96–1.00 and with WLSMV 0.98–1.00. ΔCFI values for the MLR estimator were −0.003–−0.010 and for the WLSMV estimator −0.006–0.000. RMSEA values with the MLR estimator ranged 0.00–0.07, while with the WLSMV estimator 0.00–0.07. SRMR values with the MLR estimator ranged 0.00–0.06 and with the WLSMV estimator 0.00–0.05. Based on ΔCFI values by both WLSMV and MLR estimators, neither strong nor strict invariance holds.

The high-level construal subscale (Fhlc) and the low-level construal subscale (Fllc) did not demonstrate significant differences in preserving measurement invariance across gender.

Study 4: Concurrent Validity

Participants

In Study 4, MPSp was included in the validation questionnaire with 10 concurrent instruments. This validation questionnaire was sent out to 624 principals and teachers of secondary schools. One hundred and sixty-one questionnaires were returned in the 2 weeks the survey was open. Fifteen questionnaires contained no information at all, 12 were excluded because the respondents used the same scale points throughout the test, and six had no demographic information and a lot of missing answers. The statistical analysis was carried out on the remaining sample (n6 = 128). In nv, the mean age for respondents was 48.64 years (SD = 13.18), and the proportion of women was 89.80%. In n6, the educational level was represented as follows: PhD or higher = 1.2%, bachelor = 2.5%, master = 94.1%, and other = 2.2%. As the sample is relatively small, the 80% statistical power (two-tailed, α ≤ 0.05) of the Spearman's correlation is valid for correlation values rs ≥ 0.25 (Bonett and Wright, 2000).

Data Analysis

Internal consistency and reliability of the concurrent instruments were checked to obtain a comparison with the findings of the original validation studies. For scales demonstrating a unidimensional character, composite reliability was computed. For scales not showing unidimensionality, subscale reliabilities are presented. The “laavan,” “userfreindlyscience,” and “effsize” R packages, FACTOR and SPSS version 25, were used to carry out the statistical analyses. As all instruments contain ordered categorical variables, scale scores were compared using Spearman's correlations (Yu et al., 2018). Correlations were interpreted using Cohen's (1992) recommendations (small = 0.10, medium = 0.30, and large = 0.50). Results about the relevant facets of mindfulness in earlier concurrent validity tests were considered to evaluate the validity of MPSp (Baer et al., 2004, 2008; Lilja et al., 2011; Christopher et al., 2012; Desrosiers et al., 2013; Tejedor et al., 2014; Aguado et al., 2015; Medvedev et al., 2018; Schutte and Malouff, 2018; Carpenter et al., 2019; Mattes, 2019; Sadowski et al., 2020).

Instruments used in the study: A six-item Nature Relatedness Scale (NR-6) is a brief measure of nature relatedness developed by Nisbet and Zelenski (2013). In our study, its reliability was good (ordinal Cronbach's alpha = 0.89), and the unidimensionality was confirmed (UniCo = 0.98, ECV = 0.87, MIREAL = 0.25).

Questionnaire for Eudaimonic Well-Being (QEWB) is a 21-item scale developed by Waterman et al. (2010) to measure well-being as interpreted by eudemonist philosophy, and the scale was found to be unidimensional. As in the original validation, the instrument showed an excellent reliability (ordinal Cronbach's alpha = 0.95), but did not prove to be unidimensional (UniCo = 0.88, ECV = 0.76, MIREAL = 0.27).

MAAS 10-item shortened scale was created by Chiesi et al. (2017) by omitting badly performing items of the original MAAS. The IRT (Item Response Theory) analysis revealed that the ten-item version had good psychometric properties. In our study, its reliability was good (ordinal Cronbach's alpha = 0.89), and the unidimensionality was confirmed (UniCo = 0.97, ECV = 0.88, MIREAL = 0.21).

Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI) 14-item shortened form is a scale developed by Walach et al. (2006) from the 30-item Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI). It showed good psychometric properties and was tentatively termed as unidimensional. In our study, the scale reliability was good (ordinal Cronbach's alpha = 0.87) and unidimensionality was confirmed (UniCo = 0.94, ECV = 0.80, MIREAL = 0.28).

CAMS-R 10-item shortened version was developed by Feldman et al. (2007). In the validation, one higher-order factor and four first-order factors were revealed. In our study, it showed good reliability (ordinal Cronbach's alpha = 0.86) and its deeper level unidimensional character was also confirmed (UniCo = 0.93, ECV = 0.81, MIREAL = 0.27).

FFMQ 15-item shortened version is a short version of the 36-item FFMQ developed by Baer et al. (2008). In our study, no unidimensional character was demonstrated (UniCo = 0.67, ECV = 0.60, MIREAL = 0.41). Subscale reliabilities as measured by ordinal Cronbach's alpha were acceptable (Observe = 0.72; Act.aware = 0.81; Non-react = 0.64; Non-judge = 0.75; Describe = 0.81).

PHLMS is a 20-item bidimensional scale created by Cardaciotto et al. (2008). In the present study, the reliability of both the Awareness (ordinal Cronbach's alpha = 0.87) and the Acceptance (ordinal Cronbach's alpha = 0.88) subscales was good. The instrument was not found to be unidimensional (UniCo = 0.74, ECV = 0.55, MIREAL = 0.46).

Social Connectedness Scale (SCS) is one of the two subscales of the Social Belongingness Scale developed by Lee and Robbins (1995). This eight-item measure demonstrated an excellent reliability (ordinal Cronbach's alpha = 0.95), and unidimensionality was confirmed (UniCo = 0.99, ECV = 0.95, MIREAL = 0.18).

Resistance to Change Scale (RCS) is an 18-item instrument created by Oreg (2003). In our study, the scale reliability was excellent (ordinal Cronbach's alpha = 0.92), and unidimensionality was confirmed (UniCo = 0.92, ECV = 0.87, MIREAL = 0.24).

Hospital Anxiety and Depression (HAD) Scale is a 14-item instrument that was originally developed for clinical populations (Zigmond and Snaith, 1983), but later tested successfully on general populations as well (Mykletun et al., 2001; Djukanovic et al., 2017). In our study, it showed an excellent reliability (ordinal Cronbach's alpha = 0.91) and was found to be unidimensional (UniCo = 0.94, ECV = 0.84, MIREAL = 0.25).

The NR-6, the EWBQ, and the RCS were included in the concurrent study with further research objectives in mind. With the escalating global socioecological crisis, certain skills and values such as the attitude to nature, eudemonic values, or the ability to change will become crucially important. In recent years, mindfulness as a possible catalyst of the transition to a sustainable lifestyle has been receiving increasing attention from researchers.

Results and Discussion

Table 8 contains concurrent correlations between MPSp and the seven concurrent instruments.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Concurrent correlations.

In the case of QEWB, FMI, SCS, and RCS, no subscale scores were calculated as they were found to be unidimensional both in the original study and in ours. Correlations range from small to large with most values being in the small and medium range. The Fllc of MPSp had a medium correlation with the composite scores of FFMQ, and large correlations with the Observing facet of FFMQ and the Awareness subscale of PHLMS. The strongest correlations of the Fhlc subscale of MPSp were in the medium range with FMI and FFMQ composite scores and the Observing facet of FFMQ. MPSp subscale and composite scores had a small correlation with MAAS. Acting with awareness is moderately correlated with MPSp and both of its subscales. Interestingly, the Fllc of MPSp, consisting of items closely corresponding to FFMQ Observing variables, is weakly but significantly negatively correlated with both anxiety and depression variables of HADS. The Non-judging FFMQ facet is uncorrelated with Fhlc consisting of Acting with awareness type of variables, while in previous studies, they are typically positively correlated. The Describe facet of FFMQ in our study is uncorrelated with either subscales of MPSp.

The positive correlation between MPSp composite scores and NR-6 supports the earlier literature; however, the correlation is higher than what is reported in earlier studies (r between 0.17 and 0.25). The strong correlation of the Fllc subscale with NR-6 seems to be in line with recent findings of similar studies where Observing-type items of existing scales were used.

There is no earlier data in the literature about the relationship between social connectedness and Observing-type (low-level construal) and Acting with awareness-type (high-level construal) mindfulness scale items. In our study, except for the weak correlation of the short-term focus subscale of RCS with the Fllc subscale, these constructs were found to be uncorrelated with MPSp subscales.

General Discussion

The primary goal of this study was to carry out the initial validation of a six-item trait mindfulness measure consisting of three high-level and three low-level construal scale items. While the five-item MAAS and six-item MAAS are valid short instruments to measure a dimension of trait mindfulness, all of their items are general (high-level construal) statements about certain MP skills in general. As a unique contribution to existing mindfulness measurement methodology, our research used an equal number of high-level construal (not action-specific) and low-level construal (action-specific) items to test whether the construal level of scale items significantly affects respondent's recollection of mindful moments and thus the psychometric properties of the scale.

The MPSp demonstrated good psychometric properties with above 0.6 homogeneously distributed factor loadings across the two construal-level subscales. As a contribution to existing statistical methodology in mindfulness research, our study used various EFA indices (Ferrando and Lorenzo-Seva, 2018) relatively new to mindfulness literature. The stable factor structure found in Study 1 was confirmed in the CFA tests in Study 2 and the measurement invariance tests of Study 3. Although, based on the EFA indices, the high-level construal subscale performed better in Study 1, this difference was not consistent across the other studies and samples; thus, statistical point of view nothing conclusive can be stated about how the two different construal-level subscales behave. Including concrete, action-specific items in the scale proved to be an incremental value over the short MAAS scale versions as there is a marked difference in how high- and low-level construal items relate to certain concurrent instruments. In Study 4, correlations with well-known mindfulness instruments are of the magnitude and in the direction that had been expected. The strong correlation of the low-level construal subscale of MPSp (Fllc) with the Observing subscale of FFMQ and the Awareness subscale of PHLMS can be attributed to the fact that items of these subscales are very similar.

Concurrent correlations showed a consistent pattern with the low-level construal subscale (Fllc) being the stronger predictor of the concurrent instruments except for the present focus subscale of CAMS-R where the high-level subscale (Fhlc) had a slightly stronger correlation. The difference between the two subscales of MPSp was especially marked when correlated with NR-6, the Observing subscale of FFMQ, the Awareness subscale of PHLMS, the Short term focus subscale of RCS and the HADS and its two subscales Anxiety and Depression.

The relatively strong correlation between Fllc and NR-6 can only be hypothetically explained as there are no prior studies on the relationship between nature-relatedness and the construal level of mindfulness scale items. A possible explanation can be the fact that nature is something to be enjoyed physically in its particularity and concreteness, that is, with a low-level construal mindset. The everyday activities described in the Fllc scale items prime the respondents to adopt a low-level construal type of thinking. In their study introducing the NR-6 scale, Nisbet and Zelenski (2013) argue that a higher level of nature relatedness corresponds to more sustainable behavior. From this, it might logically follow that Fllc could be a good predictor of sustainable behavior. However, in an earlier study, Amel et al. (2009) found that while the Acting with awareness facet of FFMQ positively correlated with sustainable behavior, Observing items were uncorrelated. Further research is definitely warranted into how Fhlc and Fllc items are related to sustainable behavior as there are several studies emphasizing the role mindfulness could play in transforming our unsustainable lifestyle (Wamsler and Brink, 2018; Thiermann and Sheate, 2020).

While there are several conceptual articles arguing for mindfulness being a catalyst of change (Gärtner, 2013), original research articles on the topic are virtually non-existent. As a result of this, our findings can only be explained tentatively. The fact that the low-level construal subscale (Fllc) negatively correlates with the Short-term focus subscale of RCS might be attributable to the fact that people with more abstract thinking are more likely to see the “wider picture,” which means that they might not see changes as something inherently bad. Further research is needed to verify this hypothetical proposition.

As for the relationship between mindfulness in general and certain facets of it and various eudemonic well-being measures, the research results in the literature so far are controversial. Baer et al. (2004) found that the Observe facet of FFMQ did not have a significant relationship with eudemonic well-being and Caldwell et al., 2010) reported a positive correlation only in meditating samples. In Hollis-Walker and Colosimo's (2011) study, only the Acting with awareness and Describing facets of FFMQ had a significant correlation with psychological well-being. Baer et al. (2008) reported that with the exception of the Observing facet, all other facets of FFMQ were related to psychological well-being. Certainly, these comparisons have to be taken with caution as the PWB scales used in those studies are not identical with the QEWB used in our study.

As there are no prior data on how the SCS relates to facets of mindfulness, only indirect comparisons are possible with studies using similar instruments. In Adair, Fredrickson, Castro-Schilo, Kim and Sidberry (2018) study, composite FFMQ mindfulness scores were positively correlated with pro-social behavior. It has to be noted that the FFMQ version they used was without the Observing subscale. In our study, the Describing, Acting with awareness, and Non-judging facets of FFMQ were moderately correlated (r = 0.25–0.39, p ≤ 0.05) with SCS composite scores. Both MPSp subscales are uncorrelated with SCS. In the previous chapter, it was pointed out that the Observing facet of FFMQ, being similar to the Fllc subscale, also has an ambiguous relationship with anxiety and depression measures.

In summary, it can be stated that this study has not found evidence that the construal level of trait mindfulness scale items has a significant effect on the recollection of mindful episodes by respondents. Both the high-level construal (Fhlc) and the low-level construal (Fllc) subscales demonstrated similar factorial stability, reliability, and invariance. However, there were remarkable differences between them in the concurrent validity test. Consequently, our hypothesis that the high- and low-level construal items would behave differently in the statistical analysis has been only partly confirmed. Our hypothesis, predicting that in spite of the single facet construct of MP and the expected unidimensional tendencies, items would gravitate toward two hidden variables according to their level of construal, has been confirmed.

Conclusions

Using samples from various base populations, ample evidence was found that the positively worded scale version MPSp is a valid instrument with clear and stable factor structure across samples and gender, good reliability, and adequate concurrent validity. The bifactorial structure was confirmed in two different samples through EFA and CFA. In spite of the fact that a bifactorial structure was consistently evidenced in Study 1, Study 2, and Study 3, indices (UniCo, ECV, MIREAL) used to measure the construct's closeness to unidimensionality and the interfactor correlation signaled an underlying common factor. This result lends support to our initial claim hypothesizing that both high- and low-level construal scale items in MPSp tap into a central aspect of the complex construct termed “Mindful Presence,” which encompasses skills and abilities to bring attention to and anchor it in the present moment. We feel that our study adds to the existing literature in several ways. Besides introducing a really short but valid trait mindfulness measure, we directed attention to the yet unresearched issue of the construal level of scale items. The MPSp scale's main fields of application can be validation studies where several concurrent instruments are used at the same time as well as for target populations with very limited time for self-report tests.

Limitations and Further Research

In Study 1, the three samples were drawn from the same base population of students of economics at the University of Debrecen with no significant difference between the samples regarding age and gender makeup; however, group effects cannot be ruled out with certainty. While in Study 1 and Study 3, the age and gender of students who did not send back the questionnaire were known, in Study 2 and Study 4, it could not be determined, which raises the possibility of these samples being selectively biased. Although diverse samples were used, in the future the scale should be tested on general and clinical populations to see if results can be replicated. The sample in Study 4 (n6) was relatively small, reducing the statistical power of the Spearman correlations under a given threshold of rs value. Ten concurrent instruments were incorporated into this initial validation process, and although short-scale versions were used, respondent fatigue may have had an impact on scores. The response rate was relatively low in all four studies. One possible explanation is the lack of financial or other rewards often granted to student respondents. In the case of the two adult samples, limited free (Study 2 and Study 4) time and the length of the questionnaire (Study 4) might have been a possible cause of the low response rate. This validation study is based on Classic Test Theory (CTT). IRT in future research can provide important information (i.e., item difficulty) not addressed by CTT (Sébille et al., 2010; Ye et al., 2018, 2019). Even though MPSp items have been categorized into construal-level groups based on whether they are action-specific or general statements about mindfulness, as construal level is a continuum, the procedure can be considered arbitrary.

In the future, MPSp's relationship with other instruments could also be tested. Because of the dynamism of mindful and mindless moments in non-meditating samples and the fact that self-report instruments can only measure respondents' recollection of mindful experience at a particular moment, trait mindfulness can only be established with certainty based on time-series data of repeated measures done on the same sample; thus, the notion “trait” should be taken with caution. The application of generalizability theory (Truong et al., 2020; Ye et al., 2020) might help to grasp the trait or state nature of MPSp scale items in future studies.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Debrecen Clinical Center, Regional and Institutional Committee of Scientific Ethics. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

AL, DK, RO, SS, ÉB, ZB, and AM: conceptualization, investigation, and writing—review and editing. AL, DK, RO, and SS: formal analysis, methodology, and writing—original draft. ÉB and ZB: funding acquisition. ÉB, ZB, and AM: project administration, resources, and visualization. AL, DK, RO, SS, and AM: validation. All authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript, read, and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

The publication was supported by the GINOP-2.3.2-15-2016-00005 Project. The project was co-financed by the European Union under the European Regional Development Fund.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.626084/full#supplementary-material

References

Adair, K. C., Fredrickson, B. L., Castro-Schilo, L., Kim, S., and Sidberry, S. (2018). Present with you: does cultivated mindfulness predict greater social connection through gains in decentering and reductions in negative emotions? Mindfulness 9, 737–749. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0811-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aguado, J., Luciano, J. V., Cebolla, A., Serrano-Blanco, A., Soler, J., and García-Campayo, J. (2015). Bifactor analysis and construct validity of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire (FFMQ) in non-clinical Spanish samples. Front. Psychol. 6:404. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ainur, A. K., Sayang, M. D., Jannoo, Z., and Yap, B. W. (2017). Sample size and non-normality effects on goodness of fit measures in structural equation models. Pertanika J. Sci. Technol. 25, 575–586.

Google Scholar

Alter, A. L., and Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). Effects of fluency on psychological distance and mental construal (or why New York is a large city, but New York is a civilized jungle). Psychol. Sci. 19, 161–167. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02062.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amel, E. L., Manning, C. M., and Scott, B. A. (2009). Mindfulness and sustainable behavior: pondering attention and awareness as means for increasing green behavior. Ecopsychology 1, 14–25. doi: 10.1089/eco.2008.0005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anālayo, B. (2019). Adding historical depth to definitions of mindfulness. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 28, 11–14. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bado, F. M. R., Rebustini, F., Jamieson, L., Cortellazzi, K. L., and Mialhe, F. L. (2018). Evaluation of the psychometric properties of the Brazilian version of the Oral Health Literacy Assessment in Spanish and development of a shortened form of the instrument. PLoS ONE 13:e0207989. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0207989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., and Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: the Kentucky inventory of mindfulness skills. Assessment 11, 191–206. doi: 10.1177/1073191104268029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E., Button, D., Krietemeyer, J., Sauer, S., et al. (2008). Construct validity of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire in meditating and nonmeditating samples. Assessment 15, 329–342. doi: 10.1177/1073191107313003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basto, M., and Pereira, J. M. (2012). An SPSS R-menu for ordinal factor analysis. J. Statist. Softw. 46, 1–29. doi: 10.18637/jss.v046.i04

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentler, P. M. (1977). Factor simplicity index and transformations. Psychometrika 42, 277–295. doi: 10.1007/BF02294054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergomi, C., Tschacher, W., and Kupper, Z. (2013). Measuring mindfulness: first steps towards the development of a comprehensive mindfulness scale. Mindfulness 4, 18–32. doi: 10.1007/s12671-012-0102-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, A., Hadash, Y., and Fresco, D. M. (2019). Metacognitive processes model of decentering: emerging methods and insights. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 1:19. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bigras, N., Daspe, M. È., Godbout, N., Briere, J., and Sabourin, S. (2017). Cumulative childhood trauma and adult sexual satisfaction: mediation by affect dysregulation and sexual anxiety in men and women. J. Sex Marital Therapy 43, 377–396. doi: 10.1080/0092623X.2016.1176609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 11, 230–241. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bph077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Black, D. S., Sussman, S., Johnson, C. A., and Milam, J. (2012). Psychometric assessment of the mindful attention awareness scale (MAAS) among Chinese adolescents. Assessment 19, 42–52. doi: 10.1177/1073191111415365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonett, D. G., and Wright, T. A. (2000). Sample size requirements for estimating Pearson, Kendall and Spearman correlations. Psychometrika 65, 23–28. doi: 10.1007/BF02294183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 84:822. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. A. (2014). Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research. New York, NY: Guilford Publications.

Google Scholar

Caldwell, K., Harrison, M., Adams, M., Quin, R. H., and Greeson, J. (2010). Developing mindfulness in college students through movement-based courses: effects on self-regulatory self-efficacy, mood, stress, and sleep quality. J. Am. Coll. Health 58, 433–442. doi: 10.1080/07448480903540481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardaciotto, L., Herbert, J. D., Forman, E. M., Moitra, E., and Farrow, V. (2008). The assessment of present-moment awareness and acceptance the Philadelphia mindfulness scale. Assessment 15, 204–223. doi: 10.1177/1073191107311467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmody, J., Baer, R. A. L. B., Lykins, E., and Olendzki, N. (2009). An empirical study of the mechanisms of mindfulness in a mindfulness-based stress reduction program. J. Clin. Psychol. 65, 613–626. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carpenter, J. K., Conroy, K., Gomez, A. F., Curren, L. C., and Hofmann, S. G. (2019). The relationship between trait mindfulness and affective symptoms: a meta-analysis of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ). Clin. Psychol. Rev. 74:101785. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chadwick, P., Hember, M., Symes, J., Peters, E., Kuipers, E., and Dagnan, D. (2008). Responding mindfully to unpleasant thoughts and images: reliability and validity of the Southampton mindfulness questionnaire (SMQ). Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 47, 451–455. doi: 10.1348/014466508X314891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, E. Y., and Wang, Y. (2019). Mindfulness changes construal level: an experimental investigation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 148, 1656–1664. doi: 10.1037/xge0000654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, G. W., and Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct. Eq. Model. 9, 233–255. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiesi, F., Donati, M. A., Panno, A., Giacomantonio, M., and Primi, C. (2017). What about the different shortened versions of the mindful attention awareness scale? Psychol. Rep. 120, 966–990. doi: 10.1177/0033294117711132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christopher, M. S., Neuser, N. J., Michael, P. G., and Baitmangalkar, A. (2012). Exploring the psychometric properties of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire. Mindfulness 3, 124–131. doi: 10.1007/s12671-011-0086-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychol. Bullet. 112:155. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, K. M., Lau, M. A., and Cairns, D. R. (2009). Development and preliminary validation of a trait version of the toronto mindfulness scale. J. Cogn. Psychother. 23, 185–197. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.23.3.185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desrosiers, A., Klemanski, D. H., and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2013). Mapping mindfulness facets onto dimensions of anxiety and depression. Behav. Therapy 44, 373–384. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2013.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Djukanovic, I., Carlsson, J., and Årestedt, K. (2017). Is the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) a valid measure in a general population 65–80 years old? A psychometric evaluation study. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 15:193. doi: 10.1186/s12955-017-0759-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreyfus, G. (2011). Is mindfulness present-centred and non-judgmental? A discussion of the cognitive dimensions of mindfulness. Contemp. Buddhism 12, 41–54. doi: 10.1080/14639947.2011.564815

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksson, M., and Boman, E. (2018). Short is beautiful: dimensionality and measurement invariance in two length of the Basic Psychological need Satisfaction at work scale. Front. Psychol. 9:965. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabrigar, L. R., and Wegener, D. T. (2011). Exploratory Factor Analysis. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780199734177.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, G., Hayes, A., Kumar, S., Greeson, J., and Laurenceau, J. P. (2007). Mindfulness and emotion regulation: the development and initial validation of the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised (CAMS-R). J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 29:177. doi: 10.1007/s10862-006-9035-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrando, P. J., and Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2018). Assessing the quality and appropriateness of factor solutions and factor score estimates in exploratory item factor analysis. Educ. Psychol. Measur. 78, 762–780. doi: 10.1177/0013164417719308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finch, H., Stage, A. K., and Monahan, P. (2008). Comparison of factor simplicity indices for dichotomous data: DETECT R, Bentler's simplicity index, and the loading simplicity index. Appl. Measur. Educ. 21, 41–64. doi: 10.1080/08957340701796365

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finney, S. J., and DiStefano, C. (2013). “Nonnormal and categorical data in structural equation modeling,” in Structural Equation Modeling: A Second Course, eds G. R. Hancock and R. O. Mueller (IAP Information Age Publishing), 439–492.

Forero, C. G., Maydeu-Olivares, A., and Gallardo-Pujol, D. (2009). Factor analysis with ordinal indicators: a Monte Carlo study comparing DWLS and ULS estimation. Struct. Eq. Model. 16, 625–641. doi: 10.1080/10705510903203573

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank, J. L., Jennings, P. A., and Greenberg, M. T. (2016). Validation of the mindfulness in teaching scale. Mindfulness 7, 155–163. doi: 10.1007/s12671-015-0461-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagne, P., and Hancock, G. R. (2006). Measurement model quality, sample size, and solution propriety in confirmatory factor models. Multivar. Behav. Res. 41, 65–83. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr4101_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gärtner, C. (2013). Enhancing readiness for change by enhancing mindfulness. J. Change Manag. 13, 52–68. doi: 10.1080/14697017.2013.768433

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gethin, R. (2011). On some definitions of mindfulness. Contemp. Buddhism 12, 263–279. doi: 10.1080/14639947.2011.564843

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gong, H., and Medin, D. L. (2012). Construal levels and moral judgment: some complications. Judgm. Decision Making. 7, 628–638.

Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Babin, B. J., and Black, W. C. (2009). Multivariate Data Analysis: A Global Perspective, 7th edn. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Hartley, J. (2014). Some thoughts on Likert-type scales. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 14, 83–86. doi: 10.1016/S1697-2600(14)70040-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heene, M., Hilbert, S., Draxler, C., Ziegler, M., and Bühner, M. (2011). Masking misfit in confirmatory factor analysis by increasing unique variances: a cautionary note on the usefulness of cutoff values of fit indices. Psychol. Methods. 16:319. doi: 10.1037/a0024917

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heeren, A., Van Broeck, N., and Philippot, P. (2009). The effects of mindfulness on executive processes and autobiographical memory specificity. Behav. Res. Therapy 47, 403–409. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollis-Walker, L., and Colosimo, K. (2011). Mindfulness, self-compassion, and happiness in non-meditators: a theoretical and empirical examination. Personal. Individ. Diff. 50, 222–227. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoyle, R. H. (2000). Confirmatory factor analysis. Handb. Appl. Multivar. Statist. Math. Model. 3, 465–497. doi: 10.1016/B978-012691360-6/50017-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordan, C., Vitoratou, S., Siew, Y., and Chalder, T. (2019). Cognitive behavioural responses to envy: development of a new measure. Behav. Cogn. Psychotherapy 48, 408–418. doi: 10.1017/S1352465819000614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jorgensen, T. D., Pornprasertmanit, S., Schoemann, A. M., Rosseel, Y., Miller, P., Quick, C., et al. (2019). Package “semTools”. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/semTools/semTools.pdf

Google Scholar

Jung, S., and Lee, S. (2011). Exploratory factor analysis for small samples. Behav. Res. Methods 43, 701–709. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0077-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koran, J. (2020). Indicators per factor in confirmatory factor analysis: more is not always better. Struct. Eq. Model. Multidiscipl. J. 27, 765–772. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2019.1706527

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koziol, N. A., and Bovaird, J. A. (2018). The impact of model parameterization and estimation methods on tests of measurement invariance with ordered polytomous data. Educ. Psychol. Measur. 78, 272–296. doi: 10.1177/0013164416683754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, R. M., and Robbins, S. B. (1995). Measuring belongingness: the social connectedness and the social assurance scales. J. Counsel. Psychol. 42:232. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.42.2.232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, C. H. (2016a). The performance of ML, DWLS, and ULS estimation with robust corrections in structural equation models with ordinal variables. Psychol. Methods. 21:369. doi: 10.1037/met0000093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, C. H. (2016b). Confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data: comparing robust maximum likelihood and diagonally weighted least squares. Behav. Res. Methods 48, 936–949. doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0619-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liberman, N., and Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: a test of temporal construal theory. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 75:5. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lilja, J. L., Frodi-Lundgren, A., Hanse, J. J., Josefsson, T., Lundh, L. G., Sköld, C., et al. (2011). Five facets mindfulness questionnaire—reliability and factor structure: a Swedish version. Cogn. Behav. Therapy 40, 291–303. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2011.580367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lilja, J. L., Lundh, L. G., Josefsson, T., and Falkenström, F. (2013). Observing as an essential facet of mindfulness: a comparison of FFMQ patterns in meditating and non-meditating individuals. Mindfulness 4, 203–212. doi: 10.1007/s12671-012-0111-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Millsap, R. E., West, S. G., Tein, J. Y., Tanaka, R., and Grimm, K. J. (2017). Testing measurement invariance in longitudinal data with ordered-categorical measures. Psychol. Methods 22:486. doi: 10.1037/met0000075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2003). A factor simplicity index. Psychometrika 68, 49–60. doi: 10.1007/BF02296652

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mantzios, M., and Wilson, J. C. (2014). Making concrete construals mindful: a novel approach for developing mindfulness and self-compassion to assist weight loss. Psychol. Health 29, 422–441. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2013.863883

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattes, J. (2019). Systematic review and meta-analysis of correlates of FFMQ mindfulness facets. Front. Psychol. 10:2684. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNeish, D. (2018). Thanks coefficient alpha, we'll take it from here. Psychol. Methods 23, 412–433. doi: 10.1037/met0000144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meade, A. W., Johnson, E. C., and Braddy, P. W. (2008). Power and sensitivity of alternative fit indices in tests of measurement invariance. J. Appl. Psychol. 93:568. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.3.568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medvedev, O. N., Norden, P. A., Krägeloh, C. U., and Siegert, R. J. (2018). Investigating unique contributions of dispositional mindfulness facets to depression, anxiety, and stress in general and student populations. Mindfulness 9, 1757–1767. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-0917-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mîndrila, D. (2010). Maximum likelihood (ML) and diagonally weighted least squares (DWLS) estimation procedures: a comparison of estimation bias with ordinal and multivariate non-normal data. Int. J. Dig. Soc. 1, 60–66. doi: 10.20533/ijds.2040.2570.2010.0010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morera, O. F., and Stokes, S. M. (2016). Coefficient α as a measure of test score reliability: review of 3 popular misconceptions. Am. J. Public Health 106, 458–461. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2015.302993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mykletun, A., Stordal, E., and Dahl, A. A. (2001). Hospital Anxiety and Depression (HAD) scale: factor structure, item analyses and internal consistency in a large population. Br. J. Psychiatry 179, 540–544. doi: 10.1192/bjp.179.6.540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbet, E. K., and Zelenski, J. M. (2013). The NR-6: a new brief measure of nature relatedness. Front. Psychol. 4:813. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norman, E. (2017). Metacognition and mindfulness: the role of fringe consciousness. Mindfulness 8, 95–100. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0494-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oreg, S. (2003). Resistance to change: developing an individual differences measure. J. Appl. Psychol. 88:680. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.4.680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osborne, J. W., Costello, A. B., and Kellow, J. T. (2014). Best Practices in Exploratory Factor Analysis. Louisville, KY: CreateSpace Independent Publishing Platform, 86–99. doi: 10.4135/9781412995627.d8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osman, A., Lamis, D. A., Bagge, C. L., Freedenthal, S., and Barnes, S. M. (2016). The mindful attention awareness scale: further examination of dimensionality, reliability, and concurrent validity estimates. J. Personal. Assess. 98, 189–199. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2015.1095761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, T., Reilly-Spong, M., and Gross, C. R. (2013). Mindfulness: a systematic review of instruments to measure an emergent patient-reported outcome (PRO). Qual. Life Res. 22, 2639–2659. doi: 10.1007/s11136-013-0395-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratscher, S. D., Wood, P. K., King, L. A., and Bettencourt, B. A. (2019). Interpersonal mindfulness: scale development and initial construct validation. Mindfulness 10, 1044–1061. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-1057-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putnick, D. L., and Bornstein, M. H. (2016). Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: the state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Dev. Rev. 41, 71–90. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raiche, G. (2010). nFactors: An R Package for Parallel Analysis and Non Graphical Solutions to the Cattell Scree Test. R Package Version 2. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/nFactors/nFactors.pdf

Revelle, W. (2016). How to: Use the Psych Package for Factor Analysis and Data Reduction. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University, Department of Psychology (2016).

Rodriguez, A., Reise, S. P., and Haviland, M. G. (2016). Applying bifactor statistical indices in the evaluation of psychological measures. J. Personal. Assess. 98, 223–237. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2015.1089249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ropovik, I. (2015). A cautionary note on testing latent variable models. Front. Psychol. 6:1715. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruscio, J., and Roche, B. (2012). Determining the number of factors to retain in an exploratory factor analysis using comparison data of known factorial structure. Psychol. Assess. 24:282. doi: 10.1037/a0025697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadowski, I., Böke, N., Mettler, J., Heath, N., and Khoury, B. (2020). Naturally mindful? the role of mindfulness facets in the relationship between nature relatedness and subjective well-being. Curr. Psychol. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-01056-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sass, D. A., Schmitt, T. A., and Marsh, H. W. (2014). Evaluating model fit with ordered categorical data within a measurement invariance framework: a comparison of estimators. Struct. Eq. Model. Multidiscipl. J. 21, 167–180. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.882658

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satorra, A., and Bentler, P. M. (2001). A scaled difference chi-square test statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika 66, 507–514. doi: 10.1007/BF02296192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schermelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H., and Müller, H. (2003). Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods Psychol. Res. Online 8, 23–74.

Google Scholar

Schutte, N. S., and Malouff, J. M. (2018). Mindfulness and connectedness to nature: a meta-analytic investigation. Personal. Individ. Differ. 127, 10–14. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.01.034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sébille, V., Hardouin, J. B., Le Néel, T., Kubis, G., Boyer, F., Guillemin, F., et al. (2010). Methodological issues regarding power of classical test theory (CTT) and item response theory (IRT)-based approaches for the comparison of patient-reported outcomes in two groups of patients-a simulation study. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 10, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-10-24

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegling, A. B., and Petrides, K. V. (2016). Zeroing in on mindfulness facets: similarities, validity, and dimensionality across three independent measures. PLoS ONE 11:e0153073. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sijtsma, K. (2009). On the use, the misuse, and the very limited usefulness of Cronbach's alpha. Psychometrika 74:107. doi: 10.1007/s11336-008-9101-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinval, J., Pasian, S., Queirós, C., and Marôco, J. (2018). Brazil-Portugal transcultural adaptation of the UWES-9: internal consistency, dimensionality, and measurement invariance. Front. Psychol. 9:353. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, O. R., Melkevik, O., Samdal, O., Larsen, T. M., and Haug, E. (2017). Psychometric properties of the five-item version of the Mindful Awareness Attention Scale (MAAS) in Norwegian adolescents. Scand. J. Public Health 45, 373–380. doi: 10.1177/1403494817699321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugiura, Y. (2004). Detached mindfulness and worry: a meta-cognitive analysis. Personal. Individ. Differ. 37, 169–179. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2003.08.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., and Segal, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: empirical evidence. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 70, 275–287. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.70.2.275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tejedor, R., Feliu-Soler, A., Pascual, J. C., Cebolla, A., Portella, M. J., Trujols, J., et al. (2014). Psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale. Revista de Psiquiatría y Salud Mental 7, 157–165. doi: 10.1016/j.rpsmen.2014.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiermann, U. B., and Sheate, W. R. (2020). The way forward in mindfulness and sustainability: a critical review and research agenda. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 5, 118–139. doi: 10.1007/s41465-020-00180-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trope, Y., and Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychol. Rev. 117:440. doi: 10.1037/a0018963

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Truong, Q. C., Krägeloh, C. U., Siegert, R. J., Landon, J., and Medvedev, O. N. (2020). Applying generalizability theory to differentiate between trait and state in the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ). Mindfulness 11, 953–963. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01324-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dam, N. T., Earleywine, M., and Borders, A. (2010). Measuring mindfulness? An item response theory analysis of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale. Personal. Individ. Differ. 49, 805–810. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dam, N. T., van Vugt, M. K., Vago, D. R., Schmalzl, L., Saron, C. D., Olendzki, A., et al. (2018). Mind the hype: a critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation. Perspectiv. Psychol. Sci. 13, 36–61. doi: 10.1177/1745691617709589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhaeghen, P., and Aikman, S. N. (2020). How the mindfulness manifold relates to the five moral foundations, prejudice, and awareness of privilege. Mindfulness 11, 241–254. doi: 10.1007/s12671-019-01243-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vess, M., Arndt, J., and Schlegel, R. J. (2011). Abstract construal levels attenuate state self-esteem reactivity. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47, 861–864. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.02.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walach, H., Buchheld, N., Buttenmüller, V., Kleinknecht, N., and Schmidt, S. (2006). Measuring mindfulness—the Freiburg mindfulness inventory (FMI). Personal. Individ. Differ. 40, 1543–1555. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wamsler, C., and Brink, E. (2018). Mindsets for sustainability: exploring the link between mindfulness and sustainable climate adaptation. Ecol. Econ. 151, 55–61. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2018.04.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waterman, A. S., Schwartz, S. J., Zamboanga, B. L., Ravert, R. D., Williams, M. K., Bede Agocha, V., et al. (2010). The questionnaire for eudaimonic well-being: psychometric properties, demographic comparisons, and evidence of validity. J. Posit. Psychol. 5, 41–61. doi: 10.1080/17439760903435208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, M. W. (2017). The reliability of multidimensional neuropsychological measures: from alpha to omega. Clin. Neuropsychol. 31, 1113–1126. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2017.1317364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson-Singleton, N. N., Walker, J. H., LoParo, D., Mack, S. A., and Kaslow, N. J. (2018). Psychometric evaluation of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire in a clinical sample of African Americans. Mindfulness 9, 312–324. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0776-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, A. (2000). GAD, metacognition, and mindfulness: an information processing analysis. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 9, 95–100. doi: 10.1093/clipsy/9.1.95

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, A. (2002). Emotional Disorders and Metacognition: Innovative Cognitive Therapy. Chichester: Wiley. doi: 10.1002/9780470713662

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, E. J., Harrington, K. M., Clark, S. L., and Miller, M. W. (2013). Sample size requirements for structural equation models: an evaluation of power, bias, and solution propriety. Educ. Psychol. Measur. 73, 913–934. doi: 10.1177/0013164413495237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ye, Z. J., Liang, M. Z., Zhang, H. W., Li, P. F., Ouyang, X. R., Yu, Y. L., et al. (2018). Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of resilience scale specific to cancer: an item response theory analysis. Qual. Life Res. 27, 1635–1645. doi: 10.1007/s11136-018-1835-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ye, Z. J., Zhang, Z., Tang, Y., Liang, J., Sun, Z., Zhang, X. Y., et al. (2019). Development and psychometric analysis of the 10-item resilience scale specific to cancer: a multidimensional item response theory analysis. Eur. J. Oncol. Nurs. 41, 64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.ejon.2019.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ye, Z. J., Zhang, Z., Zhang, X. Y., Tang, Y., Chen, P., Liang, M. Z., et al. (2020). State or trait? measuring resilience by generalisability theory in breast cancer. Eur. J. Oncol. Nurs. 46:101727. doi: 10.1016/j.ejon.2020.101727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, Y., Liu, Z. W., Zhou, W., Chen, X. C., Zhang, X. Y., Hu, M., et al. (2018). Assessment of burden among family caregivers of schizophrenia: psychometric testing for short-form Zarit burden interviews. Front. Psychol. 9:2539. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, K. H., Chan, W., Marcoulides, G. A., and Bentler, P. M. (2016). Assessing structural equation models by equivalence testing with adjusted fit indexes. Struct. Eq. Model. Multidiscipl. J. 23, 319–330. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2015.1065414

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, D. C., Foster, G. C., and McKenna, M. G. (2019). Is the DOSPERT gender invariant? A psychometric test of measurement invariance. J. Behav. Decision Making 32, 203–211. doi: 10.1002/bdm.2105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zigmond, A. S., and Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 67, 361–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix

Below is the English translation of MPSp.

MPSp

Imagine that you are at home, alone, spending your free time. How much are the following statements true for you? Check one of the six options.

The six-scale points were the following: almost always, very frequently, somewhat frequently, somewhat infrequently, very infrequently, and almost never.

• When I'm doing routine activities, I focus only on what I am doing.

• When I'm doing everyday tasks, I re-live the experience with full attention and awareness every time.

• I don't rush routine activities only to be able to do something more interesting afterward.

• When I'm having a shower or a bath, I pay all my attention to that activity. For example, the sound of the water, the feeling as it runs down my body, the way my body reacts etc.

• When I'm eating, I pay all my attention to eating. For example, how the flavors of the food appear while chewing, how the texture of the food changes, the feeling when I swallow it, etc.

• When I'm getting dressed, I focus all my attention on that activity. For example, the smell of the clothes, the material touching my skin, the touch of the buttons etc.

Keywords: methodological issues, construal level, scale validation, short scale, trait mindfulness, mindfulness

Citation: Lengyel A, Keczeli D, Orosz R, Bács Z, Müller A, Szőke S and Bácsné Bába É (2021) Initial Validation of the Mindful Presence Scale: The Issue of the Construal Level of Scale Items. Front. Psychol. 12:626084. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.626084

Received: 04 November 2020; Accepted: 09 June 2021;
Published: 21 July 2021.

Edited by:

Sai-fu Fung, City University of Hong Kong, China

Reviewed by:

Zeng-Jie Ye, Guangzhou University of Chinese Medicine, China
Garry Kuan, Universiti Sains Malaysia Health Campus, Malaysia

Copyright © 2021 Lengyel, Keczeli, Orosz, Bács, Müller, Szőke and Bácsné Bába. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Attila Lengyel, guszfraba@gmail.com; lengyel.attila@econ.unideb.hu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.