Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 20 May 2021
Sec. Movement Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Futsal research and challenges for sport development View all 23 articles

Stressors in Indoor and Field Brazilian Soccer: Are They Perceived as a Distress or Eustress?

\r\nMaria Regina Ferreira Brando*Maria Regina Ferreira Brandão*Luis Felipe PolitoLuis Felipe PolitoVania HernandesVania HernandesMariana CorreaMariana CorreaAna Paula MastrocolaAna Paula MastrocolaDaniel OliveiraDaniel OliveiraAlessandra OliveiraAlessandra OliveiraLarissa MouraLarissa MouraMarcelo Villas Boas JuniorMarcelo Villas Boas JuniorDaniela Angelo\r\nDaniela Angelo
  • Master’s and Doctoral Programme in Physical Education, São Judas Tadeu University, São Paulo, Brazil

Soccer players inescapably live under stress during the sportive career, and many real-life aspects of soccer situations operate in the ongoing performance. This study’s main objective was to elaborate the List of Stressors in Professional Indoor and Field Soccer, a self-report instrument designed to measure the impact of 77 soccer situations upon the sport performance. Participants were 138 indoor and field soccer players from the Brazilian Premier League. Each situation was evaluated on a 7-point scale, ranging from the most negative (−3) to the most positive (+3). Data were analyzed according to the players’ perception of the items: distress or eustress and its intensity, and after that, situations perceived as plus −1 and +1 were compared by time in which they were experienced and distributed among five categories established by the literature: Expectations about the Performance, Personal Factors, Competition Aspects, Training Demands, and Relationship with Significant People. Narratives of athletes’ experiences were also used to discuss the results. An Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling using Bi-factorial (BI-ESEM) was employed to assess the factor structure. For the total participants, 49 situations were perceived as distress and 28 as eustress. Using the criteria established a priori, the distribution was among the five categories in the remaining 32 situations. Differences in perception between less and more experienced players were found in 11 situations. The results revealed that Brazilian professional soccer players experience various stressful situations. These events are important representations of environmental demands and could predict the performance as they are perceived as eustress or distress. Some of these stressful situations are inherent in sport and others adjacent to the sports system or environment. Coach pressure to win and conflicts with teammates are examples of stressors in-sport, family problems and disputes with press or fans are examples of stressors external to the team, also called peripheral opponents, and showed the relative social influence of significant others in soccer performance. We can conclude that the knowledge of the direction of a given stress situation has important practical implications in preparing athletes and helping them face the performance stressors that are part of soccer daily life.

Introduction

The term stress started to be used only at the beginning of the 20th century. Until then, the term was used in physics to describe the force that tends to deform an object. In 1910, Sir William Osler, a British cardiologist, first suggested that stress contributed to coronary heart disease (Robinson, 2018). Since then, stress has moved from a term in physics to a cultural construct, and numerous studies have been carried out to detect the effects of stress on physical and mental health.

Selye (1976), considered the patriarch of research on stress in human beings, refers to a “General Adaptation Syndrome,” a syndrome due to the presence of generalized individual manifestations of the organism in the face of a harmful stimulus, adaptation by stimulating the body’s defenses, and general because it is produced only by agents that have a widespread effect on large parts of the body.

From the point of view of psychological stress research, the main emphasis of the studies is not on the physiological genesis, but on the psychological genesis of stress, changes in the well-being of individuals, cognitive processes, and appraisal of stressors and their psychological control (Nitsch, 1981; Jones, 1990). According to this conception, the following principles are important: the starting point of the study of stress is in the reciprocal relationship between the individual and the environment, in subjective perception and in the attribution of value judgments that the individual has about the situation of this environment (Folkman and Lazarus, 1984; Lazarus, 2000).

So, what makes a stressful situation is the nature of this circumstance and the interpretation, the way people subjectively perceive and evaluate the situation. This perception depends on the psychic dispositions (attitudes, beliefs, and values) and previous learning. People do not behave passively in the face of environmental stimuli but rather give them personal importance. In the relationship between individual and environment, an intermediate cognitive process is characterized by subjective evaluation, which is considered a decisive point for the emergence of stress (Nitsch, 1981).

In other words, stress is not necessarily debilitating and can facilitate performance. Then, it is vital to research the “directional perception” of the sources of stress, that is, the nature of the individual interpretation of the sources in terms of whether they are positive or negative about the next performance (Jones et al., 1990, 1993; Swain and Jones, 1993; Jones, 1995a; Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi, 2000; Fletcher and Hanton, 2003).

Hypothetically, athletes can report identical sources of stress, but because of variations in “directional perception,” they can differ considerably in their interpretations of the consequences on performance. Parfitt et al. (1990) still claim that the “directional perception” of stress sources can predict sports performance better than just the simple indication of a source.

Soccer players, indoor or field, are inevitably exposed to a potential number of stressors that operate on athletic performance during their sports career (Poulus et al., 2020). Professional players are under considerable stress, and they need to compete for a place on the starting lineup and, once they win the place, they must be able to keep it. Low performance in just one game can take the player to the bench in the next round. The risk of injury is always present and can mean the absence of practice for more or less long periods. The athlete must deal with the stress produced by the so-called peripheral opponents, spectators, the press and family members, and the expectations of the coach and his teammates (Apitzsch, 1994; Teipel et al., 1994; Noblet and Gifford, 2002; Mellalieu et al., 2009; Olmedilla et al., 2019).

During training and competitions, soccer players are faced with a series of requirements, stressors, which can vary in terms of the content and intensity of their effect on sports performance (Jones et al., 1993; Swain and Jones, 1993; Jones, 1995a; Brandão, 2000; De Rose Junior et al., 2004; Zahariadis et al., 2006; Thelwell et al., 2008, Main and Grove, 2009; Garcia-Maás et al., 2010). But, for Urhausen et al. (1998) and De Rose Junior et al. (2004), it is not only during training and competitions that stressful situations occur. They also appear in events inside and outside of sport and influence an athlete’s mental and physical readiness. Thus, an important aspect pointed out in evaluating sport stressors concerns the sources of stressful situations and the perception of these sources’ direction and intensity. The psychological and physiological demands in soccer associated with training, competitions, and the social organization of sport show that performance is a complex phenomenon affected by factors inherent to the sport modality and environmental factors.

A literature review (Cohn, 1990; Scanlan et al., 1991; Samulski and Chagas, 1992; De Rose Junior and Vasconcellos, 1993; VanYperen, 1994; Brandão, 2000; De Rose Junior et al., 2004) showed that there are 5 major sources of stress inherent to various sports: expectations about performance, personal factors, aspects of competition, physical demands, and relationships with significant people and traumatic experiences. The category expectations about performance is composed of two subcategories: goals and pressure. The goal refers to the athlete’s ambition to achieve a certain sporting result and the pressure, the demands of the media, himself, and others that indicate performance expectations, pressure to meet the established goals, “obligation” to win a certain game and to achieve expected results. Personal factors refer to the demands (costs or psychological benefits) of sports practice. This category includes the athlete’s disposition, his psychological and organic state, and other aspects of professional practice such as the employment contract. The competition aspects refer to the events that commonly occur during the competition. A negative stress response occurs when the athlete has feelings or thoughts of concern about some aspect of the game, the conditions of the field, the crowd, the press assessment, etc. Physical demands refer to the role that factors inherent to the sporting event play in the stress process. If the athlete does not have a personal fitness to deal with the demands of training and competitions, consequently there is a risk of failure or a decrease in performance. Finally, the dimension relationship with significant people refers to the extent and nature of the bonds with people that are significant for athletes and that influence their performance. These people are fully involved in the structure, dynamics, and social environment of the sport practiced. Teipel et al. (1994), in a study with soccer players, showed that previous defeats, the influence of the fans, and the unexpected results of the opposing team were motivators and, therefore, facilitators of sports performance. On the other hand, being physically weak, uncomfortable competitive conditions, failed actions at the start of competitions, conflicts with the coach, problems with the referees, the coach’s continued criticism from the bench, failed actions, and the negative criticism of teammates was harmful to the performance.

If there is a positive or negative impact of these stressors on soccer players’ sports performance, they need to be investigated. So, the purpose of this study was six, including (a) development of a List of Stressors in Professional Indoor and Field Soccer to facilitate the identification of situations that can cause stress in professional indoor and field players, (b) confirm if the stressful situations can be perceived as both distress and eustress, (c) identify the situations perceived as distress and eustress, (d) check which items make up the five previously established categories (Expectations about the Performance, Personal Factors, Competition Aspects, Training Demands, Relationship with Significant People), (e) examine the differences in the perception of the situations by age, and time as professional, and (f) assess the factor structure of the list.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 138 indoor and field male soccer players (17 goalkeepers, 41 defenders, 47 midfielders, and 33 forward) from the Brazilian Premier League participated in the study (age = 24.46 years, SD = 3.93; sports experience = 11.25 years, SD = 4.61, years as a professional player = 5.62 years, SD = 3.77, and years of soccer practice = 11,68, SD = 4,71 years). The participants comprised players from four different clubs, including Palmeiras, Grêmio, Internacional, and Paulista (Jundiaí). As professional players, they train an average of 10 h per week of football per se and 8 h of physical preparation. In addition, in the main championship of the country they compete twice a week, on Wednesdays and Sundays. It is essential to highlight that from all the participants, 18 field soccer players had played for the Brazilian National team.

Measurements

(1) Socio-demographic and background information.

We assessed age, sports experience, years as a professional player.

(2) Measurement of the stress situations in indoor and field soccer.

List Development

The list of football situations was designed based on the Life Events Checklist, the dominant method used by researchers in the last 50 years (Dohrenwend, 2006), defined as occurrences, fundamentally critical environmental incidents, that were likely to bring about readjustment-requiring changes in people’s usual activities. Moreover, it was elaborated considering the theoretical review of stress in soccer (Frester, 1976; Samulski and Chagas, 1992, 1996; Teipel, 1993; Brandão, 2000). To develop an initial list of soccer situations, a focus group composed of two specialist soccer coaches and five professional players (three field soccer and two indoor soccer) besides the main researcher of this study was created. The focus group’s objective was to help reflect not only on the situations they considered important but also on the different points of view presented and the strength of each situation to be part of the list of stressful situations in soccer. The group was gathered three times to achieve this goal. The focus group listed 72 situations that ranged from the regular activities involved in the football environment, training, competitions, on the one hand, to the relationship with significant people, such as coaches, referees, teammates and the press, on the other.

After that, five event categories defined a priori according to the literature review were used to classify the 72 items. Some examples of events for each of the five categories are: for Category 1: Expectations about the Performance (Being the favorite, Coach pressure to win); Category 2: Personal Factors (Not sleeping well the night before the competition, Being with the contract already due or close to its maturity date); Category 3: Competition Aspects (Playing against a hostile audience, Not scoring a goal that was practically attained); Category 4: Training Demands (Being blatantly dribbled during tactical training, training in two periods); Category 5: Relationship with Significant People (Having problems or conflicts with the coach, Being jeopardized by the referees).

For the structuring of a procedure that would provide the recording of the conditions of the situations by the athlete, it was interesting to know not only which situations were experienced as distress or eustress but, at the same time, how intensely each situation could act as a disturbance or, on the contrary, as a stimulus to the athletic performance. To this end, a seven-level scale was developed, with three dimensions (eustress, distress, and a performance-neutral dimension). Each dimension had three levels of intensity. Thus, 1, 2, 3 identify the positive or negative impact intensity of the situation to the performance, as follows: +1 (a small amount of positive impact); +2 (a moderate amount of positive impact); +3 (a huge amount of positive impact); −3 (a huge amount of negative impact; −2 (a moderate amount of negative impact); −1 (a small amount of negative impact). The number 0 is the center of the scale and means that the situation neither stimulates positively nor negatively the performance.

The events list was tested in an exploratory study with 24 players to verify the clarity of the items’ instructions and phrasing, the item suitability, and the possibility of inclusion, revision, or rejection of each item in the item pool. Seven item events that presented ambiguous responses and one item that referred only to goalkeepers were neglected. Thirteen items were included to increase the instrument’s credibility. The inclusions were based on the testimonies of the players obtained during the application of the exploratory project. This process generated a second version of the list composed of 77 events.

Then, an initial revision of the second version was made by a panel of judges, composed by a Ph.D. in Sport Psychology, specialist in the study of stress in sport and especially in soccer, by the coach of the Brazilian Soccer National Team and his assistant, the goalkeeper coach of one of the evaluated teams, a Ph.D. in sports training and the main researcher of the study that worked with high performance professional soccer teams, including the Brazilian team. Their suggestions were primarily regarding the understanding of the response to items by players with a low level of education. They recommended using graphic symbols (“faces”) that represented the perception of the direction and intensity of stress. Thus, the following symbols appeared in the test body:www.frontiersin.org which represent −3, −2, −1, 0, +1, +2, +3, respectively.

Then, these procedures generated the final “List of Stressors in Professional Indoor and Field Brazilian Soccer” of this study, which consists of 77 soccer events and five categories, seven items for Category 1; six items for Category 2; 40 items for Category 3 (subdivided into three groups: opponents six items; imminent or real failure eight items; aspects of the game 26 items); 14 items for Category 4; and 10 items for Category 5. It is important to highlight that the item pool was developed so that the language was common to both indoor soccer players and field soccer players. The categories of stressors, definition, and list of the 77 events are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Categories of stressors, definition, and list of the 77 events.

Data Collection and Procedures

The study was conducted according to international guidelines for ethical principles of scientific research with human beings. The study procedures of this research were also approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University São Judas Tadeu. Indoor and field soccer players from the Brazilian First Division were invited to participate in this study. Upon obtaining participants’ written informed consent, the data were collected, individually, outside training hours, in pre-season periods (preparation period before the competitive period). Participants were encouraged to read each situation and provide honest responses about their effect on the performance and its magnitude. Special emphasis was placed on confidentiality and participation was not mandatory.

After the list evaluation, all athletes undertook a semi-structured interview. The main objective of the individual interviews was to understand the perception of stressful situations from what each player experiences and to give him the possibility to talk about that experience. The interviews also explored how well the items captured the athletes’ experiences about the item’s content. Interviews were audio-recorded and then transcribed verbatim.

Analysis

All items on the stressor list were analyzed in descriptive statistics and considered if they were perceived as a distress or eustress. Subsequently, a first analysis of the data was made according to the following criteria: the items evaluated between −1 and −3, and +1 and +3 were classified according to five stressors categories defined a priori. This interval was chosen because the objective of the study was to identify situations that had a negative or positive impact on performance, and situations assessed close to neutral indicate that according to the perception of the Brazilian players they do not interfere in any way to the performance and, therefore, can be excluded. The one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and a post hoc test Tukey was then used to compare the players by age (younger, average, and older players) and time as a professional. The level of significance adopted was 0.05.

An Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling using Bi-factorial (BI-ESEM) was employed to assess the factor structure. We chose to use a BI-ESEM model to allow estimates of direct relationships between items and specific and global factors; thus, it is possible to separate the variation attributed to specific factors, from that attributed to the general factor (Holzinger and Swineford, 1937).

Bi-factorial models assume that the covariance between a set of items can be explained by a set of orthogonal factors, including global factor (G-factor; in this case they would be 2 global factors) and specific factors (S-factor). The recent development of bi-factorial rotation for EFA has made it possible to incorporate bi-factorial modeling into the ESEM framework. In BI-ESEM, the G factors were specified separately, outside the rotation process (Arens and Morin, 2017; Howard et al., 2018; Marsh et al., 2020).

To assess the levels of reliability of the latent variables, it was used and the Composite Reliability (CR). Values above 0.7 are considered adequate (Hair et al., 2014).

Results

The mean and standard deviation of the 77 items of the list of stressors are shown in Table 2. To better view the results, the events were plotted into a figure (Figure 1) according to the perception of distress (A) or eustress (B). As one can observe, 49 soccer events were perceived as distress and 28 as eustress.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean and standard deviation of the 77 items of the list of stressors.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. List of stressors perceived as distress (A) or eustress (B).

The soccer events evaluated between −1 and −3, and +1 and +3, comprising a total of 32 items, were selected to be analyzed. In Table 3, it is possible to observe the number of athletes who have chosen each negative (A) or positive (B) dimension of the 32 situations. Two items draw attention, numbers 1 (Not being in good shape) and 75 (Lack of group cohesion), experienced as having a large amount of negative impact for 111 and 100 players, representing 80 and 72% of the total athletes, respectively. Other items that have a high negative impact on performance for more than 50% of the players were: 9 (Having problems or conflicts with the coach), 10 (Having problems or disputes with the teammates), 35 (Losing by a dilated score), and 59 (Playing injured). On the other hand, items 6 (Establishing high goals) and 40 (Playing a derby) were considered as having a high positive impact on the performance for 87 and 82% of the total athletes. The other items that have a high positive impact were: 19 (Playing at home), 20 (Assuming responsibilities inside the team), 25 (Some other team wanting to book you), 32 (Doing a speed training), 34 (Stretching), 49 (Self-pressure to play well), and 52 (Knowing in advance that you are going to play).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Number of athletes who have chosen each negative, neutral, or positive dimension of the 32 situations (in yellow the negatives, and in green the positive situations chosen by 50% or more).

The 32 situations were then classified according to the five categories defined a priori (Category 1: Expectations about the Performance, 4 items; Category 2: Personal Factors, 5 items; Category 3: Competition Aspects, 12 items; Category 4: Training Demands, 6 items; Category 5: Relationship with Significant People, 5 items). The 32-item instrument showed good reliability (CR = 0.74). We assessed the CR of each category that presented the following results: CR = 0.65 for Expectations about the performance; CR = 0.51 for Personal factors; CR = 0.64 for Competition aspects; CR = 0.62 for Training demands; and CR = 0.84 for Relationship with significant people. The soccer events are plotted in Figure 2, from the most negative to the most positive impact on the performance.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. List of soccer event stressors by categories of stress, Expectations about Performance (A), Personal Factor (B), Competition Aspects (C), Training Demands (D), and Relationship with Significant People (E).

As shown in Figure 2, a wide variability of the performance’s impact exists among the stress categories. In Category 1, all the events were perceived as positives, and in Category 5, all the events were perceived as negatives. In Categories 2, 3, and 4, some of the events were perceived as negative, while others were perceived as positive. Special attention was given to Category 3 that was divided into three as previously described: opponents, imminent or real failure, and aspects of the game. None of the opponents’ soccer events reached the established result criterion regarding average being from −1 to −3 or +1 to +3.

These 32 situations were compared between age (younger, average, and older players), and time as professional (less experience, average, and more experience players). The results by age can be observed in Table 4 and the significant items in Figure 3, and by time as professional in Table 5 and the significant items in Figure 4. Compared by age we can observe differences (p < 0.01) in 10 situations (3, 4, 19, 25, 34, 35, 36, 51, 59, and 73); younger players tend to evaluate situations as more negative or more positive to performance when compared mainly with older players. Compared by time as professionals, we can observe differences (p < 0.01) in eight situations (3, 4, 19, 25, 36, 51, 59, and 73) and notice the same trend as before: less experienced players tend to evaluate situations as more negative or more positive to performance when compared mainly with more experienced players.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. 32 items for younger (≤22 years), average (23–27 years), and older (≥28 years) players.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Significant items for younger (≤22 years), average (23–27 years), and older (≥28 years) players.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. 32 items for less (−3 years), average (4–8 years), and more (+9 years) experienced athletes.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Significant items for less (–3 years), average (4–8 years), and more (+9 years) experienced athletes.

A structural model with the 32 items that had the most significant impact (negative or positive) on performance and classified according to the five categories was used to previously evaluate the Field and Indoor Professional Stressors List’s factor structure. Analysis of the players’ responses leads us to believe that the 32 items of the list that had the most impact (negative or positive) on the performance can be explained by two distinct latent variables. The stress items were divided by category; however, within the same category, some items were perceived as eustress while others as distress (although the items were divided by category, it was not possible to conclude based only on the category which one generates stress or eustress). The Composite Reliability value was 0.88 for distress and 0.78 for eustress. Based on the assumption that there is a solution for the set of items with two global factors (distress and eustress) and specific factors (the categories in which the items were classified), a Bi-factorial Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling (BI-ESEM) was employed to assess the factor structure.

The adequacy of the model was assessed by the adjustment meanings Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR), Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI). According to the literature (Brown, 2015), RMSEA and SRMR values must be less than 0.08, CFI and TLI values must be above 0.90, or preferably 0.95.

The assumption of normality of the data was not satisfied using the Shapiro–Wilk test and the estimation methods were chosen according to the results of the Mardia’s coefficient for data that violate the assumption of multivariate normality (coefficient of multivariate kurtosis = 36.08). The method of extracting the Weighted Least Squares Adjusted by Average and Variance (WLSMV) was implemented in a polychoric data matrix, considering the ordinal nature of the data (Holgado-tello, 2015).

The model’s adjustment indexes are acceptable (Chi-Square = 354.77; df = 288; RMSEA = 0.041; CFI = 0.966; TLI = 0.945; SRMR = 0.059). However, the G-factors were not well defined by strong and significant loads (Table 6). For illustration purposes, we present the results of the path diagrams of model in Figure 5.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Standardized factorial loads of the model.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Path diagrams of model. Bifactor solution including global factors (eustress and distress) and specific factors (rel, ac, td, pf, and ep). Note that path diagram figures are only intended to be illustrative as providing detailed labels would make the diagrams too large to present.

In the Relationship with Significant People category, except for item 15, all the others had significant loads above 0.40 in the G-factor. In the Training Demand category, items 16, 32, 34, and 69 were better defined in the G-factor, and item 68 had an acceptable factor load in its respective factor. In the category Aspects of the Competition, items 21, 28, 35, 36, 37, 38, 59, and 63 showed high levels of specificity associated with the S-factor ranging between 0.45 and 0.75 and the other items (21, 19, and 26) presented significant loads, but below the desired level. In the Expectations about the Performance category, items 51 and 6 showed factorial loads of 0.51 and 0.64 in factors S and G, respectively. In the Personal Factors category, except for item 1, all items presented significant loads, however, weak in the respective S-factors, and were poorly defined in the G-factor.

Discussion

The aims of this study were six, including (a) develop a List of Stressors in Professional Indoor and Field Soccer to facilitate the identification of situations that can cause stress in professional indoor and field players, (b) confirm if the stressful situations can be perceived as both distress and eustress, (c) identify the situations perceived as distress and eustress, (d) check which items make up the five previously established categories, (e) examine the differences in the perception of the situations by age, and time as professional, and (f) assess the factor structure of the list.

The conception considered initially for elaborating the list of stressful situations postulated that the level of stress of a soccer player, indoor or field, is expressed by the intensity of impact on the performance caused by events in the athletes’ sportive lives. Another important point is that the events are not positive or negative per se. Still, these soccer events are objective occurrences experienced by the players with enough intensity to impact their performance. During training and competitions, soccer players face a series of requirements, called stressors, which can vary in terms of the content and intensity of their effect on sports performance. Nevertheless, training and competitions are only a part of the player’s sporting experience; according to Noblet and Gifford (2002), other events directly and indirectly related to them, such as family, contract negotiations, transfers, injuries, and rehabilitation, also need to be considered in the investigation of the sources of stress. So, the list was formed by situations that can be classified as (a) inherent in the competitive process, that is, that are directly part of the competition process, are related to the individual or the environment, and (b) adjacent to the competitive process, which happen in everyday life and are independent of the competitive process, such as family problems (De Rose Junior et al., 2004).

Our study confirms that soccer is a challenging environment composed of a spectrum of inherent and adjacent situations, which can impact the performance of the players in a positive (eustress) or negative (distress) way depending on how athletes perceive the situations. In summary, it refers to the extent to which soccer players label the intensity of the cognitive and somatic symptoms of stress experienced in a debilitating-facilitative continuum. This agrees with the studies made by Pargman (1986), Anshel (1990), Jones et al. (1990, 1993), Samulski and Chagas (1992, 1996), Jones and Swain (1995), Jones (1995b), Ntoumanis and Biddle (1998), Brandão et al. (2002), Aschbacher et al. (2013), Kung and Chan (2014), Hargrove et al. (2015), Madigan et al. (2017), McCormick et al. (2018); and have shown that it is not enough to analyze whether certain situations generate stress or not, but mainly, analyze the directional perception of the situations, that is, the nature of interpretation of situations in terms of having a positive or negative relationship with the subsequent performance.

Thus, it is not surprising that the stress phenomenon in sport has been considerably studied in the past decades since high performance sport is characterized by a demand to perform at optimal levels in often intense pressure situations that can interfere with the athletes’ actions, thoughts, and feelings (Brandão, 2000). By its nature, high-performance sport is highly competitive and invariably generates stress in athletes. To Scanlan et al. (1991), with rare exceptions, elite athletes experience stress during their long and arduous sports career to achieve sports excellence, and that would be extremely hard for an athlete to invest so much time and energy in such a challenging environment without feeling any stress or pressure.

However, there is one aspect to consider, as we can observe a large dispersion of responses in some situations (Table 2) which leads us to affirm that the player’s perception in the experience of a particular situation is not uniform, indicating that part of them experience the situation as a performance stimulus and another part as having a negative impact in the performance. These results confirm a classical study of Frester (1976) that considers that equal conditions are often experienced and psychically elaborated differently. This understanding leads to the need for an individual analysis of the subjective perception of the situations.

However, it is interesting to highlight the situations perceived negatively and positively by more than 50% of players (Table 3). The most intensely disturbing situations chosen by players showed firstly how the relationship with significant others can impair the performance of the players, which is in accordance with Ommundsen and Vaglum (1991), Horn (2008), Weinberg and Gould (2008), Kristiansen et al. (2012), and Chan et al. (2019), which studies showed that one of the main sources of negative stress in sport are the reprimands and criticism of the coaches. A poor relationship between the coach and their players, and between players among themselves, can influence the players’ cognitive, emotional, and behavioral processes and how it has consequences in the performance. One player explained that situation: “Under the command of T., the team lost 5 games in a row. Explosive and even fearless, he soon became incompatible with the team.”

Secondly is the decrements in performance that arise from playing under condition of injury or not being in a good shape. According to Hägglund et al. (2013), Foster et al. (2001), and Nobari et al. (2020) the workouts’ poorly intensity and volume, the inadequate total load of daily training, training monotony, strain, and accumulated fatigue can lead to injuries and poor physical conditions and interfere on the players’ perceived ability, resulting in feelings of pressure. But a special personal disturbance factor, coming into play injured, despite being considered a debilitating item, appears to be a routine for soccer players. A player’s narrative can show this situation: “I have suffered a sprain in my right ankle in a dispute with opposing defenders ten days ago, but even so I continued to play.” Conflictive situations like that can interfere with the player’s physical/psychic balance and game behavior, contribute to burnout and some kind of “hiding” behavior in practice (Semerci, 2019), and consequently, activity engagement is impaired when there are conflicts (Chan et al., 2019).

Thirdly is a fact inherent to soccer concerns to the ongoing or definitive score of a game. Losing from a special score seems like a difficult time that can decide future performance in games. Being imminent or real failures, these stressful situations during competitions provoke psychological disorders and may negatively affect the athlete’s performance (Bailey et al., 2010; Bennett and Maynard, 2017; Brown and Fletcher, 2017). According to Bransen et al. (2019) soccer players are often confronted with in-game situations that could affect their performance, which attest to the need to have a special look at the relationship between certain stressful situations and performance.

However, the most intensely stimulating situations showed that the possibility of success is the motivational key for a soccer athlete. Elite athletes indicate that perceiving success increases their desire to continue practicing the sport, the desire to try harder, and the actual level of effort employed. In this sense, the goal refers to the athlete’s ambition to achieve a certain sporting result. His pressure indicates performance expectations, pressure to meet the established goals, and “positive obligation” to win a certain game and achieve expected results. These agree with Scanlan et al.’s (1989) studies and is confirmed by a narrative of a player: “I am very critical of myself and I always want to play well. After the game, I analyze everything I did and try to get even better.” But it is interesting to note that according to Cohn (1990) and Woodman and Hardy (2001) from a cognitive point of view, the perception of pressure from the player to perform well implies great tension on him to the extent where the demands of the sport can exceed the rewards. Our results show the opposite: self-pressure items were evaluated as positive factors, therefore, a performance facilitator.

Specifically, in our study some stressors from the in-game demands have been linked with positive emotions. Traditionally in soccer it is expected that since a team plays in their own “home,” they will already have an advantage in terms of results because they have the support of the fans. Coaches and players believe in the advantage of playing at home (Snyder and Purdy, 1985; Arboix Alió et al., 2020), and about that one player said, “The pressure of the spectators at home usually influences the performance of the team, which falls in production when is not supported.” Diana et al. (2017) evaluated the game structure in different contexts (home and away) and showed that the game location strongly influences the game tactics. Another stressor, especially for Brazilian players, is related to positive emotion when playing a derby. Certain games are important by their own characteristics. Because of their impact, significant events have special meanings for players, and playing a derby is almost always associated with memorable memories (Brandão, 2000) as it involves situations with great expectation of performance, experienced as decisive. Beyond that, a “derby” in soccer is a match that mobilizes players, coaches, fans, and the press, even if its result does not interfere with the team’s position on the championship. The following assertion illustrates its importance: “A derby has a shirt, players, mystique and fans.”

It is well known in the literature that strength, power, and speed make a difference in the physical (and tactical) performance of soccer players. The determinant abilities in a soccer match involve high demands of speed, agility (change of direction without previous knowledge about the local where the change of direction would be done, involving decision making), and change of direction (in this case, the athletes know the exact time and local of the change of direction), as signaled by Polito et al. (2017). Furthermore, in soccer, speed is a physical problem, but it also involves decision making that will lead to movements. Thus, speed can influence the game’s outcome (Ekblom, 1995) and it is perceived by the players: “A player who has great speed, has a fundamental characteristic for the game tactical scheme.”

One aspect that was considered highly positive by the athletes was to know in advance that they are going to be titular. For Lachman (1974) stimulus that is unexpected or suddenly introduced can generate negative emotional reactions. Being warned that they will be a titular minutes before the game require them a quick adaptation and a mental preparation to the new situation according to the demands of unexpected situations: “The undefinition of the coach about who is going to play often brings a certain emotional instability to the group.” However, some coaches prefer to decide or disclose the squad at the time of the game, which can generate negative feelings and behaviors, as said one player: “When the player does not know if he will play, he has 2 thoughts, one positive and one negative. He thinks: if I am going to play, I pay attention to the pep talk before the game. But if he thinks: I can stay out, he doesn’t even care about the game. So, if he finds out in the locker room that he is going to play, it takes him by surprise.” According to Brandão (2000), ideally, the player should be informed in advance that he is going to play, so that he can prepare himself and have an appropriate mental tactic for the game. Besides, he should know his opponent’s level of performance, physical and mental skills and disabilities, and tactics: “It is always better to know in advance that you are going to play, you can prepare psychologically better for the game,” said one of the players.

Apitzsch (1995) stated that the perception of individuals from the external world is not objective but is influenced by subjective interpretations derived from past experiences. According to him, the experience is usually linear with age and with time as professional, meaning that physical, technical, tactical, and psychological skills should increase as a result of increased practice in training and competitions. Younger athletes with little competitive experience do not know the different moments and situations they will face. As a result, they tend to respond to stimuli from the environment differently from more experienced athletes, who have experienced identical or similar situations. Based on this premise, we sought to investigate the differences in the perception of stressful situations according to age and time as a professional, testing the hypothesis that there would be a difference due to these categorical variables. Older and more experienced athletes were expected to perceive situations as less negative to sports performance.

Our results first confirmed the hypothesis and are in accordance with studies conducted by Mahoney et al. (1987) who observed that the most experienced athletes had better concentration before and during competitions, were more self-confident, had a high capacity to recover from errors, experienced less stress before and during competitions, and interpreted anxiety as a facilitator unlike the less experienced (“This is my first derby against Palmeiras as a professional. I was sleepless in the face of such a responsibility”), who tended to interpret anxiety as more debilitating to performance (“Some players lost their emotional balance at the wrong time. Lack tranquility, calm, it’s a very young team”). Gould and Krane (1992) showed that experienced athletes have different standards when compared to less experienced athletes in terms of stress reactions; Régnier et al. (1993) studied those who have a systematic difference in how increasingly experienced athletes perceive and respond to different environmental stimuli; Brandão (2000), when assessing more experienced soccer players, noted that they evaluated competition situations as less unfavorable to performance than players with less experience. Evidence suggests that, based on previous experiences, more experienced athletes subjectively estimate the probabilities of events and react to them less negatively (“Insecurity only gets hold of the athlete when he doesn’t have enough experience to believe that he knows how to play”). The knowledge of the probability of the different events in the sports environment is for Abernethy (1993) a great advantage for the most experienced athletes.

Identifying the antecedents of stress in sport has been an important area of investigation for both theoretical and practical reasons (Ruiz et al., 2019). Familiarity with the categories of stress that are supposed to influence athletes’ emotions and can trigger negative or positive responses will clarify the concept of stress and identify which situations potentially depress performance and which one facilitates performance. Still, it will also help in planning the regime of physical, technical, and tactical training and the preparation of players for the competitive process. Furthermore, when stress categories are chronic negative this can result in “burnout,” lack of motivation, poor sports performance, and even abandonment of competitive sport (Anshel, 1990). A special aspect to be addressed at this point is how the perception of stressful situations also can be different depending on the player’s culture, as analyzed by Brandão et al. (2013). This perception opens an avenue of interesting studies for colleagues from other countries who want to assess the perception of stress in their players and compare it with the Brazilian players.

Related to the List of Stressors in Soccer’s factor-structure, although the model had satisfactory adjustment rates, not all items loaded the respective S-factor or G-factor as strongly as expected. Some of the estimated factor loads for this model proved to be insignificant, which is consistent with the nature of the two-factorial models, in which each item cannot realistically be assumed to have equally strong associations with global and specific factors (Morin et al., 2016). Specific patterns of significant versus non-significant loads may help us to interpret specific items and those that should be classified as stress and/or distress. Future research should look for ways to refine the items designed to improve stress assessment and test them on a large sample to increase this list’s significance.

In conclusion, the results revealed that Brazilian professional soccer players in this study experience various stressful situations. These events are important representations of environmental demands and could predict the performance as they are perceived as distress or eustress. Some of these stressful situations are inherent to the sport environment, and others are adjacent to the sports system or environment. Coach pressure to win and conflicts with teammates are examples of stressors within the team. Family problems and conflicts with press or fans are examples of stressors external to the team, also called peripheral opponents, and showed the relative social influence of significant others in soccer performance.

It is important to highlight that the List of Stressors in Professional Indoor and Field Brazilian Soccer was elaborated to fit a specific sport, soccer, indoor and field, and use it to observe the debilitating or facilitating character of stress factors. Knowing the directionality of a given stress factor has important practical implications in preparing athletes and helping them face the performance stressors that are part of the daily life of soccer. This knowledge can also help future athletes and coaches minimize the factors considered negative, which play a critical role in the appearance of psychological and psychosocial disorders and strengthen the positive ones, which significantly impact the players’ motivation.

In this sense, it is believed that the perception of stressful situations and the intensity they affect players behavior should be examined as the players’ set of psychosocial situations; individual experiences in terms of training, team’s social relationships, contract negotiations, player transfers, in addition to competitive and non-competitive aspects and the team’s physical, social, and cultural factors in which the player plays, their infrastructure, media attention, and player support. Furthermore, in a broader sense, in the historical and contemporary contexts in which the athletes are involved, they cannot be satisfied only with this knowledge, but rather convert it into practical training for stress control.

An important delimitation for this study is the following: the small number of futsal players in relation to soccer players, due to the pandemic, so it was not possible to test the hypothesis that there would be no difference in the perception of stress due to this categorical variable.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Comitê de ética em pesquisa Universidade São Judas Tadeu. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MRFB and LFP conceived the original idea of the study. VH, MC, APM, DO, AO, and LM selected the researches and contributed to data processing and analysis. DA analyzed and presented the data. MRFB, DA, and MVBJ wrote and organized the manuscript. All authors reviewed the manuscript and approved the final version for submission.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Special thanks to all teams and players that made possible this research.

References

Abernethy, B. (1993). “Attention,” in Handbook of Research on Sport Psychology, eds R. N. Singer, M. Murphey, and L. K. Tennant (New York, NY: Macmillan).

Google Scholar

Anshel, M. H. (1990). Toward validation of a model for coping with acute stress in sport. Int. J. Sports Psychol. 21, 58–83.

Google Scholar

Apitzsch, E. (1994). La personalidad del jugador de fútbol de élite. Rev. Psicol. Deporte 3, 89–99.

Google Scholar

Apitzsch, E. (1995). “Psychodynamic theory of personality and sport performance,” in European Perspectives on Exercise and Sport Psychology, ed. S. J. H. Biddle (Wallingford: CABI), 111–127.

Google Scholar

Arboix Alió, J., Aguilera-Castells, J., Trabal Taña, G., and Sánchez-Lopez, M. J. (2020). Comparison of home advantage in men’s and women’s Portuguese roller hockey league. Cuad. Psicol. Deporte 20, 181–189. doi: 10.6018/cpd.363041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arens, A. K., and Morin, A. J. (2017). Improved representation of the self-perception profile for children through bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling. Am. Educ. Res. J. 54, 59–87. doi: 10.3102/0002831216666490

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aschbacher, K., O’Donovan, A., Wolkowitz, O. M., Dhabhar, F. S., Su, Y., and Epel, E. (2013). Good stress, bad stress and oxidative stress: insights from anticipatory cortisol reactivity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 1698–1708. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, C. M., Samples, H. L., Broshek, D. K., Freeman, J. R., and Barth, J. T. (2010). The relationship between psychological distress and baseline sports-related concussion testing. Clin. J. Sport Med. 20, 272–277. doi: 10.1097/JSM.0b013e3181e8f8d8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bennett, J., and Maynard, I. (2017). Performance blocks in sport: recommendations for treatment and implications for sport psychology practitioners. J. Sport Psychol. Action 8, 60–68. doi: 10.1080/21520704.2016.1227414

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandão, M. R., Valdés, H. M., and González, E. (2002). Estrés en jugadores de fútbol: una comparación Brasil y Cuba. Cuad. Psicol. Dep. 2, 7–14.

Google Scholar

Brandão, M. R. F., Leite, G., Ferreira, R. A., Gomes, S. S., Pedrinelli, V. J., and Oliveira, R. S. (2013). Percepção do estresse competitivo e cultura em futebolistas de três países participantes do Mundial de 2006: Brasil, Portugal e Arábia Saudita (Perception of competitive stress and culture in professional soccer players of Brazil, Portugal and Saudi Arabia). Motricidade 8, 925–936.

Google Scholar

Brandão, M. R. F. (2000). Fatores de Estresse em Jogadores de Futebol Profissional. (Stress Factors in Professional Soccer Players). Doctoral’s Thesis. São Paulo, SP: University of Campinas.

Google Scholar

Bransen, L., Robberechts, P., Van Haaren, J., and Davis, J. (2019). “Choke or Shine? Quantifying soccer players’ abilities to perform under mental pressure,” in Proceedings of the 13th MIT Sloan Sports Analytics Conference, Berlin, 1–25.

Google Scholar

Brown, D. J., and Fletcher, D. (2017). Effects of psychological and psychosocial interventions on sport performance: a meta-analysis. Sports Med. 47, 77–99. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-0552-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. (2015). Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Guilford Publications.

Google Scholar

Chan, D. K., Keegan, R. J., Lee, A. S., Yang, S. X., Zhang, L., Rhodes, R. E., et al. (2019). Toward a better assessment of perceived social influence: the relative role of significant others on young athletes. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 29, 286–298. doi: 10.1111/sms.13320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohn, P. J. (1990). An exploratory study on sources of stress and athlete burnout in youth golf. Sport Psychol. 4, 95–106. doi: 10.1123/tsp.4.2.95

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Rose, D. Jr., Sato, T. C., Selingardi, D., Bettencourt, E. L., Barros, J. C. T. S., and Ferreira, M. C. M. (2004). Situações de jogo como fonte de “stress” em modalidades esportivas coletivas. Rev. Bras. Educ. Fís. Esporte 18, 385–395. doi: 10.1590/S1807-55092004000400007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Rose Junior, D., and Vasconcellos, E. G. (1993). Situações de “stress” específicas do basquetebol. Rev. Paulista Educ. Física 7, 25–34.

Google Scholar

Diana, B., Zurloni, V., Elia, M., Cavalera, C. M., Jonsson, G. K., and Anguera, M. T. (2017). How game location affects soccer performance: T-pattern analysis of attack actions in home and away matches. Front. Psychol. 8:1415. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dohrenwend, B. P. (2006). Inventorying stressful life events as risk factors for psychopathology: toward resolution of the problem of intracategory variability. Psychol. Bull. 132:477. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekblom, B. (1995). Football (Soccer). Londres: Blackwell Scientific Publications.

Google Scholar

Fletcher, D., and Hanton, S. (2003). Sources of organizational stress in elite sports performers. Sport Psychol. 17, 175–195. doi: 10.1123/tsp.17.2.175

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folkman, S., and Lazarus, R. S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company.

Google Scholar

Foster, C., Florhaug, J. A., Franklin, J., Lori, G., Lauri, A. H., Suzanne, P., et al. (2001). Anewapproach to monitoring exercise training. J. Strength Cond. Res. 15, 109–115. doi: 10.1519/00124278-200102000-00019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frester, R. (1976). “El “Test” del Sintoma de Carga: un procedimiento para el análisis de la elaboración de condiciones de carga psíquica en los desportistas,” in Aportes a la Psicologia Deportiva, ed. P. Kunath (Havana: Editorial Orbe), 1–14. doi: 10.33017/reveciperu2006.0010/

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia-Maás, A., Pajou, P., Gili, M., Ponseti, X., Borras, P. A., Vidal, J., et al. (2010). Commitment, enjoyment and motivation in young soccer competitive players. Spanish J. Psychol. 13, 609–616. doi: 10.1017/s1138741600002286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gould, D., and Krane, V. (1992). “The arousal-athletic performance relationship: current status and future directions,” in Advances in Sport Psychology, ed. Horn (Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Publishers).

Google Scholar

Hägglund, M., Waldén, M., Magnusson, H., Kristenson, K., Bengtsson, H., and Ekstrand, J. (2013). Injuries affect team performance negatively in professional football: an 11-year follow-up of the UEFA Champions League injury study. Br. J. Sports Med. 47, 738–742. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2013-092215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Hult, T. M., Ringle, C. M. E., and Sarstedt, M. (2014). A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage

Google Scholar

Hargrove, M. B., Becker, W. S., and Hargrove, D. F. (2015). The HRD eustress model: generating positive stress with challenging work. Hum. Resource Dev. Rev. 14, 279–298. doi: 10.1177/1534484315598086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holgado-tello, F. (2015). Confirmatory factor analysis: recommendations for unweighted least squares method related to Chi-Square and RMSEA Type I error. Accion Psicol. 12, 79–90. doi: 10.5944/ap.12.1.14362

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holzinger, K. J., and Swineford, F. (1937). The bi-factor model. Psychometrika 1, 41–54.

Google Scholar

Horn, T. S. (2008). Advances in Sport Psychology. Champaign: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Howard, J. L., Gagné, M., Morin, A. J., and Forest, J. (2018). Using bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling to test for a continuum structure of motivation. J. Manag. 44, 2638–2664. doi: 10.1177/0149206316645653

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, G. (1990). “A cognitive perspective on the processes underlying the relationship between stress and performance in sport,” in Stress and Performance in Sport, eds J. G. Jones and L. Hardy (Chichester: John Wilwy & Sons), 17–42.

Google Scholar

Jones, G. (1995b). More than just a game: research developments and issues in competitive anxiety in sport. Br. J. Psychol. 86, 449–478. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1995.tb02565.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, G. (1995a). “Anxiety and performance: research issues and developments,” in European Perspectives on Exercise and Sport Psychology, ed. S. Biddle (Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics), 128–153.

Google Scholar

Jones, G., and Swain, A. (1995). Predispositions to experience debilitative and facilitative anxiety in elite and nonelite performers. Sports Psychol. 9, 201–211. doi: 10.1123/tsp.9.2.201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, G., Swain, A., and Cale, A. (1990). Antecedents of multidimensional competitive state anxiety and self-confidence in elite intercollegiate middle-distance runners. Sports Psychol. 4, 107–118. doi: 10.1123/tsp.4.2.107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, G., Swain, A., and Hardy, L. (1993). Intensity and direction dimensions of competitive state anxiety and relationships with performance. J. Sports Sci. 11, 525–532. doi: 10.1080/02640419308730023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kristiansen, E., Murphy, D., and Roberts, G. C. (2012). Organizational stress and coping in U.S. Professional Soccer. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 24, 207–223. doi: 10.1080/10413200.2011.614319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kung, C. S. J., and Chan, C. K. Y. (2014). Differential roles of positive and negative perfectionism in predicting occupational eustress and distress. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 58, 76–81. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.10.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lachman, S. J. (1974). Distúrbios Psicossomáticos - Uma Interpretação Behaviorista. Rio de Janeiro: Livros Técnicos e Científicos Editora.

Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S. (2000). How emotions influence performance in competitive sports. Sport Psychol. 14, 229–252. doi: 10.1123/tsp.14.3.229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madigan, D. J., Stoeber, J., and Passfield, L. (2017). Perfectionism and training distress in junior athletes: a longitudinal investigation. J Sports Sci. 35, 470–475. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2016.1172726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahoney, M., Gabriel, T., and Perkins, T. (1987). Psychological skills and exceptional athletic performance. Sport Psychol. 1, 181–199. doi: 10.1123/tsp.1.3.181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Main, L., and Grove, J. R. (2009). A multi-component assessment model for monitoring training distress among athletes. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 9, 195–202. doi: 10.1080/17461390902818260

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Guo, J., Dicke, T., Parker, P. D., and Craven, R. G. (2020). Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM), and Set-ESEM: optimal balance between goodness of fit and parsimony. Multiv. Behav. Res. 55, 102–119. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2019.1602503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, A., Meijen, C., and Marcora, S. (2018). Effects of a motivational self-talk intervention for endurance athletes completing an ultramarathon. Sport Psychol. 32, 42–50. doi: 10.1123/tsp.2017-0018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mellalieu, S. D., Neil, R., Hanton, S., and Fletcher, D. (2009). Competition stress in sport performers: stressors experienced in the competitive environment. J. Sports Sci. 27, 729–744. doi: 10.1080/02640410902889834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morin, A., Tran, A., and Caci, H. (2016). Factorial validity of the ADHD adult symptom rating scale in a french community sample: results from the ChiP-ARD study. J. Attent. Disord. 20, 530–541. doi: 10.1177/1087054713488825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsch, J. R. (1981). Stress: Theorien, Untersuchungen, Massnahmen. Bern: Hogrefe.

Google Scholar

Nobari, H., Tubagi Polito, L. F., Clemente, F. M., Pérez-Gómez, J., Ahmadi, M., and Garcia-Gordillo, M. Á, et al. (2020). Relationships between training workload parameters with variations in anaerobic power and change of direction status in elite youth soccer players. Int. J. Environ. Res. Publ. Health 17, 01–15. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17217934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noblet, A. J., and Gifford, S. M. (2002). The sources of stress experienced by professional Australian footballers. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 14, 1–13. doi: 10.1080/10413200209339007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ntoumanis, N., and Biddle, S. (1998). The relationship between competitive anxiety, achievement goals, and motivational climates. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 69, 176–187. doi: 10.1080/02701367.1998.10607682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olmedilla, A., Moreno-Fernández, I. M., Gómez-Espejo, V., Robles-Palazón, F. J., Verdú, I., and Ortega, E. (2019). Psychological intervention program to control stress in youth soccer players. Front. Psychol. 10:2260. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ommundsen, Y. N. G. V. A. R., and Vaglum, P. (1991). Soccer competition anxiety and enjoyment in young boy players. the influence of perceived competence and significant others’ emotional involvement. Int. J. Sports Psychol. 2235–49.

Google Scholar

Parfitt, C. G., Jones, G., and Hardy, L. (1990). “Multidimensional anxiety and performance,” in Stress and Performance in Sport, eds G. Jones and L. Hardy (Chichester, IL: John Wiley), 43–80.

Google Scholar

Pargman, D. (1986). Stress and Motor Performance: Understanding and Coping. New York, NY: Mouvement Publications.

Google Scholar

Polito, L. F. T., Figueira, A. J. Jr., Miranda, M. L. J., Chtourou, H., Miranda, J. M., and Brandão, M. R. F. (2017). Psychophysiological indicators of fatigue in soccer players: a systematic review. Sci. Sports 32, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.scispo.2016.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poulus, D., Coulter, T. J., Trotter, M. G., and Polman, R. (2020). Stress and coping in esports and the influence of mental toughness. Front. Psychol. 11:628. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Régnier, G., Salmela, J., and Russell, S. J. (1993). “Talent detection and development in sport,” in Handbook of Research on Sport Psychology, eds R. Singer, M. Murphey, and L. K. Tennant (Nova Jersey: John Wiley & Sons).

Google Scholar

Robinson, A. M. (2018). Let’s talk about stress: history of stress research. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 22, 334–342. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruiz, M. C., Robazza, C., Tolvanen, A., Haapanen, S., and Duda, J. L. (2019). Coach-created motivational climate and athletes’ adaptation to psychological stress: temporal motivation-emotion interplay. Front. Psychol. 10:617. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samulski, D., and Chagas, M. H. (1992). Análise do stress na competição em jogadores de futebol de campo das categorias infantil e juvenil (15-18 anos). Rev. Brasil. Ciênc. Movim. 4, 12–18. doi: 10.18511/rbcm.v6i4.224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samulski, D., and Chagas, M. H. (1996). Análise do estresse psíquico na competição em jogadores de futebol de campo das categorias juvenil e júnior. Rev. Assoc. Prof. Educ. Física Londr. 11, 03–11.

Google Scholar

Scanlan, T. K., Ravizza, K., and Stein, G. L. (1989). An in-depth study of former elite figure skaters: I. Introduction to the project. J. Sports Exerc. Psychol. 11, 54–64. doi: 10.1123/jsep.11.1.54

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scanlan, T. K., Stein, G. L., and Ravizza, K. (1991). An in-depth study of former elite figure skaters: III. Sources of stress. J. Sports Exerc. Psychol. 13, 103–120. doi: 10.1123/jsep.13.2.103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E. P., and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: an introduction. Am. Psychol. 55, 5–14. doi: 10.1037//0003-066X.55.1.5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selye, H. (1976). “Stress without distress,” in Psychopathology of Human Adaptation, 137–146. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4684-2238-2_9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semerci, A. B. (2019). Examination of knowledge hiding with conflict, competition and personal values. Int. J. Conf. Manag. 30, 111–131. doi: 10.1108/IJCMA-03-2018-0044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, E. E., and Purdy, D. A. (1985). The home advantage in collegiate basketball. Sociol. Sport J. 2, 352–356. doi: 10.1123/ssj.2.4.352

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, A., and Jones, G. (1993). Intensity and frequency dimensions of competitive state anxiety. J. Sports Sci. 11, 533–542. doi: 10.1080/02640419308730024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teipel, D. (1993). “Evaluations of environmental and game-related stress conditions in soccer players,” in Proceedings of the 8th World Congress of Sport Psychology, eds S. Serpa, J. Alves, V. Ferreira, and A. P. Brito (Lisboa: ACTAS), 312–314.

Google Scholar

Teipel, D., Matsumoto, M., Sugiyama, Y., and Uemukai, K. (1994). Attitudes towards specific stress conditions in Japanese soccer players. Bull. Inst. Health Sport Sci. Univ. Tsukuba 17, 115–122.

Google Scholar

Thelwell, R. C., Weston, N. J., Greenlees, I. A., and Hutchings, N. (2008). Stressors in elite sport: a coach perspective. J. Sports Sci. 26, 905–918. doi: 10.1080/02640410801885933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urhausen, A., Gabriel, H. H. W., Weiler, B., and Kindermann, W. (1998). Ergometric and psychological findings during over-training: a long-term follow-up study in endurance athletes. Int. J. Sports Med. 19, 114–120. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-971892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

VanYperen, N. W. (1994). Estrés psicosocial, apoyo parental, y rendimiento en jóvenes futbolistas con talento. Rev. Psicol. Dep. Dossier 6, 119–138.

Google Scholar

Weinberg, R., and Gould, D. (2008). Fundamentos da Psicologia do Esporte e do Exercício. Porto Alegre: Artmed.

Google Scholar

Woodman, T., and Hardy, L. (2001). A case study of organizational stress in elite sport. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 13, 207–238. doi: 10.1080/104132001753149892

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zahariadis, P., Tsorbatzoudis, H., and Alexandris, K. (2006). Self-determination in sport commitment. Percept. Motor Skills 102, 405–420. doi: 10.2466/pms.102.2.405-42

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: eustress and distress, perception, performance, soccer (football), stress

Citation: Brandão MRF, Polito LF, Hernandes V, Correa M, Mastrocola AP, Oliveira D, Oliveira A, Moura L, Junior MVB and Angelo D (2021) Stressors in Indoor and Field Brazilian Soccer: Are They Perceived as a Distress or Eustress? Front. Psychol. 12:623719. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.623719

Received: 30 October 2020; Accepted: 24 March 2021;
Published: 20 May 2021.

Edited by:

Cesar Mendez, Rey Juan Carlos University, Spain

Reviewed by:

Kirsten Spencer, Auckland University of Technology, New Zealand
Roberto Ruiz Barquín, Autonomous University of Madrid, Spain

Copyright © 2021 Brandão, Polito, Hernandes, Correa, Mastrocola, Oliveira, Oliveira, Moura, Junior and Angelo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maria Regina Ferreira Brandão, mrfbrandao@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.