Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 14 April 2021
Sec. Comparative Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Domestication and Evolution in Dogs: Current Issues and Questions That Remain View all 5 articles

Two Ends of the Leash: Relations Between Personality of Shelter Volunteers and On-leash Walking Behavior With Shelter Dogs

  • 1School of Veterinary Science, University of Queensland, Gatton, QLD, Australia
  • 2Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
  • 3School of Mathematical and Physical Sciences, University of Newcastle, Callaghan, NSW, Australia
  • 4School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD, Australia
  • 5Curtin University Sustainable Policy (CUSP) Institute, Bentley, WA, Australia

Human personality influences the way people interact with dogs. This study investigated the associations between the personality of animal shelter volunteers and behavior during on-leash walks with shelter dogs. Video recording and a canine leash tension meter were used to monitor the on-leash walking. Personality was measured in five dimensions (neurotic, extroverted, open, agreeable and conscientious) with the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI). Neurotic volunteers pulled the leash harder and tended to interact with dogs using more body language; dogs being walked by neurotic volunteers in turn displayed more lip-licking and body shaking and were more likely to be rated as well-behaved. Extroverted volunteers were associated with stronger maximal leash tension at both the human and dog ends of the leash, and they praised the dog more, often in a high pitched voice. These volunteers eliciting more tail-wagging and body shaking by the dog. Extroverted volunteers were also more tolerant of different dog behaviors. Volunteers with personalities characterized by “openness to experiences” were less likely to verbally attract the attention of dogs, praise dogs and talk to them in a high-pitched voice; however, dogs walked by these volunteers were more likely to pull on the leash, and engaged in more lip-licking but less sniffing. “Agreeable” volunteers liked to verbally attract the attention of the dogs and more commonly initiated hand gestures and physical contact, causing the dogs to pull less frequently; dogs in these dyads displayed more gazing and lip-licking behaviors. Conscientious volunteers were less likely to pull the leash and tended to have more physical contact with the dogs but did not favor verbal communication and did not use a high pitched voice.

Introduction

Dogs (Canis familiaris) show high sociability toward humans due to domestication and artificial selection, which enables them to share close relationships with us (Wheat et al., 2019). There are ~4,759,700 dogs in Australia, and 3,555,000 Australian households (38.5%) own at least one dog (Animal Medicines Australia, 2016). In the U.S. and U.K., there are an estimated 77,000,000 (American Veterinary Medical Association, 2018) and 9,000,000 (Pet Food Manufacturers' Association, 2019) pet dogs, respectively. Therefore, a thorough understanding of human-dog interactions is warranted, particularly in the fields of interspecific communicative skills (Hare and Tomasello, 2005), play (Rooney et al., 2000), stress modulation (Bergamascoa et al., 2010) and the gender differences in human-dog interaction (Aliabadi et al., 2011).

Despite the comprehensive literature on human-dog interactions, limited research has focused on the influence of human personality (Kis et al., 2012). Human personality is widely accepted to be associated with our perceptions and behaviors (Tasa et al., 2010; Kis et al., 2012), including the way we interact with dogs (Wedl et al., 2010; Kis et al., 2012; Cimarelli et al., 2016, 2017). The “similarity-attraction hypothesis,” which proposes that we share similar personality traits, physical attractiveness, and attitudes with our partners, has also been used to describe the owner-dog partnership (Turcsán et al., 2012). For instance, neurotic owners are more likely to own anxious dogs (Turcsán et al., 2012), and owners who score higher on psychoticism on the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire tend to own a dog of an allegedly “aggressive” breed, such as German shepherd or Rottweiler (Wells and Hepper, 2012). However, other studies have shown that dogs of neurotic owners were usually confident and friendly, with a lower corticoid reactivity (Kotrschal et al., 2009; Schöberl et al., 2012). One study even found no associations between dog and human personality (Walker, 2014). It is likely that the most satisfied owner-dog relationships occur between active owners and similarly active dogs (Curb et al., 2013).

It is expected that human personality influences the way people interact with dogs. Open and neurotic owners tend to use more gestural and verbal cues when commanding their dogs, while extroverted owners praise their dogs more often (Kis et al., 2012). Owners scoring high for openness score low for owner control (the tendency to use commands) (Cimarelli et al., 2017) but high on owner warmth (enthusiasm and prone to communicating with rather than commanding their dogs) (Cimarelli et al., 2016). Working dog handlers with high ratings for agreeableness tended not to clash with dogs and were less inclined to verbally correct them during the training (Payne et al., 2015). Owners scoring high for conscientiousness score lower on owner social support (inclined toward praising and petting) (Cimarelli et al., 2017). As for the dogs, they take longer to obey a command if owners score high on neuroticism (Kis et al., 2012). Dogs of owners scoring low on agreeableness, emotional stability, extraversion and conscientiousness are more likely to display owner-directed aggression, stranger-directed fear, and/or house soiling (Dodman et al., 2018; Gobbo and Zupan, 2020).

The majority of studies related to human personality and the human-dog dyad used owners and pet dogs (Kis et al., 2012; Turcsán et al., 2012; Cimarelli et al., 2017), thereby ensuring a relatively long-term relationship with a strong bond in most relationships. Similar studies have not been conducted in animal shelters (Walker, 2014), where dogs generally have had a shorter relationship and a weaker bond with shelter volunteers/personnel. Additionally, most of the studies were conducted when dogs were off leash (Kotrschal et al., 2009; Kis et al., 2012). Compulsory dog leash laws have been implemented worldwide to protect wildlife (Bowes et al., 2017), reduce disease transmission (Day et al., 2012), prevent dog attacks and dog involvement in traffic accidents on roads (Thompson, 1997; Klainbart et al., 2018). Therefore, training dogs to walk loosely on a leash is becoming increasingly important, because a tense leash may compress the trachea and damage their eyes through increased intraocular pressure (Pauli et al., 2006).

Equine rein tension meters have been developed to measure the force exerted on the reins during horse riding (Warren-Smith et al., 2007; Hawson et al., 2014). In our study, a similar concept was adopted to develop a custom-made leash tension meter that measures the leash tension when a dog is walked on a leash. This device also differentiates the direction (dog vs. human handler) of the pulling during the walk, using a three-axis accelerometer (Dewhirst et al., 2017; Shih et al., 2020a).

This study was part of a larger research project exploring the on-leash interaction between shelter dogs and volunteers. The project found that compared to men, women tended to engage in the interaction using more verbal commands, and language typically used with babies, while men were more inclined to have physical contact with dogs (Shih et al., 2020b). Dogs showed more stress-related behaviors (e.g., less time holding the tail in the high position, more frequent gazing and lip-licking) when interacting with men than women (Shih et al., 2020b). Other dog-related characteristics also affect the interaction, such as young (Shih et al., 2020a) and male (Shih et al., 2020b) dogs doing more pulling, more socialized dogs displaying more positive behavioral signals (tail in a high position and exploring the environment); thus volunteers utilized fewer verbal cues and body language when interacting with them (Shih et al., 2020c).

In this study, we focused on the relationships of human personality and the human-dog dyad when volunteers walked dogs on a leash. It was hypothesized that volunteers with higher scores on neuroticism would use more gestural and verbal cues (Kis et al., 2012) and pull harder on the leash, in order to have a better control over the dog (Kotrschal et al., 2009). In line with a previous study, volunteers scoring high on extraversion were expected to praise the dogs more (Kis et al., 2012), and those that were more open (Cimarelli et al., 2017) or agreeable (Payne et al., 2015) would pull the leash less frequently and allow dogs to explore without interfering with them by commanding or attempting to attract their attention. Frequency of dog pulling might increase in volunteers with more open personalities if they allowed the dogs to explore freely when walking (Cimarelli et al., 2017). Finally, agreeable volunteers may be more sensitive to the need of dogs and less inclined to compete with dogs, leading to less leash pulling (Payne et al., 2015).

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Human Research Ethics and Animal Ethics Committees (Approval numbers: 2018001570 and SVS/400/18, respectively), of The University of Queensland.

Study Site

The research was conducted at the Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals, Queensland (RSPCA, QLD) shelter. Dogs were housed individually in rows of adjacent kennels (1.8 m wide × 1.2 m long × 3 m high) indoors and were able to make visual but not physical contact with each other across the central passage. Every enclosure was furnished with a metal crate, a raised mattress, a water bowl and enrichment (e.g., rubber toys or cardboard boxes). Each dog was walked twice daily, once in the morning from 08:00 to 10:00 h, and once in the afternoon from 14:00 to 16:00 h. The walk started from the kennel and ended with return to the kennel, taking about 15 min to complete. Moreover, dogs had plenty of time interacting with shelter volunteers, staff and visitors. These interactions provided the dogs with abundant social stimuli and enrichment. In this study we only recorded the interaction when dogs were on the designated pathway, with dogs walked uni-directionally, so as not to be confounded by turning around. The time on the designated pathway was ~5 min. The pathway ground had several sections with different coverings to provide tactile and olfactory enrichment for the dogs. The first 40% was covered with gravel, followed by 20% on a concrete section, then 5% on wooden boards, and finally the last 35% was covered with earth. Equipment and infrastructure provided added stimulation and play including two bridges, two dog jumps, some tennis balls and some hanging plastic milk bottles.

Subjects

Dogs

This study investigated 370 walks from July to early October 2019, involving 111 shelter dogs and 74 volunteers. All participating dogs had to have been resident at the RSPCA, QLD, for at least a week to enable them to become accustomed to the living and walking areas. Dogs' walking behavior was categorized into four levels by RSPCA animal attendants who had been closely working with those dogs. Levels were assigned according to the ease of walking the dogs and was based on their performance during the daily walk. Level 1 dogs walked on a loose leash most of the time. Level 2 dogs pulled on the leash during the walk occasionally and had more undesirable behaviors than level 1 dogs. Level 3 dogs tended to pull on the leash fiercely due to excitement or timidity. Level 3+ dogs had severe behavioral issues, such as overt excitement or fearfulness; however, they were not believed to necessarily pull on the leash harder than level 3 dogs. Dogs with severe behavioral or medical issues that might affect the experiment were excluded from the study because of safety and welfare concerns. All included dogs had undergone an RSPCA behavioral assessment (Clay et al., 2019).

Volunteers

Volunteers were trained progressively in four stages, allowing them initially to walk level 1 dogs; at each learning stage they learnt how to walk the more challenging dogs. Volunteers could only walk level 1 dogs during their 1st month of volunteering and level 3+ dogs were allowed to be walked only by volunteers who were most experienced and had gone through a series of standardized training programs. Volunteers could only walk dogs that had the same or lower behavioral level than their training level. Dogs were assigned to volunteers by RSPCA staff for a daily walk based on each volunteer's training level.

Canine Leash Tension Meter

A custom-designed canine leash tension meter (sampling rate: 10 Hz; measuring range: 0–100 kg force; resolution: 100 g force) was commissioned for this project (RobacScience Australia) (Shih et al., 2020a). The device measured the force exerted on the leash and detected the direction of the pull (handler vs. dog). One end of the device had a metal handle to be held by the handler. The opposite end of the device had a stainless-steel eyebolt to allow a simple connection to a l.4 m long commercial dog leash (Rogz Snake Lead). A Windows® 10 personal computer program was written for data collection and real-time displays. Recorded data was processed using MATLAB® (MATLAB® and Statistics Toolbox Release 2018b, The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, Massachusetts, United States) (Shih et al., 2020a).

Measures

Participants completed a consent form for the research, and demographic and personality tests prior to participating in the study if they received the research information by email/poster, or after the experiment if they were approached by RSPCA staff and our researcher in person on the day. The personality test used for the study, the NEO Five-Factor Inventory, measures human personality in five factors: neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness and conscientiousness (McCrae and Costa, 2007). Neuroticism indicates a tendency to experience negative emotions, such as sadness, guilt, fear, and embarrassment. Extraversion represents a preference to be sociable and excited. Openness describes people's active imagination and their preference for variety. Agreeableness reflects individuals' orientations to people, displaying traits such as altruism, trusting, and wellness obtained from helping others. Conscientiousness includes a proclivity for impulse-control and self-discipline (Costa and McCrae, 1992).

Study Design

Volunteers who walked dogs were recruited via e-mail, poster, and direct recruitment by RSPCA staff and the lead researcher (H.-Y.S.). Prize draws for $20 RSPCA World for Pets vouchers were offered as incentives. Since there were very few level 1 dogs available during the research period, only volunteers with level 2 and above training levels were recruited. All participants had sufficient English proficiency to follow the research instructions and complete required documents. The research process was explained before the observational study and participants were informed on the research consent form that the overall study aim was to improve the interaction between volunteers and shelter dogs.

Dogs classified at the different levels were matched to volunteers of the right experience and training level by RSPCA staff. Dogs were walked on a designated pathway away from distractions at the shelter. Before every walk, the researcher held the leash tension meter vertically downward for 10 s without connecting it to the dog. The signals generated were later used to calibrate the recorded data using MATLAB®. The volunteer connected the leash to the dog's collar and harness in front of its chest via the leash tension meter. A laptop (Swift 3, Acer Inc., New Taipei City, Taiwan) was carried by the volunteer in a backpack for data collection, and a camera (GoPro Hero 7 Silver, GoPro®, California, United States) was mounted on the volunteer's head to record the interaction. Before walking, the volunteer was directed to pull the two ends of the device and hold the pull for 3 s by counting slowly “1, 2, 3.” This procedure was repeated three times, in order to synchronize the tension data with the video. During the walk, the volunteer was instructed not to touch the leash unless the dog became tangled. The researcher also recorded the walk using an i-Phone 7 (Apple Inc., California, United States) from 10 m behind. At the end of each walk, participants completed an exit questionnaire (Table 1) containing 13 questions about their perspective of the walk.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Exit questionnaire for volunteers (n = 74) following walking dogs (n = 111) on a designated route at RSPCA Queensland, requiring them to rate each question on a 5-point scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) (Shih et al., 2020b).

Data Analysis

Video Recordings of Dog and Human Behavior

Three hundred and sixty-eight (n = 368/370) videos were coded in their entirety with behavior observation software (Boris©, Friard and Gamba, 2016) using a continuous recording method. Two videos (n = 2/370) were unavailable due to technical problems. Canine behaviors (Table 2), human verbal cues (Table 3) and human body language (Table 4) were coded using ethograms developed based on previous research (Beerda et al., 1998; Palestrini et al., 2010; Kis et al., 2012; Cimarelli et al., 2016; Grainger et al., 2016; McGowan et al., 2018) and modified during practice sessions. Behaviors were coded as point or state events, these being the number of times an event was observed and the duration of the event, respectively. Videos were coded by the researcher who is a veterinarian and a certified dog trainer, trained by two senior Ph.D. students in the use of ethograms and the software. To blind the coder, video coding was completed prior to any analysis of human personality. Twenty percent of the videos were chosen at random and double-coded to check intra-rater reliability. The average Cohen's Kappa was 0.89.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Ethogram of canine behavior.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Ethogram of human verbal cues.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Ethogram of human body language.

Leash Tension Analysis

Thirty-one (n = 31/370) leash tension files were lost, which we believe to be because the metal handle and case used to contain the components blocked the signal in a certain orientation. This left 339 (n = 339/370) files for analysis (Shih et al., 2020a).

Leash tension and pulling directions were calculated using MATLAB® (MATLAB ®and Statistics Toolbox Release 2018b, The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, Massachusetts, United States). The start and end of each file were determined by matching the timestamps of video and the leash tension file and also by matching three signal peaks at the beginning of the walk with the three repeated “1, 2, 3” verbal cues counted by the volunteer. Data were interpolated in order to make the sample times evenly spaced. Tension was tarred by deducting the minimal value, which visually equaled the baseline value when the device was not connected to the dog, from all measured values. Peak and average tensions over the walk were calculated (Shih et al., 2020a).

A “pull event” was defined as a sharp peak of tension greater than the baseline tension, which corresponded to a sudden burst of pulling initiated by either the dog, the handler or both at the same time. A peak-finding algorithm was used to determine when “pull events” occurred, with 0.1% of the body weight force set as the threshold. An event started when the filtered tension exceeded the threshold and ended when either the filtered tension returned to less than the threshold or the sign of the filtered tension gradient changed from negative to positive (indicating the start of a new pull event). Additionally, the directional signal of the accelerometer during the sample immediately prior to the start of a “pull event” was used to determine the pulling direction.

Net maximal tension (NTmax), maximal tension by dog (DTmax) and handler (HTmax) were defined as the maximal tension throughout the walk, recorded for the dog and handler, respectively. Mean tension was calculated by averaging all tension peaks above the threshold. Net mean tension (NTmean), mean tension by dog (DTmean) and mean tension by handler (HTmean) were defined as the mean tension throughout the walk, recorded for the dog and handler, respectively. Dog pulling frequency (DPF) and handler pulling frequency (HPF) were calculated by dividing the number of pulling events caused by the dog and the handler, respectively, by the total walking time.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted using RStudio Version 1.2.1335 (R Core Team, 2019) with packages leaps (Miller, 2020), MASS (Venables and Ripley, 2002), car (Fox and Weisberg, 2019), carData (Fox et al., 2020), Matrix (Bates and Maechler, 2019), polycor (Fox, 2019), plyr (Wickham, 2011), psych (Revelle, 2020), ggpubr (Kassambara, 2020), and nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2020).

To describe the exit questionnaire, exploratory factor analysis was performed with 13 questions, which revealed 2 factors (Shih et al., 2020b). Negative question wording was deliberately used for one half of the questions; for these questions, reverse scores were used for the calculation of mean scores of the 2 factors. A human satisfaction factor (factor H, Cronbach's alpha = 0.88) indicated the handler's feelings about the walk. A higher factor H score indicated that the handler was more satisfied with the interaction. A walker's “perception of dog” factor (factor D, Cronbach's alpha = 0.83) represented the human's perception of the dog's behavior. A high factor D score indicated that the handler considered the dog well behaved, more supportive and being helped by the handler (Table 1).

The 370 interactions were not independent, as dogs were assigned to participants according to the training levels due to safety and welfare considerations. Also, dogs that had been staying in the shelter longer during the research period tended to be walked more often. Generalized linear mixed models were used for analysis to address the repeated measurements. To reduce the numbers of predictors, a bivariate generalized linear model was used to analyze each combination of outcome variables (leash tension, pulling frequency, dog and human behavior and exit questionnaire scores) and predictors (human and dog demographics, human personality, canine behavioral assessment). In the analysis of the exit questionnaire, apart from the above predictors, canine and human behaviors, HTmax, and HTmean were also entered as predictors. Along with these predictors, NT max and NT mean were included for the analyses of human and canine behaviors. Predictors with p < 0.2 (Bursac et al., 2008; Cecatto et al., 2015) in the bivariate generalized linear model and those that were logically expected to influence the outcome variables (human personality and canine behavioral assessment results), regardless of the p-value, were included in the generalized linear mixed model as fixed effects. Participants' ID number and dogs' ID number were entered as random effects. Regression analysis started with a full model, in which all candidate variables were defined as predictors of interest. To reduce the potential type I error, predictors with the highest p-values were then removed in a stepwise manner until the result of the model became consistent. In addition to assessing significance, the change in the Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC) was used to assess whether the model improved by entering or removing variables.

Outcome variables were manually transformed for statistical analyses to meet the following assumptions of generalized linear mixed models: (1) residual normality (assessed by observation of quantile–quantile plots), (2) normality of the random effects (assessed by observation of quantile–quantile plots), and (3) homogeneity of variance of residuals (confirmed with either Levene's Test or visual inspection of boxplots). The assumption of no collinearity between covariates was evaluated from variance inflation factors (VIF, ensuring that VIF<2) (Zuur et al., 2010).

This research was part of a larger research project that explored the behavioral interaction between shelter dogs and volunteers during walks. In this paper, emphasis is placed on how human personality influenced the behavioral interaction while shelter dogs were being walked on leash by volunteers. The effects of human gender (Shih et al., 2020b) and canine factors (Shih et al., 2020c) have been reported in previous articles. The effects of body size, body weight, age and the behavioral level of dogs were also reported (Shih et al., 2020a). Other human factors (e.g., working experience, volunteering time and frequency, history of dog ownership and educational level) will be reported separately.

Results

Demographics

This study involved 111 shelter dogs, including 58 (52.3%) females and 53 (47.7%) males, all gonadectomized (Shih et al., 2020b). Participants were 47 (63.5%) women, 26 (35.1%) men and 1 (1.4%) person self-nominating as the third gender (Shih et al., 2020b), with an average age of 28.26 (± 14.6) years. The mean scores of the five personality traits were 24.97 (± 8.80, neuroticism), 27.24 (± 7.77, extraversion), 29.59 (± 6.41, openness), 34.35 (± 5.97, agreeableness) and 30.54 (± 7.23, conscientiousness) (Table 5). Compared to the general population, the median score (but not the mean score) of neuroticism of participants is at the average-high level. Fourteen men (8 were high and 6 were very high) and 22 women (14 were high and 8 were very high) had neurotic scores higher than the average of the reported norms. Scores of other personality dimensions all fell within the average range (McCrae and Costa, 2007).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Minimum, quartile 1 (Q1), median, mean (± SD), quartile 3 (Q3), and maximum scores or five personality traits.

Human Personality and Leash Tension

Neuroticism was positively correlated with maximal net tension (p = 0.003), maximal tension created by the dog (p = 0.014) and maximal (p = 0.0293) and mean (p = 0.0425) tension created by the human. Extraversion was positively correlated with maximal net tension (p = 0.036), maximal tension created by the dog (p = 0.021) and maximal (p = 0.041) tension created by the human. Openness was positively correlated (p = 0.0031), and agreeableness negatively correlated (p = 0.044) with dog pulling frequency. Conscientiousness was negatively correlated with human's pulling frequency (p = 0.039) (Table 6).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Generalized linear mixed model of the effect of human personality on leash tension and pulling frequency.

Human Personality and Human Behavior

The extraversion score was positively correlated with the frequencies of praise (p = 0.027) and high-pitched voice (p < 0.001). The openness score was negatively correlated with the frequencies of total verbal cues (p = 0.0039), attention seeking (p < 0.001), praise (p < 0.001) and high-pitched voice (p = 0.016). The agreeableness score was positively correlated with the frequency of attention seeking (p = 0.04). Finally, the conscientiousness score was negatively correlated with frequencies of communication (p < 0.001) and high-pitched voice (p = 0.035) (Table 7).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Generalized linear mixed model of the effect of human personality on human verbal cues during the walk.

With respect to body language, volunteers who scored high on neuroticism (p = 0.0094), extraversion (p = 0.038) and agreeableness (p < 0.001) interacted with dogs using a higher frequency of total body language. Specifically, a high frequency of hand gestures was observed when the participant scored higher on neuroticism (p = 0.016) and agreeableness (p = 0.0083); a high frequency of physical contact was seen in people scoring high on neuroticism (p = 0.0078), agreeableness (p = 0.0025) and conscientiousness (p = 0.0091) (Table 8). Finally, there was no significant correlation between human personality and the success rate of asking dogs to sit.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Generalized linear mixed model of the effect of human personality on human body languages during the walk.

Human Personality, Canine Behavior, and Walking Experience

The score of neuroticism was positively correlated with the frequency of dogs' shaking (p < 0.001). A high percentage of time spent tail wagging (p = 0.047) and a high frequency of shaking (p = 0.0017) were observed in dogs while interacting with more extravert volunteers. When interacting with volunteers scoring high on openness, dogs presented a high frequency of lip-licking (p = 0.012) but lower percentage of time spent sniffing (p = 0.017). A high score for volunteers' agreeableness was associated with dogs with a high frequency of gaze (p = 0.0077) and lip-licking (p = 0.023) (Table 9). Finally, volunteers who scored high on neuroticism (p = 0.039) and extraversion (p = 0.0079) were more likely to rate higher on factor D (Table 10).

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Generalized linear mixed model of the effect of human personality on canine behavior during the walk.

TABLE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. Generalized linear mixed model of the effect of human personality on volunteers' walking experience.

Leash Tension, Human Behavior, and Walking Experience

Net maximal tension of the leash was positively correlated with the frequency of total verbal cues (p = 0.035) and commands (p < 0.001). However, net mean tension of the leash was negatively associated with the frequency of total verbal cues (p = 0.027), use of a high-pitched voice (p = 0.017) and commands (p = 0.0011) (Appendix Table 1).

The frequency of using negative verbal cues was negatively correlated with the Factor D score (p = 0.046) while the frequency of physical contact was positively related to the Factor H score (p = 0.038). The mean leash tension created by dog was negatively associated with the factor H (p = 0.0066) and factor D (p = 0.011) score (Appendix Table 2).

Discussion

Neuroticism

Volunteers scoring high on neuroticism pulled the leash harder during the walk, which may be related to the frequent body language used by more neurotic people (Kis et al., 2012). Additionally, mothers with increased neuroticism are more assertive and less adaptive, often using a more controlling or forceful style in discipline contexts (Clark et al., 2000). Similarly, owners scoring high on neuroticism tend to use more gestural and verbal commands when asking their dogs to sit (Kis et al., 2012). However, in our study, volunteers who scored high on neuroticism did not use more verbal cues during walks, although they did use more body language (e.g., hand gestures and physical contacts). These inconclusive outcomes may result from the different nature of the human-dog partnerships in this study. Dogs of owners high in neuroticism obeyed commands with a longer latency (Kotrschal et al., 2009; Kis et al., 2012). In another study regarding the personality effect on human-dog working tasks revealed no correlation between neuroticism and the human-dog dyad in working tasks (Hoummady et al., 2016). In our study, the success rate of asking the dog to sit was not associated with human personality, although volunteers were not specifically instructed to ask the dog to sit during the walk.

When walking with people scoring high on neuroticism, dogs licked their lips and shook their body more frequently, which may be a potential indicator of stress (Kogan et al., 2012). This assumption can be further supported by the high leash tension that neurotic volunteers exerted, and the high maximal tension dogs responded with. However, a previous study found that dogs with owners high in neuroticism were more confident and friendly (Kotrschal et al., 2009). Physiologically, dogs with owners scoring high in neuroticism often have a low cortisol level (Schöberl et al., 2012, 2017; Sundman et al., 2019). This indicates that dogs may be less stressed when interacting with neurotic owners because they tend to perceive their dogs as social support and stay in closer proximity with their dogs, and such intimacy may dampen the canine stress level (Schöberl et al., 2012, 2017; Sundman et al., 2019), which is supported by our finding that neuroticism is positively correlated with physical contact initiated by handlers. Therefore, the high incidence of lip-licking of dogs when interacting with more neurotic volunteers may be interpreted as appeasement signals, which are important components of greeting and peaceful intentions that occur commonly with reduced inter-individual distance (Firnkes et al., 2017). Moreover, appeasement signals are more commonly observed in mild but not overtly threatening situations and are believed to help manage stress (Firnkes et al., 2017). In shelters generally, dogs may be slightly uncertain but still enjoy the interaction with neurotic volunteers (Glenk et al., 2014).

Extraversion

Volunteers' extraversion was positively correlated with the leash tension from the human, in line with the more assertive and disciplinary parental style observed in more extraverted mothers (Clark et al., 2000). In line with previous research, our results revealed that more extravert volunteers praised dogs more often (Kis et al., 2012) and their conversations were more likely to be at a higher pitch (Imhof, 2010), which in our study was exemplified by talking to dogs with an excited high-pitched voice. They also tended to communicate with dogs using more body language generally. Since extravert people often find social stimuli to be rewarding (Wu et al., 2014), all these behaviors may be interpreted as a mean of actively engaging in social interaction with dogs.

Dogs increased maximal leash tension and kept their tails high more often when walking with extravert volunteers, potentially because dogs were aroused by the excited human praise and high-pitched voice, and were more engaged in the interaction with volunteers (McGowan et al., 2018). However, dogs might be mildly tense and anxious at the same time by more frequently shaking their bodies (Harper, 2011; Meyer and Forkman, 2014).

Openness

Owners with increased openness were reported to use more gestural and verbal cues when asking their dogs to sit (Kis et al., 2012). They also prefer communicating with, instead of commanding, their dogs (Cimarelli et al., 2016, 2017). However, in this study, volunteers scoring high on openness used fewer verbal cues, including fewer attention-seeking phrases, praises and a high-pitched voice, which again may be due to the short-term relationship between volunteers and shelter dogs. Our results agree with previous research about human personality and interpersonal interaction quality, that people with more openness tend to pay more attention but talk less (Berry and Hansen, 2000). Another possible explanation may be that open people respect the autonomy of dogs more (Booth-Butterfield and Sidelinger, 1997; Cimarelli et al., 2017), preferring not to control and command their dogs (Cimarelli et al., 2016, 2017). Although increased communication has not been observed in open volunteers (Cimarelli et al., 2016), they tended not to verbally interfere with the dogs' behaviors, such as by attracting their attention. This assumption is supported by the fact that when walking with more open volunteers, dogs pulled more frequently, and this was not as prominent at the human end.

More lip-licking was observed in dogs walked by more open volunteers, which may be considered as a similar outcome, an appeasement behavior, as interacting with more neurotic volunteers. However, since open volunteers verbally interacted with dogs less frequently, and dogs spent less time sniffing, an indicator of avoiding an uncomfortable conflict situation (Cohen, 2007), the high incidence of lip-licking was more likely to be a sign of mild discomfort (Harper, 2011) resulting from the frequent dog pulls when they were allowed to freely explore.

Agreeableness

Agreeableness refers to the characteristic and tendency of compliance, cooperation, altruism (Costa et al., 1991) and being more responsive to social cues (Wu et al., 2014). Working dog handlers scoring high on agreeableness were reported to use lower rates of verbal corrections and preferred to cooperate with their dogs (Payne et al., 2015). Similarly, more agreeable volunteers tended not to clash with dogs, and in response, when interacting with more agreeable volunteers, dogs did not need to frequently struggle and pull on the leash. Also, people with increased agreeableness enjoy lively social interactions (Wu et al., 2014), which may explain why more agreeable volunteers tended to attract the attention of dogs both verbally (e.g., attention seekers) and physically (e.g., hand gestures and physical contacts).

Conscientiousness

Conscientious mothers are more responsive to their children by being more sensitive to children needs, providing support and comfort and following their lead (Clark et al., 2000). Our result showed that handlers with a high score on conscientiousness pulled the leash less frequently. A possible explanation may be that, similarly, conscientious handlers sensitively captured and promptly responded to the dogs' signals. They respected autonomy of the dog and adjusted their own behavior to its current state or needs instead of attempting to correct the dog.

Cimarelli et al. found that owners scoring high on conscientiousness praise and pet their dogs less frequently (Cimarelli et al., 2017). However, our results revealed no significant correlation between volunteers' conscientiousness and verbal praise, and a positive association with physical contact. Such disagreements may demonstrate the difference in interspecific relationships between owners and pet dogs vs. volunteers and shelter dogs. Important components of conscientiousness refer to individuals' level of self-control and is related to competent communication and effectively evaluating responses to interpersonal problems (Hullman et al., 2010). Therefore, more conscientious volunteers might have perceived verbal communications as a less effective approach in human-dog interactions and preferred using non-verbal communications, such as physical contact (Mills, 2005). This is supported by previous research about interpersonal communication that the positive link between conscientiousness and communication might primarily be through writing but not verbal communication (Macht and Nembhard, 2015). Finally, a high voice and less emotional stability has been associated with decreased conscientiousness (Imhof, 2010), which explains why more conscientious volunteers were less inclined to talk to shelter dogs with an excited high-pitched voice.

Personality and Experience

Although extravert owners are more likely to return dogs to the shelter post-adoption (Walker, 2014), owners scoring high on extroversion mainly consider their dog as a companion for shared activities while owners who score highly in neuroticism view their dogs as social support and spend more time with them (Kotrschal et al., 2009). Similarly, we found that volunteers who scored highly on neuroticism and extraversion were both likely to feel supported by the dogs and liked to engage in more shared activities. Also, they were more likely to report that the dogs they interacted with were well-behaved. In a cat study, high owner agreeableness was associated with a high level of satisfaction with their cats (Finka et al., 2019). However, such a result was not found in our research, possibly due to the species difference.

Leash Tension, Human Behavior, and Experience

Volunteers talked to dogs more frequently, using more commands, when the net maximal tension was higher, probably because there was greater need for verbal commands when there were strong and sudden pulls. However, the opposite results were observed with fewer high-pitched voices and commands when the net mean tension was higher. In the scenario of higher mean tension, dogs were generally more determined and pulling, resulting in a lack of interaction between dogs and handlers.

Volunteers perceived the dog as less obedient and less supportive when they more frequently corrected the dog with negative verbal cues. However, volunteers were more satisfied with the interaction when there was more frequent physical contact between the dog and the human (Protopopova and Wynne, 2014). Finally, volunteers were less satisfied with the interaction and perceived the dog as less obedient and less supportive when the dog was causing a higher mean tension on the leash.

A limitation of this study was that dogs were not randomly matched with participants. Due to concerns for animal welfare and human safety, dogs were assigned to participants based on canine behavior and participants' experiences. Also, the cultural backgrounds of participated volunteers were unclear, which might potentially influence their interaction with dogs (Hood, 1998). Nevertheless, this article suggests that human personality can affect the human-dog interaction when walking on a leash.

Conclusions

This research provides a qualitative description of associations between human personality and the behavioral dyads that exist between volunteers and shelter dogs when walking on a leash. Neurotic volunteers pulled the leash harder during the walk and tended to interact with dogs using more body language; dogs in turn displayed more lip-licking and body shaking. Extroverted volunteers were associated with stronger maximal leash tension at both the human and dog ends, and they praised the dog more, often accompanied with a high-pitched voice. This elicited more tail-wagging and body shaking by the dog. Open volunteers were less likely to verbally attract the attention of dogs, praise the dogs and talk to them with a high-pitched voice; however, dogs were more likely to pull on the leash, accompanied by more lip-licking but less sniffing. Agreeable volunteers liked to verbally attract the attention of dogs and initiate more hand gestures and physical contact, causing the dogs to pull less frequently, often coupled with more gazing and lip-licking behaviors during the walk. Conscientious volunteers were less likely to pull the leash and tended to have more physical contact with dogs, but they did not prefer verbal communication and a high-pitched voice.

In this study, dogs were more likely to be rated as well-behaved when walking with neurotic and extroverted volunteers. A more satisfactory human-dog relationship was detected if the owner and dog shared a similar activity level (Curb et al., 2013), while a mismatch between owner and dog personality increased the likelihood of dogs developing behavior issues (Dubé et al., 2020). Our study might contribute to a better matching of volunteers and dogs in animal shelters to improve the working experience and the welfare of dogs. Also, it may be used to better pair potential owners and dogs for more satisfying partnerships.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Queensland Science, Low & Negligible Risk Ethics Sub-Committee. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin. The animal study was reviewed and approved by Animal Ethics Unit, Office of Research Ethics, The University of Queensland.

Author Contributions

H-YS, CP, MP, NP, LM, and FG: conceptualization. H-YS, CP, FG, MP, NP, and LM: methodology. FG and H-YS: software and writing—original draft preparation. H-YS, FG, and CP: validation, formal analysis, and data curation. H-YS: investigation. MP and CP: resources. H-YS, CP, MP, NP, and LM: writing—review and editing. H-YS and CP: visualization and funding acquisition. CP, MP, NP, and LM: supervision. H-YS, CP, and MP: project administration. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded by the Higher Degree Research Fund of University of Queensland and the RSPCA Fund donated by philanthropists.

Conflict of Interest

MP was employed as the principal scientist by RSPCA, QLD.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Authors would like to thank RSPCA, QLD for allowing us to conduct this research at the shelter. We also appreciate assistances of all shelter dogs, volunteers and RSPCA involved in this study. Finally, we acknowledge Solomon Woldeyohannes for statistical advice.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.619715/full#supplementary-material

References

Aliabadi, I., Wedl, M., Schoberl, I., Bauer, B., and Kotrschal, K. (2011). Effects of gender on performance in human-dog dyads in an agility parcours. J. Vet. Behav. 6:57. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2010.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Veterinary Medical Association (2018). U.S. Pet Ownership Statistics. Available online at: https://www.avma.org/resources-tools/reports-statistics/us-pet-ownership-statistics (accessed January 12, 2021).

Animal Medicines Australia (2016). Pet Ownership in Australia | 2016. Available online at: https://animalmedicinesaustralia.org.au/report/pet-ownership-in-australia-2016/ (accessed October 20, 2020).

Bates, D., and Maechler, M. (2019). Matrix: Sparse and Dense Matrix Classes and Methods. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/Matrix/Matrix.pdf (accessed October 20, 2020).

Google Scholar

Beerda, B., Schilder, M. B. H., van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., de Vries, H. W., and Mol, J. A. (1998). Behavioural, saliva cortisol and heart rate responses to different types of stimuli in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 58, 365–381. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(97)00145-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergamascoa, L., Osella, M. C., Savarino, P., Larosa, G., Ozella, L., Manassero, M., et al. (2010). Heart rate variability and saliva cortisol assessment in shelter dog: human–animal interaction effects. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 125, 56–68. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2010.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, D. S., and Hansen, J. S. (2000). Personality, nonverbal behavior, and interaction quality in female dyads. Per. Soc. Psych. Bull. 26, 278–292. doi: 10.1177/0146167200265002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Booth-Butterfield, M., and Sidelinger, R. J. (1997). The relationship between parental traits and open family communication: affective orientation and verbal aggression. Commun. Res. Rep. 14, 408–417. doi: 10.1080/08824099709388684

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowes, M., Keller, P., Rollins, R., and Gifford, R. (2017). The effect of ambivalence on onleash dog walking compliance behavior in parks and protected areas. J. Park Recreat. Admi. 35, 81–93. doi: 10.18666/JPRA-2017-V35-I3-7440

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bursac, Z., Gauss, C. H., Williams, D. K., and Hosmer, D. W. (2008). Purposeful selection of variables in logistic regression. Source Code Biol. Med. 3:17. doi: 10.1186/1751-0473-3-17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cecatto, S. B., Monteiro-Soares, M., Henriques, T., Monteiro, E., and Moura, C. I. F. P. (2015). Derivation of a clinical decision rule for predictive factors for the development of pharyngocutaneous fistula postlaryngectomy. Braz. J. Otorhinolaryngol. 81, 394–401. doi: 10.1016/j.bjorl.2014.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cimarelli, G., Turcsán, B., Bánlaki, Z., Range, F., and Virányi, Z. (2016). Dog owners' interaction styles: their components and associations with reactions of pet dogs to a social threat. Front. Psychol. 7:1979. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cimarelli, G., Turcsán, B., Range, F., and Virányi, Z. (2017). The other end of the leash: an experimental test to analyze how owners interact with their pet dogs. J. Vis. Exp. 128, 1–11. doi: 10.3791/56233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, L. A., Kochanska, G., and Ready, R. (2000). Mothers' personality and its interaction with child temperament as predictors of parenting behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 274–285. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.2.274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clay, L., Paterson, M., Bennett, P., Perry, G., and Phillips, C. (2019). Early recognition of behaviour problems in shelter dogs by monitoring them in their kennels after admission to a shelter. Animals 9:875. doi: 10.3390/ani9110875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, H.-Y. (2007). Calming signals. Vet. Nurs. J. 22, 26–28. doi: 10.1080/17415349.2007.11013596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, P. T. Jr. and McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-RTM) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

Google Scholar

Costa, P. T. Jr., McCrae, R. R., and Dye, D. A. (1991). Facet scales for agreeableness and conscientiousness: a revision of the NEO personality inventory. Pers. Individ. Differ. 12, 887–898. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(91)90177-D

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curb, L. A., Abramson, C. I., Grice, J. W., and Kennison, S. M. (2013). The relationship between personality match and pet satisfaction among dog owners. Anthrozoos 26, 395–404. doi: 10.2752/175303713X13697429463673

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Day, M. J., Breitschwerdt, E., Cleaveland, S., Karkare, U., Khanna, C., Kirpensteijn, J., et al. (2012). Surveillance of zoonotic infectious disease transmitted by small companion animals. Emerg. Infect. Dis. 18:e2. doi: 10.3201/eid1812.120664

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewhirst, O. P., Roskilly, K., Hubel, T. Y., Jordan, N. R., Golabek, K. A., McNutt, J. W., et al. (2017). An exploratory clustering approach for extracting stride parameters from tracking collars on free-ranging wild animals. J. Exp. Biol. 220, 341–346. doi: 10.1242/jeb.146035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodman, N. H., Brown, D. C., and Serpell, J. A. (2018). Associations between owner personality and psychological status and the prevalence of canine behavior problems. PLoS ONE 13:e0192846. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0192846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubé, M. B., Asher, L., Würbel, H., Riemer, S., and Melotti, L. (2020). Parallels in the interactive efect of highly sensitive personality and social factors on behaviour problems in dogs and humans. Sci. Rep. 10:5288. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-62094-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finka, L. R., Ward, J., Farnworth, M. J., and Mills, D. S. (2019). Owner personality and the wellbeing of their cats share parallels with the parent-child relationship. PLoS ONE 14:e0211862. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0211862

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Firnkes, A., Bartels, A., Bidoli, E., and Erhard, M. (2017). Appeasement signals used by dogs during dog-human communication. J. Vet. Behav. 19, 35–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2016.12.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, J. (2019). polycor: Polychoric and Polyserial Correlations. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/polycor/polycor.pdf (accessed October 20, 2020).

Fox, J., and Weisberg, S. (2019). An {R} Companion to Applied Regression. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Fox, J., Weisberg, S., and Price, B. (2020). carData: Companion to Applied Regression Data Sets. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/carData/carData.pdf (accessed October 20, 2020).

Friard, O., and Gamba, M. (2016). BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-loggingsoftware for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol. Evol. 7, 1325–1330. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.12584

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glenk, L. M., Kothgassner, O. D., Stetina, B. U., Palme, R., Kepplinger, B., and Baran, H. (2014). Salivary cortisol and behavior in therapy dogs during animal-assisted interventions: a pilot study. J. Vet. Behav. 9, 98–106. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gobbo, E., and Zupan, M. (2020). Dogs' sociability, owners' neuroticism and attachment style to pets as predictors of dog aggression. Animals 10:315. doi: 10.3390/ani10020315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grainger, J., Wills, A. P., and Montrose, V. T. (2016). The behavioral effects of walking on a collar and harness in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). J. Vet. Behav. 14, 60–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2016.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hare, B., and Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 439–444. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harper, S. (2011). Noise-related anxiety in dogs: improving management. Vet. Nurse 2, 378–386. doi: 10.12968/vetn.2011.2.7.378

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawson, L. A., Salvin, H. E., McLean, A. N., and McGreevy, P. D. (2014). Riders' application of rein tension for walk-to-halt transitions on a model horse. J. Vet. Behav. Clin App. Res. 9, 164–168. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hood, J. (1998). “Pet ownership and Asian multiculturalism,” in Proceedings of the Urban Animal Management Conferences in Australia (Perth, WA), 7–9.

Google Scholar

Hoummady, S., Péron, F., Grandjean, D., Cléro, D., Bernard, B., Titeux, E., et al. (2016). Relationships between personality of human–dog dyads and performances in working tasks. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 177, 42–51. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.01.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hullman, G. A., Planisek, A., McNally, J. S., and Rubin, R. B. (2010). Competence, personality, and self-efficacy: relationships in an undergraduate interpersonal course. Atl. J. Commun. 18, 36–49. doi: 10.1080/15456870903340506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Imhof, M. (2010). Listening to voices and judging people. Intl. J. List. 24, 19–33. doi: 10.1080/10904010903466295

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kassambara, A. (2020). ggpubr: 'ggplot2' Based Publication Ready Plots. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/ggpubr/ggpubr.pdf (accessed October 20, 2020).

Kis, A., Turcsán, B., Miklósi, Á., and Gácsi, M. (2012). The effect of the owner's personality on the behaviour of owner-dog dyads. Interact. Stud. 13, 373–385. doi: 10.1075/is.13.3.03kis

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klainbart, S., Bibring, U., Strich, D., Chai, O., Bdolah-Abram, T., Aroch, I., et al. (2018). Retrospective evaluation of 140 dogs involved in road traffic accidents. Vet. Rec. 182:196. doi: 10.1136/vr.104293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kogan, L. R., Schoenfeld-Tacher, R., and Simon, A. A. (2012). Behavioral effects of auditory stimulation on kenneled dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 7, 268–275. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2011.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotrschal, K., Schöberl, I., Bauer, B., Thibeaut, A.-M., and Wedl, M. (2009). Dyadic relationships and operational performance of male and female owners and their male dogs. Behav. Process. 81, 383–391. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macht, G. A., and Nembhard, A. D. (2015). Measures and models of personality and their effects on communication and team performance. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 49, 78–89. doi: 10.1016/j.ergon.2015.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R., and Costa, P. T. Jr. (2007). Brief versions of the NEO-PI-3. J. Individ. Differ. 28, 116–128. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001.28.3.116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGowan, R. T. S., Bolte, C., Barnett, H. R., Perez-Camargo, G., and Martin, F. (2018). Can you spare 15 min? The measurable positive impact of a 15-min petting session on shelter dog well-being. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 203, 42–54. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2018.02.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, I., and Forkman, B. (2014). Nonverbal communication and human–dog interaction. Anthrozoös 27, 553–568. doi: 10.2752/089279314X14072268687925

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, T. L. A. (2020). leaps: Regression Subset Selection. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/leaps/leaps.pdf (accessed October 20, 2020).

Mills, D. S. (2005). What's in a word? A review of the attributes of a command affecting the performance of pet dogs. Anthrozoös 18, 208–221. doi: 10.2752/089279305785594108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palestrini, C., Minero, M., Cannas, S., Rossi, E., and Frank, D. (2010). Video analysis of dogs with separation-related behaviors. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 124, 61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2010.01.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pauli, A. M., Bentley, E., Diehl, K. A., and Miller, P. E. (2006). Effects of the application of neck pressure by a collar or harness on intraocular pressure in dogs. J. Am. Anim. Hosp. Assoc. 42, 207–211. doi: 10.5326/0420207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, E. M., Arnott, E. R., Early, J. B., Bennett, P. C., and McGreevy, P. D. (2015). Dogmanship on the farm: analysis of personality dimensions and training styles of stock dog handlers in Australia. J. Vet. Behav. 10, 471–478. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2015.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pet Food Manufacturers' Association (2019). Dog Population 2019. Available online at: https://www.pfma.org.uk/dog-population-2019 (accessed Feburary 4, 2021).

Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D., and Team, R. C. (2020). nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/nlme/nlme.pdf (Accessed October 20, 2020).

Google Scholar

Protopopova, A., and Wynne, C. D. L. (2014). Adopter-dog interactions at the shelter: Behavioral and contextual predictors of adoption. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 157, 109–116. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2019). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Revelle, W. (2020). psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/psych/psych.pdf (accessed October 20, 2020].

Google Scholar

Rooney, N. J., Bradshaw, J. W. S., and Robinson, I. H. (2000). A comparison of dog–dog and dog–human play behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 66, 235–248. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00078-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schöberl, I., Wedl, M., Bauer, B., Day, J., Möstl, E., and Kotrschal, K. (2012). Effects of owner–dog relationship and owner personality on cortisol modulation in human–dog dyads. Anthrozoös 25, 199–214. doi: 10.2752/175303712X13316289505422

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schöberl, I., Wedl, M., Beetz, A., and Kotrschal, K. (2017). Psychobiological factors affecting cortisol variability in human-dog dyads. PLoS ONE 12:e0170707. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shih, H.-Y., Georgiou, F., Curtis, R. A., Paterson, M. B. A., and Phillips, C. J. C. (2020a). Behavioural evaluation of a leash tension meter which measures pull direction and force during human-dog on-leash walks. Animals 10:1382. doi: 10.3390/ani10081382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shih, H.-Y., Paterson, M. B. A., Georgiou, F., Pachana, N. A., and Phillips, C. J. C. (2020b). Who is pulling the leash? Effects of human gender and dog sex on human–dog dyads when walking on-leash. Animals 10:1894. doi: 10.3390/ani10101894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shih, H.-Y., Paterson, M. B. A., Georgiou, F., Pachana, N. A., and Phillips, C. J. C. (2020c). Do canine behavioural assessments and characteristics predict the human-dog interaction when walking on a leash in a shelter setting? Animals 11:26. doi: 10.3390/ani11010026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundman, A.-S., Poucke, E. V., Holm, A.-C. S., Faresjö, Å., Theodorsson, E., Jensen, P., et al. (2019). Long-term stress levels are synchronized in dogs and their owners. Sci. Rep. 9:7391. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-43851-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tasa, K., Sears, G. J., and Schat, A. C. H. (2010). Personality and teamwork behavior in context: the cross-level moderating role of collective efficacy. J. Organ. Behav. 32, 65–85. doi: 10.1002/job.680

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, P. G. (1997). The public health impact of dog attacks in a majorAustralian city. Med. J. Aust. 167, 129–132. doi: 10.5694/j.1326-5377.1997.tb138810.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turcsán, B., Range, F., Virányi, Z., Miklósi, Á., and Kubinyi, E. (2012). Birds of a feather flock together? Perceived personality matching in owner–dog dyads. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 140, 154–160. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venables, W. N., and Ripley, B. D. (2002). Modern Applied Statistics With S, 4th ed. New York, NY: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-21706-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, S. L. (2014). Human and Canine Personality Assessment Instruments to Predict Successful Adoptions With Shelter Dogs. [dissertation]. West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University.

Google Scholar

Warren-Smith, A. K., Curtis, R. A., Greetham, L., and McGreevy, P. D. (2007). Rein contact between horse and handler duringspecific equitation movements. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 108, 157–169. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2006.11.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wedl, M., Schöberl, I., Bauer, B., Day, J., and Kotrschal, K. (2010). Relational factors affecting dog social attraction to human partners. Interac. Stud. 11, 482–503. doi: 10.1075/is.11.3.09wed

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, D. L., and Hepper, P. G. (2012). The personality of aggressive and non-aggressive dog owners. Pers. Individ. Differ. 53, 770–773. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.05.038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheat, C. H., Fitzpatrick, J. L., Rogell, B., and Temrin, H. (2019). Behavioural correlations of the domestication syndrome are decoupled in modern dog breeds. Nat. Commun. 10:2422. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-10426-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickham, H. (2011). The split-apply-combine strategy for data analysis. J. Stat. Softw. 40, 1–29. doi: 10.18637/jss.v040.i01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, D. W.-L., Bischof, W. F., Anderson, N. C., Jakobsen, T., and Kingstone, A. (2014). The influence of personality on social attention. Pers. Individ. Differ. 60, 25–29. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuur, A. F., Ieno, E. N., and Elphick, C. S. (2010). A protocol for data exploration to avoid common statistical problems. Methods Ecol. Evol. 1, 3–14. doi: 10.1111/j.2041-210X.2009.00001.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: personality, dog, leash tension, dog-waking, shelter, human behavior, canine behavior, human-dog interaction

Citation: Shih H-Y, Paterson MBA, Georgiou F, Mitchell L, Pachana NA and Phillips CJC (2021) Two Ends of the Leash: Relations Between Personality of Shelter Volunteers and On-leash Walking Behavior With Shelter Dogs. Front. Psychol. 12:619715. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.619715

Received: 17 December 2020; Accepted: 19 March 2021;
Published: 14 April 2021.

Edited by:

Katherine Ely Bruce, University of North Carolina at Wilmington, United States

Reviewed by:

Franziska Kuhne, University of Giessen, Germany
Camila María Cavalli, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET), Argentina

Copyright © 2021 Shih, Paterson, Georgiou, Mitchell, Pachana and Phillips. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Clive J. C. Phillips, clive.phillips@curtin.edu.au; Hao-Yu Shih, haoyu.shih@uq.net.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.