- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Alexandru Ioan Cuza University of Iasi, Iasi, Romania
Recent research has investigated how the sense of relational entitlement (SRE, the extent to which a person expects that his/her needs and wishes will be fulfilled by the romantic partner) diminishes couple satisfaction, but little is known about how SRE affects the daily quality of close, romantic relationships. Moreover, the evidence on how SRE interacts with other features of a satisfying relationship (such as the variables of the interpersonal process model of relationships—self-disclosure, perceived partner disclosure, and perceived partner responsiveness) is scarce. Using an electronic daily diary, we examined 99 couples (198 participants) for 7 days, with two daily measurements for each partner. We used a dyadic double intercept multilevel model, which simultaneously computes effects for men and women. We tested a model where one partner's daily couple satisfaction was predicted by their overall levels of SRE (excessive, restricted, and assertive) and by their daily and overall levels of self-disclosure, perceived partner self-disclosure, and perceived partner responsiveness. The model also included person-level interactions and cross-level interactions between the SRE types and variables of the interpersonal process model of relationships for each gender. The analysis indicated that person-level excessive SRE lowers couple satisfaction. Also, day and person-level perceived partner responsiveness and person-level self-disclosure are related to couple satisfaction, but the latter association is significant only for men. Finally, we found some significant person-level interactions that account for changes in couple satisfaction. For men, the links between couple satisfaction, excessive and restricted SRE were moderated by self-disclosure and perceived partner responsiveness, respectively, perceived partner self-disclosure and perceived partner responsiveness. For women, the associations between couple satisfaction, restricted and assertive SRE were moderated by self-disclosure, respectively, perceived partner self-disclosure. This study advances our understanding of the general implications of SRE in the dynamics of couple relationships. More specifically, it shows how SRE interacts with other couple-specific variables in shaping day-to-day couple satisfaction. The theoretical and clinical implications for couple therapy are discussed.
Introduction
Inside their romantic relationships, people express and fulfill some of the most intimate needs. Hence, intimate relationships become a crucial context where entitlement-related behaviors take shape (George-Levi et al., 2014). Although generally considered a negative personality trait (Campbell et al., 2004; Grubbs and Exline, 2016) or a facet of narcissism (Miller et al., 2012), some scholars indicate that entitlement also has some adaptive characteristics (Levin, 1970; Moses and Moses-Hrushovski, 1990). Entitlement refers to an outcome that individuals believe they deserve to receive from their relationships (Attridge and Berscheid, 1994). This outcome is important because it allows the distribution of resources within romantic relationships (Lerner and Mikula, 1994). Moreover, other scholars refer to the entitlement as a crystallization of early attachment bonds (Tolmacz, 2011). Thus, just as attachment can be secure or insecure, entitlement may be adaptive or maladaptive.
Previous research showed that the maladaptive forms of entitlement are detrimental to couple satisfaction (Tolmacz and Mikulincer, 2011; George-Levi et al., 2014), but the more adaptive forms were not related to couple satisfaction (George-Levi et al., 2014). However, no study, to our knowledge, explored whether these relationships are influenced by other variables. For example, previous studies have shown that the expression of needs and the partner's responsiveness to those needs shape couple satisfaction (Bar-Kalifa et al., 2015; Unger et al., 2015). For this reason, the main goal of this study was to explore the moderating role of self-disclosure, perceived partner disclosure, and perceived partner responsiveness (PPR) in the relationship between sense of relational entitlement (SRE) and couple satisfaction. To do so, we used a dyadic daily-diary design that allowed us to examine whether and how the daily fluctuations in self-disclosure, perceived partner disclosure, and PPR, and the person level of these variables moderated the association between SRE and couple satisfaction.
The Sense of Relational Entitlement
The first conceptualization of entitlement most likely belongs to Freud (George-Levi et al., 2014). If Freud (1916) talked about the patients who claimed more compensation for their congenital deficiencies, Jacobson (1959) suggested that some people may think they deserve more because of the exceptional qualities they believe they have. Later, the concept has been included among the five factors of narcissism, indicating the tendency to expect favored treatment from others (Exline et al., 2004). It is well-documented that narcissism has a negative impact on couple relationships by increasing vengefulness (Brown, 2004), interpersonal aggression (Reidy et al., 2010), and vindictive behavior (Ogrodniczuk et al., 2009). Also, some studies indicate that narcissism may predict higher marital satisfaction and commitment, but only in cases of narcissistic individuals with high self-esteem (Sedikides et al., 2004) and with communal feelings for the partner (Finkel et al., 2009). However, narcissism and entitlement are distinct constructs (Brown et al., 2009). First, more recent research showed a clear distinction between two forms of excessive entitlement, grandiose and vulnerable, both of them being unrelated to narcissism (Crowe et al., 2016). Second, entitlement and narcissism show different relationships with other psychological constructs. For example, while grandiose narcissism is negatively associated with short-term psychological distress, anxiety and depression, entitlement shows no relationships with them (Brown et al., 2009). Third, narcissism is a purely intrapersonal construct, while entitlement is a more interpersonal one (Williams et al., 2018). Finally, narcissism can be conceptualized as a personality trait and a personality disorder (Lamkin et al., 2017), while entitlement is a trait-like characteristic that can take both adaptive (assertive) and pathological (restricted or inflated) forms.
Although the concept of entitlement was initially described as a negative individual characteristic, researchers understood that people can have a healthy assertion of their need and wishes (Levin, 1970; Kriegman, 1983; Moses and Moses-Hrushovski, 1990). The concept of sense of entitlement assumed and promoted this positive dimension. Thus, this concept integrated three basic entitlement-related attitudes: excessive, restricted, and assertive entitlement (Levin, 1970; Kriegman, 1983; Moses and Moses-Hrushovski, 1990). The authors suggest that an excessive sense of entitlement characterizes people who believe that their need must be fulfilled regardless of the needs or emotional states of those around. A restricted sense of entitlement is present in people characterized by unassertiveness, timidity, which are less independent and less self-assured. Finally, people who are characterized by assertive sense of entitlement can realistically estimate what they can expect from others, are assertive and confident that they can achieve their needs and rights. It is an adaptive form of entitlement, essential for the well-being of individuals (George-Levi et al., 2014). As we can see, unlike narcissistic entitlement, the sense of entitlement has both negative and positive dimensions and implications. Moreover, several clinical reports that underlined the important role of the sense of entitlement is couple relationships (e.g., Blechner, 1987; Billow, 1999).
In this context, Tolmacz and Mikulincer (2011) proposed another development of the concept, the sense of relational entitlement (SRE), in order to explain individual differences in expression of need and rights inside dyadic relationships. The authors defined the concept as the extent to which a person expects that his/her needs and wishes will be fulfilled by the romantic partner, and as a person's affective and cognitive responses to a romantic partner's failure to fulfill these needs and hopes (Tolmacz and Mikulincer, 2011). In recent years, the concept of SRE was applied in various studies concerning interpersonal relationships, being linked to caregiving style (George-Levi et al., 2016), attachment orientations (Shadach et al., 2017; Brenner et al., 2019), pathological concern (Shavit and Tolmacz, 2014), dating abuse (Warrener and Tasso, 2017), relationship with parents (Tolmacz et al., 2016), and the quality of one's intimate relationship (Bar-Kalifa et al., 2016; Tolmacz et al., 2017; Williams et al., 2018; Candel and Turliuc, 2019; Turliuc and Candel, 2019). The conceptual innovation of Tolmacz and Mikulincer (2011) was completed with the development of a specific scale for measuring the sense of entitlement in couple relationships: The Sense of Relational Entitlement Scale (SRES). The scale includes subscales for the dimensions of entitlement (assertive, restricted, and excessive), this time in the context or the couple relationship. In other words, the authors indicated that people may be characterized by an assertive (confidence in the relationship and the ability to ask for their rights), restricted (a lack of assertiveness and deservingness), and excessive sense of relational entitlement (negative evaluations of the partner and exaggerated expectations). It seems that people with excessive SRE are more sensitive to negative aspects of the partner and relationship and have higher expectations for their partner attention and understanding (Tolmacz and Mikulincer, 2011). Also, an inflated sense of entitlement has been associated with inadequate early parental care, maldaptive attachment styles, and early trauma, such as sexual abuse (Shadach et al., 2017; Brenner et al., 2019). Thus, it was presumed that this type of relational entitlement will have the highest impact on couple satisfaction because excessively entitled people have stronger reaction to the degree of fulfillment of their needs and wishes (Bar-Kalifa et al., 2016). According to Tolmacz and Mikulincer (2011), two or more types of entitlement (for example, assertive and exaggerated) can coexist in the same individual. Given that this concept measures a type of dispositional entitlement and the fact that a relationship consists of hundreds of daily interactions, Tolmacz and Mikulincer (2011) suggest that a person can have high scores on more than one dimension of entitlement. This reflects the various interactions between partners, some of which are more assertive and adaptive, and others more exaggerated or maladaptive.
The Sense of Relational Entitlement and Couple Satisfaction
Couple satisfaction represents the “people's global subjective evaluation of the quality of their marriage” (Li and Fung, 2011, p. 246). It can vary as a function of different interpersonal and intrapersonal characteristics of the couple, such as the partner's background and traits (Bradbury et al., 2000). Studies assessing the sense of relational entitlement and its link with couple satisfaction are scarce. As the Sense of Relational Entitlement Scale is being developed and validated only recently, the situation is to be expected. In their study in which they present the construction of the SRE scale, Tolmacz and Mikulincer (2011) presented the associations of SRE with couple satisfaction among young adults (Tolmacz and Mikulincer, 2011). These findings also received support when examining middle-aged long-term dyadic relationships (George-Levi et al., 2014). People with an inflated sense of relational entitlement are more sensitive to the partner's transgressions. This leads them to use more negative tactics in conflict resolution, such as more verbal aggression, more dominance, and less compromise (Williams et al., 2018). Moreover, excessive relational entitlement was strongly related to abusive behaviors in couple relationships (Wood, 2004; Warrener and Tasso, 2017). Other research showed that it was related to divorce rates (Sanchez and Gager, 2000), selfishness in romantic relationships (Exline et al., 2004) and chronic relationship conflict over a period of 10 weeks (Moeller et al., 2009). On the contrary, individuals with a restricted sense of entitlement are more avoidant when resolving conflicts and suffer from more pathological concern (Shavit and Tolmacz, 2014; Williams et al., 2018). Previous studies showed that both excessive and restricted SRE were strong predictors of lower relational satisfaction (Tolmacz and Mikulincer, 2011; George-Levi et al., 2014; Candel and Turliuc, 2019). Having an assertive sense of relational entitlement was suggested to be linked with positive outcomes, such as higher life satisfaction, more self-esteem and self-efficacy (Tolmacz et al., 2016). However, previous studies that tested it in the context of romantic relationships found no link between assertive SRE and satisfaction (Tolmacz and Mikulincer, 2011; Candel and Turliuc, 2019). Thus, previous research points out that SRE is a trait-like psychological characteristic that has a negative impact on satisfaction. In this study, we were interested in exploring whether the between-person differences in SRE (excessive, restricted, and assertive) will lead to different levels of daily couple satisfaction. We hypothesized that:
(H1) Excessive and restricted relational entitlement will be negatively associated with couple satisfaction. Given that assertive entitlement was not related to relational satisfaction, as previous studies have concluded (e.g., Tolmacz and Mikulincer, 2011), we did not make any hypothesis for its associations with couple satisfaction, but it was included in all the analyses.
Perceived Partner Responsiveness, Self-Disclosure, and Couple Satisfaction
Romantic intimacy is one of the strongest positive predictors of physical health (e.g., lower illness rates, better recovery rates etc.; Hook et al., 2003), psychological well-being (e.g., lower risk for depression, higher levels of life satisfaction; Hook et al., 2003), and couple satisfaction (Dandurand and Lafontaine, 2013; Bar-Kalifa et al., 2015). The Interpersonal Process Model of Intimacy (Reis and Shaver, 1988) indicates that intimacy is built through two fundamental processes: self-disclosure and empathic response from the partner. The model suggests that the ability of both partners to communicate essential information about their wishes, needs, or expectancies, and the perception that a partner is responsive to one's needs is a central construct when it comes to determining the quality of a relationship (Reis et al., 2004). According to this model, when the expression of needs and the response toward those needs are higher, people perceive their romantic relationships as being more intimate (Reis and Shaver, 1988).
Self-disclosure (namely, the verbal communication of information about the self, including personal thoughts, states, dispositions, needs, events in the past, and plans for the future) is a central concept in the study of romantic relationships (Finkenauer et al., 2004). It can be used to maintain proximity to the partner (Lee and Pistole, 2012), it leads to greater intimacy (Laurenceau et al., 1998), and it is generally associated with positive couple outcomes over extended periods of time (Sprecher and Hendrick, 2004). Also, due to the capitalization of these positive outcomes, people who self-disclose more feel an improvement in other aspects of their romantic relationships (Langston, 1994). Previous studies offer evidence that greater self-disclosure is associated with greater couple satisfaction (Hendrick, 1981; Sprecher and Hendrick, 2004; Unger et al., 2015). This may be, in part, due to the role that self-disclosure has in relationship maintenance (Sprecher and Hendrick, 2004), and to its contribution to greater intimacy, which is an important indicator for relational success (Reis and Shaver, 1988). In addition to one's own self-disclosure, perceived partner disclosure might also play a role in the level of satisfaction a person feels. First, one's higher self-disclosure may lead to higher levels of partner disclosure (Laurenceau et al., 2005). Second, Rosenfeld and Bowen (1991) found that the partner's level of disclosure is important for one's couple satisfaction, but people usually overestimate the partner's disclosure. Thus, the perception overcomes the reality of the degree of partner's self-disclosure. Finally, giving and receiving self-disclosure are associated with love, commitment, and couple satisfaction, meaning that these processes sustain the desire to continue the relationship (Sprecher and Hendrick, 2004).
Perceived partner responsiveness (PPR), described as the perception that a partner understands, values, and responds supportively to one's needs, is a cardinal process in the study of relational quality (Reis et al., 2004). When people feel that their partners are more responsive, they believe their relationship is more intimate and that it offers more satisfaction (Laurenceau et al., 1998; Canevello and Crocker, 2010). It has been shown that PPR can mediate or moderate the relationship between various behaviors or traits (e.g., sexual behavior, attachment, or social anxiety) and couple satisfaction (Kane et al., 2007; Bar-Kalifa et al., 2015; Gadassi et al., 2016), and that it can influence variables such as investment, alternatives, or commitment for the relationship (Segal and Fraley, 2016). Moreover, the temporal link between PPR and couple satisfaction was previously validated in multiple studies that employed longitudinal or dyadic diary analysis (Bar-Kalifa et al., 2015; Gadassi et al., 2016; Segal and Fraley, 2016).
Expressing their needs (self-disclosure) and perceiving the partner's response to those needs (PPR) represent organizing constructs that change the way one feels in their relationship. Previous reports showed that perception of enthusiastic, engaged responses from one's partner was associated with more couple satisfaction (see Reis, 2014 for a review). Also, self-disclosure can promote couple satisfaction and endurance (see Finkenauer et al., 2018 for a review). Moreover, past daily diary studies showed that disclosing and receiving disclosure and responsiveness from the partner on a day-to-day basis represent central components of a well-functioning intimate relationship (Laurenceau et al., 2005). Based on these prior findings, we may consider that both the general levels of self-disclosure, perceived partner self-disclosure, and PPR, and the day-to-day expressions of these functional behaviors would positively impact couple satisfaction. Thus, we hypothesized that:
(2) Self-disclosure will be positively associated with couple satisfaction on person-level and day-level. (3) The perceived self-disclosure of the partner will be positively associated with couple satisfaction on person-level and day-level. (4) PPR will be positively associated with couple satisfaction on person-level and day-level.
The Moderation Role of Self-Disclosure and Perceived Partner Responsiveness
While SRE refers to the extent a person expects his/her needs and wishes will be fulfilled inside a romantic relationship, self-disclosure includes the verbal communication of the person's needs, and perceived partner's responsiveness represents the perception that a partner understands and responds supportively to the person's needs. It is important to note that all these aspects gravitate around need fulfillment in the romantic relationship, being relevant for the person's perception of coupe satisfaction (Patrick et al., 2007). Following the previously mentioned works, we consider that PPR and self-disclosure would change the nature (would be a moderator) of the associations between SRE and couple satisfaction by diminishing the strength of the previously found negative associations.
First, both PPR and self-disclosure foster positive outcomes that may influence the levels of couple satisfaction. For example, daily PPR encourages individuals to express more joy, excitement, contentment, and gratitude (Ruan et al., 2020). Also, PPR was positively related to forgiveness after a real live hurtful event (Pansera and la Guardia, 2011). In regard to self-disclosure, the partners who are more involved in such behaviors experience greater emotional involvement, greater satisfaction, and positive affect after being taking part in couple conflicts (Prager et al., 2015).
Second, we found evidence that SRE is associated with relationship quality variables (Tolmacz and Mikulincer, 2011; George-Levi et al., 2014; Candel and Turliuc, 2019). While the expression of needs is crucial in one's relationships, the inter-individual differences can make the most important difference in how people present their needs in their romantic relationships (Clark et al., 2001). Coming from a person's earlier experiences with fulfillment needs (Tolmacz, 2011), SRE plays a relevant role in an individual's general expectation toward a relationship and in the outcome of the said relationship. However, previous studies have shown that positive intimate experiences can counter a person's maladaptive expectation from a relationship (Stanton et al., 2017). For the individuals with an avoidant attachment style, engaging in intimacy-promoting behaviors led to a higher relational quality immediately after engaging in the said behaviors (Stanton et al., 2017). Experiences with people who are understanding, trustworthy, and responsive to one's needs will lead to positive views of others, whereas relationships with people who are unresponsive and rejecting will lead to negative views of others (Bretherton, 1990; Grabill and Kerns, 2000).
Third, there is evidence for the moderating role of the intimacy-related variables on the association between various personal or couple constructs and relationship quality. For example, self-disclosure can soften the harmful effects of negative interactions on need fulfillment and can alleviate the negative effects of trauma on satisfaction (Prager and Buhrmester, 1998; Monk and Nelson Goff, 2014). A study assessing the moderation role of PPR showed that at higher levels of PPR, more self-focused talk was associated with higher sexual satisfaction, and at lower levels of PPR, more self-focused talk was associated with lower sexual satisfaction (Merwin and Rosen, 2020). Also, emotional intimacy was found to moderate the relationship between the use of Sexually Explicit Media (SEM) and relationship satisfaction in men's case, with a higher SEM significantly associated with lower relationship satisfaction among men reporting lower levels of emotional intimacy (Veit et al., 2017).
Based on this evidence, it is reasonable to expect that the impact of adaptive or maladaptive types of SRE on couple satisfaction may vary as a function of self-disclosure and PPR.
Moreover, with evidence coming from both cross-sectional (Kane et al., 2007; Unger et al., 2015) and daily diary studies (Bar-Kalifa et al., 2015; Gadassi et al., 2016; Segal and Fraley, 2016), we consider that both the person-level and day-level effects should be taken into account. We consider that having a relationship characterized by positive and intimacy-promoting behaviors such as PPR, self-disclosure, and perceived partner self-disclosure (in general and on a day-to-day basis) would buffer the negative effects of SRE on relational satisfaction. Individuals with excessive or restricted SRE feel more negativity and, in the case of the former, are more conflictual in nature. This can be counteracted by positive partner behaviors (PPR and perceived partner self-disclosure) and by more intimacy-promoting behavior on their part (self-disclosure). Thus, we hypothesized that:
(5) The association between SRE (exaggerated, restricted, or assertive), on the one hand, and couple satisfaction, on the other hand, will be weaker for those characterized by high levels of self-disclosure, perceived partner self-disclosure or PPR.
Finally, although we did not propose any hypothesis for it, we explored the role of gender in shaping these relationships.
Method
Participants
Ninety-nine couples (198 participants) responded to a 7-day, two measurements each day dyadic diary. For men, the mean age was 25.74 years (SD = 5.63, Min. = 18, Max. = 42). For women, the mean age was 23.13 years (SD = 4.92, Min. = 18, Max. = 39). The mean length of the relationship was 42.78 months (SD = 44.02, Min. = 6, Max. = 204). From the entire sample, 15 couples were married. At least one partner from each couple was enrolled in a Psychology course at a Romanian University. Both their and their partner's participation was voluntary. For their participation, the participants received credits for their course.
Procedure
Each participant received an online form containing the informed consent, the Sense of Relational Entitlement scale, and some demographic questions. In addition, they were asked to offer their email address and phone number. After sending back this information, each participant received another unique online form (containing an open-ended question regarding the most important topic of conversation for that day and the items measuring PPR, self-disclosure, perceived partner self-disclosure, and couple satisfaction), especially designed for him/her. They were asked to complete it twice a day (once at noon and once in the evening) for 7 days, from Monday to Sunday. Each day, one of the researchers sent personalized emails and phone messages to the participants in order to emphasize the importance of their adherence to the research. The protocol for this study was approved by the ethical committee of the university.
Measures
The Sense of Relational Entitlement
We used the Romanian version of the Sense of Relational Entitlement scale (Candel, 2018). This scale contains 18 items that assess each person's relational entitlement type. The scale offers different scores for excessive (eight items), restricted (three items), and assertive entitlement (seven items; it includes both assertive and expectation items, as recommended by George-Levi et al., 2014). The items are rated on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much). Each measurement offered good internal consistency, for both male and female participants (for restricted entitlement: Cronbach's α = 0.69 for males and 0.71 for females; for excessive entitlement: Cronbach's α = 0.86 for males and 0.87 for females; for assertive entitlement: Cronbach's α = 0.70 for males and 0.73 for females).
Couple Satisfaction
The participant's couple satisfaction was assessed using a single item (“Today I am satisfied with my relationship”) rated on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (total disagreement) to 6 (total agreement).
The other variables were measured using a single item: self-disclosure (“How much did you self-disclose since the last answers?”), perceived partner self-disclosure (“How much did your partner self-disclose since the last answers?”), and PPR (“How responsive was your partner since the last answers?”). All three items were measured on a Likert scale form 1 (not at all) to 6 (very much).
Data Analytic Approach
The analysis used a total of 2,772 units of observations (99 couples × 2 members × 14 assessments). We analyzed these data using a multilevel model for dyadic diary data that treats the three levels of distinguishable dyadic diary data (days, nested within persons, nested within couples) as two levels of random variation. This method estimated separate intercepts and slopes for the male and female partner. The lower level represents variability due to day-level repeated measures for male partners and female partners, and the upper level represents person-level variability across male partners and across female partners (Bolger and Laurenceau, 2013). For each type of SRE, which were separately considered as upper-level predictors, we were interested in the person-level effects (e.g., the degree to which a person was characterized by greater excessive SRE at the beginning of the diary). For the moderators, we were interested in both person-level effects (e.g., the degree to which a person was characterized by greater self-disclosure over the course of the diary) and day-level effects (e.g., the degree to which a certain day was characterized by greater self-disclosure than the person's average). For this reason, we tested a model in which couple satisfaction was predicted by each type of SRE, the participants' averages of self-disclosure, perceived partner, and PPR, alongside daily deviations from these averages. In addition, we tested the interaction of each type of SRE with both the day-level moderators (cross-level interactions) and person-level averages of the moderators (level-2/person-level interaction).
Satisfactionijk = (male)i[γ10Self-disclosureijk + γ20Partner self-disclosureijk + γ30PPRijk + γ01Self-disclosureij + γ02Partner self-disclosureij + γ03PPRij + γ04Excessive SREij + γ05Restricted SREij + γ06Assertive SREij + um0i + um1iSelf-disclosureijk + um2iPartner self-disclosureijk + um3iPPRijk + γ07(Self-disclosureij Excessive SREij) + γ08(Self-disclosureij Restricted SREij) + γ09(Self-disclosureij Assertive SREij) + γ010(Partner self-disclosureij Excessive SREij) + γ011(Partner self-disclosureij Restricted SREij) + γ012(Partner self-disclosureij Assertive SREij) + γ013(PPRij Excessive SREij) + γ014(PPRij Restricted SREij) + γ015(PPRij Assertive SREij) + γ11(Self-disclosureijk Excessive SREij) + γ12(Self-disclosureijk Restricted SREij) + γ13(Self-disclosureijk Assertive SREij) + γ21(Partner self-disclosureijk Excessive SREij) + γ22(Partner self-disclosureijk Restricted SREij) + γ23(Partner self-disclosureijk Assertive SREij) + γ31(PPRijk Excessive SREij) + γ32(PPRij Restricted SREij) + γ33(PPRijk Assertive SREij)] + (female)1[γ40Self-disclosureijk + γ50Partner self-disclosureijk + γ60PPRijk + γ016Self-disclosureij + γ017Partner self-disclosureij + γ018PPRij+ γ019Excessive SREij + γ020Restricted SREij + γ021Assertive SREij + uf0i + uf1i Self-disclosureijk + uf2i Partner self-disclosureijk + uf3i PPRijk + γ022(Self-disclosureij Excessive SREij) + γ023(Self-disclosureij Restricted SREij) + γ024(Self-disclosureij Assertive SREij) + γ025(Partner self-disclosureij Excessive SREij) + γ026(Partner self-disclosureij Restricted SREij) + γ027(Partner self-disclosureij Assertive SREij) + γ028(PPRij Excessive SREij) + γ029(PPRij Restricted SREij + γ030(PPRij Assertive SREij) + γ41(Self-disclosureijk Excessive SREij) + γ42(Self-disclosureijk Restricted SREij) + γ43(Self-disclosureijk Assertive SREij) + γ51(Partner self-disclosureijk Excessive SREij) + γ52(Partner self-disclosureijk Restricted SREij) + γ53(Partner self-disclosureijk Assertive SREij) + γ61(PPRijk Excessive SREij) + γ62(PPRijk Restricted SREij) + γ63(PPRijk Assertive SREij)] + eijk
In this double intercept model, Satisfactionijk is the predicted couple satisfaction for participant i in couple j on day k; malei and femalei represent each gender's intercepts. On the day level, we introduced as predictors the daily levels of self-disclosure, perceived partner self-disclosure, and PPR for participant i in couple j on day k. At this level, γ 10 and γ40 represent self-disclosure, γ20 and γ50 represent perceived partner self-disclosure, and γ30 and γ60 represent PPR. On the person level, we introduced as predictors the overall levels of excessive SRE, restricted SRE, assertive SRE, self-disclosure, perceived partner self-disclosure, and PPR for participant i in couple j. At this level, γ01 and γ016 represent self-disclosure, γ02 and γ017 represent perceived partner self-disclosure, γ03 and γ018 represent PPR, γ04 and γ019 represent excessive SRE, γ05 and γ020 represent restricted SRE, and γ06 and γ21 represent assertive SRE. um0i and uf0i represent random intercepts, um1i and uf1i represent random slopes for self-disclosure, um2i and uf2i represent random slopes for perceived partner self-disclosure, and um3i and uf3i represent random slopes for PPR. eijk is a residual component for this subject on the particular day. Additionally, this model also included person-level interactions and cross-level interactions between the SRE types and self-disclosure, perceived partner self-disclosure, and PPR for men and women, respectively.
All level-1 predictors were group-mean centered. All day-level effects were considered random and thus were allowed to vary from person to person. Each level-2 predictor was grand mean centered. All the analyses were computed using the IBM SPSS 20 software. To explore the interaction slopes, we estimated simple slopes for low (−1 SD), average, and high (+1 SD) levels of the moderators using the Preacher et al. (2006) computational tool for testing interaction effects in multilevel analysis.
Results
Preliminary Analysis
Means, standard deviations, and the paired-sample t-tests for the gender differences of each of the studied variables are presented in Table 1. Women report higher daily self-disclosure and assertive SRE. Men report higher levels of daily couple satisfaction. Table 2 presents the correlations between the variables. Couple satisfaction is positively related to self-disclosure, perceived partner self-disclosure, and PPR at both levels of the analysis. SRE is unrelated to the person-level self-disclosure, perceived partner self-disclosure, and PPR and negatively related to couple satisfaction.
The Person-Level Effects on Couple Satisfaction
The results of the hierarchical linear models regarding couple satisfaction are presented in Table 3. At person-level, excessive entitlement is significantly associated with couple satisfaction, meaning that the participants with greater excessive entitlement also report lower couple satisfaction. These associations are significant for both men and women (men: b = −0.25, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001; women: b = −0.28, SE = 0.06, p < 0.001). Restricted and assertive SRE were not related to couple satisfaction. The PPR was associated with greater couple satisfaction for men (b = 0.28, SE = 0.09, p < 0.001) and women (b = 0.28, SE = 0.09, p = 0.003). This indicates that when the participants perceived greater partner responsiveness over the course of the diary, they also reported higher couple satisfaction. Self-disclosure was associated with satisfaction for men (b = 0.24, SE = 0.10, p = 0.03), meaning that men that self-disclose more have, in general, higher levels of couple satisfaction.
Table 3. Self-disclosure, perceived partner self-disclosure, perceived partner responsiveness (PPR), and sense of relational entitlement (SRE), as predictors of couple satisfaction.
The Day-Level Effects on Couple Satisfaction
The day-level analysis yielded fewer significant results. Only PPR significantly predicted couple satisfaction, for both men and women (for men: b = 0.13, SE = 0.03, p < 0.001; for women: b = 0.15, SE = 0.03, p < 0.001). In the days when men and women perceived greater partner responsiveness, they reported higher satisfaction with their relationship. No other result was significant.
The Moderation Effect of Self-Disclosure, Perceived Partner Disclosure, and Perceived Partner Responsiveness
No cross-level interactions were found. However, several person-level interactions were significant. The plots for all the significant interactions are included as Supplementary Material. The relationship between excessive SRE and couple satisfaction is moderated by self-disclosure and PPR. These effects were significant only for men. Estimation of simple slopes indicate that men's higher excessive SRE is not associated with couple satisfaction at low levels of self-disclosure (b = −0.03, SE = 0.13, p = 0.80), but is significantly associated with lower couple satisfaction at medium (b = −0.25, SE = 0.09, p < 0.01) and high levels of self-disclosure (b = −0.48, SE = 0.13, p < 0.001). Also, men's higher excessive SRE is associated with lower couple satisfaction at low (b = −0.69, SE = 0.14, p < 0.001) and medium levels of PPR (b = −0.25, SE = 0.09, p < 0.01). However, it is not associated with couple satisfaction at high levels of PPR (b = 0.18, SE = 0.13, p = 0.19).
The link between restricted SRE and couple satisfaction was moderated by perceived partner disclosure and PPR (for men only) and by self-disclosure (for women only). For men, the relationship between restricted SRE and couple satisfaction is not significant at low (b = −0.15, SE = 0.10, p = 0.13) and medium levels of perceived partner disclosure (b = 0.003, SE = 0.07, p = 0.96), but becomes barely significant and positive at high levels of perceived partner disclosure (b = 0.16, SE = 0.09, p = 0.07). In addition, the relationship between restricted SRE and couple satisfaction is not significant at low (b = 0.17, SE = 0.10, p = 0.11) and medium levels of PPR (b = 0.003, SE = 0.06, p = 0.96), but becomes barely significant and negative at high levels of PPR (b = −0.16, SE = 0.09, p = 0.08). Finally, at low (b = 0.14, SE = 0.13, p = 0.28) and medium levels of self-disclosure (b = 0–0.03, SE = 0.07, p = 0.63), women's level of restricted SRE is not related to couple satisfaction. However, the relationship becomes significant and negative at high levels of self-disclosure (b = −0.21, SE = 0.10, p = 0.04).
Self-disclosure and perceived partner disclosure moderate the relationship between assertive SRE and couple satisfaction (for women only). Assertive SRE has a barely significant negative association with couple satisfaction at low levels of self-disclosure (b = −0.30, SE = 0.18, p = 0.07). This relationship becomes non-significant at medium levels of self-disclosure (b = 0.05, SE = 0.11, p = 0.62). At high levels of self-disclosure, the association is significant and positive (b = 0.44, SE = 0.14, p < 0.01). At low levels of perceived partner disclosure, there is a significant and positive association between assertive SRE and couple satisfaction (b = 0.51, SE = 0.17, p ≤ 0.01). This association becomes non-significant at medium levels of perceived partner disclosure (b = −0.05, SE = 0.11, p = 0.62), and significant and negative at high levels of partner disclosure (b = −0.40, SE = 0.14, p ≤ 0.01).
Discussions
Past research has shown that people that are either excessively or restrictedly entitled use maladaptive ways of need expression and that they may report lower levels of couple satisfaction (Tolmacz and Mikulincer, 2011; George-Levi et al., 2014). Besides, disclosing information about the self and about the current needs toward the romantic partner and the way the partner responds to this can affect couple satisfaction (Sprecher and Hendrick, 2004; Canevello and Crocker, 2010; Unger et al., 2015). Using dyadic diary data from romantic couples, the current study explored the possibility that self-disclosure, perceived partner self-disclosure, and PPR would moderate the relationships between excessive, restrictive, and assertive SRE and couple satisfaction.
The first hypothesis, regarding the relationship between SRE and couple satisfaction, was only partially supported. People with higher levels of excessive SRE reported lower levels of couple satisfaction. However, contrary to previous results (Tolmacz and Mikulincer, 2011), restricted SRE was not related to couple satisfaction. In another study on the same model of the SRE, George-Levi et al. (2014) suggested that excessive and restricted entitlement should be grouped in one new factor called conflicted entitlement. As such, these two types of entitlement may share some variance when it comes to explaining the variation in couple satisfaction. Given that excessive entitlement was previously shown to have a stronger relationship with couple satisfaction (George-Levi et al., 2014), this may account for the non-significant association between restricted SRE and couple satisfaction. Finally, assertive SRE was not related to couple satisfaction, a finding that confirms previous studies (Tolmacz and Mikulincer, 2011).
We proposed that self-disclosure is related to couple satisfaction. This hypothesis was only partially supported. Men's self-disclosure is related to their couple satisfaction, but only at the personal level. Day-to-day self-disclosure does not seem to be related to daily levels of couple satisfaction, a finding that contradicts some previous results (e.g., Rosenfeld and Bowen, 1991). These findings may be explained by the fact that self-disclosure, although it has some aspects of personality construct, is also greatly influenced by relational or environmental conditions (Sprecher and Hendrick, 2004). As such, the relationship between daily self-disclosure and daily couple satisfaction might be affected by other variables. Also, emotional self-disclosure seems to be more important than factual self-disclosure (Laurenceau et al., 2005), but in this study, we did not differentiate between the two. As for the gender differences, previous studies (Dindia and Allen, 1992) have shown that women disclose more than men, and the present results confirm these findings. However, only men's higher general levels of self-disclosure are associated with higher levels of couple satisfaction. Laurenceau et al. (2005) found that self-disclosure is more important for a male than it is for women in predicting intimacy. Although not identical, the process regarding couple satisfaction can be similar. Male partners can be more reliant on engaging in self-revealing disclosure, while female partners may derive their couple satisfaction from other components of the process (such as PPR).
The third hypothesis stated that perceived-partner self-disclosure is associated with couple satisfaction. We found no significant association at any level and for either gender. Although surprising, this may be explained by people's overestimation of their partner's disclosure. Rosenfeld and Bowen (1991) state that people have a tendency to consider their partner's self-disclosure similar to their own. These present results show a very strong correlation between self-disclosure and perceived partner self-disclosure, a fact that supports this assumption. As such, partner disclosure might act in a very similar way to self-disclosure.
The fourth hypothesis was confirmed. Perceived Partner Responsiveness was significantly related to higher couple satisfaction for all the participants, at both day and person levels. These findings support the previous results (Bar-Kalifa et al., 2015; Segal and Fraley, 2016) and show that feelings of understanding, validation, and acceptance from the partner are extremely important in shaping one's couple satisfaction toward the relationship.
The hypothesis concerning the moderating role of self-disclosure, perceived partner self-disclosure, and PPR was only partially supported. First, a significant association between higher excessive SRE and lower couple satisfaction was observed only for the men that used more self-disclosure. Although contrasting with the proposed hypothesis, this result finds its support in the studies showing the negative effects of too much self-disclosure (Cozby, 1972; Collins and Miller, 1994). High levels of self-disclosure can leave the recipient unsure of how to respond, leading to their constant retreat from the relationship. This might be particularly damaging for an excessively entitled person who might not easily forgive such a transgression, feeling that their personal needs are not fulfilled by the partner. Moreover, entitled individuals use various self-promotion behaviors, self-disclosure being one of them, but are also unethical in their decision-making style (Tamborski et al., 2012; Abell and Brewer, 2014). They can promote their needs in the relationship with their partner by self-disclosing, but might not reciprocate when the partners also express their needs. Thus, the partners can distance themselves from the entitled individuals, with the latter becoming less satisfied. Finally, people can also self-disclose their negative feelings, which might lead to negative reciprocity from the partner and further dissatisfaction (Finkenauer et al., 2018). Excessively entitled individuals might be more prone to self-disclose their disappointments following their partner's transgressions. When the level of their negative self-disclosure gets stronger, their satisfaction might become weaker. On the contrary, perceiving one's partner as being more responsive is beneficial for the more entitled individuals. In agreement with our hypothesis, perceived partner responsiveness buffers the negative effect of excessive SRE on couple satisfaction. Having a partner that is more sensible and responsive toward one's needs was found to be related to higher couple satisfaction (Gadassi et al., 2016). This seems to play an important role in determining someone with strong unmet emotional needs to feel more satisfied. Feeling that the partner is caring and understanding is beneficial for men with higher levels of excessive entitlement. Finally, perceived partner responsiveness might also appear due to the individual's own projection of responsiveness (Lemay et al., 2007). Due to an underlying narcissism, those with an excessive entitlement can consider themselves as being more responsive to their partners' needs. Thus, they might maintain the perception of a responsive partner and their relational satisfaction due to their personality traits.
The moderator analysis for the relationship between restricted entitlement and couple satisfaction provided some contradictory findings. First, the link is negative only at high levels of either PPR or self-disclosure (the former moderator was significant for men, while the latter was significant for women). Restricted entitlement consists of the belief that one does not deserve to get anything from the partner. However, both high PPR and high self-disclosure foster intimacy, a process where the partners listen to each other and are attentive to one another (Prager, 1995). This might contradict the core beliefs of inadequacy that a restrictively entitled person possesses, leading to confusion, guilt, shame, and low couple satisfaction. Paradoxically, higher levels of restricted SRE and higher levels of perceived partner self-disclosure interact and predict higher levels of couple satisfaction for men. Tolmacz (2011) proposes that a restricted sense of entitlement can emerge from maternal messages that communicate dissatisfaction with the child. Therefore, the individual starts believing in their usefulness. Later, the adult would act in such a way to satisfy the partner to compensate for their perceived ineptness. Our results suggest that partner disclosure offers the ideal opportunity for individuals with a restricted entitlement to feel useful. Specifically, by allowing their partners to disclose, they consider that they atone for their past unfitness, which makes them feel more satisfied. Nevertheless, the positive impact of perceived partner self-disclosure might also be explained by the capitalization theory (Langston, 1994). Self-disclosing about positive events can lead to more trust toward the target person (Reis et al., 2010). Although we measured just the perception of partner self-disclosure and not the actual disclosure, it is possible for them to be positively correlated. This means that the partners after they self-disclose, offer more trust to the restrictive entitlement individuals, which might determine the latter to capitalize on these positive experiences. In the end, it is possible for the individuals with a restricted sense of entitlement to capitalize more from their partners' positive experiences than from their own.
Assertive entitlement is significantly and positively related to couple satisfaction only when the person discloses more. This result was found only among women. For assertively entitled individuals it is important to obtain what they feel they deserve. This attitude, combined with a higher ability to self-disclose and communicate in a non-aggressive way about their needs, can determine the partner to pay more attention to the person's needs. They might also capitalize on their positive self-disclosure and increase their satisfaction by talking about their positive experiences. Moreover, when low levels of self-disclosure are achieved, the assertive persons lack one important mechanism used to express their needs. Therefore, they seem to be less satisfied. On the contrary, the level of perceived partner disclosure intensifies dissatisfaction among assertively entitled women. This shows that while assertive women need to disclose more to be more satisfied, they do not want to reciprocate and allow the partners to disclose. Previous studies showed that assertive entitlement was positively related to some facets of narcissism, such as superiority and vanity (Tolmacz and Mikulincer, 2011). Despite being the more adaptive type of entitlement, assertive entitlement still overlaps with some narcissistic traits. Also, taking into account the results of Crowe et al. (2016) and Hart et al. (2019), assertive entitlement can be described as a more emotionally stable and less vulnerable form of entitlement, but not completely devoided of the antagonistic behavior that can be found in maladaptive entitlement. As such, when faced with greater partner disclosure and greater expression of needs form their partner, the individuals with greater assertive entitlement might not feel prepared to respond and thus might report less couple satisfaction.
Significant gender differences emerged during the analysis. Self-disclosure, perceived partner disclosure, and PPR mostly played different roles among men and women and interacted differently with the facets of SRE. One potential explanation for this is that there are gender differences in the process of intimacy. Women generally self-disclose more than men (Dindia and Allen, 1992; Horne and Johnson, 2018), and this result was supported by our findings. Moreover, other studies suggest that women feel more satisfied with the process of intimacy when the partner self-discloses more, while for men, both partners must disclose (Manne et al., 2004). Moreover, women tend to respond with better accuracy to their partner's expression of needs, being responsive when the partners experience greater stress. On the contrary, men offer both responsiveness and negative behaviors when the partner needs support (Neff and Karney, 2005).
As a summary, this study shows that the components of the interpersonal process of intimacy can both buffer and aggravate the effects of SRE on couple satisfaction. For excessively entitled individuals, offering more self-disclosure seems to be counterproductive. However, having a more responsive partner allows for greater couple satisfaction. Still, this positive effect of PPR can be only temporary, depending on the ability and willingness of the partner to be responsive toward partners who greatly exaggerate their needs and concentrate mostly on themselves. It is worth noting that these results were found only in males. As previously mentioned, women are generally better at responding to their partner's moments of greater stress. Thus, excessively entitled men might risk taking this ability for granted. For restricted individuals, higher intimacy promoting behaviors (higher self-disclosure and PPR) may contrast with their low or non-existent expectations, bringing a decline in their satisfaction. On the contrary, greater perceived partner disclosure may come with the opportunity to feel useful and increase their couple satisfaction. In the end, assertive entitled individuals profit from greater self-disclosure and report more couple satisfaction, but seem to be affected by greater partner self-disclosure. Although the least damaging form on entitlement, assertiveness can also bring negative consequences when the partners insist too much on their needs. Alternatively, given that this result was found only on women, assertive women can achieve lower couple satisfaction when their partner discloses more because self-disclosure is not a behavior that fits with the gender role expected from men. Our results support the view of Finkel et al. (2017) on the role of responsiveness in romantic relationships. While being responsive promotes couple satisfaction, this is highly dependent on the individual's predispositions. In conjunction with self-disclosure and PPR, the different types of entitlement lead to different levels of couple satisfaction.
In addition to theoretical advances, this study also proposes some methodological strengths. To our knowledge, this is one of the first studies that used the concept of SRE and all three of its forms in a diary design. This kind of research is very useful because it allows the study of the participants in a more ecologically valid way, partially suppressing the shortcomings of a cross-sectional design. Also, while previous studies that investigated this concept concentrated in more experienced couples, our results point to some similar findings in a sample of young couples.
However, this study is not without its limits. While the diary design allows for a long investigation, it remains correlational, and thus, it does not allow for inferring a causal association between the variables. Also, all the concepts were measured with self-reporting questionnaires and the sample was mostly composed of couples with a relatively high socioeconomic status that presents higher than average couple satisfaction. In the future, other methods (such as direct observation) and other samples can be used to extend these results. Finally, although entitlement is distinct from narcissism, the two concepts are related. Future studies should control the role of narcissism to explore how SRE affects couple satisfaction above and beyond it.
Conclusion
This present study examined the moderation effect of self-disclosure and PPR on the associations between SRE and couple satisfaction. Our main findings indicate a negative association of daily couple satisfaction with excessive SRE, but not with restricted SRE. Self-disclosure was related to couple satisfaction, but only for men and only at person-level. Perceived-partner self-disclosure was related with couple satisfaction for men and women at both day-level and person-level. All three types of SRE (assertive, restricted, and excessive) interact with self-disclosure, perceived partner disclosure, and perceived partner responsiveness and account for changes in couple satisfaction. To our knowledge, this is the first study to use the variables included in the interpersonal process model of intimacy (Reis and Shaver, 1988) as an organizing construct for the interactions between SRE and couple satisfaction. The research on relational entitlement is recent, and only a few studies have examined its importance in shaping the levels of couple satisfaction. Still, our results are important, because other than their empirical strengths, they can point to some clinical and therapeutic implications too. For example, they suggest that careful expression of needs thought self-disclosure and a responsive answer from the partner can overcome the effects of some of the more damaging types of entitlement. Based on these results, the therapists would be able to create programs that take into account the level of excessive, restricted, and assertive relational entitlement when advising greater use of self-disclosure and PPR. Finally, these programs should use different techniques depending on gender.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethical Committee of the Alexandru Ioan Cuza University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
OSC and MNT wrote the manuscript, conceived and designed the study. OSC collected and analyzed the data.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.609232/full#supplementary-material
References
Abell, L., and Brewer, G. (2014). Machiavellianism, self-monitoring, self-promotion and relational aggression on Facebook. Comput. Hum. Behav. 36, 258–262. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.076
Attridge, M., and Berscheid, E. (1994). “Entitlement in romantic relationships in the United States,” in Entitlement and the Affectional Bond. Critical Issues in Social Justice, eds. M. J. Lerner and G. Mikula, (Boston, MA: Springer), 117–147.
Bar-Kalifa, E., Bar-Kalifa, L., Rafaeli, E., George-Levi, S., and Vilchinsky, N. (2016). Relational entitlement moderates the associations between support matching and perceived partner responsiveness. J. Res. Pers. 65, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2016.08.009
Bar-Kalifa, E., Hen-Weissberg, A., and Rafaeli, E. (2015). Perceived partner responsiveness mediates the association between social anxiety and relationship satisfaction in committed couples. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 34, 587–610. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2015.34.7.587
Blechner, M. J. (1987). Entitlement and narcissism: paradise sought. Contemp. Psychoanal. 23, 244–255. doi: 10.1080/00107530.1987.10746178
Bolger, N., and Laurenceau, J. P. (2013). Methodology in the Social Sciences. Intensive Longitudinal Methods: An Introduction to Diary and Experience Sampling Research. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Bradbury, T. N., Fincham, F. D., and Beach, S. R. (2000). Research on the nature and determinants of marital satisfaction: a decade in review. J. Marriage Fam. 62, 964–980. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.00964.x
Brenner, I., Bachner-Melman, R., Lev-Ari, L., Levi-Ogolnic, M., Tolmacz, R., and Ben-Amitay, G. (2019). Attachment, sense of entitlement in romantic relationships, and sexual revictimization among adult CSA survivors. J. Interpers. Violence. doi: 10.1177/0886260519875558
Bretherton, I. (1990). Communication patterns, internal working models, and the intergenerational transmission of attachment relationships. Infant. Mental. Hlth. J. 11, 237–252. doi: 10.1002/1097-0355(199023)11:3<237::AID-IMHJ2280110306>3.0.CO;2-X
Brown, R. P. (2004). Vengeance is mine: narcissism, vengeance, and the tendency to forgive. J. Res. Pers. 38, 576–584. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2003.10.003
Brown, R. P., Budzek, K., and Tamborski, M. (2009). On the meaning and measure of narcissism. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 35, 951–964. doi: 10.1177/0146167209335461
Campbell, W. K., Bonacci, A. M., Shelton, J., Exline, J. J., and Bushman, B. J. (2004). Psychological entitlement: interpersonal consequences and validation of a self-report measure. J. Pers. Assess. 83, 29–45. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8301_04
Candel, O. S. (2018). “Sense of relational entitlement – Romanian version. Factor structure and associations with romantic attachment and dyadic satisfaction,” in Proceedings of CIEA 2018 The Fifth International Conference on Adult Education Education for Values - Continuity and Context (Iasi, 2018), eds. R. Ungureanu and M. Mocanu (Bologna: Edlearning), 79–86.
Candel, O. S., and Turliuc, M. N. (2019). The effect of the sense of relational entitlement on relational satisfaction, positive and negative emotions. J. Psychol. Educ. Res. 27, 46–60.
Canevello, A., and Crocker, J. (2010). Creating good relationships: responsiveness, relationship quality, and interpersonal goals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 99, 78–106. doi: 10.1037/a0018186
Clark, M. S., Fitness, J., and Brissette, I. (2001). “Understanding people's perceptions of relationships is crucial to understanding their emotional lives,” in Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Vol 2. Interpersonal Processes, eds. M. Hewstone and M. Brewer, (Oxford: Basil Blackwell), 253–278.
Collins, N. L., and Miller, L. C. (1994). Self-disclosure and liking: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 116, 457–475. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.457
Cozby, P. C. (1972). Self-disclosure, reciprocity and liking. Sociometry 35, 151–160. doi: 10.2307/2786555
Crowe, M. L., LoPilato, A. C., Campbell, W. K., and Miller, J. D. (2016). Identifying two groups of entitled individuals: cluster analysis reveals emotional stability and self-esteem distinction. J. Pers. Disord. 30, 762–775. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2015_29_229
Dandurand, C., and Lafontaine, M. F. (2013). Intimacy and couple satisfaction: the moderating role of romantic attachment. Int. J. Psychol. Stud. 5, 74–90. doi: 10.5539/ijps.v5n1p74
Dindia, K., and Allen, M. (1992). Sex differences in self-disclosure: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 112, 106–124. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.106
Exline, J. J., Baumeister, R. F., Bushman, B. J., Campbell, W. K., and Finkel, E. J. (2004). Too proud to let go: narcissistic entitlement as a barrier to forgiveness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 894–912. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.894
Finkel, E. J., Campbell, W. K., Buffardi, L. E., Kumashiro, M., and Rusbult, C. E. (2009). The metamorphosis of Narcissus: communal activation promotes relationship commitment among narcissists. Pers. Soc. Psychol. B. 35, 1271–1284. doi: 10.1177/0146167209340904
Finkel, E. J., Simpson, J. A., and Eastwick, P. W. (2017). The psychology of close relationships: fourteen core principles. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 68, 383–411. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044038
Finkenauer, C., Engels, R. C., Branje, S. J., and Meeus, W. (2004). Disclosure and relationship satisfaction in families. J. Marriage Fam. 66, 195–209. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-2445.2004.00013.x-i1
Finkenauer, C., Kerkhof, P., and Pronk, T. (2018). “Self-disclosure in relationships: REVEALING and concealing information about oneself to others,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Personal Relationships, eds. A. L. Vangelisti and D. Perlman (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 271–281. doi: 10.1017/9781316417867.022
Freud, S. (1916). “Some character-types met with in psycho-analytic work,” in The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, Volume XIV (1914 − 1916): On the History of the Psycho-Analytic Movement, Papers on Metapsychology and Other Works (London: Hogarth Press), 309–333.
Gadassi, R., Bar-Nahum, L. E., Newhouse, S., Anderson, R., Heiman, J. R., Rafaeli, E., et al. (2016). Perceived partner responsiveness mediates the association between sexual and marital satisfaction: a daily diary study in newlywed couples. Arch. Sex. Behav. 45, 109–120. doi: 10.1007/s10508-014-0448-2
George-Levi, S., Vilchinsky, N., Tolmacz, R., Khaskiaa, A., Mosseri, M., and Hod, H. (2016). “It takes two to take”: Caregiving style, relational entitlement, and medication adherence. J. Fam. Psychol. 30, 743–751. doi: 10.1037/fam0000203
George-Levi, S., Vilchinsky, N., Tolmacz, R., and Liberman, G. (2014). Testing the concept of relational entitlement in the dyadic context: further validation and associations with relationship satisfaction. J. Fam. Psychol. 28, 193–203. doi: 10.1037/a0036150
Grabill, C. M., and Kerns, K. A. (2000). Attachment style and intimacy in friendship. Personal Relationships 7, 363–378. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2000.tb00022.x
Grubbs, J. B., and Exline, J. J. (2016). Trait entitlement: a cognitive-personality source of vulnerability to psychological distress. Psychol. Bull.142, 1204–1226. doi: 10.1037/bul0000063
Hart, W., Richardson, K., Tortoriello, G. K., and Breeden, C. J. (2019). Revisiting profiles and profile comparisons of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism on self-presentation tactic use. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 151, 109523. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.109523
Hendrick, S. S. (1981). Self-disclosure and marital satisfaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1150–1159. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.40.6.1150
Hook, M. K., Gerstein, L. H., Detterich, L., and Gridley, B. (2003). How close are we? Measuring intimacy and examining gender differences. J. Couns. Dev. 81, 462–472. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6678.2003.tb00273.x
Horne, R. M., and Johnson, M. D. (2018). Gender role attitudes, relationship efficacy, and self-disclosure in intimate relationships. J. Soc. Psychol. 158, 37–50. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2017.1297288
Jacobson, E. (1959). The “Exceptions” An Elaboration of Freud's Character Study. Psychoanal. Stud. Chil. 14, 135–154. doi: 10.1080/00797308.1959.11822826
Kane, H. S., Jaremka, L. M., Guichard, A. C., Ford, M. B., Collins, N. L., and Feeney, B. C. (2007). Feeling supported and feeling satisfied: how one partner's attachment style predicts the other partner's relationship experiences. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 24, 535–555. doi: 10.1177/0265407507079245
Kriegman, G. (1983). Entitlement attitudes: psychosocial and therapeutic implications. J. Am. Acad. Psychoan. 11, 265–281. doi: 10.1521/jaap.1.1983.11.2.265
Lamkin, J., Lavner, J. A., and Shaffer, A. (2017). Narcissism and observed communication in couples. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 105, 224–228. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.09.046
Langston, C. A. (1994). Capitalizing on and coping with daily-life events: expressive responses to positive events. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 1112–1125. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1112
Laurenceau, J. P., Barrett, L. F., and Pietromonaco, P. R. (1998). Intimacy as an interpersonal process: the importance of self-disclosure, partner disclosure, and perceived partner responsiveness in interpersonal exchanges. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1238–1251. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1238
Laurenceau, J. P., Barrett, L. F., and Rovine, M. J. (2005). The interpersonal process model of intimacy in marriage: a daily-diary and multilevel modeling approach. J. Fam. Psychol. 19, 314. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.2.314
Lee, J. Y., and Pistole, M. C. (2012). Predictors of satisfaction in geographically close and long-distance relationships. J. Couns. Psychol. 59, 303. doi: 10.1037/a0027563
Lemay, E. P., Clark, M. S., and Feeney, B. C. (2007). Projection of responsiveness to needs and the construction of satisfying communal relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 834–853. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.5.834
Lerner, M. J., and Mikula, G. (1994). “Entitlement and the affectional bond,” in Entitlement and the Affectional Bond. Critical Issues in Social Justice, eds. M. J. Lerner and G. Mikula (Boston, MA: Springer), 117–147.
Levin, S. (1970). On the psychoanalysis of attitudes of entitlement. Bull. Philadelphia Association Psychoanalysis, 20, 1–10.
Li, T., and Fung, H. H. (2011). The dynamic goal theory of marital satisfaction. Rev. Gen. Psycho. 15, 246–254. doi: 10.1037/a0024694
Manne, S., Ostroff, J., Rini, C., Fox, K., Goldstein, L., and Grana, G. (2004). The interpersonal process model of intimacy: The role of self-disclosure, partner disclosure, and partner responsiveness in interactions between breast cancer patients and their partners. J. Fam. Psychol. 18, 589–599. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.18.4.589
Merwin, K. E., and Rosen, N. O. (2020). Perceived partner responsiveness moderates the associations between sexual talk and sexual and relationship well-being in individuals in long-term relationships, J. Sex. Res. 57, 351–364, doi: 10.1080/00224499.2019.1610151
Miller, J. D., Price, J., Gentile, B., Lynam, D. R., and Campbell, W. K. (2012). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism from the perspective of the interpersonal circumplex. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 53, 507–512. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.026
Moeller, S. J., Crocker, J., and Bushman, B. J. (2009). Creating hostility and conflict: effects of entitlement and self-image goals. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 45, 448–452. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.11.005
Monk, J. K., and Nelson Goff, B. S. (2014). Military couples' trauma disclosure: moderating between trauma symptoms and relationship quality. Psycho. Trauma-US. 6, 537–545. doi: 10.1037/a0036788
Moses, R., and Moses-Hrushovski, R. (1990). Reflections on the sense of entitlement. Psychoanal. Stud. Chil. 45, 61–78. doi: 10.1080/00797308.1990.11823511
Neff, L. A., and Karney, B. R. (2005). Gender differences in social support: a question of skill or responsiveness?. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 79–90. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.79
Ogrodniczuk, J. S., Piper, W. E., Joyce, A. S., Steinberg, P. I., and Duggal, S. (2009). Interpersonal problems associated with narcissism among psychiatric outpatients. J. Psychiat. Res. 43, 837–842. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.12.005
Pansera, C., and la Guardia, J. (2011). The role of sincere amends and perceived partner responsiveness in forgiveness. Pers. Relationships. 19, 696–711. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2011.01386.x
Patrick, H., Knee, C. R., Canevello, A., and Lonsbary, C. (2007). The role of need fulfillment in relationship functioning and well-being: a self-determination theory perspective. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 434–457. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.3.434
Prager, K. J., and Buhrmester, D. (1998). Intimacy and need fulfillment in couple relationships. K. Soc. Pers. Relat. 15, 435–469. doi: 10.1177/0265407598154001
Prager, K. J., Shirvani, F., Poucher, J., Cavallin, G., Truong, M., and Garcia, J. J. (2015). Recovery from conflict and revival of intimacy in cohabiting couples. Pers. Relationships. 22, 308–334. doi: 10.1111/pere.12082
Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., and Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. J. Educ. Behav. Stat. 31, 437–448. doi: 10.3102/10769986031004437
Reidy, D. E., Foster, J. D., and Zeichner, A. (2010). Narcissism and unprovoked aggression. Aggressive Behav. 36, 414–422. doi: 10.1002/ab.20356
Reis, H. T. (2014). “Responsiveness: affective interdependence in close relationships in the herzliya series on personality and social psychology,” in Mechanisms of Social Connection: From Brain to Group, eds. M. Mikulincer and P. R. Shaver (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 255–271. doi: 10.1037/14250-015
Reis, H. T., Clark, M. S., and Holmes, J. G. (2004). “Perceived partner responsiveness as an organizing construct in the study of intimacy and closeness,” in Handbook of Closeness and Intimacy, eds. D. J. Mashek and A. Aron (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 201–225,
Reis, H. T., and Shaver, P. (1988). “Intimacy as an interpersonal process,” in Handbook of Personal Relationships: Theory, Research and Interventions. eds. S. Duck et al. (Chichester: Wiley), 367–389.
Reis, H. T., Smith, S. M., Carmichael, C. L., Caprariello, P. A., Tsai, F.-F., Rodrigues, A., et al. (2010). Are you happy for me? How sharing positive events with others provides personal and interpersonal benefits. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 99, 311–329. doi: 10.1037/a0018344
Rosenfeld, L. B., and Bowen, G. L. (1991). Marital disclosure and marital satisfaction: direct-effect versus interaction-effect models. Western J. Comm. 55, 69–84. doi: 10.1080/10570319109374371
Ruan, Y., Reis, H. T., Clark, M. S., Hirsch, J. L., and Bink, B. D. (2020). Can I tell you how I feel? Perceived partner responsiveness encourages emotional expression. Emotion. 20, 329–342. doi: 10.1037/emo0000650
Sanchez, L., and Gager, C. T. (2000). Hard living, perceived entitlement to a great marriage, and marital dissolution. J. Marriage Fam. 62, 708–722. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.00708.x
Sedikides, C., Rudich, E. A., Gregg, A. P., Kumashiro, M., and Rusbult, C. (2004). Are normal narcissists psychologically healthy?: Self-esteem matters. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 400–416. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.400
Segal, N., and Fraley, R. C. (2016). Broadening the investment model: an intensive longitudinal study on attachment and perceived partner responsiveness in commitment dynamics. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 33, 581–599. doi: 10.1177/0265407515584493
Shadach, E., Rappaport, S., Dollberg, D., Tolmacz, R., and Levy, S. (2017). Relational Entitlement, Early Recollections of Parental Care, and Attachment Orientation. Curr. Psychol. 37, 781–791. doi: 10.1007/s12144-017-9559-y
Shavit, Y., and Tolmacz, R. (2014). Pathological concern: scale construction, construct validity, and associations with attachment, self-cohesion, and relational entitlement. Psychoanal. Psychol. 31, 343–356. doi: 10.1037/a0036560
Sprecher, S., and Hendrick, S. S. (2004). Self-disclosure in intimate relationships: associations with individual and relationship characteristics over time. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 23, 857–877. doi: 10.1521/jscp.23.6.857.54803
Stanton, S. C. E., Campbell, L., and Pink, J. C. (2017). Benefits of positive relationship experiences for avoidantly attached individuals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 113, 568–588. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000098
Tamborski, M., Brown, R. P., and Chowning, K. (2012). Self-serving bias or simply serving the self? Evidence for a dimensional approach to narcissism. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 52, 942–946. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.030
Tolmacz, R. (2011). Sense of entitlement in couple relationships: an attachment theory perspective. Am. J. Psychoanal., 71, 37–57. doi: 10.1057/ajp.2010.40
Tolmacz, R., Efrati, Y., and Ben-David, B. M. (2016). The sense of relational entitlement among adolescents toward their parents (SREap) – Testing an adaptation of the SRE. J. Adolescence. 53, 127–140. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.09.003
Tolmacz, R., Mahajna, S., and Efrati, Y. (2017). The sense of entitlement in romantic relationships: ethnic and religious aspects. Mental Health Religion Culture 20, 728–740. doi: 10.1080/13674676.2017.1405249
Tolmacz, R., and Mikulincer, M. (2011). The sense of entitlement in romantic relationships—Scale construction, factor structure, construct validity, and its associations with attachment orientations. Psychoanal. Psychol. 28, 75–94. doi: 10.1037/a0021479
Turliuc, M. N., and Candel, O. S. (2019). The Assertive Sense of Relational Entitlement, Emotions and Couple Satisfaction: A Mediation Model. Annals of the Al. I. Cuza University, Psychology Series 28, 41–56.
Unger, D., Sonnentag, S., Niessen, C., and Kuonath, A. (2015). The longer your work hours, the worse your relationship? The role of selective optimization with compensation in the associations of working time with relationship satisfaction and self-disclosure in dual-career couples. Hum. Relat. 68, 1889–1912. doi: 10.1177/0018726715571188
Veit, M., Štulhofer, A., and Hald, G. M. (2017). Sexually explicit media use and relationship satisfaction: a moderating role of emotional intimacy?. Sex. Relatsh. Ther. 32, 58–74. doi: 10.1080/14681994.2016.1193134
Warrener, C. D., and Tasso, A. (2017). An empirical examination of the relationship between dating abuse and entitlement within a college population. Partner Abuse 8, 377–394. doi: 10.1891/1946-6560.8.4.377
Williams, D. N., Smith, D. B., Sukach, T., and Minaiy, C. (2018). Expecting more in relationships – implications of relational entitlement. Am. J. Fam. Ther. 46, 321–340. doi: 10.1080/01926187.2018.1539354
Keywords: entitlement, couple satisfaction, self-disclosure, perceived partner responsiveness, daily diary
Citation: Candel OS and Turliuc MN (2021) The Role of Relational Entitlement, Self-Disclosure and Perceived Partner Responsiveness in Predicting Couple Satisfaction: A Daily-Diary Study. Front. Psychol. 12:609232. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.609232
Received: 22 September 2020; Accepted: 16 February 2021;
Published: 23 March 2021.
Edited by:
Dominik Schoebi, Université de Fribourg, SwitzerlandReviewed by:
Andrea B. Horn, University of Zurich, SwitzerlandThomas Ledermann, Florida State University, United States
Copyright © 2021 Candel and Turliuc. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Maria Nicoleta Turliuc, dHVybGl1YyYjeDAwMDQwO3VhaWMucm8=