Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 20 July 2021
Sec. Human-Media Interaction

Commentary: A Tablet-Based Assessment of Rhythmic Ability

  • 1International Laboratory for Brain, Music and Sound Research (BRAMS), University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 3Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music (CRBLM), Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 4Department of Cognitive Psychology, University of Economics and Human Sciences in Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland

A Commentary on:
A Tablet-Based Assessment of Rhythmic Ability

Zanto, T.P., Padgaonkar, N.T., Nourishad, A., and Gazzaley, A. (2019). A Tablet-Based Assessment of Rhythmic Ability. Front. Psychol. 10:2471. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02471

Introduction

Humans are well-equipped to move along with auditory rhythms via finger or foot tapping, body swaying or walking (Leman et al., 2013; Sowiński and Dalla Bella, 2013). Individual differences in auditory-motor synchronization abilities (AMS) are observed in the general population (Repp, 2010; Sowiński and Dalla Bella, 2013; Palmer et al., 2014), and exacerbated by disorders (e.g., language/speech disorders, Lundetræ and Thomson, 2018; Ladanyi et al., 2020; Parkinson, Yahalom et al., 2004; Puyjarinet et al., 2019). Describing these individual differences can shed light on the cognitive mechanisms underlying the rhythm system in healthy and patient populations (Dalla Bella, 2020; Damm et al., 2020; Ladanyi et al., 2020).

Finger tapping to test AMS (Repp, 2005; Repp and Su, 2013) is used in test batteries like the Battery for the Assessment of Auditory Sensorimotor and Timing Abilities (BAASTA, Dalla Bella et al., 2017), and the Harvard Beat Assessment Test (H-BAT, Fujii and Schlaug, 2013). Tapping performance is typically measured in the lab with tapping pads or dedicated sensors, which afford high temporal precision (≤ 1 ms), but make the method quite unsuitable to be used outside the lab.

Using Mobile Devices for Testing Rhythmic Abilities

The portability of tablets and smartphones makes them an appealing solution for testing cognitive functions (Koo and Vizer, 2019), and rhythm abilities (tablet version of BAASTA; Puyjarinet et al., 2017; Bégel et al., 2018; Dauvergne et al., 2018). The study by Zanto et al. (2019) contributes to the demonstration that mobile technologies can serve purposefully for assessing AMS abilities. With their AMS task on tablet, Zanto et al. aimed at replicating outcomes of well-known AMS tasks, such as tapping to a metronome. The results are broadly consistent with previous studies, showing, for example, that musicians are more consistent than non-musicians in paced tapping (Franěk et al., 1991; Repp, 2010). Nevertheless, we notice that AMS performance (see Figure 1) is relatively low compared to other studies. This discrepancy may be linked to some of the limitations inherent in using touchscreen technology for tap detection.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Comparison of synchronization consistency obtained during AMS tasks using a metronome or music. Synchronization consistency, a common measure of AMS, is a value from 0 to 1 (0 = lack of synchronization, high variability; 1 = perfect phase-locking between the taps and the beat, no variability). The results of AMS with a metronome are the mean of three different tempi that are comparable across studies (slow–around 450 ms, medium–around 600 ms, and fast–around 750 ms). The music stimuli were excerpts with an inter-beat interval around 600 ms. The dots on the plot represent the mean consistency, and the bars, the standard deviation. NM, non-musician; M, musician; ADHD, attention deficit disorder with or without hyperactivity; PD, Parkinson's disease; PS, poor synchronizers. *The task used in this study involved tapping to stimuli with 15% randomly omitted beats. Synchronization to these stimuli, less predictable than a standard metronome, may have yielded lower consistency values than in most of the other studies.

Limitations and Indications For Future Research

Timing inaccuracy in AMS tapping tasks on tablet can arise from: (1) a delay in the audio output, (2) the temporal uncertainty arising from the sampling rate of touch detection, and (3) the processing delay between the touch detection and the recording of a tap. Some of these limitations stem from the precision of the device touchscreen (sampling rate between 60 and 240 Hz) which is much lower as compared to lab measurements (1,000 Hz or more). Lower sampling rate imparts an unavoidable uncertainty about when the touch event occurred (Kousa, 2017). This variability (jitter) of error in individual touch events cannot be removed by subtracting an average delay. Because of this limitation, the participant's taps would appear more variable than their actual performance when measured in the lab. This hinders the capacity of the task to capture fine grained differences in AMS, and potentially to distinguish between good and poor synchronizers.

We compared the results from Zanto et al. (2019) with other in-lab studies and those using tablet devices (Figure 1), by taking synchronization consistency as a measure of variability in paced tapping (Fujii and Schlaug, 2013; Sowiński and Dalla Bella, 2013; Woodruff Carr et al., 2014). This measure corresponds to the vector length of the distribution of tap times within the inter-beat-interval, obtained with circular statistics. This measure shows high sensitivity to poor synchronization (e.g., Bégel et al., 2017; Lagrois and Peretz, 2019). Hence, synchronization consistency is a well-suited metric to assess the temporal precision of an AMS task, irrespective of constant latency in the task's technological implementation. It is apparent that non-musicians from Zanto et al.'s study generally obtained lower synchronization consistency (i.e., performed worse; mean consistency = 0.73; range = 0.64–0.80) than healthy adults from all the other studies (mean consistency = 0.93; range = 0.82–0.97) considered here. Their results are sometimes comparable to or show poorer synchronization than individuals with rhythm disorders (Bégel et al., 2017, 2018; Puyjarinet et al., 2017; Lagrois and Peretz, 2019). The observed generally lower synchronization consistency is likely to reflect low timing precision of touchscreen devices, a fact that may hinder quantitative comparison with validated norms and other laboratory-based studies of synchronization consistency. However, the performance reported by Zanto et al. is more comparable to values in the literature when considering other measures of tapping variability (e.g., standard deviation of asynchrony). In spite of these discrepancies, however, the precision afforded by Zanto et al.'s task is sufficient to distinguish musicians from non-musicians, and young from older adults, while providing high test-retest reliability. Thus, it may have general diagnostic value (e.g., for screening purposes). In addition, it is worth noting that Zanto's protocol also extends to visual and audio-visual synchronization, which is usually not tested by other batteries.

When synchronizing with audio stimuli, these issues with measurement precision on a tablet device can be solved by relying on an audio recording of the sound the taps produce when they reach the touchscreen. This solution, which is device-independent and capable of high temporal precision ( ≤ 1 ms) without requiring prior calibration, is already implemented in a tablet version of BAASTA (Dalla Bella and Andary, 2020). By recording the combined audio of stimulus and response, and resolving each during analysis, it bypasses possible sources of delay and jitter. This may explain why the tablet version of BAASTA successfully replicated the results previously obtained in the lab on a computer for AMS.

Conclusion

Mobile devices such as tablets or smartphones are very promising methods for screening AMS abilities. Solutions based on audio recording can compensate the current limitations of mobile touchscreens, thus reducing measurement uncertainty and matching the precision of laboratory measurement.

Author Contributions

AZ, NF, and SDB: wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by a Discovery Grant (RGPIN-2019-05453) to SDB from the Natural Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Dr. Theodore Zanto, who shared data from his study with us.

References

Bégel, V., Benoit, C.-E., Correa, A., Cutanda, D., Kotz, S., and Dalla Bella, S. (2017). “Lost in time” but still moving to the beat. Neuropsychologia 94:22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bégel, V., Verga, L., Benoit, C.-E., Kotz, S. A., and Dalla Bella, S. (2018). Test-retest reliability of the Battery for the Assessment of Auditory Sensorimotor and Timing Abilities (BAASTA). Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 61, 395–400. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2018.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalla Bella, S. (2020). “The use of rhythm in rehabilitation for patients with movement disorders,” in Music and the Aging Brain, eds L. L. Cuddy, S. Belleville, and A. Moussard (London: Academic Press), p. 383–406. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-817422-7.00015-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalla Bella, S., and Andary, S. (2020). High-Precision Temporal Measurement of Vibro-Acoustic Events in Synchronisation With a Sound Signal on a Touch-Screen Device. International Patent No WO 2020/128088 A1. Geneva: World Intellectual Property Organization (WIPO).

Google Scholar

Dalla Bella, S., Berkowska, M., and Sowiński, J. (2015). Moving to the beat and singing are linked in humans. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:663. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalla Bella, S., Farrugia, N., Benoit, C.-E., Begel, V., Verga, L., Harding, E., and Kotz, S. A. (2017). BAASTA: battery for the assessment of auditory sensorimotor and timing abilities. Behav. Res. Methods 49, 1128–1145. doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0773-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damm, L., Varoqui, D., De Cock, V. C., Dalla Bella, S., and Bardy, B. (2020). Why do we move to the beat? A multi-scale approach, from physical principles to brain dynamics. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 112, 553–584. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.12.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dauvergne, C., Bégel, V., Gény, C., Puyjarinet, F., Laffont, I., and Dalla Bella, S. (2018). Home-based training of rhythmic skills with a serious game in Parkinson's disease: usability and acceptability. Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 61, 380–385. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2018.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falk, S., Müller, T., and Dalla Bella, S. (2015). Non-verbal sensorimotor timing deficits in children and adolescents who stutter. Front. Psychol. 6:847. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franěk, M., Mates, J., Radil, T., Beck, K., and Pöppel, E. (1991). Finger tapping in musicians and nonmusicians. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 11, 277–279. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(91)90022-P

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujii, S., and Schlaug, G. (2013). The Harvard Beat Assessment Test (H-BAT): a battery for assessing beat perception and production and their dissociation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:771. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koo, B. M., and Vizer, L. M. (2019). Mobile technology for cognitive assessment of older adults: a scoping review. Innov. Aging 3:igy038. doi: 10.1093/geroni/igy038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kousa, M. (2017). Design, implementation and evaluation of a low-cost, high accuracy feedback latency measurement system (Master's thesis). Aalto University, Espoo, Finland. Retrieved from: http://urn.fi/URN:NBN:fi:aalto-201712188209

Google Scholar

Ladanyi, E., Persici, V., Fiveash, A., Tillmann, B., and Gordon, R. (2020). Is atypical rhythm a risk factor for developmental speech and language disorders? WIREs Cogn. Sci. 11:e1528. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lagrois, M.-É., and Peretz, I. (2019). The co-occurrence of pitch and rhythm disorders in congenital amusia. Cortex 113, 229–238. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.11.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leman, M., Moelants, D., Varewyck, M., Styns, F., van Noorden, L., and Martens, J.-P. (2013). Activating and relaxing music entrains the speed of beat synchronized walking. PLoS ONE 8:e67932. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundetræ, K., and Thomson, J. M. (2018). Rhythm production at school entry as a predictor of poor reading and spelling at the end of first grade. Read. Writ. 31, 215–237. doi: 10.1007/s11145-017-9782-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmer, C., Lidji, P., and Peretz, I. (2014). Losing the beat: deficits in temporal coordination. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 369:20130405. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puyjarinet, F., Bégel, V., Gény, C., Driss, V., Cuartero, M.-C., Kotz, S. A., Pinto, S., and Dalla Bella, S. (2019). Heightened orofacial, manual, and gait variability in Parkinson's disease results from a general rhythmic impairment. NPJ Park. Dis. 5:19. doi: 10.1038/s41531-019-0092-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puyjarinet, F., Bégel, V., Lopez, R., Dellacherie, D., and Dalla Bella, S. (2017). Children and adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder cannot move to the beat. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-11295-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of the tapping literature. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 969–992. doi: 10.3758/BF03206433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Repp, B. H. (2010). Sensorimotor synchronization and perception of timing: effects of music training and task experience. Hum. Mov. Sci. 29, 200–213. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2009.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Repp, B. H., and Su, Y.-H. (2013). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of recent research (2006-2012). Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 403–452. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowiński, J., and Dalla Bella, S. (2013). Poor synchronization to the beat may result from deficient auditory-motor mapping. Neuropsychologia 51, 1952–1963. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodruff Carr, K., White-Schwoch, T., Tierney, A. T., Strait, D. L., and Kraus, N. (2014). Beat synchronization predicts neural speech encoding and reading readiness in preschoolers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 14559–14564. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1406219111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yahalom, G., Simon, E. S., Thorne, R., Peretz, C., and Giladi, N. (2004). Hand rhythmic tapping and timing in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 10, 143–148. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2003.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zanto, T. P., Padgaonkar, N. T., Nourishad, A., and Gazzaley, A. (2019). A tablet-based assessment of rhythmic ability. Front. Psychol. 10:2471. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02471

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: music, rhythm, movement, auditory-motor synchronization, assessment mobile technologies, individual differences

Citation: Zagala A, Foster NEV and Dalla Bella S (2021) Commentary: A Tablet-Based Assessment of Rhythmic Ability. Front. Psychol. 12:607676. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.607676

Received: 17 September 2020; Accepted: 14 June 2021;
Published: 20 July 2021.

Edited by:

Nicola Bruno, University of Parma, Italy

Reviewed by:

Theodore Zanto, University of California, San Francisco, United States

Copyright © 2021 Zagala, Foster and Dalla Bella. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Agnès Zagala, agnes.zagala@umontreal.ca; Simone Dalla Bella, simone.dalla.bella@umontreal.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.