Skip to main content

CLINICAL TRIAL article

Front. Psychol., 05 March 2021
Sec. Health Psychology

Effects of Combining Meditation Techniques on Short-Term Memory, Attention, and Affect in Healthy College Students

Samani Unnata Pragya&#x;Samani Unnata Pragya1Neelam D. Mehta&#x;Neelam D. Mehta2Bassam AbomoelakBassam Abomoelak3Parvin UddinParvin Uddin4Pushya VeeramachaneniPushya Veeramachaneni5Naina MehtaNaina Mehta3Stephanie MooreStephanie Moore6Melissa Jean-FrancoisMelissa Jean-Francois6Stephanie GarciaStephanie Garcia6Samani Chaitanya PragyaSamani Chaitanya Pragya1Devendra I. Mehta
Devendra I. Mehta3*
  • 1Department of Religions and Philosophies, University of London, London, United Kingdom
  • 2Sidney Kimmel Medical College, Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA, United States
  • 3Gastrointestinal Translational Laboratory, Arnold Palmer Hospital for Children, Orlando, FL, United States
  • 4College of Arts, Sciences and Education, Florida International University, Miami, FL, United States
  • 5College of Law, Florida International University, Miami, FL, United States
  • 6Department of Biostatistics, Robert Stempel College of Public Health and Social Work, Florida International University, Miami, FL, United States

Meditation refers to a family of self-regulation practices that focuses on training attention and awareness to foster psycho-emotional well-being and to develop specific capacities such as calmness, clarity, and concentration. We report a prospective convenience-controlled study in which we analyzed the effect of two components of Preksha Dhyāna – buzzing bee sound meditation (Mahapran dhvani) and color meditation (leśyā dhyāna) on healthy college students. Mahapran and leśya dhyāna are two Preksha Dhyāna practices that are based on sound and green color, respectively. The study population represents a suitable target as college students experience different stress factors during the school year. This study measures the individual and combined effects of two techniques (one focusing on sound and one focusing on color), on short-term memory, attention, and affect, in novice meditators. We used a battery of cognitive, performance, and compared results with baseline and control values. We found improved cognition, especially attention, short-term memory, and affect in terms of positivity and reduced negativity. Overall, the two techniques produced variable benefits and subjects showed improved scores over baseline for short-term memory, cognitive function, and overall wellbeing. Further studies are required to understand underlying mechanisms for the observed differences between the two techniques and to elucidate mechanisms underlying the more pronounced and global benefits observed with the combined techniques. These results underscore a need to examine individual components of meditation practices in order to individualize treatment approaches for attention disorders in young adults.

Clinical Trail Registration:ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT03779269.

Introduction

Meditation is defined as a group of self-regulation practices used to focus an individual’s attention in order to achieve increased voluntary control over various psycho-emotional and physiologic states (Davis and Hayes, 2011). It is an ancient practice originating around 1,500 BCE in India and is increasingly used as a modality to treat physical and psycho-emotional disorders, including anxiety (Mehta et al., 2011; Hoge et al., 2013; Satsangi and Brugnoli, 2018), attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Poissant et al., 2019), chronic pain (Reiner et al., 2013; Banth and Ardebil, 2015; Ball et al., 2017; Majeed et al., 2018), depression (Tolahunase et al., 2018; Carpena et al., 2019), irritable bowel syndrome (Gaylord et al., 2011), and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Gallegos et al., 2017). Meditation also has been found to increase focus and attention among adolescents and young adults (Jha et al., 2007; Black et al., 2009; Serwacki and Cook-Cottone, 2012). Recently, data showed that mindfulness is inversely associated with depression and stress among medical students (Alzahrani et al., 2020).

Many meditation practices involve a multistep process using two or more techniques to focus the mind on a specific stimulus (Hu et al., 2011).

Some studies have compared different whole-systems meditation practices in experienced meditators. A comparison of the effects of mindfulness-based meditation, a form of meditation with roots from Buddhism, and focused meditation (Dunn et al., 1999; Sevinc et al., 2018) showed each practice produced different electroencephalography (EEG) patterns in meditation groups compared with controls. A study of highly-trained Buddhist monks showed no observable changes resulting from compassion meditation (techniques to focus on cultivating deep concern for others; Hofmann et al., 2011), but a significant increase in the duration of perceptual dominance resulting from meditation focused on one point of the body (such as the navel).

Many studies have compared the effects of different meditation practices in experienced meditators (Amihai and Kozhevnikov, 2014; Sevinc et al., 2018), but few have assessed the effects of individual techniques or the impact of those techniques in novice meditators. Hirshberg et al. (2018) reported divergent effects of brief meditation in response to an acute stressor in a randomized controlled trial of 156 undergraduate students. The participants were novice to mindfulness, and they were allocated to one of four brief practices including breath awareness, loving-kindness, gratitude, or to an attention control condition. The study revealed an increased positivity although effects differences were noticed, especially after a stressor (Hirshberg et al., 2018). Our goal was to study the effects in novice meditators of two individual techniques used in the meditative practice, preksha dhyana. Furthermore, our study planned to assess the impact when the techniques were combined. In addition, we wanted to embed these techniques in their normal college time rather than vacation or summer camps to assess the impact at times of highest need. Although techniques used in preksha dhyana are similar to those used in mindfulness-based meditation, preksha dhyana also uses two techniques that focus on sound and on imagery (color). Enhanced understanding of these individual techniques could enable specific combinations to be used for personalized therapeutic approaches.

Materials and Methods

Ethical Considerations

The proposal was approved by the Florida International University (FIU) ethics, and the IRB committees. All participants (n = 142) signed consent forms in order to be enrolled in the study. All participants were given ID numbers according to the IRB rules (IRB-17-0108-CR02). Students received an IRB-approved small stipend for participating. The study was registered in Clinicaltrials.gov as NCT03779269. Exclusion criteria included previous use of any form of meditation and attendance of less than 80% of the intervention or control sessions.

Techniques Studied

Preksha Dhyana is based on wisdom culture of ancient Jain ascetics that aims for pure consciousness. Meditation (Dhyana) and relaxation are core to this practice and have recently been compiled and formulated as a complete system by the renowned Jain ascetic, Acharya Mahapragya. The PI is a pupil of the former Acharya and an expert exponent of the system (Mahapragya, 1995). Mahapran is a sound-based technique (the English translation of naad is “sound”) created using either inner bodily sounds, ambient sounds, or basic universal sounds (Suriji Maharaja, 1997). We used a basic universal sound of bee-like buzzing (achieved using several deep breaths and exhaling with closed lips for several seconds at a time while focusing on the vibrations) to achieve intense concentration. We also studied a technique that includes focusing on an imaginary color. Participants concentrated on a visualized green point on the forehead (as used in Jain Yoga; Mahapragya, 1995), since bright green is associated with attention arousal (Valdez and Mehrabian, 1994) and with the heart chakra (one of several psychic energy centers in the body regarded as the ultimate focal point linking emotional, physical, and spiritual energies; Tulsi, 1994). We assessed both techniques individually and combined, using a battery of cognitive, performance, and compared results with baseline and control values. The study items were selected based on theoretical considerations that cognition, memory, and affect are important in overall assessment of any changes from such interventions. The selected scales were based on previous experience as well as validation with appropriate normative values for our age group.

Procedure

The study was completed over the course of six school semesters (one semester = 3 calendar months) with new participants each semester. From the outset of the study, the design was to assess the two techniques separately and when combined. While randomization into the groups was initially planned, logistics of needing adequate time per session time available during college required adjustment. Participants were initially randomly allocated either to one of the intervention groups, and if unable to attend based on class schedules, re-allocated to the control group. This approach was adopted to meet logistics constraints which included the intervention time of about 1 h and allowing 5–10 min prior to intervention session and 20–30 min after the intervention. In addition, our previous recruitment strategies revealed that sessions before or after college classes will have limited study participants with potential dropout. Advantage of embedding the intervention during the college hours secured better recruitment and attendance. This strategy also allowed consistent use of limited access of meditation room in campus as well as availability of the lead instructor (UP), for each intervention. We used the same lead instructor throughout the study, but different, trained associates each semester for the assessment and data entry. Only the statisticians were blinded to the groups.

Meditation Sessions

A trained instructor guided the students through a 25-min meditation three times per week for 8 weeks. During each session, participants sat in a simple cross-legged position and followed the instructor through a meditation process (Table 1). Each session was preceded by a 3-min relaxation period and concluded with a 2-min relaxation period, as well as 5 min for students’ questions. Each group was administered a pre-test at the beginning of the study and a post-test at or after the 8th week to assess affect at baseline and post-intervention.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Interventions applied in each study group.

Study Measures

Short-Term Working Memory

We assessed digital recall and recall processing; language recall and recall processing; and spatial recall and recall processing in all groups using the Alloway Working Memory Assessment short form (AWMA-2 SF; Alloway, 2007), a tool validated for individuals up to age 22. The digital recall test requires participants to hold an increasing number of digits in short-term memory and repeat the digits to the tester. The sentence recall test requires participants to listen to a sentence, remember the last word of each sentence, and then recite the last word to the tester. The spatial recall test requires individuals to mentally manipulate a series of shapes and to remember the sequence in which the shapes are placed. Spatial recall was available for control, color focus and sound and color focus groups only.

Affect

Affect was measured using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). This consists of 2, 10-item self-report mood scales measuring the distinct dimensions of positive and negative affect (Watson et al., 1988). Using a scale from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (extremely), participants rated the extent to which they felt positive mood (e.g., enthusiastic, active, and alert) or negative mood (e.g., anger, disgust, and fear) throughout the previous week. The two scales, uncorrelated with each other, have shown high internal consistency and stability over time.

Selective Attention Capacity, Sustained Attention Capacity, and Impulsivity

These were measured using Connor’s Continuous Performance test 3rd edition (CPT-3™). This 14-min test consists of six groups of letters in which each letter of the alphabet is flashed on a computer screen individually in random order. Time between letters is variable, but averages 250 ms. Respondents are asked to press the spacebar after each letter except for letter X (which appears 36 times out of the 324 letters presented). Individual scores of variables measured indicate attention and impulsivity, and also allows derivation of Inattentiveness score from variables including detectability, omission and commission of errors as t scores as well as correct hit response time (Hit RT), and Impulsivity score from variables including Hit RT, commission of error and perseverations. This current version was used to assess the impact of mindfulness training in young adults with ADHD (Bueno et al., 2015).

Adherence to the Meditation Practice

This was assessed three times per session in all groups by measuring the duration of buzzing during mahapran. Participants were asked to take a deep breath, close their eyes, and emit a humming or buzzing noise for as long as they could with each breath.

Data Management and Statistical Analyses

Data were retrieved from CPT II software or directly entered into an Excel spreadsheet. The statistics team was blinded to the identity of the groups and participants and used IBM SPSS Statistics (Armonk, NY, United States) for data analysis. Generally, the data were presented as means and SDs (Essex et al., 2017) for continuous variables, while categorical variables were presented in percentages. Differences in means were used especially in pre- and post-treatment analyses. t-tests, Mann-Whitney U test (smaller sample sizes), and ANOVA were used to determine significant changes. Given number of variables, to avoid type 1 error, Bonferroni correction was applied and alpha < 0.0013 was the cut off for significance for the three intervention groups combined and controls. However, in comparing each variable between groups, sound focus, color focus, sound and color focus, and controls, for a priori hypothesis of interaction between methods of meditation, trends in values of p where variables had conventional alpha <0.05 were tabulated. For magnitude of effect changes from baseline were calculated using the confidence intervals from paired t-tests. Pearson’s correlation coefficient was used to assess the relationship between independent variables. Linear regression analysis was performed post hoc to assess group as dependent and change in measures from baseline as independent variables.

Results

Participants and Demographics

Following IRB approval, we allocated students (n = 178) at Florida International University (FIU) to one of three preksha dhyana intervention groups or to a control group: sound-only (n = 40), color-focus-only (n = 40), a combination of the two techniques, sound and color-focus (n = 60), or control group (n = 38; Table 2). The enrolled student population at FIU is 61% Hispanic or Latino, 13% black or African American, 15% white non-Hispanic, 4% Asian or Pacific Islander, and 7% other minority groups. About 58% of enrolled students are female and 42% male.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Study participants.

We used control group data from our previous study to estimate the sample size (n = 30) for a single intervention group (Pragya et al., 2014). Of enrolled participants, 142 completed the study. Thirty-six participants (20.2%) were lost to follow-up (12 controls, nine from the sound-only technique, six from color-focus technique, and nine from sound plus color-focus group). The flowchart of the study is illustrated in Figure 1. Overall, there were fewer males than females in all groups (p < 0.01), but all groups were equivalent in age (p = 0.32; Table 2).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The flowchart of the study.

Assessment of Preksha Meditation

Overall, the three preksha dhyana groups combined (N = 116), after Bonferroni correction, did show average and maximum Buzzing time, Commission of Errors, Hit RT, Digital Recall, Listening Recall and Listening Recall Processing, Positivity, and Negativity improve significantly (p < 0.001). Using same stringency, none of the control group variables showed changes from baseline (Supplementary Table S1). In terms of magnitude of effect, changes in affect from baseline in the three preksha dhyana groups were calculated from the paired t-test confidence intervals. Negativity decreased by 19.8% (95% CI 9.0–30.6) and positivity increased by 10.4% (95% CI 2.7–18.1). This entire group also showed significant improvement from baseline in short-term working memory with digital recall increasing by 4.34 SD 11.8, CI 2.11–6.57, equivalent to 9.7% (95% CI 5.7–13.6) from baseline, and listening recall increasing by 8.79 SD 14.5 CI 6.1–11.5, equivalent to 11.1% (95% CI 6.2–15.9) from baseline. Spatial recall was assessed only in the color and sound and color focus groups and showed an increase of 4.41 SD 14.4 CI 1.0–7.9, equivalent to 5.7% (95% CI 1.1–10.3) from baseline (Supplementary Table S2).

To test whether differences existed between control group and the three preksha dhyana groups combined, independent sample t-test was run and Commission of errors t score (F = 0.316; p < 0.02), HitRT score (F = 1.904; p = 0.04), and Negative affect (F = 1.635; p = 0.04) showed trends but did not meet cut off or p < 0.0013 (Supplementary Table S3). In the three preksha dhyana groups combined, age was not correlated with differences in maximum duration of buzzing (r = 0.04, p = 0.72). In the control group, however, age showed a trend with the change in duration of buzzing (r = 0.45, p = 0.02; Supplementary Table S4).

In the three Preksha groups combined, the average buzzing time was correlated with differences in Omission T scores (r = 0.21, p = 0.03). Gender differences were seen in the control group, using the Mann-Whitney U test given sample size, for attention (improved Commission of error t score, U = 37.50; p = 0.03 in females) were noted. However, no gender differences were seen in the three preksha dhyana groups combined using t-test (Supplementary Table S5).

Comparison of Individual Meditation Practices

Results comparing the three meditation groups and controls using conventional alpha 0.05 as threshold to select variables for display are summarized in Table 3. In general values of p are lower in combined sound and color focus group than other groups. In addition, Spatial recall (M = -5.39; SD = 15.71; p = 0.02) and Spatial recall processing score (M = 6.06; SD = 14.71; p < 0.01) also showed trends of improvement in the combined sound and color-focus group, but not the color-focus only group nor control. Data of individual meditation practice are illustrated in Supplementary Tables S6–S8. Figure 2 depicts the differences in negativity, positivity, and commission of errors in every group (8 weeks postintervention compared to baseline).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Paired t-test results.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Differences in negativity and positivity from baseline (A) Commission of error differences from baseline (B) ***p < 0.01, **p = 0.01, *p < 0.05.

We used ANOVA to compare the difference in the change in score from baseline to post- intervention across groups (control group and the each of the three meditation groups). At least one group was significantly different for HitRT and Digital Recall (Supplementary Table S9). Since each group varied in number of participants, we applied the Bonferroni procedure to determine whether statistically significant differences existed between the groups. Change in digital recall from pre- to post- meditation differed significantly between the combined techniques and sound-only (mahapran) intervention groups (F = 5.618; p < 0.01,) and between the combined techniques and color-focus-only intervention groups (F = 7.307; p = 0.02), but did not differ significantly with the control group (F = 5.490; p = 0.24). Interestingly, change in maximum buzzing duration was significantly positively correlated with a change in listening recall (r = 0.53, p = 0.01) and change in average buzzing duration was significantly positively correlated with the change in attention (Commission of Error t score, r = 0.21, p = 0.03). In terms of semesters, no specific differences were seen that were not accounted for by changes in groups, especially the control group (Supplementary Table S10). A multiple regression was calculated to predict group based on variables. A significant regression equation was found [F(4,130) = 4.018, p < 0.004], with an R2 0.11. The predicted group is equal to 1.501–0.028 (Delta Negativity) – 0.01 (Delta Commission of Errors) + 0.021 (Delta Digital recall) + 0.008 (Delta Listening Recall), where Delta variable is differences from baseline in respective scores. Group allocation increased by 1.501–0.028 in Negativity, −0.01 in Commission of errors, + 0.08 in increase in Listing Recall, + 0.021 in Digital Recall. Thus, Group allocation increased in increments of + 0.021 in Digital Recall, 0.008 Learning Recall and decrements of 0.028 in Negativity, and 0.01 in Commission of Error. By adding Gender as a variable, the regression equation was F(5,129) = 3.885, p < 0.003 with R2 improved to 0.13. The variables Negativity, Commission of Errors, Digital Recall and Learning Recall as well as gender were significant predictors accounting for 13% of responses to intervention.

Discussion

Combined Preksha dhyana meditation groups demonstrated significant improvements in affect, attention, and short-term memory in a prospective cohort-controlled study. Improvements over baseline in affect for both Positivity and Negativity, as well as inattention and impulsivity, and improved short-term memory for numbers and words were seen. Benefits were seen in both genders. Having shown efficacy of preksha dhyana we focused on the primary goal of this study, which, was to evaluate the impact of individual techniques used in the preksha dhyana meditation practice with the long term goal of harnessing individual meditation techniques in isolation or varied combinations to personalize therapy for specific disorders, such as ADHD.

In terms of trends within the three preksha dhyana meditation groups, study participants showed some improvement over baseline for affect, short-term memory, and attention. The degree of improvement varied between the sound-only and color-focus only meditation techniques and was larger and more widespread when both techniques were combined. Participants using the sound technique (mahapran) showed cognitive performance improvement on CPT-3™ with reduced commission of errors, also a marker of attention. Students in this group also exhibited decreased negative affect from baseline. We previously reported the benefits of this technique on students with learning disabilities and noted the ease with which the technique was learned by this group (Pragya et al., 2014).

Participants using the color-focus technique alone also showed improved scores for (commission of errors, also a marker of attention) and affect (decreased negativity). Short-term working memory improved significantly from baseline (listening recall and listening recall processing). In participants who used both meditation techniques (combined sound and color focus), short term memory for digital recall, spatial recall, and spatial recall processing improved from baseline. Affect improved significantly from baseline (both decreased negativity and increased positivity), an outcome not achieved when using either technique individually. This suggests that both techniques are associated with beneficial outcomes and suggest these benefits to be incremental when combined, despite same total time of meditating.

In terms of magnitude of effect changes in affect in the study group using both techniques were substantial, with negativity decreasing (mean 19.8%) and positivity improving (mean 10.4%). This group also showed significant improvement from baseline in short-term working memory with digital recall (mean 9.7%), listening recall (mean 11.1%), and spatial recall (mean 5.7%). Similar changes were shown for attention, with decreased commission of errors. The buzzing duration was associated with increased attention (Commission of Errors t score). Using multiple regression, five variables accounted for 13% of the improvement seen. These were Negativity reduction, Commission of Error reduction, and an increase in digital memory and learning memory. Gender as female was also associated with response. This further underscores incremental benefit from single intervention to combined intervention.

In a similar sample size, mindfulness, a comparable form of meditation with roots from ancient Buddhism and Yoga, revealed measurable effects in 156 college students (Hirshberg et al., 2018). The participants were novices to mindfulness, and they were allocated to one of four brief practices, breath awareness, loving-kindness, gratitude, or to an attention control condition. The study revealed that gratitude training significantly improved positive affect compared to breath awareness, while loving-kindness led to significantly greater reductions in implicit negative affect compared to the control condition immediately after brief practice. The study did not seek to assess cumulative impact of these techniques on the same population. In other studies, mindfulness showed improvement of mood in college students, especially depression as well as stress reduction (Mendes and Palmer, 2016; Sundquist et al., 2017; Alzahrani et al., 2020).

The prefrontal cortex has been associated with enhanced cognitive focus and the parietal lobe with improved spatial cognition (Somers and Sheremata, 2013). Our results support these data: the study group using a focused meditation technique (in this case, color) showed increased attentiveness and spatial recall ability, while this was not the pattern seen in the sound-based group. In a small but randomized controlled trial, short-term training with meditation over 8 weeks also lead to increased functional connectivity between the amygdala and a region implicated in emotion regulation [ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC); Kral et al., 2018]. This finding suggests neuroplasticity even in short term intervention.

Other studies showed that different techniques activate different brain areas and stimulate different brain wave patterns. In experienced meditators, EEG (Dunn et al., 1999; Lehmann et al., 2001) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI; Wang et al., 2011) have shown that different meditative techniques correlate with variable activation in different brain areas. Wang et al. (2011) showed that focused meditation practice affected subjects’ prefrontal cortex, while a breathing-based practice affected the limbic system and other areas associated with mood or affect (Wang et al., 2011). Taken together, these studies showed how different techniques, added together, can lead to a cumulative effect. However, further work is warranted to understand the affected physical regions in the brain, but also the cellular level mechanisms that may be involved in such responses even in the short term as we are reporting.

The study strengths included the large group size, subject retention in the intervention group, and using the length of buzzing as in internal marker of adherence. In addition, the use of Bonferroni correction reduced type 1 error. Study limitations include the inability to fully randomize groups due to students’ schedule constraints, and availability of appropriate space, although we do not believe this added significant systematic bias. The gender differences in the groups may also introduce a bias in the study. In addition, the attrition rate of the control group was larger than expected and reduced the effective power of the study for cross-group comparison. Ideally, the control group would have been exposed to a sham or active intervention with a similar program pattern; however, to date, such control groups have not been universally standardized or validated for studies on meditation.

Conclusion

Preksha dhyana is a multi-step Jain meditation process using two specific techniques: mahapran (a sound and breathing technique) and a color-focused imagery technique. This study showed incremental as well as differential benefits from both techniques, with the greatest benefits when both techniques are combined. The magnitude of beneficial effects on affect and on short-term memory was statistically significant, even with a brief intervention. Further study to assess both cumulative and differential effects of other preksha dhyana techniques specifically and all meditative practices in general can contribute to individualized meditation modalities to treat disorders such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Florida International University (FIU; IRB-17-0108-CR02). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

SUP and NM contributed equally to this work. Psychological tests were applied by PU and NM. Additionally, SM, MJ-F, and SG were involved in the statistical analyses. BA, NM, PV, SM, SCP, and DM were involved in data collection, data management, and writing the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the Jain Education and Research Foundation and the Florida International University faculty support program for their support. We also thank Amy N. McBride, MS, MAP, and Allan Salinas for writing and editing assistance.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.607573/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Table S1 | The control group (n = 26) assessment data. Significant differences for digital recall score (mean = −3.33, p-value = 0.03), listening recall processing score (mean = 9.36, p-value = 0.01) were observed. Values of p did not meet Bonferroni criteria.

Supplementary Table S2 | Assessment of different variables among all the meditation groups (n = 116). Values of p are shown for some of the variables including those with uncorrected p < 0.05. *indicates significance after Bonferroni correction.

Supplementary Table S3 | Analysis between and within groups. At least, one group showed statistical differences (p < 0.05) in HitRt score and digital recall score.

Supplementary Table S4 | Correlations between age and improvement.

Supplementary Table S5 | Significant differences in improvement between genders for meditation group.

Supplementary Table S6 | The sound focus (Mahapran) meditation (n = 31). (p < 0.05 was considered significant).

Supplementary Table S7 | The color focus meditation (n = 34). (p < 0.05 was considered significant).

Supplementary Table S8 | Assessment of the combined group meditation (n = 51). (p < 0.05 was considered significant).

Supplementary Table S9 | Post hoc test (Bonferroni test was used to account for sample size differences) for the three statistically significant variables. *Denotes statistically significant mean value.

Supplementary Table S10 | Significant differences in improvement between semesters for control group.

References

Alloway, T. P. (2007). Working memory, reading, and mathematical skills in children with developmental coordination disorder. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 96, 20–36. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alzahrani, A. M., Hakami, A., AlHadi, A., Batais, M. A., Alrasheed, A. A., and Almigbal, T. H. (2020). The interplay between mindfulness, depression, stress and academic performance in medical students: a Saudi perspective. PLoS One 15:e0231088. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0231088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amihai, I., and Kozhevnikov, M. (2014). Arousal vs. relaxation: a comparison of the neurophysiological and cognitive correlates of Vajrayana and Theravada meditative practices. PLoS One 9:e102990. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0102990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ball, E. F., Nur Shafina Muhammad Sharizan, E., Franklin, G., and Rogozinska, E. (2017). Does mindfulness meditation improve chronic pain? A systematic review. Curr. Opin. Obstet. Gynecol. 29, 359–366. doi: 10.1097/GCO.0000000000000417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banth, S., and Ardebil, M. D. (2015). Effectiveness of mindfulness meditation on pain and quality of life of patients with chronic low back pain. Int. J. Yoga 8, 128–133. doi: 10.4103/0973-6131.158476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Black, D. S., Milam, J., and Sussman, S. (2009). Sitting-meditation interventions among youth: a review of treatment efficacy. Pediatrics 124, e532–e541. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-3434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bueno, V. F., Kozasa, E. H., da Silva, M. A., Alves, T. M., Louza, M. R., and Pompeia, S. (2015). Mindfulness meditation improves mood, quality of life, and attention in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biomed. Res. Int. 2015:962857. doi: 10.1155/2015/962857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carpena, M. X., Tavares, P. S., and Menezes, C. B. (2019). The effect of a six-week focused meditation training on depression and anxiety symptoms in Brazilian university students with 6 and 12 months of follow-up. J. Affect. Disord. 246, 401–407. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.12.126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, D. M., and Hayes, J. A. (2011). What are the benefits of mindfulness? A practice review of psychotherapy-related research. Psychotherapy 48, 198–208. doi: 10.1037/a0022062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, B. R., Hartigan, J. A., and Mikulas, W. L. (1999). Concentration and mindfulness meditations: unique forms of consciousness? Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 24, 147–165. doi: 10.1023/A:1023498629385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Essex, H., Parrott, S., Atkin, K., Ballard, K., Bland, M., Eldred, J., et al. (2017). An economic evaluation of Alexander technique lessons or acupuncture sessions for patients with chronic neck pain: a randomized trial (ATLAS). PLoS One 12:e0178918. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0178918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallegos, A. M., Crean, H. F., Pigeon, W. R., and Heffner, K. L. (2017). Meditation and yoga for posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analytic review of randomized controlled trials. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 58, 115–124. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaylord, S. A., Palsson, O. S., Garland, E. L., Faurot, K. R., Coble, R. S., Mann, J. D., et al. (2011). Mindfulness training reduces the severity of irritable bowel syndrome in women: results of a randomized controlled trial. Am. J. Gastroenterol. 106, 1678–1688. doi: 10.1038/ajg.2011.184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirshberg, M. J., Goldberg, S. B., Schaefer, S. M., Flook, L., Findley, D., and Davidson, R. J. (2018). Divergent effects of brief contemplative practices in response to an acute stressor: a randomized controlled trial of brief breath awareness, loving-kindness, gratitude or an attention control practice. PLoS One 13:e0207765. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0207765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, S. G., Grossman, P., and Hinton, D. E. (2011). Loving-kindness and compassion meditation: potential for psychological interventions. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 1126–1132. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoge, E. A., Bui, E., Marques, L., Metcalf, C. A., Morris, L. K., Robinaugh, D. J., et al. (2013). Randomized controlled trial of mindfulness meditation for generalized anxiety disorder: effects on anxiety and stress reactivity. J. Clin. Psychiatry 74, 786–792. doi: 10.4088/JCP.12m08083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, X., Chang, F., Prakash, R., and Chaudhury, S. (2011). A theoretical model of efficacy of concentrative meditation for cognitive rehabilitation of dementia. Med. Hypotheses 77, 266–269. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2011.04.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., and Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 109–119. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.2.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kral, T. R. A., Schuyler, B. S., Mumford, J. A., Rosenkranz, M. A., Lutz, A., and Davidson, R. J. (2018). Impact of short- and long-term mindfulness meditation training on amygdala reactivity to emotional stimuli. NeuroImage 181, 301–313. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehmann, D., Faber, P. L., Achermann, P., Jeanmonod, D., Gianotti, L. R., and Pizzagalli, D. (2001). Brain sources of EEG gamma frequency during volitionally meditation-induced, altered states of consciousness, and experience of the self. Psychiatry Res. 108, 111–121. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(01)00116-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahapragya, A. (1995). 01.01 What is Preksha. The Mirror of The Self. JVB.

Google Scholar

Majeed, M. H., Ali, A. A., and Sudak, D. M. (2018). Mindfulness-based interventions for chronic pain: evidence and applications. Asian J. Psychiatr. 32, 79–83. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2017.11.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehta, S., Mehta, V., Mehta, S., Shah, D., Motiwala, A., Vardhan, J., et al. (2011). Multimodal behavior program for ADHD incorporating yoga and implemented by high school volunteers: a pilot study. ISRN Pediatr. 2011:780745. doi: 10.5402/2011/780745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendes, A., and Palmer, S. J. (2016). The value of mindfulness in people with long-term conditions. Br. J. Community Nurs. 21:532. doi: 10.12968/bjcn.2016.21.10.532

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poissant, H., Mendrek, A., Talbot, N., Khoury, B., and Nolan, J. (2019). Behavioral and cognitive impacts of mindfulness-based interventions on adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review. Behav. Neurol. 2019:5682050. doi: 10.1155/2019/5682050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pragya, S. U., G, C., Chiu, R., Mehta, N., Johnson, P., Mehta, D. I., et al. (2014). Can the cognitive parameters of college students with learning disabilities benefit from using Mahapraan, a breathing based Preksha meditation. J. College Teach. Learn. 11:169. doi: 10.19030/tlc.v11i4.8854

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiner, K., Tibi, L., and Lipsitz, J. D. (2013). Do mindfulness-based interventions reduce pain intensity? A critical review of the literature. Pain Med. 14, 230–242. doi: 10.1111/pme.12006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satsangi, A. K., and Brugnoli, M. P. (2018). Anxiety and psychosomatic symptoms in palliative care: from neuro-psychobiological response to stress, to symptoms' management with clinical hypnosis and meditative states. Ann. Palliat. Med. 7, 75–111. doi: 10.21037/apm.2017.07.01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serwacki, M. L., and Cook-Cottone, C. (2012). Yoga in the schools: a systematic review of the literature. Int. J. Yoga Therap. 22, 101–109. doi: 10.17761/ijyt.22.1.7716244t75u4l702

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sevinc, G., Holzel, B. K., Hashmi, J., Greenberg, J., McCallister, A., Treadway, M., et al. (2018). Common and dissociable neural activity after mindfulness-based stress reduction and relaxation response programs. Psychosom. Med. 80, 439–451. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000590

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Somers, D. C., and Sheremata, S. L. (2013). Attention maps in the brain. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 4, 327–340. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundquist, J., Palmer, K., Johansson, L. M., and Sundquist, K. (2017). The effect of mindfulness group therapy on a broad range of psychiatric symptoms: a randomised controlled trial in primary health care. Eur. Psychiatry 43, 19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.01.328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suriji Maharaja, V. K. (1997). "Dhyana-vicara:Savivecana. Haridwara" (Indian Journal).

Google Scholar

Tolahunase, M. R., Sagar, R., Faiq, M., and Dada, R. (2018). Yoga- and meditation-based lifestyle intervention increases neuroplasticity and reduces severity of major depressive disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 36, 423–442. doi: 10.3233/RNN-170810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulsi (1994). Transmutation of personality through preksha meditation. Jain Vishva Bharati: Ladnun.

Google Scholar

Valdez, P., and Mehrabian, A. (1994). Effects of color on emotions. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 123, 394–409. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.123.4.394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, D., Rao, H., Korczykowski, M., Wintering, N., Pluta, J., Khalsa, D. S., et al. (2011). Celebral blood flow changes associated with different meditation practices and perceived depth of meditation. Psychiatry Res. 191, 66–67. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: meditation, short-term memory, attention, affect, college students

Citation: Pragya SU, Mehta ND, Abomoelak B, Uddin P, Veeramachaneni P, Mehta N, Moore S, Jean-Francois M, Garcia S, Pragya SC and Mehta DI (2021) Effects of Combining Meditation Techniques on Short-Term Memory, Attention, and Affect in Healthy College Students. Front. Psychol. 12:607573. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.607573

Received: 18 September 2020; Accepted: 09 February 2021;
Published: 05 March 2021.

Edited by:

David Facal, University of Santiago de Compostela, Spain

Reviewed by:

Miguel Angel Ruiz-Carabias, University of Murcia, Spain
Irene Estores, University of Florida, United States

Copyright © 2021 Pragya, Mehta, Abomoelak, Uddin, Veeramachaneni, Mehta, Moore, Jean-Francois, Garcia, Pragya and Mehta. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Devendra I. Mehta, ZGV2ZW5kcmEubWVodGFAb3JsYW5kb2hlYWx0aC5jb20=, orcid.org/0000-0002-2267-866X

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.