Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 28 January 2021
Sec. Psychology of Language

The Trade-Off Between Format Familiarity and Word-Segmentation Facilitation in Chinese Reading

Updated
\r\nMingjing ChenMingjing ChenYongsheng Wang*Yongsheng Wang*Bingjie ZhaoBingjie ZhaoXin LiXin LiXuejun Bai*Xuejun Bai*
  • Key Research Base of Humanities and Social Sciences, Institute of Psychology and Behavior, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin, China

In alphabetic writing systems (such as English), the spaces between words mark the word boundaries, and the basic unit of reading is distinguished during visual-level processing. The visual-level information of word boundaries facilitates reading. Chinese is an ideographic language whose text contains no intrinsic inter-word spaces as the marker of word boundaries. Previous studies have shown that the basic processing unit of Chinese reading is also a word. However, findings remain inconsistent regarding whether inserting spaces between words in Chinese text promotes reading performance. Researchers have proposed that there may be a trade-off between format familiarity and the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces. In order to verify this, this study manipulated the format familiarity via reversing the Chinese reading direction from right to left to investigate this issue in Experiment 1 and Experiment 2. The purpose of Experiment 1 was to examine whether inter-word spaces facilitated Chinese reading in an unfamiliar format. Experiment 1 was conducted that 40 native Chinese undergraduates read Chinese sentences from right to left on four format conditions. The results showed faster reading speed and shorter total reading time for the inter-word spaced format. Based on this finding, Experiment 2 examined whether the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces would reduce or disappear after improving the format familiarity; this experiment was conducted that 40 native Chinese undergraduates who did not participate in Experiment 1 read Chinese sentences from right to left on four format conditions after ten-day reading training. There was no significant difference between the total reading time and reading speed in the inter-word spaced format and unspaced format, which suggests that the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces in Chinese reading changed smaller. The combined results of the two experiments suggest that there is indeed a trade-off between format familiarity and the facilitation of word segmentation, which supports the assumption of previous studies.

Introduction

As the saying goes, “Read wide, and you will wisely write.” In other words, reading is the basic way for humans to acquire information, as well as an effective way to inherit human knowledge and civilization. Different civilizations have produced different languages with different characteristics. For example, Chinese is an ideographic language that differs from an alphabetic language like English, whose texts contain intrinsic inter-word spaces as word boundary markers. The basic independent unit of reading processing whether in Chinese or English is the word (Rayner, 1998, 2009; Rayner et al., 1998; Reichle et al., 1999; Inhoff et al., 2000; Yan et al., 2006; Li et al., 2009; Perea and Acha, 2009). The primary task for readers of Chinese is to segment words from Chinese texts which do not contain inter-word spaces as an indicator of word segmentation. The characteristic of no inter-word spaces has been the subject of many studies focusing on the mechanism of word segmentation in Chinese reading (Rayner, 1998; Bai et al., 2008; Bassetti, 2009; Li et al., 2009, 2014; Cui et al., 2014; Zang et al., 2016; Ma, 2017; Liu and Lu, 2018; Ma and Zhuang, 2018; Zhou et al., 2020). Moreover, the reading direction is one of the significant characteristics among languages. In some languages, such as Hebrew and Arabic, text by default is read from right to left (Deutsch and Rayner, 1999). The default direction of ancient Chinese texts was also from right to left and from top to bottom. However, the direction of modern Chinese texts has changed to a left to right direction, which is a familiar reading format for native Chinese readers. This poses the following questions: when reading texts with an unfamiliar format which differs from the default format (for example, Arabic readers whose default format is reading from right to left in arabic texts read Chinese texts from left to right in unfamiliar format), how do readers understand and process the information given in the unfamiliar format? Does reading performance change under unfamiliar format? Are there any differences between reading processing in the unfamiliar format and the familiar format? These questions need to be explored in psychology of reading.

It is a subject of debate whether inter-word spaces as indicators of word segmentation could promote Chinese reading. Many studies have supported inter-word spaces being explicit indicators that promote word recognition and reading processing (Yan et al., 2010; Li et al., 2011a; Zang et al., 2013; Bai et al., 2015). However, some researchers did not find evidence of inter-word spaces facilitating reading; a representative study is that by Bai et al. (2008). In this study, the segmentation was manipulated to present Chinese sentences in different formats (unspaced format, inter-word spaced format, inter-character spaced format, non-word spaced format) to native Chinese readers in natural reading. However, the results did not completely support the research hypothesis: there was no facilitation effect of inter-word spaces. To explain this finding, it was proposed that there may be a trade-off between format familiarity and word-segmentation facilitation in Chinese reading. This means that inter-word spaces do indeed facilitate Chinese reading; however, the format unfamiliarity of inserting spaces into texts offsets the facilitation of word segmentation. To verify this proposal, the following studies found that inter-word spaces facilitated Chinese reading for foreign students and children with dyslexia (Bai et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2010; Shen et al., 2012; Zang et al., 2013). What these participants had in common was a low level of proficiency in Chinese reading; in addition, they were unfamiliar with the format of unspaced Chinese texts, which was caused by their insufficient reading experience. Therefore, inter-word spaces facilitated reading in this unfamiliar format. However, the studies using Chinese native readers as participants did not find that inter-word spaces promoted the reading process in the familiar format (Shen et al., 2010; Yan et al., 2012). In summary, the format familiarity of Chinese text was the distinguishing factor between the studies finding that inter-word spaces facilitated reading and those that found no evidence of facilitation.

Based on previous findings, this study manipulated word segmentation and format familiarity to investigate whether there is a trade-off between format familiarity and word-segmentation facilitation in Chinese reading. In Experiment 1, the Chinese sentences were presented in four segmented formats (unspaced format, inter-character spaced format, inter-word spaced format, non-word spaced format). Chinese readers were required to read from right to left, which was an unfamiliar format for them. In Experiment 2, the reading training (reading the Chinese texts from right to left) was manipulated to improve participants’ familiarity with the format. Following this, the same Chinese sentences were presented in the same four segmented formats and the Chinese readers were required to read from right to left after having improved their familiarity with the format. Based on the assumption that there is a trade-off between format familiarity and word-segmentation facilitation in Chinese reading, if the results of Experiment 1 showed that inserting spaces between words did indeed facilitate the reading process, this would prove that inter-word spaces did facilitate reading in the unfamiliar format. On the other hand, if inserting spaces between words did not promote Chinese reading, it would imply that inter-word spaces did not facilitate reading in the unfamiliar format.

After obtaining the results in Experiment 1, the study turned to Experiment 2. The unfamiliar format was changed to a familiar format via reading training in Chinese reverse texts. If the facilitation of word segmentation reduced or disappeared under this condition, it would verify that the facilitation was offset or partly offset by the format familiarity. On the other hand, if nothing changed with the facilitation of word segmentation, then it would imply that the format familiarity did not affect inter-word spaces facilitating Chinese reading by giving word-segmentation indications.

Experiment 1

The purpose of Experiment 1 was to determine whether inter-word spaces facilitate Chinese reading in an unfamiliar format without relevant reading experience.

Method

Participants

The participants in Experiment 1 were 40 undergraduate students (mean ages 20.78 ± 1.21 years). There were 28 females and 12 males; their mother tongue was Chinese; they had normal or corrected vision; and they were right-handed dominant. Each participant signed a project agreement form before the experiment commenced. Based on the Declaration of Helsinki (BMJ 1991; 302: 1194), Tianjin Normal University’s Medical Ethical Committee approved the experiment.

Experimental Design and Materials

The experimental design was a single-factor (word segmentation) within-subjects design which contained four conditions: (1) Unspaced format: Chinese default format with no spaces in sentences; (2) Inter-character spaced format: spaces between adjacent characters in sentences; (3) Inter-word spaced format: spaces between words in sentences; (4) Non-word spaced format: spaces were randomly inserted between sentences to turn adjacent words into non-words. Examples of sentences in the four conditions are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Example Chinese stimuli from the four conditions used in the experiment.

A total of 60 Chinese sentences were constructed, which all ranged between 15 and 17 characters in length (M = 15.8 characters, SD = 0.80 characters). The experimental sentences were rated on a seven-point scale for their naturalness by 30 participants who did not take part in the eye-tracking experiment. The mean naturalness score was 6.69 (where a score of 7 was very natural) and the consistency of the word segmentation in sentences was 91% by 12 participants who did not take part in the eye-tracking experiment. Experiment 1 constructed four files, each of which had 60 sentences. The 60 sentences were allocated to the four conditions, which were rotated in the form of a Latin square and contained 15 sentences. The blocked format was the presented format of sentences in each condition, which had a random order. Each file contained 12 practical sentences which was allocated to four conditions, each of which contained three sentences. The 12 practical sentences were presented to be read first in each file. Participants needed to answer yes/no after each of 22 sentences, after which followed a comprehension question where the number of yes and no responses was equal. Each participant read 72 sentences in total.

Apparatus

The experiment recorded right-eye movements using EyeLink 1000 (SR Research, Canada); the sampling rate was 1,000 Hz, while the accuracy rate was a 0.5° visual angle. We adjusted the resolution of the stimulus presented on a 19-inch Dell monitor to 1,024 × 768. In the experiment, participants maintained a distance of 70 cm from the screen. The characters had a size of 25 × 25 pixels and the visual angle was 0.80°, and they were presented in sentences in the Song font.

Procedure

The participants were told to read sentences from right to left in different conditions before the experiment. Participants needed to understand the meaning of sentences as quickly as possible and press the space bar to read the next sentence. In some sentences a comprehension question followed, which participants had to then answer as correctly as possible. Chin rests were used to ensure that participants’ heads remained in a resting position to compensate for head movement. A calibration was completed before the experiment to calculate the position of the fixed point. Participants started the test after successful calibration. If necessary, the eye location would be recalibrated during the experiment. The experiment lasted about 20 min. The participants’ responses to the comprehension questions achieved a correctness rate of 91.0%, which indicated that the sentences had been read and understood.

Data Preparation and Analysis

According to the following criteria (Rayner et al., 2006; Bai et al., 2008; Rayner, 2009; Liu et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2018), the analysis of fixation durations excluded data shorter than 80 ms and longer than 800 ms. The data were excluded if: (1) participants pressed the key incorrectly during the experiment, which resulted in an interruption; (2) data were lost due to accidental factors (such as head movement); (3) there were fewer than four gazes; (4) the data were outside three standard deviations. After excluding invalid data (2.8% of the total data), data analysis was conducted.

The experiment computed eye-movement measures of target words as follows: (1) Mean fixation duration (average fixation duration in all fixated points in the sentence); (2) Mean saccadic length (average length of all saccades from the current fixated point to the next one in the sentence); (3) Number of fixations (number of all fixations in the sentence); (4) Total fixation duration (sum of the fixation duration in all fixation points in the sentence); (5) Reading speed (average number of reading words per second in the sentences); (6) Forward saccadic length (saccadic length reading from right to left). The units of the time index (mean fixation duration; total fixation duration) were milliseconds; the units of the mean saccadic length and forward saccadic length were characters; while characters per second were the units of reading speed. SPSS 20 for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, United States) was used to process statistical data. A repeated-measures analysis of variance, including subject analysis (F1) and item analysis (F2), was conducted (Shen et al., 2010; Bai et al., 2012a, 2015; Zang et al., 2013).

Results

The results of the global analysis are shown in Tables 2A, 2B, 3.

TABLE 2A
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2A. Global eye movement measure under four conditions.

TABLE 2B
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2B. The 95% confidence intervals of global eye movement measure under four conditions.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. F values, degrees of freedom, p values, η2 values, and post hoc comparisons for each index.

The results showed a significant word-segmentation effect in the mean fixation duration: F1(3,117) = 61.82, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.61; F2(3,177) = 59.46, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.50. Furthermore, the results of the post hoc test showed no significant difference between the inter-word spaced condition and the non-word spaced condition, p > 0.05. The difference between inter-character spaced condition and inter-word spaced condition was also not significant, p > 0.05. There were significant differences between each of the other two conditions, ps < 0.05. The mean fixation duration of the unspaced format was the longest, ps < 0.001. The mean fixation duration of the inter-character spaced format was the shortest, ps < 0.05. This meant that participants took a shorter duration to process words and understand the Chinese sentences. Inserting spaces into characters in Chinese sentences could reduce the difficulty of Chinese reading.

In Chinese, the saccade length, which reflects the language information in the unit space, is generally only two to three Chinese characters (Inhoff and Liu, 1998; Yan and Bai, 2007; Yan et al., 2013). A longer saccade length indicates that the participants obtained relatively more information during the saccade fixation (Irwin, 1998; Yan and Bai, 2000; Yan et al., 2013). There was a significant word-segmentation effect in the mean saccadic length: F1(3,117) = 188.44, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.83; F2(3,177) = 149.32, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.72. The results of the post hoc test showed that there was a significant difference between each of the two conditions, ps < 0.01. The mean saccadic length in the inter-character spaced format was the longest, ps < 0.001. The mean saccadic length in the inter-word spaced format was longer than that in the unspaced format which was the shortest, p < 0.001. The main effect of word segmentation was significant in the forward saccadic length format, F1 (3,117) = 128.84, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.77; F2 (3,177) = 127.65, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.68. Furthermore, the results of the post hoc test showed that forward saccadic length in the inter-character spaced format was the longest, significantly longer than in the other conditions, ps < 0.001. Forward saccadic length in the inter-word spaced format was significantly longer than that of the unspaced format, p < 0.001. The forward saccadic length in the non-word spaced format was significantly longer than in the unspaced format, p < 0.05, which was marginally shorter than that in the inter-word spaced format, p = 0.068.

The number of fixations refers to the number of all fixation points, which could reflect the cognitive load of the reading material (Yan et al., 2013). The more difficult and complex the reading materials are, the higher the number of fixations (Henderson and Ferreira, 1990; Yan et al., 2013). The higher the reading level, the lower the number of fixations for the same reading material (Rayner et al., 2011). We found that there was a significant word-segmentation effect in the number of fixations: F1(3,117) = 11.61, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.23; F2(3,177) = 3.90, p = 0.010, η2 = 0.062. The results of the post hoc test showed that the number of fixations in the inter-character spaced format was the highest, ps < 0.05. The number of fixations in the inter-word spaced format was less than that of the non-word spaced format, p < 0.05. The difference between other conditions was not significant, ps > 0.05. The number of fixations in the inter-word spaced condition was significantly less than that under non-word spaced condition, which may be due to non-word spaces interfering with word segmentation and increasing the difficulty of reading.

The total reading time is sensitive to slower and longer cognitive processing, which can reflect the processing difficulty of reading sentences (Rayner et al., 2011; Yan et al., 2013). In the results, the main effect of word segmentation was significant, F1(3,117) = 10.88, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.22; F2(3,177) = 2.82, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.05 The results of the post hoc test showed that the total fixation duration in the inter-word spaced format was the shortest of the four conditions, ps < 0.01. The difference between each of the other conditions was not significant, ps > 0.05. This implies that inter-word spaces as word-segmentation clues could shorten the reading time and reduce the difficulty of Chinese texts. The reading speed represented the Chinese characters read per second. A faster reading speed means faster processing to understand the words and sentences and a lower difficulty with word recognition and reading comprehension. The results showed that the main effect of word segmentation on reading speed was significant, F1(3,117) = 10.84, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.22; F2(3,177) = 1.849, p = 0.140. Furthermore, the results of the post hoc test showed that the reading speed in the inter-word spaced format was significantly faster than in the other conditions, ps < 0.05. The difference between each other conditions was not significant, ps > 0.05. The reading speed in the inter-character spaced format was slower than that in the unspaced format, but the difference between them was not significant, ps > 0.05. The reading speed on inter-character spaced format was slowest, which was not significant with that on the unspaced format and non-word spaced format, ps > 0.05. This showed that the inter-word spaces as word boundaries provided visual clues that facilitated Chinese reading and improved the reading speed, which is a finding consistent with previous results (Bai et al., 2009, 2015, 2014; Shen et al., 2010, 2012; Yan et al., 2010; Li and Pollatsek, 2011; Li et al., 2011b; Zang et al., 2013). In sum, the results in experiment 1 supported the research hypothesis.

Discussion

The purpose of Experiment 1 was to determine whether inter-word spaces could facilitate Chinese reading in an unfamiliar format without relevant reading experience. We used four word-segmentation conditions to compare the eye movements of participants reading Chinese sentences from right to left in an unfamiliar format.

The previous results did not show the effect of inter-word spaces facilitating Chinese reading (Bai et al., 2008). The assumption proposed was that there was a trade-off between format unfamiliarity and facilitation effect of inter-word spaces in Chinese reading. The results showed that inserting spaces into words as indicators of word segmentation facilitated Chinese reading in the unfamiliar format, which supports the assumption. The logic behind this finding is that if there is a trade-off between format unfamiliarity and the facilitation of inter-word spaces, then the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces is offset by the format unfamiliarity of inserting spaces between words. However, the texts in the unspaced format had the format familiarity of the Chinese default format. There was no difference in the total reading time and reading speed between the inter-word spaced format and unspaced format. Therefore, if readers were both unfamiliar with the unspaced format and inter-word spaced format, the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces would not be offset by the unfamiliarity of the format. The results showing the total reading time was shorter and the reading speed was faster in the inter-word spaced format compared to the unspaced format indeed supported the assumption that there was a trade-off between format unfamiliarity and the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces in Chinese reading. In addition, the total reading time in the inter-word spaced format was shorter than that in the inter-character spaced format and non-word spaced format, which had the longest reading time. This may be due to the basic unit of information processing Chinese being a word rather than a character.

According to the results in Experiment 1, the readers’ familiarity with the format affected the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces; the inter-word spaces significantly facilitated Chinese reading where readers lacked reading experience of reverse text from right to left. We can therefore assume that with increased reading experience of Chinese reversed texts, the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces would gradually decrease or even disappear. This problem will be further verified in Experiment 2.

Experiment 2

Experiment 1 showed that the inter-word spaces facilitated Chinese reading in an unfamiliar format in participants without relevant reading experience. Based on this finding, Experiment 2 focused on whether the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces would change with increased reading experience from right to left following reading training. Therefore, the combined results of Experiment 1 and 2 will demonstrate whether there is a trade-off between format unfamiliarity and the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces in Chinese reading.

Method

Participants

The participants in Experiment 2 were 40 undergraduate students who did not participate in Experiment 1 (mean ages 20.50 ± 1.63 years). There were 31 females and 9 males; their mother tongue was Chinese; they had normal or corrected vision; and they were right-handed dominant. Each participant signed a project agreement form before the experiment commenced. Based on the Declaration of Helsinki (BMJ 1991; 302: 1194), Tianjin Normal University’s Medical Ethical Committee approved the experiment.

Experimental Design and Materials

The experimental design was a single-factor (word segmentation) within-subjects design which contained four condition, same as that in experiment 1. Examples of sentences in the four conditions are shown in Table 1. The materials used for the eye-movement tests were the same as in Experiment 1.

The materials in the reading training were 60 Chinese essays (average number of words M = 936) chosen from Chinese high-school textbooks, which were reversed from left–right format to right–left format through reversing software (see the Appendix for examples of reading materials).

Apparatus

The apparatus was same as that in Experiment 1.

Procedure

The procedure involved stages of reading training and eye movement. Firstly, participants entered the laboratory and were made familiar with the environment every day. Then the participants sat in their own seats where the experiment book was presented, which contained essays that the participants needed to read every day. The participants then read essays. Before the reading commenced, the researcher would give the following instruction: “Below you will read some articles. The sentences in the article will be presented from right to left. Please read carefully word by word and understand the article as much as possible. Seven reading comprehension questions will appear after each article. You are required to select the most appropriate answer based on the article and fill in the answer.” The participants began to read an article after understanding the instruction and then answered seven questions, before moving on to the next question.

The reading training lasted for 30 min every day for ten days. After 10 days of reading training, eye-movement testing began, followed the same procedure as Experiment 1. The participants’ responses to the comprehension questions in the eye-movement stage achieved a correctness rate 93.0%, which indicated that the sentences had been read and understood the sentences seriously.

Data Preparation and Analysis

According to the following criteria which were same as that in Experiment 1 (Rayner et al., 2006; Bai et al., 2008; Rayner, 2009; Li et al., 2017; Liang et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2018), analysis data was selected. After excluding invalid data (1.65% of the total data), data analysis was conducted.

The experiment computed eye-movement measures of target words which were same as that in the experiment 1. The same method in experiment 1 was used to process the statistical data. A repeated-measures analysis of variance, including subject analysis (F1) and item analysis (F2), was conducted (Shen et al., 2010; Bai et al., 2012a, b, 2015; Yu et al., 2018).

Results

The results of the global analysis are shown in Tables 4A, 4B, 5.

TABLE 4A
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4A. Global eye movement measure under four conditions.

TABLE 4B
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4B. The 95% confidence intervals of global eye movement measure under four conditions.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. F values, degrees of freedom, p values, η2 values, and post hoc comparisons for each index.

The results of time indicators including mean fixation time, number of fixations, total time and reading speed, and space indicators including mean saccadic length and forward saccadic length are presented as follows.

Firstly, in the time metric results, there was a significant word-segmentation effect in the mean fixation duration: F1(3,117) = 76.6, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.66; F2(3,177) = 25.70, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.30. The post-test results showed no significant difference between the inter-word spaced condition and inter-character spaced condition, p > 0.05. There were significant differences between each of the other two conditions, ps < 0.05. The mean fixation duration of the unspaced condition was the longest, ps < 0.001, which implies that the lack of word spaces caused interference in Chinese reading. It is surprising that the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces did not appear in the total time and reading speed. The main effect of word segmentation was significant on total time, F1(3,117) = 18.01, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.42; F2(3,177) = 6.05, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.09. The post hoc test showed that there was not a significant difference between the total time in the unspaced format and the inter-word spaced format, ps > 0.05. This result was consistent with the assumption that the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces changed smaller. Furthermore, there was not a significant difference in the total reading time between the inter-character spaced format and the non-word spaced format, ps > 0.05. This may be the interference of inter-characters offset that of non-word spaces. The difference between each of the other conditions was significant, ps < 0.01. Combined with reading speed, the main effect of word segmentation was significant on reading speed, F1(3,117) = 20.92, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.35; F2(3,177) = 5.84, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.09. The post-test results showed that the reading speed in the inter-word spaced format was the fastest, but was not significantly different to that in the unspaced format, ps > 0.05. The reading speed in the inter-character spaced format was not significantly different to that in the non-word spaced format, ps > 0.05. However, before the reading training, the reading speed in the inter-word spaced format was significantly faster than that in the unspaced format in Experiment 1, which meant the facilitation of inter-word spaces changed smaller in the Experiment 2.

Secondly, in the results on spacing indicators, we found a significant word-segmentation effect in the number of fixations: F1(3,117) = 28.31, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.42; F2 (3,177) = 21.96, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.27. The post hoc test showed that there was significant difference between each condition, ps < 0.05. Except the comparison between the inter-character word format and the non-word format, which was not significant, p = 0.21. The highest number of fixations was found in the inter-character spaced format, ps < 0.05. This may be because the inter-character spaces caused reading interference in Chinese texts. Furthermore, there was a significant word-segmentation effect on the mean saccadic length: F1(3,117) = 209.98,p < 0.001, η2 = 0.84; F2(3,177) = 221.88, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.79. The post hoc test results showed the difference between inter-word spaced format and the non-word spaced format was not significantly, p > 0.05. There was a significant difference between each of the other conditions, ps < 0.01. The mean saccadic length in the non-word spaced format was longer than that in the unspaced condition, ps < 0.001. This was because the language information per unit space was the largest under the unspaced format. Furthermore, the main effect of word segmentation on forward saccadic length was significant, F1 (3,117) = 189.06, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.83; F2 (3,177) = 258.55, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.81. The results in post hoc test showed that the difference between each other conditions was significant, ps < 0.001. In sum, the results in the experiment 2 showed that the facilitation of word segmentation changed smaller, which supported research hypothesis.

Discussion

The purpose of Experiment 2 was to determine whether the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces would change after reading training to improve participants’ familiarity with the format. Combined with the results in the Experiment 1, whether there was a trade-off between format familiarity and the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces was verified.

The results found that the difference between the inter-word spaced format and unspaced format was not significant on the total fixation duration and reading speed, which meant that the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces as word-segmentation clues changed smaller or disappeared. This is consistent with previous studies where no facilitation of inter-word spaces was found for Chinese native readers (Bai et al., 2008, 2012a; Shen et al., 2010; Yan et al., 2012). The common feature of these studies is that the participants were readers with abundant Chinese reading experience, which meant high level of format familiarity in the default Chinese text. Therefore, inter-word spaces did not facilitate the process of Chinese reading when readers had rich experience, that is in the familiar format. In Experiment 2, because reverse-order reading training increased participants’ relevant reading experience, the unfamiliarity of the reverse-order format changed to format familiarity. In a familiar format, the inter-word spaces as word-segmentation clues did not facilitate Chinese reading. The results supported the research hypothesis in Experiment 2: after increasing participants’ familiarity with the format, the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces in Chinese reading changed smaller. There is the other possible explanation, which is “floor effect,” the subjects have reached the best reading performance after reading training in the experiment 2, the inter-word spaces could not facilitate the best reading performance. The “floor effect” may be explain the findings in experiment 2, which could not explain why there is facilitation of inter-word spaces on unfamiliar format in the experiment 1 for Chinese native readers, who also have the best reading ability and best reading performance. The Chinese reading ability would not disappear for just reversing the reading direction. Of course, the “floor effect” should be investigated in the further research via reading training of inter-word spaces for the other subjects.

The combined results of Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 show that there was indeed a trade-off between format unfamiliarity and the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces in Chinese reading, which verified the research assumption (Bai et al., 2008).

Supplementary Analysis

The facilitation effect of inter-word spaces in a familiar format was examined in Experiment 1 and that in an unfamiliar format was examined in Experiment 2. Combining Experiment1 and Experiment 2, supplementary analysis was conducted to investigate the role of word segmentation and format familiarity in Chinese reading. A repeated-analysis measurement was conducted: 4 (word segmentation: unspaced condition, inter-character spaced condition, inter-word spaced condition, non-word spaced condition) 2 (format familiarity: unfamiliar format and familiar format). The analysis are shown in Table 6.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. F values, p values, ηP2 values of word segmentation and format familiarity and that of Interaction for each index.

The main effect of word segmentation was significant on the mean fixation duration, F1 (3,234) = 133.58, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.63; F2 (3,177) = 94.27, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.62; the main effect of format familiarity on the mean fixation duration was not significant, F1(1,78) = 0.02, p > 0.1; F2 (1,59) = 2.902, p = 0.09; the interaction between word segmentation and format familiarity was not significant, F1 (3,234) = 1.88, p > 0.1; F2(3,177) = 0.824, p > 0.1. See Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The mean fixation time under the four conditions in the unfamiliar format and familiar format.

The main effect of word segmentation on the mean saccadic length was significant, F1(3,234) = 382.85, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.83; F2(3,177) = 300.42, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.84; the main effect of format familiarity was significant, F1(1,78) = 22.40, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.22; F2 (1,59) = 1590.65, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.96; the interaction between word segmentation and reading training was significant, F1(3,234) = 9.37, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.11; F2 (3,177) = 8.91, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.13. See Figure 2. A simple-effects analysis found that the main effect of word segmentation on the unfamiliar format was significant, F1 (3,76) = 139.16, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.85; F2 (3,57) = 155.64, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.89. In the unfamiliar format, there was a significant difference between each condition (ps < 0.01). On the other hand, there was a significant difference between the four conditions in the familiar format, F1(3,76) = 81.83, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.76, F2 (3,57) = 327.16, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.95. There was only one difference in the familiar format, where the difference between inter-word spaced format and non-word spaced format was not significant, p = 0.24. This meant the interference of non-word and facilitation of inter-word reduced were decreased at the same time on the familiar format.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The mean saccadic length under four conditions in the unfamiliar format and familiar format.

There was a significant word-segmentation effect [F1(3,234) = 29.198, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.27; F2 (3,177) = 12.96, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.18] and a significant format-familiarity effect [F1(1,78) = 57.82, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.43; F2 (1,59) = 762.48, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.93] on the number of fixations. The interaction was significant on the subjective analysis, F1 (3,234) = 3.06, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.04; F2 (3,177) = 0.985, p = 0.401. See Figure 3. The simple-effects analysis found that there was a significant word-segmentation effect on the unfamiliar format, F1(3,76) = 16.25, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.39; F2(3, 57) = 4.14, p = 0.01, ηp2 = 0.18. The subject-analysis results showed that the number of fixations in the inter-character spaced condition was larger than the other conditions (ps < 0.01). However, there was no significant difference between the unspaced condition and the inter-word spaced condition (p = 1.00), and the difference between the unspaced and the non-word spaced conditions was not significant (p = 0.60). There were significant differences between each of other conditions (ps < 0.01). The item analysis results were not totally consistent with those of the subject analysis. Firstly, there was no significance between the unspaced condition and the inter-character spaced condition (p = 0.14). Secondly, there was not significant between the inter-character spaced and non-spaced condition (p = 1.00). On the other hand, there was a significant word-segmentation effect on the familiar format, F1(3, 76) = 16.42, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.39; F2(3, 57) = 16.51, p < 0.001. ηp2 = 0.47. The subject-analysis results showed that the number of fixations in the inter-character spaced condition was the largest, with no significant difference with that in the non-word spaced condition (p = 0.23). At the same time, there was no significant difference between the unspaced condition and the inter-word spaced condition (p = 0.12). There were significant differences between each other condition (ps < 0.05). The item analysis results were not totally consistent with those of the subject analysis. Firstly, there was no significant difference between the unspaced condition and the inter-character spaced condition (p = 0.23). There were significant differences between each of the other conditions (ps < 0.05).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The number of fixations under four conditions in the unfamiliar format and familiar format.

The main effect of word segmentation [F1(3,231) = 20.01, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.21; F2(3,177) = 6.98, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.11) and format-familiarity [F1(1,77) = 18.42, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.19; F2(1,59) = 789.81, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.93] on the total fixation duration were significant. The interaction was significant in the subject analysis but not in the item analysis, F1(3,231) = 4.17, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.05; F2 (3,177) = 0.77, p = 0.51. See Figure 4. A simple-effects analysis found the main effect of word segmentation was significant in the unfamiliar format, F1 (3, 75) = 14.71, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.37. The difference between unspaced format and inter-character spaced format was not significant (p = 1.00), and that between unspaced format and non-spaced format was not significant (p = 1.00). These difference was significant (p = 0.008) or marginally significant (p = 0.06) in the experiment 2, which meant the interference of inter-character spaces and non-word spaces was appeared on the familiar format. Interestingly, the facilitation of inter-word spaces changed smaller in the familiar format. The total fixation duration under the inter-word spaced format was the significantly shortest (ps < 0.001). However, the difference between the inter-word spaced format and unspaced format was not significant on the familiar format (p = 1.00). The other results under familiar format were consistent with the results under unfamiliar format.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The total fixation time under four conditions on the unfamiliar format and familiar format.

The main effect of word segmentation [F1(3,234) = 28.17, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.27; F2(3,177) = 6.61, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.10] and format-familiarity [F1(1,78) = 10.55, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.12; F2(1,59) = 657.47, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.91] on reading speed were significant. The interaction was significant in the subject analysis but not on the item analysis, F1(3,234) = 4.22, p < 0.01,ηp2 = 0.051; F2(3,177) = 0.68, p > 0.1. See Figure 5. Simple-effects analysis found the main effect of word segmentation was significant in the unfamiliar format, F1(3, 76) = 12.25, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.33; the subject-analysis results found the reading speed in the inter-word spaced condition was significantly faster than the other conditions, ps < 0.001. The difference between unspaced format and inter-word spaced format was no more significant under familiar format (p = 1.00), which implied that the facilitation of word segmentation was changed smaller. Interestingly, the difference between the unspaced format and inter-character spaced format was not significant under unfamiliar format, which was significant under familiar format. This implied that the interference of inter-character spaces only appeared under the familiar format. On the other hand, there was significant differences under familiar format, F1(3,76) = 19.46, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.43. The subject-analysis results showed that the difference in the unspaced and inter-word spaced condition was not significant, which means that the facilitation of word segmentation changed smaller under familiar format.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The reading speed under four conditions on the unfamiliar format and familiar format.

The main effect of word-segmentation conditions [F1(3,234) = 294.39, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.79; F2(3,177) = 278.54, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.83] on forward saccadic length was significant; however, it was not significant on format-familiarity [F1 (1,78) = 0.000, p > 0.1; F2 (1,59) = 11.27, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.16]. The interaction was not significant in the subject analysis, but was marginally significant in the item analysis, F1 (3,234) = 1.53, p > 0.1; F2 (3,177) = 2.61, p = 0.05, ηp2 = 0.04. See Figure 6. Simple-effects analysis found that the main effect of word segmentation was significant in the unfamiliar format, F2(3, 116) = 279.94, p < 0.001. The subject-analysis results found that the difference was significant in the other conditions (ps < 0.001) except for the inter-word spaced and non-word spaced conditions (p > 0.05).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. The forward saccadic length under four conditions on the unfamiliar format and familiar format.

General Discussion

This study aimed at investigating whether there was a trade-off between format familiarity and word-segmentation facilitation in Chinese reading. The reading training was manipulated to control the format familiarity, and EyeLink 1000 (SR Research, Canada) was used to record the eye tracking of adult participants in four word-segmentation conditions. The results showed there was a trade-off between format unfamiliarity and word-segmentation facilitation in Chinese reading.

Based on this, the primary task for readers is to segment words in unspaced texts in Chinese reading. However, a consensus has not been reached over whether inserting spaces between words facilitates Chinese reading. Bai et al. (2008) did not find that inter-word spaces facilitated the reading of native Chinese undergraduates, which was explained on the basis of a trade-off between inter-word spaces and format familiarity. The facilitating effect was offset by the format unfamiliarity of inter-word spaces. However, in the unspaced condition, where there was neither facilitation of word segmentation or format unfamiliarity, and there was no significant difference in reading performance. Therefore, the format familiarity was controlled in Experiment 1. For readers, the unspaced format and inter-word spaced format in the Chinese reverse texts read from right to left were unfamiliar formats. The results showed that inter-word spaces facilitated reading in the unfamiliar format, where the reading speed in the inter-word spaced condition was faster than that in the unspaced condition. The results supported the prior assumption and were consistent with the research hypothesis. Based on this finding, Experiment 2 was designed to investigate whether the facilitation effect would change, that was disappear or reduce. We improved participants’ familiarity with the unspaced text format which was just like normal Chinese texts via reading reverse Chinese texts for 30 min per day for 10 days. In Experiment 2, the eye tracking of new participants reading the Chinese sentences was recorded; the reason for changing the participants was to retain the same Chinese sentences in the eye-movement experiment. Surprisingly, the results found that there was no significant difference between the reading speed and total fixation time in the unspaced and inter-word spaced conditions. This meant that the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces as word-segmentation indicators changed smaller. The combined results in Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 showed a trade-off between format familiarity and word-segmentation facilitation in Chinese reading.

The default format of the reading and writing system in contemporary Chinese language is from left to right, which is regarded as a familiar format by native Chinese readers. There is a trade-off between format familiarity and the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces in Chinese reading; native Chinese readers have a high level of format familiarity, and so the facilitation of word segmentation did not appear as in prior studies (Bai et al., 2008; Shen et al., 2010; Li et al., 2011b; Yan et al., 2012). The common feature of these previous studies was that the participants had sufficient reading experience to offset the word-segmentation facilitation. This provides a reasonable explanation for this finding. On the other hand, the previous studies which found that inter-word spaces facilitated Chinese reading had a common feature; that is, the participants who were foreign undergraduates or Chinese children with dyslexia had insufficient Chinese reading experience. Lack of Chinese reading experience caused format unfamiliarity, which could not offset the inter-word facilitation. Therefore, there was word-segmentation facilitation of the Chinese reading process in these studies (Bai et al., 2009, 2011; Wang et al., 2010; Blythe et al., 2012; Shen et al., 2012; Gu et al., 2017). The inconsistency of word-segmentation facilitation comes from having sufficient Chinese reading experience. The reverse Chinese texts presented single characters from right to left, and then the readers needed to read from right to left in an unfamiliar format. Therefore, this study used the reading training of reverse Chinese texts in an unfamiliar format, which improved the relevant reading experience for readers. In Experiment 2, the results showed that the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces found in Experiment 1 changed smaller via improving the participants’ familiarity with the format of reverse Chinese texts. In sum, the results supported the research hypothesis.

In addition, the trade-off can also explain the assumption in the previous studies, which proposed that inter-word spaces could play a certain role in promoting Chinese reading of difficult or ambiguous texts (Hsu and Huang, 2000; Inhoff et al., 2000; Inhoff and Radach, 2002; Li et al., 2011a). Hsu and Huang (2000) found that inter-word spaces as word-segmentation indicators help readers to achieve faster word recognition and reading comprehension compared with unspaced texts. Compared with the default format, the readers have to pay a higher reading cost to process the reverse Chinese texts, where the reading difficulty was higher. This means that the inter-word spaces could facilitate Chinese reading in difficult texts.

The combined results in Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 showed that a trade-off between format unfamiliarity and word-segmentation facilitation of Chinese reading did indeed exist. For unspaced Chinese texts, word segmentation did not depend on the low-level visual clues. The mechanisms behind the trade-off were the low-level visual factor (format familiarity) and the high-level cognition factor (reading experience). The mechanism can be explained by the holistic hypothesis of the computational model on the word-segmentation mechanism in Chinese reading (Li et al., 2009). Two assumptions were included in the model: feed-forward assumption and holistic hypothesis. The first assumption supported the process whereby the character recognition system obtains visual information from characters and then transfers it to the word-segmentation stage, and finally integrates it into the word-recognition stage. There is only feed-forward from bottom to top and no feedback from top to bottom in word processing. However, the holistic hypothesis supports the notion that the visual information system, character recognition system and word-recognition system affect the word-segmentation stage and word-recognition stage interactively. Previous studies supported the holistic assumption being suitable for Chinese reading, which is consistent with the results in this study (Li et al., 2009, 2011a; Ma, 2017; Ma and Zhuang, 2018). Based on the holistic hypothesis, the inter-word spaces as low-level visual information affect reading comprehension from bottom to top, while the reading experience as a high-level cognition factor affects reading comprehension from top to bottom. Therefore, the reading experience as a high-level cognitive factor behind format familiarity and inter-word spaces as a low-level visual factor would both affect the Chinese reading, with a trade-off between them. The assumption explained that there was no facilitation effect of inter-word spaces in the previous studies, where the participants were native adults with rich Chinese reading experiences (Bai et al., 2008; Shen et al., 2010; Yan et al., 2012; Liu and Li, 2014). The richness of Chinese reading experiences affected the facilitation of inter-word spaces as the low-level visual clues in word recognition and reading comprehension. However, previous studies of foreign students who learned Chinese as a second language found that the inter-word spaces facilitated reading speed and prompted word recognition as well as reading comprehension. This is because Chinese learning beginners had insufficient Chinese reading experience, and the inter-word spaces could help them to segment word from Chinese texts, so as to acquire faster reading comprehension and word recognition (Wang et al., 2010; Shen et al., 2012; Gu et al., 2017; Zhou et al., 2020).

In the current study, the reading training just contained unspaced reverse texts to improve the format familiarity. Further research could focus on improving the format familiarity of inter-word spaced text via reading training in Chinese reverse texts where spaces have been inserted between words. Whether the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces would appear after improving the format familiarity of inter-word spaced text needs to be further verified. In addition, future studies could manipulate the inter-word spaces, word frequency and format familiarity to explore the relationship between them and examine the mechanism between word segmentation and word recognition. Moreover, The expect direction is the trade-off during learning a new language in the future, especially for non-native readers, when they start to learn Chinese which is not familiar orthography and semantic information, the format familiarity may have more influence on the facilitation of the inter-word spaces. For example, Uyghur whose mother tongue is presented from right to left start to learn Chinese, however, they are unfamiliar with the Chinese format from left to right. The reading performance of Uyghur and that of Chinese could be compared to expect new surprising findings.

Conclusion

To summarize, the results indicate that inter-word spaces as low-level visual clues facilitate Chinese reading in an unfamiliar format. After improving participants’ familiarity with the format, the facilitation effect of inter-word spaces changed smaller. There is a trade-off between format familiarity and facilitation of inter-word spaces, which supported the assumption made in previous studies.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

Based on the Declaration of Helsinki (BMJ 1991; 302: 1194), Tianjin Normal University’s Medical Ethical Committee approved the experiment. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

XB provided the research idea and the research construction. MC and YW designed the experiments and wrote the manuscript. MC conducted the data analysis. BZ participated the eye-movement experiment. XL contributed to writing the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

Ministry of Education, Humanities and Social Sciences Research Youth Project, China, 18YJC190023.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.602931/full#supplementary-material

References

Bai, X., Guo, Z., Cao, Y., Gu, J., Yan, G., and Zang, C. (2012a). Effects of word segmentation on hindi-english bilinguals reading:evidence from eye movements. [www.frontiersin.org]. J. Psychol. Sci. 35, 544–549.

Google Scholar

Bai, X., Liang, F., Yan, G., Tian, J., Zang, C., and Meng, H. (2012b). Inter-word space effects on saccadic target selection in chinese reading: evidence from second language learners. [www.frontiersin.org]. Acta Psychol. Sin. 44, 853–867.

Google Scholar

Bai, X., Li, X., and Yan, G. (2015). Eye movement control in chinese reading: a summary of 20-years research. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 31, 85–91.

Google Scholar

Bai, X., Meng, H., Wang, J., Tian, J., Zang, C., and Yan, G. (2011). The landing positions of dyslexic, age-matched and ability-matched children during reading spaced text. [www.frontiersin.org]. Acta Psychol. Sin. 43, 851–862.

Google Scholar

Bai, X., Tian, J., Yan, G., and Wang, T. L. (2009). An eye movement study:the role of word segmentation on chinese reading for American college students. Nan Kai Linguist. 1, 140–153.

Google Scholar

Bai, X., Zhang, M., Zang, C., Li, X., Chen, L., and Yan, G. (2014). The effect of word boundary information on chinese word acquisition and recognition: evidence from eye movements. [www.frontiersin.org]. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1–8.

Google Scholar

Bai, X. J., Yan, G. L., Liversedge, S. P., Zang, C. L., and Rayner, K. (2008). Reading spaced and unspaced chinese text: Evidence from eye movements. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 34, 1277–1287. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.5.1277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bassetti, B. (2009). Effects of adding interword spacing on chinese reading: a comparison of chinese native readers and English readers of chinese as a second language. Appl. Psycholinguist. 30, 757–775. doi: 10.1017/s0142716409990105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blythe, H. I., Liang, F., Zang, C., Wang, J., Yan, G., Bai, X., et al. (2012). Inserting spaces into chinese text helps readers to learn new words: an eye movement study. J. Mem. Lang. 67, 241–254. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, L., Drieghe, D., Bai, X., Yan, G., and Liversedge, S. P. (2014). Parafoveal preview benefit in unspaced and spaced chinese reading. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 67, 2172–2188.

Google Scholar

Deutsch, A., and Rayner, K. (1999). Initial fixation location effects in reading Hebrew words. Lang. Cogn. Process. 14, 393–421. doi: 10.1080/016909699386284

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, J. J., Zhang, Y., and Zheng, H. Y. (2017). An eye movement study: the role of word segmentation for Russian students on chinese reading. Psychol. Res. 10, 22–29.

Google Scholar

Henderson, J. M., and Ferreira, F. (1990). Effects of foveal processing difficulty on the perceptual span in reading - implications for attention and eye-movement control. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 16, 417–429. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.16.3.417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, S. H., and Huang, K. C. (2000). Interword spacing in chinese text layout. Percept. Mot. Skills 91, 355–365. doi: 10.2466/pms.91.6.355-365

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inhoff, A. W., and Liu, W. M. (1998). The perceptual span and oculomotor activity during the reading of chinese sentences. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24, 20–34. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.1.20

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inhoff, A. W., and Radach, R. (2002). The role of spatial information in the reading of complex words. Comments Theor. Biol. 7, 121–138.

Google Scholar

Inhoff, A. W., Radach, R., and Heller, D. (2000). Complex compounds in German: Interword spaces facilitate segmentation but hinder assignment of meaning. J. Mem. Lang. 42, 23–50. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2666

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irwin, D. E. (1998). Lexical processing during saccadic eye movements. Cogn. Psychol. 36, 1–27. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Bicknell, K., Liu, P., Wei, W., and Rayner, K. (2014). Reading is fundamentally similar across disparate writing systems: a systematic characterization of how words and characters influence eye movements in chinese reading. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 895–913. doi: 10.1037/a0033580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Liu, P., and Ma, G. (2011a). Advances in cognitive mechanisms of word segmentation during chinese reading. [www.frontiersin.org]. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 19, 459–470.

Google Scholar

Li, X., Liu, P., and Rayner, K. (2011b). Eye movement guidance in chinese reading: Is there a preferred viewing location? Vis. Res. 51, 1146–1156. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., and Pollatsek, A. (2011). Word knowledge influences character perception. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 833–839. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0115-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Rayner, K., and Cave, K. (2009). On the segmentation of chinese words during reading. Cogn. Psychol. 58, 525–552. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Huang, R., Hua, H., and Li, X. (2017). How do readers select the saccade targets? [www.frontiersin.org]. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 25, 404–412.

Google Scholar

Liang, F., Blythe, H. I., Bai, X., Yan, G., Li, X., Zang, C., et al. (2017). The role of character positional frequency on chinese word learning during natural reading. PLoS One 12:e0187656. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, P., and Li, X. (2014). Inserting spaces before and after words affects word processing differently in chinese: evidence from eye movements. Br. J. Psychol. 105, 57–68. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, P., and Lu, Q. (2018). The effects of spaces on word segmentation in chinese reading: Evidence from eye movements. J. Res. Read. 41, 329–349. doi: 10.1111/1467-9817.12106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Z., Pan, Y., Tong, W., and Liu, N. (2017). Effects of adults aging on word encoding in reading chinese: evidence from disappearing text. Peerj 5:e2897. doi: 10.7717/peerj.2897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, G. (2017). Does interword spacing influence lexical processing in chinese reading? Vis. Cogn. 25, 815–824. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2017.1338322

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, G., and Zhuang, X. (2018). Distributional analyses of word frequency effects in chinese sentence reading and lexical decision tasks. J. Res. Read. 41, S183–S196. doi: 10.1111/1467-9817.12259

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perea, M., and Acha, J. (2009). Space information is important for reading. Vis. Res. 49, 1994–2000. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychol. Bull. 124, 372–422. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 62, 1457–1506. doi: 10.1080/17470210902816461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rayner, K., Fischer, M. H., and Pollatsek, A. (1998). Unspaced text interferes with both word identification and eye movement control. Vis. Res. 38, 1129–1144. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00274-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rayner, K., Liversedge, S. P., and White, S. J. (2006). Eye movements when reading disappearing text: the importance of the word to the right of fixation. Vis. Res. 46, 310–323. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rayner, K., Yang, J., Castelhano, M. S., and Liversedge, S. P. (2011). Eye movements of older and younger readers when reading disappearing text. Psychol. Aging 26, 214–223. doi: 10.1037/a0021279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reichle, E. D., Rayner, K., and Pollatsek, A. (1999). Eye movement control in reading: accounting for initial fixation locations and refixations within the E-Z Reader model. Vis. Res. 39, 4403–4411. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00152-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, D-L., Bai, X-J., Zang, C-L., Yan, G-L., Feng, B-C., and Fan, X-H. (2010). Effect of word segmentation on beginners’ reading: evidence from eye movements. [www.frontiersin.org]. Acta Psychol. Sin. 42, 159–172.

Google Scholar

Shen, D., Liversedge, S., Tian, J., Zang, C., Cui, L., Bai, X., et al. (2012). Eye movements of second language learners when reading spaced and unspaced chinese text. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 18, 192–202. doi: 10.1037/a0027485

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Tian, J., Zang, C., and Bai, X. (2010). “Spaced chinese text reading in chinese developmental dyslexic children,” in Proceedings of the Poster Session Presented at the 4th China International Conference on Eye Movement, Tianjin.

Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Zhao, B., Chen, M., Li, X., Yan, G., and Bai, X. (2018). Influence of the frequency of fixated words and the number of strokes of parafoveal words on saccadic target selection in chinese reading. [www.frontiersin.org]. Acta Psychol. Sin. 50, 1336–1345.

Google Scholar

Yan, G., and Bai, X. (2000). A study of eye movement in chinese reading process. Dev. Psychol. 8, 19–22.

Google Scholar

Yan, G., and Bai, X. (2007). Eye movement studies of chinese reading. [www.frontiersin.org]. Stud. Psychol. Behav. 5, 229–234.

Google Scholar

Yan, G., Tian, H., Bai, X., and Rayner, K. (2006). The effect of word and character frequency on the eye movements of chinese readers. Br. J. Psychol. 97, 259–268. doi: 10.1348/000712605x70066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, G., Xiong, J., Zang, C., Yu, L., Cui, L., and Bai, X. (2013). Review of eye-movement measures in reading research. [www.frontiersin.org]. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 21, 589–605.

Google Scholar

Yan, G., Zhang, L., Bian, Q., and Xu, Z. (2012). Effect of word segmentation on good readers and poor readers: evidence from eye movements. Psychol. Explor. 32, 525–530.

Google Scholar

Yan, M., Kliegl, R., Richter, E. M., Nuthmann, A., and Shu, H. (2010). Flexible saccade-target selection in chinese reading. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 63, 705–725. doi: 10.1080/17470210903114858

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, M., Yan, H., and Yan, G. (2018). Is the word the basic processing unit in chinese sentence reading: an eye movement study. Lingua 205, 29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2017.12.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zang, C., Liang, F., Bai, X., Yan, G., and Liversedge, S. P. (2013). Interword spacing and landing position effects during chinese reading in children and adults. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 39, 720–734. doi: 10.1037/a0030097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zang, C., Wang, Y., Bai, X., Yan, G., Drieghe, D., and Liversedge, S. P. (2016). The use of probabilistic lexicality cues for word segmentation in chinese reading. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 69, 548–560. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1061030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, W., Ye, W., and Yan, M. (2020). Alternating-color words facilitate reading and eye movements among second-language learners of chinese. Appl. Psycholinguist. 41, 685–699. doi: 10.1017/s0142716420000211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix

Examples of Reading Materials in Experiment 2

www.frontiersin.org

www.frontiersin.org

Keywords: trade-off, format familiarity, word segmentation, Chinese reading, eye movements

Citation: Chen M, Wang Y, Zhao B, Li X and Bai X (2021) The Trade-Off Between Format Familiarity and Word-Segmentation Facilitation in Chinese Reading. Front. Psychol. 12:602931. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.602931

Received: 04 September 2020; Accepted: 06 January 2021;
Published: 28 January 2021.

Edited by:

Manuel Perea, University of Valencia, Spain

Reviewed by:

Claudio Mulatti, University of Trento, Italy
Anthony James Krafnick, Dominican University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Chen, Wang, Zhao, Li and Bai. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yongsheng Wang, wangyongsheng@mail.tjnu.edu.cn; Xuejun Bai, bxuejun@126.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.