Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 10 June 2021
Sec. Movement Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Esports: Playing into the Future View all 9 articles

Personalized Adaptive Training Improves Performance at a Professional First-Person Shooter Action Videogame

\r\nFrancesco NeriFrancesco Neri1Carmelo Luca SmeraldaCarmelo Luca Smeralda1Davide MomiDavide Momi1Giulia SprugnoliGiulia Sprugnoli1Arianna MenardiArianna Menardi1Salvatore FerroneSalvatore Ferrone1Simone Rossi,Simone Rossi1,2Alessandro Rossi,Alessandro Rossi1,3Giorgio Di Lorenzo,,Giorgio Di Lorenzo4,5,6Emiliano Santarnecchi*Emiliano Santarnecchi7*
  • 1Siena Brain Investigation & Neuromodulation Lab, Department of Medicine, Surgery and Neuroscience, Neurology and Clinical Neurophysiology Section, University of Siena, Siena, Italy
  • 2Human Physiology Section, Department of Medicine, Surgery and Neuroscience, University of Siena, Siena, Italy
  • 3Department of Medicine, Surgery and Neuroscience, University of Siena, Siena, Italy
  • 4Laboratory of Psychophysiology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Chair of Psychiatry, Department of Systems Medicine, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy
  • 5Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology Unit, Fondazione Policlinico Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy
  • 6IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
  • 7Berenson-Allen Center for Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States

First-Person Shooter (FPS) game experience can be transferred to untrained cognitive functions such as attention, visual short-term memory, spatial cognition, and decision-making. However, previous studies have been using off-the-shelf FPS games based on predefined gaming settings, therefore it is not known whether such improvement of in game performance and transfer of abilities can be further improved by creating a in-game, adaptive in-game training protocol. To address this question, we compared the impact of a popular FPS-game (Counter-Strike:Global-Offensive–CS:GO) with an ad hoc version of the game based on a personalized, adaptive algorithm modifying the artificial intelligence of opponents as well as the overall game difficulty on the basis of individual gaming performance. Two groups of FPS-naïve healthy young participants were randomly assigned to playing one of the two game versions (11 and 10 participants, respectively) 2 h/day for 3 weeks in a controlled laboratory setting, including daily in-game performance monitoring and extensive cognitive evaluations administered before, immediately after, and 3 months after training. Participants exposed to the adaptive version of the game were found to progress significantly faster in terms of in-game performance, reaching gaming scenarios up to 2.5 times more difficult than the group exposed to standard CS:GO (p < 0.05). A significant increase in cognitive performance was also observed. Personalized FPS gaming can significantly speed-up the learning curve of action videogame-players, with possible future applications for expert-video-gamers and potential relevance for clinical-rehabilitative applications.

Introduction

Videogame consoles are widely used by adolescents and children nowadays, with up to the 60% of 8–18 years old adolescents playing videogames on a daily basis, and a great increase of the overall daytime spent playing videogames from 1999 to 2020 (Lu et al., 2012; Özçetin et al., 2019). This has consequently led to an increase in research efforts addressing both potential harms and benefits of videogaming on cognitive functions (Oei and Patterson, 2013). The vast majority of previous studies has focused on action videogames, a category characterized by complex visual scenes and different time-locked goals (Bavelier et al., 2012; Latham et al., 2013). These games require fast responses to visual and auditory cues, constant selection between multiple action plans, and peripheral processing, ultimately resulting in a very demanding perceptual load (Green et al., 2010; Hubert-Wallander et al., 2011).

Previous investigations have reported expert action videogames players (AVGPs) to outperform non-players (NAVGPs) in a variety of cognitive and perceptual tasks, including visual selective attention (Green and Bavelier, 2003, 2006a; Feng et al., 2007; Dye et al., 2009; Spence and Feng, 2010; Clark et al., 2011; Belchior et al., 2013), visual stimuli detection (Vallett et al., 2013), visual search efficiency (Castel et al., 2005), contrast sensitivity (Li et al., 2009), visual interference suppression (Hazarika et al., 2018), shifting (Green et al., 2010), cognitive flexibility (Colzato et al., 2010), visual short-term memory (Boot et al., 2008), decision making (Green et al., 2010), multitasking (Green and Bavelier, 2006a), and multisensory integration (Di Luzio et al., 2021). Among action videogame (AVG) studies, first-person shooters (FPS) games – like Call of Duty, Counterstrike (CS:GO), Battlefield, and Fortnite – are centered on weapon combat using a first person perspective (i.e., the player looks at the scene through the eyes of the main character in the game), consequently requiring high flexibility, task switching skills, and rapid reaction times (RTs) (Colzato et al., 2010).

Apart from the direct impact on the specifically trained abilities, the success of a given intervention/training program occurs when people apply their previously accumulated experience-based skills to a new context or scenario (Thorndike and Woodworth, 1901; Tillberg, 2020). Such transfer of training/learning from one skill/ability to another is still a topic of discussion (Baldwin and Ford, 1988; Lieberman et al., 2014) as it is not yet clear whether exercising a specific skill can lead to an effect on abilities different from the practiced one (Gallagher et al., 2005; Morrison and Chein, 2011; Chooi and Thompson, 2012). The transfer of learning can be defined into a two-category model (Sala and Gobet, 2017) consisting in (i) near transfer learning: generalizing a skill to solve similar problems (e.g., once we have learned to tie a shoelaces, we will use this knowledge to tie all the shoelaces of the shoes we will buy during life, despite changing the thickness, length, or color of new shoelaces) (Alloway and Warner, 2006) and (ii) far transfer learning: use the knowledge to solve novel problems (e.g., a person who has learned the wind flow principles can transfer and use that knowledge to steer the sail on a sailboat). Moreover, recent studies have demonstrated beneficial transfer effects to brain functions that were not directly trained in the realm of video gaming itself, such as working memory or visuomotor control, which have been reported to occur even in older populations (Anguera et al., 2013; Latham et al., 2013) and possibly being transferrable to enhance performances in daily complex skills such as driving (Li et al., 2016).

Computerized virtual reality scenario are established in the context of training of various professional figures, for instance, airliner pilots using flight simulators recreating real scenarios (Haslbeck et al., 2014) or virtual trainings for surgeons (Boyle et al., 2011; Ahlborg et al., 2015). In this context, personalization of training regimen based on individual characteristics is crucial and it can be used to enhance specific cognitive functions both in healthy individuals and patients, with favorable outcomes in everyday life activities (Klingberg et al., 2005; Jaeggi et al., 2008; Alloway, 2012; Shinaver et al., 2014; Spencer-Smith and Klingberg, 2015). Specifically, tailored interventions have been frequently shown to outperform general approaches both in the clinical and no-clinical settings (Baker et al., 2015). In the case of videogames, personalization can also help in coping with aspects related to the game being either too easy or too difficult, resulting in a boring or frustrating experience, with potential adverse consequences at the research level, such as an increased risk of ceiling or floor effects (Lawrence, 2012). Furthermore, difficulty levels tailored at the individual pace – rather than on the general population sampling – could also ensure a greater control on the data collection and analysis. To the best of our knowledge, no previous studies have compared personalized or adaptive protocols to the standard in-game progression algorithms and their impact on game and cognitive abilities.

In the present study, we aimed at verifying whether a personalized/adaptive action videogame experience could elicit stronger benefits in terms of in-game performance and cognitive enhancement compared to the same videogame played according to standard, off-the-shelves parameters. We compared the in-game learning curves, as well as cognitive performance, of healthy participants following a training with a competitive FPS game called “Counter Strike: Global Offensive” (“CS:GO” hereafter; Valve and Hidden Path, 2012). A first group of participants played the “default” version of the game (Default CS:GO, “D-CS:GO”), whilst a second group was presented with a personalized adaptive training (Adaptive CS:GO, “A-CS:GO” from here on out). This allowed to carefully individualize the gaming sessions, providing equally entertaining and challenging trials both along the experiment period and across subjects. To monitor for the eventual transfer effects, participants were assessed for their cognitive profile before (T0) and after the experiment (T1: immediately after the end of the game period; T2: 3 months later). We hypothesized that (i) adaptive/personalized training might lead to a superior gaming performance than playing CS:GO using default parameters, and that (ii) the two training regimes would lead to a different modulation of cognitive abilities, with a higher performance for the adaptive group both in the short and long term.

Materials and Methods

Design and Participants

The study design included a baseline assessment (T0) consisting of a training session on the game modalities and features of CS:GO, a battery of cognitive tasks and an in-game assessment of the participants’ skills. After that, participants performed 15 daily gaming sessions, each lasting approximately 2 h for a minimum of 6 h per week, followed by a post-gaming in-game skills assessment and post-gaming cognitive assessment (T1). Follow-up cognitive assessment was repeated 3 months after the end of gaming sessions (T2).

For this study, the inclusion criterion was to be classified as AVGP, therefore every candidate compiled a questionnaire to quantify the time spent playing videogames (Green et al., 2017). A total of 21 young healthy were identified as AVGPs (action videogame experience ≥ 3–5 h per week during the last year) and were enrolled. None of the participants reported prior experience with a competitive FPS game. Each subject was randomly assigned to either one of two groups: the “A-CS:GO” group, playing a customized version of the videogame (11 subjects, four females and seven males; 24.2 ± 3.1 years), and the “D-CS:GO” group (10 subjects, two females and eight males; 24.1 ± 2.3 years), playing the off-the-shelves version of the game. Details on the progression algorithm are reported in a dedicated paragraph below.

Gaming was performed on a 19-inches screen located 80 cm away from the subject, with the prohibition of playing at home with any videogames. All participants had no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, assessed via the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Sheehan et al., 1998). Each participant provided a written informed consent. Study was approved by the local ethical committee.

First-Person Shooter Training Structure

CS:GO is an FPS game which differs from classic action games due to its competitiveness, as it requires the acquisition and the continued improvement of tactic and strategic skills related to specific game battlefields played over timed rounds. Indeed, while in a classic FPS game, the gamer has multiple scenarios to play and often switches scenario every few minutes of gameplay, CS:GO is played on a limited number of maps, thus requiring a deep knowledge of team-play strategies and fine 3D navigation. Thus, the game constantly demands high visuomotor coordination, fast RT, optimal perceptual skills, good planning behavior, high flexibility, and appropriate inhibition. For these reasons, and because it can be played cooperatively in competitive matches, the game is currently used in the professional electronic sports contexts1.

For the present study, the game was played in the offline modality (Local Area Network), allowing to play against artificial intelligence (AI) guiding computer-controlled opponents (“Bots” hereafter). The difficulty level of each bot is defined by a combination of skills, including speed, aiming accuracy with different weapons, cooperative behavior, aggressiveness, weapons at their disposal, and “swarm” intelligence (i.e., ability to coordinate with other bots in complex scenarios requiring cooperation). These characteristics are defined by the game engine and grouped to define four difficulty levels (Easy, Normal, Hard, Expert).

Introduction to the game was given throughout the first day with a 2-h session, allowing players to become familiar with the game commands and interface. Team-Deathmatch game modality was chosen for 15 daily gaming sessions, consisting of four rounds × 20 min each. In this modality, players must gain the highest possible scores, increasing their rate of survival while maximizing the number of bots being defeated at each round. Game mechanics included the player joining one of two teams and one of the four predefined maps, with the aim of increasing the CS:GO score of his/her team. Neither the same Team nor the same map could be chosen more than twice in a row to increase complexity and variability.

Each session lasted approximately 2 h, with participants playing a 10′ warm-up round without any performance monitoring, then training for 40′ (2 rounds × 20 min each), resting for 10′, and lastly, playing for an additional 40′ (2 rounds × 20 min each). In each session, participants joined one of the two teams (player + 4 AI-guided allies) with the aim of overpowering the opposite team (five bots).

Participants joined each gaming session on separate desktop PCs without the possibility of communicating with each other. After each round, the ratio between kills and death (K/D score) was registered by investigators. Gaming sessions were carried out using a dedicated desktop PC, equipped with: high performance dedicated graphic card = ATI Radeon 4GB, RAM = 8GB, 21 LCD monitor [Frame Per Second (FPS) = 60 Hz], headphones, mouse, and a keyboard.

Adaptive Algorithm

For the A-CS:GO group, the difficulty level (ranging on a scale from 1 to 24) was manually set up by the experimenter. An ad hoc algorithm was conceived for the study, allowing for the manipulation of the difficulty of each gaming session according to the participants’ performance on the previous gaming session. The personalization was done by creating a set of custom bots characterized by almost 100 combinations of in-game skills, specifically related to:

(i) Aim focus: bot’s RT and accuracy when focusing on a new target (i.e., similar to accuracy of saccades and gaze in humans);

(ii) Aim focus interval: time required to re-focus on a new target (i.e., in human executive functions terms, this could be ascribed to Flexibility/Switching ability);

(iii) Aim focus decay: duration of aim on a given target before engaging in alternative routines (i.e., sustained attention capacity);

(iv) Reaction time: RT to an event, specifically to the appearance of an enemy on the screen, new weapons available, change of strategic priority (i.e., similar to a general RT in humans);

(v) Team working: ability to cooperate with other teammates, e.g., protect wounded bots, target enemies closer to teammates;

(vi) Aggressivity: tendency to attack over retreat, chase enemies down instead of holding a safe position, and wait for back-up;

(vii) Proficiency with a specific weapon: ability with specific weapons (knife, gun, uzi, rifle, and shotgun); and

(viii) Speed: movement speed (walk, run, and spin).

Resulting bots were then combined in teams of five or more components by balancing each bot’s skill to obtain a certain level of difficulty (e.g., Elite team = 5 bots with good proficiency in each weapon, high aiming skills, fast RT, high cooperative behavior, high proactiveness, high speed; Medium team = 2 bots at Elite level, two bots with average aiming and RTs, one bot with poor aiming/RT and good proficiency in only 1–2 specific weapons, medium proactiveness, medium speed; Low-tier team = 4 bots with average aiming and RT, proficiency in only 1–2 specific weapons, one bot with low aiming and RT, proficiency in only one weapon, low cooperation, low proactiveness). The combination of bots resulted in an ad hoc difficulty level scale from 1–24, instead of the four-levels scale used in the standard game (Easy, Medium, Hard, and Expert). The 24 levels were obtained by (i) combining bots of different skills in (ii) teams of five or more bots (six from level 10 onward), by (iii) equipping them with different weapons, and by (iv) selecting maps of different sizes.

The A-CS:GO game progression was governed by a 2:1 rule: the level was manually increased by an investigator when the numbers of kills doubled individual’s death (i.e., K/D ≥ 2) for two consecutive rounds. The requirement of completing two rounds at K/D > 2 was aimed at ensuring that the increase in K/D ratio was reflective of a genuine increase in the participant’s skill. Participants in the A-CS:GO group progressed through the 1–24 difficulty levels, constantly trying to maintain at least a K/D > 2. If a player reached the 2:1 criterion at the last available level (#24), he/she restarded from level 17 and challenged with a new K/D criterion of 3:1 (Figure 1). Notably, the initial difficulty level was set by an investigator based on the performance at the first two rounds: players started their first round at level 4, if the 2:1 criterion was not met, the difficulty level was lowered up to level 1. If the player reached the K/D ≥ 2/1 at level 4, he/she was then tested at level 6; if the 2:1 criterion was satisfied again, the training session started at level 7; if not, training started at level 5.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scheme of the experiment protocol. (A) At the beginning of the game, the player could join one of the two teams available in the game: Team 1 or Team 2. The A-CS:GO group played a customized game in which the progression in the game was established on the bases of the Death/Kill (K/D) ratio. If a player reached the K/D ≥ 2/1 for two consecutive rounds, he/she could proceed into the next level. Once reaching level 24, participants restarted from level 17 and could proceed to the next level only if he/she was capable of reaching a K/D ≥ 3/1. The D-CS:GO players could choose their difficulty level between four standardized games at the beginning of each round (*players joined a team and faced some enemies. The number of team bots and enemy bots was customized, as well as their skills). (B) Weapons’ efficacy was set from the less (green–yellow) to the most (yellow–orange) dangerous on the bases of the extent of the capacity to cause damage, firing rate, and reload velocity. (C) Four different maps were available in the game. The smallest and easiest maps (Shorttrain and Shortdust) characterized the first levels of the A-CS:GO group and the Easy/Normal modality in the D-CS:GO group, whereas larger maps (Bank and Office) were proposed to challenge subjects in the A-CS:GO group and in Hard/Expert modality of the D-CS:GO group.

Importantly, the different difficulty system between D-CS:GO and A-CS:GO did not allow for a direct comparison of the K/D levels across groups. Given that each participant in the A-CS:GO group was moved to the next level in case he/she achieved a 2:1 K/D, resulting raw K/D ratios were not representative of individual performance. Therefore, a composite score obtained by multiplying K/D values for each session by their corresponding difficulty level was computed (e.g., K/D = 2.3, difficulty = 16; corrected K/D = 2.3 × 16 = 36.8). In order to calculate the same score for the D-CS:GO group, the four difficulty levels (Easy, Medium, Hard, and Expert) were mapped to corresponding ones in the A-CS:GO difficulty system: (Easy = level 1; Medium = level 5; Hard = level 7; and Expert = level 10).

Participants in the D-CS:GO group were allowed to freely decide their game difficulty level, choosing between Easy, Medium, Hard, or Expert (Figure 1). However, participants were still asked to meet the requirement of a K/D > 2, i.e., they were aware of the nature of the training and the goal of improving their in-game performance as much as possible within the 3 weeks of training period.

In-Game Performance Assessment

Both the D-CS:GO and A-CS:GO groups performed an initial evaluation of their FPS skills and were tested on two sets of tasks: (i) a training course involving navigating a map while shooting at targets and (ii) a set of firing range simulations assessing various types of coordinated visuomotor skills relevant for aiming (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Game skills assessment. (A) During the initial assessment, each player completed 40′ of tutorial to understand the game controls and the environment. (B) At T0 and T1, subjects completed the Aim Course time-constrained task. The times required to complete the Aim Course 1 and Aim Course 2 Maps were registered. (C) At T0 and T1, the participants engaged in various shooting tasks and accuracy was recorded. (D) At the initial assessment, the A-CS:GO group was further assessed on a fourth task, aiming to define the level of player game-ability. This task established the player’s starting level.

Tutorial

An initial 40-min game tutorial was carried out to ensure a complete understanding of the game dynamics, commands, and interface (Figure 2A).

Training Course

Two time-constrained training courses (Figure 2B; Aim Courses 1 and 2) were tested: players had to complete a guided path as fast as possible while shooting randomly at the appearance of the enemies’ silhouettes. Completion time was measured, representing the ability to navigate a 3D environment under pressure as well as pure aiming skills.

Firing Range

Participants were assessed on their firing accuracy in a virtual shooting gallery, completing six aiming tasks with a handgun (Figure 2C):

(i) “Static” task = participants shoot 50 targets appearing at the center of the visual field for 1 s;

(ii) “Linear” task = participants shoot 50 targets appearing on the screen for 2 s and moving in linear trajectories horizontally or vertically at two different speed levels (linears 1 and 2);

(iii) “Static + linear” task = a combination of the two previous tasks;

(iv) “Angles” task = participants shoot 50 targets moving diagonally from the center of the screen; and

(v) “Reflex” task = participants shoot to target moving randomly from the center of the screen;

Participants in both the D-CS:GO and A-CS:GO groups were tested with the same game assessment at T0 and T1.

Gaming Skills/Ability

Players played a game for 20 min on a game level (Figure 2D) to establish their gaming skills, calculated by the ratio between the number of enemies killed and number of defeats.

Cognitive Assessment

The entire cognitive battery of tasks was assessed at T0 and T1, while at T2, only a reduced battery of tasks showing a significant effect at T1 compared to T0 was administered. Tasks were performed via the E-prime 2.0 software (Psychology Software Tools Inc.2; Schneider et al., 2012) on a Windows laptop PC. The battery-tasks measured multiple cognitive domains such as visuo-spatial abilities, attention, inhibition, and switching (for details, see Supplementary Material). Moreover, ad hoc visuo-spatial tasks were administered to assess the impact of videogames as previously shown by numerous studies (Green and Bavelier, 2003, 2006a,2006b, 2007) and verify near, moderate, and far transfer. In fact, FPS games can have a direct impact on basic skills, such as attention and filtering [due to the continuous requirement to discriminate distractors among salience target (Green and Bavelier, 2006a)], hand-eye coordination and contrast sensitivity [(greater in AVGPs respect to NVGPs) (Green and Bavelier, 2012)], and even on executive functions (i.e., cognitive flexibility, planning, and decision making; Basak et al., 2011). Therefore, tasks were categorized as “Far,” “Moderate,” and “Near” transfer based on the nature of the videogame implicated in our study and considering precedent findings (Green and Bavelier, 2003, 2006a,2006b).

Near Transfer

An FPS game mainly recruits attentional and visuospatial skills. We measured these cognitive functions by means of a series of tasks: Serial RT (Robertson, 2007), Mental Rotation (Cooper and Shepard, 1973), Useful Field of View (UFOV) (Feng et al., 2007), Visual Search (Treisman and Gelade, 1980), Flanker (Green and Bavelier, 2003), and Attentional Blink (Raymond et al., 1992).

Moderate Transfer

To measure inhibition abilities, we used these tasks: Preparing to Overcome Prepotency (POP) (Rosano et al., 2005), Letter No-Go (Thorell et al., 2009), and Global-Local Features (Navon, 1977) tasks.

Far Transfer

We verified changes in higher cognitive functions such as flexibility, reasoning, and memory using Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices (Raven et al., 1998), Sandia Matrix Task (Matzen et al., 2010), Digit Span (Wechsler, 1981), and Change Localization Task (Luck and Vogel, 1997).

Statistical Analysis

Analyses were carried out using the SPSS version 16, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, United States. Three main analyses were carried out: (i) in-game progression/performance based on K/D at T0–T1; (ii) changes at in-game assessment based on training courses and firing range simulations measured at T0–T1; and (iii) transfer of cognitive abilities measured with cognitive tasks at T0–T1–T2. Subjects were not allowed to play videogames at home between T0 and T1, while no specific task was assigned to the participants between T1 and T2.

In-Game Progression (K/D Ratio) Analysis

First, the raw K/D ratio (uncorrected K/D ratio) of each round was multiplied by the level of difficulty of the same session (corrected K/D ratio). A quadratic regression was conducted considering the game performance during training (60 rounds) to test if Time significantly predicted the participants’ corrected K/D ratio.

Second, we calculated the average uncorrected and corrected K/D ratio over the four rounds played each training day. A preliminary analysis was conducted on the first training day, to verify the differences between the A-CS:GO and D-CS:GO groups at the beginning of the training. Therefore, a Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance (ANOVARM) was conducted on the corrected and the uncorrected K/D ratio, to verify the performance differences between groups during the training period with Time (15 levels: from Day 1 to Day 15) as within-subjects factor and Group (two levels: A-CS:GO and D-CS:GO) as between-subjects factor. Again, a quadratic regression was conducted to test if Time significantly predicted the participants’ game performance during the training days (15 days). A Post hoc power analysis was performed using the G-Power software (Faul et al., 2007) in order to calculate the statistical power of the ANOVA results.

In-Game Assessments Analysis

An ANOVARM was performed to verify game ability changes using Time (2 levels: T0 and T1) and Group (2 levels: A-CS:GO and D-CS:GO) as factors.

Transfer on Cognitive Abilities Analysis

Cognitive tasks were administered to both A-CS:GO and D-CS:GO groups before (T0), immediately after (T1), and 3 months after the end of the experimental procedure (T2). Due to scheduling conflicts, only 19 subjects out of 21 gamers underwent T2 re-testing. In order to verify significant long lasting changes in gamers between the pre, post, and follow up assessments, aANOVARM was conducted with Time (three levels: T0, T1, and T2) and Group (two levels: A-CS:GO and D-CS:GO) as factors.

Results

In-Game Progression Results

Figure 3A illustrates the uncorrected K/D ratio, the difficulty levels and the corrected K/D ratio of the training rounds. Regression analyses indicated that Time (i.e., number of training sessions) predicted the corrected K/D ratio in the A-CS:GO (R2 = 0.91; β = 0.51, p < 0.001) and in the D-CS:GO group (R2 = 0.86; β = 1.70, p < 0.001). Figure 3B shows the corrected K/D ratio of each participant.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Graphical depiction of the in-game progression for both A-CS:GO and D-CS:GO groups. (A) uncorrected K/D ratio, difficulty levels and corrected K/D ratio of the training rounds. (B) Individual corrected K/D ratio changes during training rounds. (C) K/D ratio during the training days. (D) Corrected K/D ratio during the training days. After an initial equivalent performance between A-CS:GO and D-CS:GO groups, the corrected K/D ratio progressively increased in the A-CS:GO group and the performance of the A-CS:GO group becomes significantly higher than the CS:GO group performance. (*p < 0.05 – higher performance in the A-CS:GO group). E: Pre-training corrected K/D was subtracted from post-training performance for both groups, highlighting a significant improvement of performance after the A-CS:GO training (*p < 0.05).

Similar to that shown in each round, the profile of the uncorrected K/D performance did not differ between trained groups during the training days (Figure 3C). The Mauchly’s test of the ANOVARM showed a violation of the assumption of sphericity (χ(104)2 = 189.93, p < 0.001) and a Greenhouse-Geisser estimation of sphericity was used to correct the degrees of freedom (ε = 0.37). The analysis revealed a significant effect of Time (F(5.27, 100.18) = 7.76, p < 0.001) and no interaction between Time and Group. On the other hand, by correcting the K/D ratio with the difficulty level and conducting ANOVARM, significant difference was highlighted between A-CS:GO and D-CS:GO groups during the training days (Figure 3D). Again, Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity was violated, (χ(104)2 = 471.09, p < 0.001) and the degrees of freedom were corrected by using Greenhouse-Geisser estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.16). Results showed a significant effect of Time (F(2.32, 44.08) = 13.25, p < 0.001) and a significant interaction of Time x Group (F(2.32, 44.08) = 4.22, p = 0.017) with an higher performance in the A-CS:GO. Input parameters of the power analysis were: effect size f: 0.4 (calculated from the significant interaction of Time x Group ANOVARM analysis: d = 0.4 90% CI [0.14–0.67]); groups: 2 (A-CS:GO and D-CS:GO); measurements: 15 (training days) alpha: 0.05; non-sphericity correction ε: 0.16. We calculated a statistical power of 0.93 (1-β error probability) to discriminate significant differences during the training period and between different conditions (Noncentrality parameter λ: 90.97; Critical F: 3.09; degrees of freedom: 2.24, 42.56). Figure 3E shows Post-Pre performance difference. The results of the t-test highlighted an higher corrected K/D ratio in participants exposed to A-CS:GO: (t(19) = 4.47, p < 0.05).

In-Game Assessment Results

A general effect of Time (T0 and T1) was observed for all game tasks (Table 1). The Aim Course 1 task analysis revealed a main effect of Time (F(1,19) = 17.758, p < 0.001), such as that both groups (A-CS:GO and D-CS:GO) showed a faster completion time at the post-training assessment compared to the pre-training assessment (t(18) = −4.27, p < 0.001). The same pattern was observed for the Aim Course 2 task (F(1,19) = 15.642, p = 0.001), with higher performances achieved after training in both groups (t(18) = −4.05, p < 0.001). No interaction between Time and Group was found, although a trend for a better performance in the A-CS:GO group compared to the D-CS:GO group was observed, with the A-CS:GO group fastening their time of task execution (−46.09 s between T0 and T1) more than the D-CS:GO group (−31.5 s between T0 and T1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Group Means and Standard Deviation (SD) for the pre (T0) and post (T1) game assessment.

In the Static, Linear 1, Linear 2, Static + Linear, and Angles aim tasks, a main effect of Time was also found (respectively: F(1,19) = 28.384, p < 0.001; F(1,19) = 18.621, p < 0.001; F(1,19) = 6.618, p = 0.019; F(1,19) = 26.239, p < 0.001; F(1,19) = 22.927, p < 0.001). Trained subjects missed less targets in the post-game assessment (respectively: t(18) = −5.11, p < 0.001; t(18) = −4.33, p < 0.001; t(18) = −4.15, p < 0.001; t(18) = −4.21, p < 0.001), simultaneously entailing higher number of hits in all tasks but Angles (respectively: t(18) = 5.90, p < 0.001; t(18) = 6.42, p < 0.001; t(18) = 3.51, p = 0.002). No significant interactions between the Time and Group were found. Finally, no significant effects were observed for the Reflex task.

Long-Term Cognitive Transfer: Effect of Training

No differences between the two groups have been observed in the cognitive tests performance analysis at T0 (p > 0.05). ANOVARM revealed a main effect of Time for the subjects’ RTs in Visual Search (F(2,16) = 7.848, p = 0.004), such as that participants responded faster at T1 than at T0 (p = 0.009). Such effect remained stable even at T2 compared to T0 (p = 0.002). No difference between T1 and T2 was observed. Concerning the Global features Accuracy of Global Local Task, a main effect of Time was found (F(2,16) = 4.483, p = 0.028) with a lower performance at T2 than T0 (p = 0.024). At the Global-Local Task, a main effect of Time for RT to the Global features was observed (F(2,16) = 12.004, p < 0.001). Responses were faster at T1 compared to T0 assessment (p = 0.002). After 3 months, reaction remained faster than the T0 (p < 0.001). For Local features RT, a main effect of Time was found (F(2,16) = 8.929, p = 0.002). Subjects were faster at T2 compared to T0 (p = 0.002), but not at T1. In the Attentional Blink task, a main effect of Time was also observed (F(2,16) = 34.143, p < 0.001). Subjects were faster in responding to the target stimulus at T1 compared to T0 (p = 0.003). Higher speed was further observed at T2 compared to T0 (p < 0.001). Similarly, an effect of Time of RT to the letter X was observed (F(2,16) = 29.413, p < 0.001), with a faster response at T1 and T2 compared to T0 (p < 0.001 for both comparisons). A main effect of Time (F(2,16) = 5.555, p = 0.015) was observed considering the participants’ accuracy in detecting the target letter X, such as that the participants were more accurate at T1 than T0 (p = 0.01). For what concerns the SRTT task, RTs lowered as a function of Time (F(2,16) = 20.069, p < 0.001), with better performances at T1 and T2 compared to T0 (p < 0.001 for both comparisons). As per the UFOV task, Time exerted a significant effect in terms of overall faster RTs (F(2,16) = 7.511, p = 0.005), leading to higher performances at T2 compared to T0 (p = 0.004). All effects are reported in Figure 4.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Long Term Cognitive Transfer results. Graphical depictions of the significant effects observed between cognitive assessments (significant difference between T1 and T0; ∗∗significant difference between T2 and T0; Global/Local Task: ∇: Global Features ○: Local Features).

Discussion

A personalized, adaptive version of a competitive FPS videogame was compared with its standard version, revealing a greater improvement in videogame performance for participants exposed to the former. Additionally, cognitive benefits were detected after 15 sessions of intensive video gaming (∼30 h) which were maintained up to 3 months after the end of the study in both experimental groups. Relevance for cognitive enhancement applications, potential implications for the professional videogame industry as well as on the impact of videogames on brain and cognition are discussed.

Individualized Video Gaming

Previous literature reported a beneficial effect of individualized gaming task on clinical populations (Davis et al., 2018). Thus, we hypothesized and expected the achievement of better videogame and cognitive performances in the adaptive trained group of our healthy subjects’ sample. Videogames provide entertainment via immediately rewarding experience, substantiated by neuroimaging findings suggesting a pivotal role of the striatum – part of the neural reward system – during video gaming (Lorenz et al., 2015; Momi et al., 2018b). This mechanism might be capitalized to the exploitation of personalized video gaming to obtain potential benefits for cognitively impaired populations as well as in the prevention of cognitive weaknesses (Mishra et al., 2016). Furthermore, to decrease the probability of drop-outs and further guarantee adequate long-lasting motivation and game competence, it is critical to create individualized game environments (Ryan et al., 2006). We did not observe significant drop-outs, confirming the validity of the adaptive algorithm, which ensured the proper balance between the increment of difficulties and positive reinforcement and entertainment (Grodal, 2000).

In particular, “close-loop tasks” might be essential in order to accelerate and reinforce the progression on videogame, as well as in everyday life skills (Sitaram et al., 2017). By receiving feedbacks on their performance, participants can better understand where it is better to allocate their effort, thus maximizing their performance on a specific goal (Mishra et al., 2016). During gaming, rewards are provided through visual or auditory feedbacks, aiming to maximize motivation (Bavelier et al., 2012; Ahlborg et al., 2015). Accordingly, the constant monitoring over measures of performance and the corresponding ongoing adjustments of gaming parameters enabled the distinction between two differential learning curves among our experimental groups: a proficiency-based profile for the A-CS:GO group and a fixed time bound practice for the D-CS:GO group. Proficiency based learning is a feasible component that concerns, for example, the way humans learn at school or new skills in general, giving the possibility to progressively overcome consecutive gaps for optimal learning (Snyder et al., 2009). For example, it has been shown that future surgeons learn better surgical skills if, during a virtual reality training program, the difficulty of simulated operations becomes progressively harder (Gallagher et al., 2005).

An interesting finding concerns the different trajectory of the learning curve across the training sessions, with the A-CS:GO group progressively outperforming the D-CS:GO group (Figure 3). In the adaptive modality, the number of enemies increases and allies decreases after the 10th level. In the meantime, available maps rise for their complexity. Starting from level 15, the number of allies keeps on decreasing (four allies vs. eight enemies bots) and ensures that the player’s ability continues to increase until the end, even after the participants have already explored all the available maps and weapons. This suggests that players reach a discrete knowledge and confidence with all available scenarios and videogame options, becoming capable of capitalizing such knowledge to face and overcome new challenging situations. As shown by trendlines and regression analysis (Figure 3D), training time is a strong predictor for the game performance in both groups. On the other hand, a stronger prediction is noticeable in the A-CS:GO group, suggesting the usefulness of a proper individualized – and relatively short in time – training in order to reach a better performance in complex tasks. Furthermore, it demonstrates how the increasing difficulty of an adaptive training, based on the player’s specific characteristics, could be a better setting than a free game.

Regarding the game assessment results, both groups improved their performance in the target shooting tasks. All participants became accurate in hitting more targets and making less mistakes. Moreover, a trend toward the significance of a decreased execution time in the Aim Course task was found for the A-CS:GO. In order to defeat the growing number of enemies, A-CS:GO participants had to quickly adapt themselves and adopt different gaming styles, compared to those of the D-CS:GO participants. Fast moving is a requisite for the A-CS:GO players: they have to hit the enemies and quickly move to another playing area in order not to be hit back. This is exactly what happened in the Aim Course task: the faster the player hits the shapes of the enemies, the faster he can move to another area and finish the course.

Recently, video gaming is no longer considered just an entertaining activity for some people, but it has become an opportunity for a real career. More and more people are investing their future in this activity, participating in competitive tournaments that reward large sums of money, and its activity is so widespread that professional videogamers are also called electronic athletes (Parke et al., 2005). The economic impact is enormous, so much that many brands have invested to advertise during professional tournaments and support players (Bányai et al., 2019). The reduction of the learning curve has an interesting implication for those who wish to approach with professional video gaming. Decreasing the acquisition time of a skill game could in fact lead the player to achieve goals faster and it is possible to hypothesize that an individualized protocol – such as the one developed in our studio for CS:GO – could be used in other FPS games, which are the ones most often used in professional tournaments.

Cognitive Transfer

The hypothesized effect of a better performance of the A-CS:GO within the gaming environment has been found, confirming previous findings on how intensive exercise produces a marked improvement in the training task (Gallagher et al., 2005; Morrison and Chein, 2011; Chooi and Thompson, 2012; Mishra et al., 2016), but similar cognitive changes between the A-CS:GO and D-CS:GO groups observed at T1 and T2 assessments open additional questions related to video gaming and effective cognitive transfer.

According to Green and Bavelier, action videogame playing can enhance the ability of an individual to learn new tasks (Bavelier et al., 2012), a phenomenon defined as “learning to learn” (Harlow, 1949; Kemp et al., 2010). Specifically, AVGPs are considered more capable to extract regular patterns from different scenarios and suppress irrelevant information from distractors (Föcker et al., 2018). These benefits are expected to be generalized to other tasks sharing some of the structure or have similarities with the videogame (near and moderate transfer), whereas, they generally fail to expand to more distant cognitive processes (far transfer). In our study, we found cognitive benefits in both the A-CS:GO and D-CS:GO groups after the game training and these benefits were maintained in the follow-up assessment performed 3 months after the end of the experiment. This result suggests the possibility of obtain a long-lasting improvement following an FPS videogame training, with potential implications even for clinical applications. Regarding near and moderate transfers, we found an enhancement of cognitive skills directly stimulated by the videogame, specifically measured via the Visual Search, UFOV, Attentional Blink, Global/Local, and SRTT tasks, and especially for the RTs which were improved after game training. These tests are used to investigate the ability of the visual system to allocate attentional resources, inhibition skills, and visuo-motor learning. The effects observed in the present study align with previous literature reporting a transfer effect on the visual-attentive and visuospatial functions implicated during video gaming, especially in shortening the RT observed at T1 (Greenfield et al., 1994; Green and Bavelier, 2003; Castel et al., 2005), also showing the persistance of the effect 3 months after the end of the training (T2).

For instance, subjects with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) show slower RTs in complex attentional tasks (Gualtieri and Johnson, 2006). In the same patients, videogame-interface trainings have been used to modify attentional abilities and executive functions that are typically altered in this neuropsychological disorder (Davis et al., 2018). Moreover, it has been demonstrated that subjects with dyslexia have a larger attentional blink and reading speed inefficiency (Visser et al., 2004). Action videogame training has been proved to be capable to improve children’s reading speed (Franceschini et al., 2013). Recently, deficits in switching abilities have also been reported in Parkinson’s disease (Hélie and Fansher, 2018) for which the use of videogames have been recently used, improving their performance in activities of daily living and motor coordination (Pompeu et al., 2012). Moreover, in patients with multiple sclerosis, protocols combining video gaming with physical exercise and movement (“exergames”) have been applied, giving positive effects in higher cognitive functions, such as attention and executive functions (Prosperini et al., 2015; Lavorgna et al., 2018).

Interestingly, a recent investigation by our group has shown how playing CS:GO in its “default” modality might induce acute and long-lasting changes in brain structures (e.g., cortical thickness, volume) relevant for the currently observed cognitive effects. For instance, in one study, long-term morphovolumetric changes in the pulvinar were found (Momi et al., 2018b). The pulvinar is strongly connected with the occipital cortex and it has been shown to be involved in cognitive functions such as selective attention (Shipp, 2004), multisensory integration (Stein and Stanford, 2008) inhibition (Mitchell, 2015), and spatial seeking (Fischer and Whitney, 2009). Remarkably, all the aforementioned cognitive functions are consistently stimulated by an FPS where players are asked to identify salient targets (e.g., enemies) among distractors (e.g., teammates, neutral players), while constantly filtering irrelevant information. After an FPS training, a long-term increase of brain cortical thickness in the somatosensory, parahippocampal, and superior parietal lobule areas has been found (Momi et al., 2018a). These anatomical structures have been linked to navigation processing, spatial knowledge in 3D environments, visual-spatial skills, visuomotor attention, motor execution, inhibition, and saccadic eye movement (Stephan et al., 1995; Mort et al., 2003; Caplan et al., 2006; Ekstrom and Bookheimer, 2007; Naito et al., 2008; Rauchs et al., 2008; Asscheman et al., 2015) and might likely represent the neural bases of our near and moderate transfer effects. It is reasonable to assume that the application of an adaptive, personalized version of CS:GO, that is more effective for cognitive improvement, could lead to even stronger brain changes in pertinent regions. More in general, the overlapping enhancement of cognitive abilities across the two groups seems to fit with the learning to learn hypothesis. In fact, both gaming groups were exposed to the same subtasks and abilities required in FPS action videogame, such as flexibility, switching, and rapid response times (Colzato et al., 2010). A possibility is that a longer training period is necessary to highlight the eventual significant differences on cognitive effects between A-CS:GO and D-CS:GO.

Conclusion

Findings further support published evidences that videogame playing might impact cognitive functioning in a beneficial way. Moreover, we demonstrate how a personalized/adaptive FPS experience can significantly speed-up the learning curve of action videogame players, opening to possible future applications for video-gamers, as well as potential rehabilitative applications in clinical populations.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors upon request to the corresponding author, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Brainsight – Dipartimento di scienze mediche chirurgiche e neuroscienze – UOC Neurologia, Neurofisiologia, Policlinico Santa Maria alle Scotte, Siena, Italy. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in the study.

Author Contributions

ES, CS, and FN conceptualized and designed the study protocol. FN, DM, GS, CS, and SF collected the data. FN and ES performed the statistical analysis. AR, GDL, and ES oversaw the study conduction. FN, AM, and ES edited the first draft. SR and AR supervised the study and manuscript. ES, SR, FN, and AR critically reviewed the manuscript for content and all authors approved the final version for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all the participants who took part in the study.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.598410/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.eslgaming.com
  2. ^ www.pstnet.com/eprime

References

Ahlborg, L., Weurlander, M., Hedman, L., Nisell, H., Lindqvist, P. G., Felländer-Tsai, L., et al. (2015). Individualized feedback during simulated laparoscopic training: a mixed methods study. Int. J. Med. Educ. 6, pgrng93–100. doi: 10.5116/ijme.55a2.218b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alloway, T. (2012). Can interactive working memory training improve learning? J. Interact. Learn. Res. 23, 197–207.

Google Scholar

Alloway, T. P., and Warner, C. (2006). Task-specific training, learning, and memory for children with developmental coordination disorder: a pilot study. Percept. Mot. Skills 107, 473–480. doi: 10.2466/pms.107.6.473-480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, J. A., Boccanfuso, J., Rintoul, J. L., Al-Hashimi, O., Faraji, F., Janowich, J., et al. (2013). Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature 501, 97–101. doi: 10.1038/nature12486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asscheman, S. J., Thakkar, K. N., and Neggers, S. F. W. (2015). Changes in effective connectivity of the superior parietal lobe during inhibition and redirection of eye movements: supplementary issue: behavioral neuroscience. J. Exp. Neurosci. 9(Suppl. 1):JEN.S32736.

Google Scholar

Baker, R., Camosso-Stefinovic, J., Gillies, C., Shaw, E. J., Cheater, F., Flottorp, S., et al. (2015). Tailored interventions to address determinants of practice. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2015:CD005470.

Google Scholar

Baldwin, T. T., and Ford, J. K. (1988). Transfer of training: a review and directions for future research. Pers. Psychol. 41, 63–105. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.1988.tb00632.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bányai, F., Griffiths, M. D., Király, O., and Demetrovics, Z. (2019). The psychology of esports: a systematic literature review. J. Gambl. Stud. 35, 351–365. doi: 10.1007/s10899-018-9763-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basak, C., Voss, M. W., Erickson, K. I., Boot, W. R., and Kramer, A. F. (2011). Regional differences in brain volume predict the acquisition of skill in a complex real-time strategy videogame. Brain Cogn. 76, 407–414. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bavelier, D., Green, C. S., Pouget, A., and Schrater, P. (2012). Brain plasticity through the life span: learning to learn and action video games. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 391–416. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-152832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belchior, P., Marsiske, M., Sisco, S. M., Yam, A., Bavelier, D., Ball, K., et al. (2013). Video game training to improve selective visual attention in older adults. Comput. Hum. Behav. 29, 1318–1324. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.01.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boot, W. R., Kramer, A. F., Simons, D. J., Fabiani, M., and Gratton, G. (2008). The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. Acta Psychol. 129, 387–398. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyle, E., Kennedy, A. M., Traynor, O., and Hill, A. D. K. (2011). Training surgical skills using nonsurgical tasks–can Nintendo WiiTM improve surgical performance? J. Surg. Educ. 68, 148–154. doi: 10.1016/j.jsurg.2010.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caplan, J. B., Luks, T. L., Simpson, G. V., Glaholt, M., and McIntosh, A. R. (2006). Parallel networks operating across attentional deployment and motion processing: a multi-seed partial least squares fMRI study. NeuroImage 29, 1192–1202. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castel, A. D., Pratt, J., and Drummond, E. (2005). The effects of action video game experience on the time course of inhibition of return and the efficiency of visual search. Acta Psychol. 119, 217–230. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chooi, W. T., and Thompson, L. A. (2012). Working memory training does not improve intelligence in healthy young adults. Intelligence 40, 531–542. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2012.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, K., Fleck, M. S., and Mitroff, S. R. (2011). Enhanced change detection performance reveals improved strategy use in avid action video game players. Acta Psychol. 136, 67–72. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colzato, L. S., van Leeuwen, P. J. A., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., and Hommel, B. (2010). DOOM’d to switch: superior cognitive flexibility in players of first-person shooter games. Front. Psychol. 1:8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, L. A., and Shepard, R. N. (1973). “Chronometric studies of the rotation of mental images,” in Visual Information Processing, ed. W. G. Chase (New York, NY: Academic Press), 75–176.

Google Scholar

Davis, N. O., Bower, J., and Kollins, S. H. (2018). Proof-of-concept study of an at-home, engaging, digital intervention for pediatric ADHD. PLoS One 13:e0189749. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0189749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Luzio, P., Borgomaneri, S., Sanchioni, S., Tessari, A., and Romei, V. (2021). Exposure to first-person shooter videogames is associated with multisensory temporal precision and migraine incidence. Cortex 134, 223–238. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dye, M. W. G., Green, C. S., and Bavelier, D. (2009). The development of attention skills in action video game players. Neuropsychologia 47, 1780–1789. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekstrom, A. D., and Bookheimer, S. Y. (2007). Spatial and temporal episodic memory retrieval recruit dissociable functional networks in the human brain. Learn. Amp. Mem. 14, 645–654. doi: 10.1101/lm.575107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., and Buchner, A. (2007). GPower 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF0319314

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, J., Spence, I., and Pratt, J. (2007). Playing an action video game reduces gender differences in spatial cognition. Psychol. Sci. 18, 850–855. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01990.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, J., and Whitney, D. (2009). Precise discrimination of object position in the human pulvinar. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 101–111. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20485

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Föcker, J., Cole, D., Beer, A. L., and Bavelier, D. (2018). Neural bases of enhanced attentional control: lessons from action video game players. Brain Behav. 8:e01019. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Viola, S., Molteni, M., and Facoetti, A. (2013). Action video games make dyslexic children read better. Curr. Biol. CB 23, 462–466. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, A. G., Ritter, E. M., Champion, H., Higgins, G., Fried, M. P., Moses, G., et al. (2005). Virtual reality simulation for the operating room. Ann. Surg. 241, 364–372.

Google Scholar

Green, C., and Bavelier, D. (2003). Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature 423, 534–537. doi: 10.1038/nature01647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, C. S., and Bavelier, D. (2006a). Effect of action video games on the spatial distribution of visuospatial attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 32, 1465–1478. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.6.1465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, C. S., and Bavelier, D. (2006b). Enumeration versus multiple object tracking: the case of action video game players. Cognition 101, 217–245. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, C. S., and Bavelier, D. (2007). Action-video-game experience alters the spatial resolution of vision. Psychol. Sci. 18, 88–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01853.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, C. S., and Bavelier, D. (2012). Learning, attentional control, and action video games. Curr. Biol. 22, R197–R206.

Google Scholar

Green, C. S., Kattner, F., Eichenbaum, A., Bediou, B., Adams, D. M., Mayer, R. E., et al. (2017). Playing some video games but not others is related to cognitive abilities: a critique of Unsworth et al. (2015). Psychol. Sci. 28, 679–682. doi: 10.1177/0956797616644837

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, C. S., Li, R., and Bavelier, D. (2010). Perceptual learning during action video game playing. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2, 202–216. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01054.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenfield, P. M., de Winstanley, P., Kilpatrick, H., and Kaye, D. (1994). Action video games and informal education: effects on strategies for dividing visual attention. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 15, 105–123. doi: 10.1016/0193-3973(94)90008-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grodal, T. (2000). “Video games and the pleasures of control,” in LEA’s Communication Series. Media Entertainment: The Psychology of Its Appea, eds D. Zillmann and P. Vorderer (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 197–213.

Google Scholar

Gualtieri, C. T., and Johnson, L. G. (2006). Efficient allocation of attentional resources in patients with ADHD: maturational changes from age 10 to 29. J. Atten. Disord. 9, 534–542. doi: 10.1177/1087054705283758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harlow, H. F. (1949). The formation of learning sets. Psychol. Rev. 56, 51–65. doi: 10.1037/h0062474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haslbeck, A., Kirchner, P., Schubert, E., and Bengler, K. (2014). A flight simulator study to evaluate manual flying skills of airline pilots. Proc. Hum. Fact. Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet. 58, 11–15. doi: 10.1177/1541931214581003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hazarika, J., Kant, P., Dasgupta, R., and Laskar, S. H. (2018). Neural modulation in action video game players during inhibitory control function: an EEG study using discrete wavelet transform. Biomed. Signal Process. Control 45, 144–150. doi: 10.1016/j.bspc.2018.05.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hélie, S., and Fansher, M. (2018). Categorization system-switching deficits in typical aging and Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology. 32, 724–734. doi: 10.1037/neu0000459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hubert-Wallander, B., Green, C. S., and Bavelier, D. (2011). Stretching the limits of visual attention: the case of action video games. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2, 222–230. doi: 10.1002/wcs.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., and Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 6829–6833.

Google Scholar

Kemp, C., Goodman, N. D., and Tenenbaum, J. B. (2010). Learning to learn causal models. Cogn. Sci. 34, 1185–1243. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01128.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlström, K., et al. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD–a randomized, controlled trial. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 44, 177–186. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200502000-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latham, A. J., Patston, L. L. M., and Tippett, L. J. (2013). Just how expert are “expert” video-game players? Assessing the experience and expertise of video-game players across “action” video-game genres. Front. Psychol. 4:941. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavorgna, L., Brigo, F., Moccia, M., Leocani, L., Lanzillo, R., Clerico, M., et al. (2018). e-Health and multiple sclerosis: an update. Mult. Scler. J. 24, 1657–1664. doi: 10.1177/1352458518799629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, D. (2012). The Effect of Musical Tempo on Video Game Performance. Ma. Jyväskylän: University of Jyväskylä.

Google Scholar

Li, L., Chen, R., and Chen, J. (2016). Playing action video games improves visuomotor control. Psychol. Sci. 27, 1092–1108. doi: 10.1177/0956797616650300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, R., Polat, U., Makous, W., and Bavelier, D. (2009). Enhancing the contrast sensitivity function through action video game training. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 549–551. doi: 10.1038/nn.2296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, D. A., Biely, E., Thai, C. L., and Peinado, S. (2014). “Transfer of learning from video game play to classroom,” in Learning by Playing: Videogaming. Education, ed. F. C. Blumberg (New York, NY: New York Univerisity Press), 189–203. doi: 10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780199896646.003.0013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenz, R. C., Gleich, T., Gallinat, J., and Kühn, S. (2015). Video game training and the reward system. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:40. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, M. K., Tsai, C. H., and Ziemann, U. (2012). Cerebellum to motor cortex paired associative stimulation induces bidirectional STDP-like plasticity in human motor cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:260. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J., and Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature 390, 279–281. doi: 10.1038/36846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matzen, L. E., Benz, Z. O., Dixon, K. R., Posey, J., Kroger, J. K., and Speed, A. E. (2010). Recreating Raven’s: software for systematically generating large numbers of Raven-like matrix problems with normed properties. Behav. Res. Methods 42, 525–541. doi: 10.3758/brm.42.2.525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mishra, J., Anguera, J. A., and Gazzaley, A. (2016). Videogames for neurocognitive optimization. Neuron 90, 214–218.

Google Scholar

Mitchell, A. S. (2015). The mediodorsal thalamus as a higher order thalamic relay nucleus important for learning and decision-making. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 54, 76–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Momi, D., Smeralda, C., Sprugnoli, G., Ferrone, S., Rossi, S., Rossi, A., et al. (2018a). Acute and long-lasting cortical thickness changes following intensive first-person action videogame practice. Behav. Brain Res. 353, 62–73. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Momi, D., Smeralda, C., Sprugnoli, G., Neri, F., Rossi, S., Rossi, A., et al. (2018b). Thalamic morphometric changes induced by first-person action videogame training. Eur. J. Neurosci. 49, 1180–1195.

Google Scholar

Morrison, A. B., and Chein, J. M. (2011). Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 46–60. doi: 10.3758/s13423-010-0034-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mort, D. J., Perry, R. J., Mannan, S. K., Hodgson, T. L., Anderson, E., Quest, R., et al. (2003). Differential cortical activation during voluntary and reflexive saccades in man. NeuroImage 18, 231–246. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00028-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naito, E., Scheperjans, F., Eickhoff, S. B., Amunts, K., Roland, P. E., Zilles, K., et al. (2008). Human superior parietal lobule is involved in somatic perception of bimanual interaction with an external object. J. Neurophysiol. 99, 695–703. doi: 10.1152/jn.00529.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 353–383. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oei, A. C., and Patterson, M. D. (2013). Enhancing cognition with video games: a multiple game training study. PLoS One 8:e58546. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Özçetin, M., Gümüştaş, F., Çaǧ, Y., Gökbay, ÝZ., and Özmel, A. (2019). The relationships between video game experience and cognitive abilities in adolescents. Neuropsychiatr. Dis.Treat. 15, 1171–1180. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S206271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parke, A., Griffiths, M., and Parke, J. (2005). Can playing poker be good for you? Poker as a transferable skill. J. Gambl. Issues 14.

Google Scholar

Pompeu, J. E., Mendes, F. A., dos, S., Silva, K. G., da Lobo, A. M., Oliveira, T., et al. (2012). Effect of Nintendo WiiTM-based motor and cognitive training on activities of daily living in patients with Parkinson’s disease: a randomised clinical trial. Physiother. Spec. Issue Adv. Technol. Incl. Pap. WCPT 98, 196–204. doi: 10.1016/j.physio.2012.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prosperini, L., Petsas, N., and Sbardella, E. (2015). Far transfer effect associated with video game balance training in multiple sclerosis: from balance to cognition? J. Neurol. 262, 774–776. doi: 10.1007/s00415-015-7640-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rauchs, G., Orban, P., Balteau, E., Schmidt, C., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., et al. (2008). Partially segregated neural networks for spatial and contextual memory in virtual navigation. Hippocampus 18, 503–518. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raven, J., Raven, J. C., and Court, J. H. (1998). Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Avaliable at: http://www.sjdm.org/dmidi/Raven’s_Standard_Progressive_Matrices.html (accessed May 3, 2021).

Google Scholar

Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., and Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink? J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 18, 849–860. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, E. M. (2007). The serial reaction time task: implicit motor skill learning? J. Neurosci. 27, 10073–10075. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2747-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosano, C., Aizenstein, H., Cochran, J., Saxton, J., De Kosky, S., Newman, A. B., et al. (2005). Functional neuroimaging indicators of successful executive control in the oldest old. NeuroImage 28, 881–889. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., Rigby, C. S., and Przybylski, A. (2006). The motivational pull of video games: a self-determination theory approach. Motiv. Emot. 30, 344–360. doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9051-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sala, G., and Gobet, F. (2017). Do the benefits of chess instruction transfer to academic and cognitive skills? A meta-analysis. Educ. Res. Rev. 18, 46–57. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2016.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, W., Eschman, A., and Zuccolotto, A. (2012). E-Prime User’s Guide. Sharpsburg, PA: Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools, Inc.

Google Scholar

Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59(Suppl. 20) 22–33. quiz 34-57,

Google Scholar

Shinaver, C. S., Entwistle, P. C., and Söderqvist, S. (2014). Cogmed WM training: reviewing the reviews. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 3, 163–172.

Google Scholar

Shipp, S. (2004). The brain circuitry of attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 223–230. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sitaram, R., Ros, T., Stoeckel, L., Haller, S., Scharnowski, F., Lewis-Peacock, J., et al. (2017). Closed-loop brain training: the science of neurofeedback. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 86–100. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, C. W., Vandromme, M. J., Tyra, S. L., and Hawn, M. T. (2009). Proficiency-based laparoscopic and endoscopic training with virtual reality simulators: a comparison of proctored and independent approaches. J. Surg. Educ. 66, 201–207. doi: 10.1016/j.jsurg.2009.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, I., and Feng, J. (2010). Video games and spatial cognition. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 14, 92–104. doi: 10.1037/a0019491

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer-Smith, M., and Klingberg, T. (2015). Benefits of a working memory training program for inattention in daily life: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One 10:e0119522. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0119522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, B. E., and Stanford, T. R. (2008). Multisensory integration: current issues from the perspective of the single neuron. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 255–266.

Google Scholar

Stephan, K. M., Fink, G. R., Passingham, R. E., Silbersweig, D., Ceballos-Baumann, A. O., Frith, C. D., et al. (1995). Functional anatomy of the mental representation of upper extremity movements in healthy subjects. J. Neurophysiol. 73, 373–386. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.1.373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thorell, L. B., Lindqvist, S., Bergman-Nutley, S., Bohlin, G., and Klingberg, T. (2009). Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Dev. Sci. 12, 106–113. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00745.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thorndike, E. L., and Woodworth, R. S. (1901). The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. Psychol. Rev. 8, 247–261. doi: 10.1037/h0074898

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tillberg, L. V. (2020). The dynamics of military skills: the role of experience-based knowledge in challenging situations. Scand. J. Milit. Stud. 3, 55+.

Google Scholar

Treisman, A. M., and Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognit. Psychol. 12, 97–136. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallett, D. B., Lamb, R. L., and Annetta, L. A. (2013). The gorilla in the room: the impacts of video-game play on visual attention. Comput. Hum. Behav. 29, 2183–2187. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visser, T. A. W., Boden, C., and Giaschi, D. E. (2004). Children with dyslexia: evidence for visual attention deficits in perception of rapid sequences of objects. Vis. Res. 44, 2521–2535. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Keywords: videogame, first-person shooter, videogame training, cognitive training, human learning

Citation: Neri F, Smeralda CL, Momi D, Sprugnoli G, Menardi A, Ferrone S, Rossi S, Rossi A, Di Lorenzo G and Santarnecchi E (2021) Personalized Adaptive Training Improves Performance at a Professional First-Person Shooter Action Videogame. Front. Psychol. 12:598410. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.598410

Received: 21 December 2020; Accepted: 07 April 2021;
Published: 10 June 2021.

Edited by:

Joanne DiFrancisco-Donoghue, New York Institute of Technology, United States

Reviewed by:

Carlos Vaz De Carvalho, Polytechnic Institute of Porto, Portugal
Alessia Tessari, University of Bologna, Italy
Peter Douris, New York Institute of Technology, United States

Copyright © 2021 Neri, Smeralda, Momi, Sprugnoli, Menardi, Ferrone, Rossi, Rossi, Di Lorenzo and Santarnecchi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Emiliano Santarnecchi, esantarn@bidmc.harvard.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.