- 1Faculty of Psychology, Swiss Distance University Institute, Brig, Switzerland
- 2Department for Psychology of Development and Education, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- 3Department of Sport Science, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany
- 4Department of Educational Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
In response to the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) schools around the world have been closed to protect against the spread of coronavirus. In several countries, homeschooling has been introduced to replace classroom schooling. With a focus on individual differences, the present study examined 138 schoolers (age range = 6 to 21 years) regarding their self-control and boredom proneness. The results showed that both traits were important in predicting adherence to homeschooling. Schoolers with higher levels of self-control perceived homeschooling as less difficult, which in turn increased homeschooling adherence. In contrast, schoolers with higher levels of boredom proneness perceived homeschooling as more difficult, which in turn reduced homeschooling adherence. These results partially hold when it comes to studying in the classroom. However, boredom threatened adherence only in the homeschooling context. Our results indicate that boredom proneness is a critical construct to consider when educational systems switch to homeschooling during a pandemic.
Introduction
“And the faculty of voluntarily bringing back a wandering attention, over and over again, is the very root of judgment, character, and will. No one is compos sui [master of himself] if he have it not. An education which should improve this faculty would be the education per excellence.” William James, The Principles of Psychology (1980).
The coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic represents a global crisis affecting nearly all countries and every sector of human life including health, economy, and education. Current theorizing highlights the relevance of social and behavioral sciences in order to contain the spread of the virus (Van Bavel et al., 2020). To slow the spread of COVID-19, countries have employed various social distancing (or physical distancing) measures. One frequently employed social distancing measure is the closing of schools and educational institutions. In March 2020, such measures were employed by 102 countries, with the effect of almost 900 million children and adolescents being asked to switch to homeschooling (Burn, 2020). This forced COVID-19 homeschooling should not be mistaken for classical homeschooling, which has emerged since the late 1970s as a dedicated educational approach (Jolly and Matthews, 2020). Contrary to classical homeschooling, the current situation was imposed to schoolers, parents, and teachers, and there was a great variation in terms of restrictions and in terms of teaching activities in general (e.g., online teaching, homeschooling, self-organized study; Thorell et al., 2020). As it is likely that homeschooling presents schoolers with challenges that differ from those that they experience during classroom schooling, it is crucial to investigate the role of individual differences in explaining children's struggles and successes in dealing with this change in educational format. Specifically, given the reduction in external structure, we expect homeschooling to present a self-regulatory challenge. In turn, we expect personality traits that affect how people deal with such challenges to be of particular importance during this time. Boredom proneness and self-control are two such traits. Here, we draw upon recent functional theorizing on boredom and self-control (Wolff and Martarelli, 2020) to investigate the role of boredom proneness and trait self-control in how schoolers cope with the challenges of homeschooling in the current situation. Our approach can help in identifying schoolers that might struggle with homeschooling and enable the development of strategies that enhance homeschooling adherence.
Self-Control and Boredom
Self-control and boredom are closely linked: High self-control has shown to be associated with low boredom proneness (Isacescu et al., 2017; Mugon et al., 2018; Bieleke et al., 2020b; Boylan et al., 2020; Wolff et al., 2020b). In addition, boredom seems to be related with poor cognitive, physical and emotional self-regulation (Isacescu et al., 2017; Wolff et al., 2020a) and while high trait self-control is rather consistently linked with positive outcomes (Moffitt et al., 2011), boredom proneness is linked with mostly negative outcomes (Eastwood et al., 2012). Based on this inverse relationship on the trait level, recent theoretical work suggests that the sensation of boredom and the sensation that accompanies the application of self-control (i.e., effort) both act as functional signals that help people orient their behavior (Wolff and Martarelli, 2020), which has been supported by empirical research (Bieleke et al., 2020a). According to this perspective, the perception of effort that arises when self-control is applied signals the costs of a current activity (e.g., the cost of foregoing the gratification of surfing the internet and to do the math assignment instead) and if the cost outweighs the activity's benefit, then this activity is terminated (Kurzban et al., 2013). Within this line of reasoning, boredom is understood as a signal that the task at hand is not worth pursuing (e.g., completing the math assignment) and that there might be other more rewarding activities (e.g., surfing the internet). Thus, boredom is a strong driver of behavioral change (Geana et al., 2016; Gomez-Ramirez and Costa, 2017; Bench and Lench, 2019; Danckert, 2019). To resist the boredom-induced drive for change, self-control is required (Milyavskaya et al., 2019), thereby making boredom an additional self-control demand, that schoolers are likely to have to deal with during homeschooling.
Self-control can be described as the effort individuals exert to overcome predominant responses to reach a goal (Shenhav et al., 2013) and has been found to emerge early in life and to continue to develop until adulthood (Pan and Zhu, 2018). Self-control is required in many situations and higher levels of self-control have been shown to be associated with several positive outcomes including transition to school and academic achievement (Tangney et al., 2004; Welsh et al., 2010; Zhu et al., 2016; Lindner et al., 2018; Martarelli et al., 2018). Given the key role of self-control in the educational context, it is plausible that high self-control helps schooler to better deal with specific demands of homeschooling (Blume et al., 2020). Indeed, studying in a more unstructured context such as at home where other (more rewarding) activities are within the reach of schoolers, possibly requires more effortful control than studying in a classroom. Thus, the role of self-control might be even more pronounced during homeschooling than in the classroom setting. In turn, to keep on studying while in home confinement might be particularly challenging for schoolers with low levels of self-control.
Boredom has been defined as the inability to successfully engage attention with the outer world or inner worlds (Eastwood et al., 2012) and has been associated with several negative outcomes also in the academic context (Bearden et al., 1989; Pekrun et al., 2010). While the detrimental effects of boredom in the classroom might partially be relieved by the teacher (e.g., by changing the task at hand, by redirecting attention to learning requirements, and more generally by highlighting the meaning of studying, or by adapting the difficulty of the learning material; Westgate and Wilson, 2018), homeschoolers might be challenged by several other opportunities (e.g., surfing the internet) and the experience of boredom will enhance the saliency of these other more rewarding activities (Elhai et al., 2017). Moreover, being in an environment that offers many alternative activities and not being supposed to engage with these activities is perceived as particularly boring (Struk et al., 2020). Thus, we predict that boredom prone schoolers will find it more difficult to study in homeschooling and some will mitigate boredom by engaging in alternative activities.
The Present Study
The rapid and exponential spread of the coronavirus across the world has generated substantial economic and social disruptions that have affected people's lives and well-being. For schoolers, parents and teachers school closures and the associated homeschooling has been a new challenge. The schoolers' perspective quickly gets out of focus when discussing about homeschooling. Therefore, we developed a short, child-friendly questionnaire, which should give information about the experiences of schoolers. Based on recent theorizing about self-control and boredom (Martarelli and Wolff, 2020; Wolff and Martarelli, 2020) we aimed to investigate the role of trait self-control and boredom proneness in the perceived difficulty of homeschooling and in the adherence to homeschooling. We expect that schoolers high in self-control will find it easier to study in homeschooling, which in turn should increase homeschooling adherence. Further, we expect that schoolers high in trait boredom proneness find it more difficult to study in homeschooling, which in turn should decrease homeschooling adherence. We expect trait self-control and boredom proneness to have direct effects on homeschooling adherence. Finally, we expect similar relationships between these variables in the classroom; however, because of how classroom lectures are structured these relationships should be less pronounced. Figure 1 summarizes the proposed mediation model.
Figure 1. Conceptual model in which the effects of self-control and boredom on adherence are mediated by the perceived difficulty of homeschooling.
Materials and Methods
Participants
One hundred thirty-eight schoolers (58.7% female) took part to the study. All of them were attending school (44.9% primary school, 19.6% secondary school, 32.6% college, 2.9% other) and in homeschooling when filling in the questionnaire (end of April 2020). The age range was between 6 and 21 years and the vast majority of the participating schoolers lived in Switzerland (98.6%). More than half of the participants were French-speaking (55.1%), the remaining participants were German-speaking. About half of the sample reported their parents held a university degree (49.28%). The schoolers who participated in this study were contacted via their parents, who first gave parental consent. The local ethics committee approved the study, which was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Procedure
Schoolers completed the questionnaire online, using the freely available open source software Limesurvey (www.limesurvey.org). All schoolers gave written informed consent, and they could ask parents for help, if needed. The study lasted on average 10 min and we used items with 5-point Likert scales throughout the study (1 = do not agree at all, 5 = fully agree) with the exception of demographics.
First, adherence to study in homeschooling was assessed with one item (“At home, I reliably work on the tasks that the teacher gives me”) and perceived difficulty of homeschooling was measured with a set of three items (“I find it hard to study in homeschooling,” “I find it boring to study in homeschooling,” “I have no desire to study in homeschooling”). Next, the same questions were asked for the classroom schooling context before the COVID-19 pandemic. We assessed adherence to study in the classroom (“In the classroom I reliably work on the tasks that the teacher gives me”) and perceived difficulty of studying in the classroom with a set of three items (“I find it hard to study in the classroom,” “I find it boring to study in the classroom,” “I have no desire to study in the classroom”).
Subsequently, schoolers worked on five items taken from the German short version of the Self-Control Scale (Tangney et al., 2004; Bertrams and Dickhäuser, 2009), which measures individuals' general ability to regulate themselves (e.g., “Sometimes I can't stop myself from doing something, even if I know it is wrong”), and four questions taken from the German Short Boredom Proneness Scale (Struk et al., 2017; Martarelli et al., 2020), which measures individuals' boredom proneness (e.g., “I find it hard, to entertain myself”). The wording of items was adapted to children and is reported in the Supplementary Material. Finally, schoolers reported demographics, including gender, age, residence, attended school, and parents' educational level.
Results
To test the proposed model (Figure 1) we used R (R core Team, 2020) with the functionalities of the lavaan package (Rosseel et al., 2018). The standard errors for the defined parameters were computed with bias-corrected bootstrap method (1,000 bootstrap repetitions). The R code to reproduce analyses as well as the data set is available on OSF at https://osf.io/32rwe/. Descriptive statistics were computed with jamovi version 1.1.9.0 (The jamovi project, 2020) and with JASP version 0.12.2 (JASP Team, 2020).
Descriptive Statistics
Adherence to study in homeschooling during the COVID-19 situation was relatively high (mean of 3.84 out of 5) but significantly lower than adherence to study in the classroom (mean of 4.36 out of 5), paired-samples t-test t(137) = 5.70, p < .001, Cohen's dz =0.48. The internal consistency of the scales was good, skewness and kurtosis were low (see Table 1).
With regard to preliminary correlation analyses, Table 2 shows significant correlations between our predictor, mediator and outcome variables. The correlations were in the expected direction, and stronger in the homeschooling context when compared to the classroom.
Homeschooling Model
The unstandardized and standardized path coefficients, standard errors, and p-values of the homeschooling model can be found in Table 3 and are depicted in Figure 2. Individual differences in self-control and boredom influenced the perceived difficulty of homeschooling, self-control: b = −0.550, β = −0.417, SE = 0.106, p = < 0.001; boredom: b = 0.208, β = 0.170, SE = 0.109, p = 0.056. Both variables together explained 26.2% of variance in perceived difficulty. The difficulty of homeschooling in turn negatively influenced the adherence to homeschooling, b = −0.252, β = −0.227, SE = 0.078, p = 0.001. Individual differences in self-control and boredom also influenced the adherence to homeschooling, self-control: b = 0.624, β = 0.426, SE = 0.114, p = < 0.001; boredom: b = −0.192, β = −0.142, SE = 0.111, p = 0.083. For self-control not only the direct effect, but also the indirect effect, b = 0.139, β = 0.095, SE = 0.052, p = 0.007, and the total effect, b = 0.762, β = 0.521, SE = 0.110, p = < 0.001, were significant. The indirect effect of boredom on adherence was not significant, b = −0.052, β = −0.039, SE = 0.033, p = 0.114, while the total effect turned out to be significant, b = −0.245, β = −0.180, SE = 0.114, p = 0.031. The model explained 42.0% of variance in adherence to homeschooling.
Figure 2. Homeschooling model N = 138. Coefficients are unstandardized ***. Difference is significant at the 0.001 level (two-tailed) and **. Difference is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed) †. Difference is significant at the 0.1 level (two-tailed).
We additionally estimated the model including age and gender. Gender (coded as 0 for male and 1 for female) had a significant impact on self-control, b = 0.290, β = 0.177, SE = 0.124, p = 0.020. Age had a significant impact on self-control, b = −0.075, β = −0.373, SE = 0.017, p < 0.001, on boredom, b = 0.070, β = 0.319, SE = 0.018, p < 0.001, and on adherence to homeschooling, b = −0.041, β = −0.139, SE = 0.021, p = 0.049. Gender and age did not change the other results (no change in accepting or rejecting the null hypotheses).
Classroom Model
The unstandardized and standardized path coefficients, standard errors and p-values of the classroom model can be found in Table 4 and are depicted in Figure 3. Individual differences in self-control and boredom influenced the perceived difficulty of studying in the classroom, self-control: b = −0.436, β = −0.342, SE = 0.100, p = < 0.001; boredom: b = 0.205, β = 0.174, SE = 0.101, p = 0.043. Both variables together explained 19.6% of variance in perceived difficulty. However, the difficulty of studying in the classroom did not influence the adherence to studying in the classroom, b = −0.098, β = −0.132, SE = 0.071, p = 0.167. The direct effect of self-control on adherence to studying in the classroom, b = 0.254, β = 0.270, SE = 0.093, p = 0.006 turned out to be significant, while the indirect effect was not significant, b = −0.020, β = −0.023, SE = 0.019, p = 0.284. The total effect of self-control on adherence was significant, b = 0.297, β = 315, SE = 0.081, p = < 0.001. The direct, indirect and total effect of boredom on adherence to studying in the classroom were not significant (ps > 0.284). The model explained 15.3% of variance in adherence to studying in the classroom.
Figure 3. Classroom model N = 138. Coefficients are unstandardized ***. Difference is significant at the 0.001 level (two-tailed) and **. Difference is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed).
We additionally estimated the model including age and gender. Gender (coded as 0 for male and 1 for female) had a significant impact on self-control, b = 0.290, β = 0.177, SE = 0.124, p = 0.020. Age had a significant impact on self-control, b = −0.075, β = −0.373, SE = 0.017, p < 0.001, on boredom, b = 0.070, β = 0.319, SE = 0.018, p < 0.001, and on adherence to studying in the classroom, b = −0.038, β = −0.198, SE = 0.019, p = 0.042. When adjusting for age and gender, we found no changes in the pattern of results with one exception: the direct effect of self-control just missed significance, b = 0.198, β = 0.210, SE = 0.105, p = 0.060.
Discussion
Here, we show that both trait self-control and boredom proneness predict adherence to homeschooling (according to the significance of their total effects and the model's explained variance). Schoolers with higher levels of self-control perceived homeschooling as less difficult, which in turn increased homeschooling adherence. In contrast, schoolers with higher levels of boredom proneness found it more difficult to study in homeschooling (effect is significant at the 0.1 level) and showed less adherence to homeschooling (effect is significant at the 0.1 level). The same pattern of association partially occurred in the classroom context. Schoolers with higher levels of self-control perceived studying in the classroom as less difficult and showed an increase in adherence. However, boredom proneness did not threaten adherence in the classroom (direct, indirect and total effects of boredom turned out to be non-significant in the classroom model). Moreover, there was a large difference in explained variance (42.0% in the homeschooling model vs. 15.3% in the classroom model).
According to these results, schoolers with low levels of self-control and high levels of boredom proneness might be disadvantaged in a homeschooling setting. Generally these findings fit well with recent theoretical work ascribing a key role to self-control and boredom as guiding signals of goal-directed behavior (Wolff and Martarelli, 2020) and with recent empirical work showing a strong link between self-control and boredom (e.g., Isacescu et al., 2017). Thus, schoolers, who tend to get bored also tend to show low self-control and this structural combination could make the continuation of their education during a pandemic challenging.
In order to create equal educational opportunities, teachers, parents, and school principals should take boredom into account in the context of homeschooling during a pandemic. One possibility to reduce the detrimental effects of boredom on learning could be to create an environment conducive to learning. First, (more rewarding) behavioral alternatives that are not linked with studying should be removed. This suggestion builds upon a recent study of Struk et al. (2020) that investigated the impact of behavioral restrictions on boredom. The results showed that a room with no behavioral alternatives (when compared with a room containing several behavioral possibilities such as a computer with an open browser) was perceived as less boring by participants that were asked to entertain themselves with their thoughts. Moreover, participants in the room with behavioral alternatives more often engaged in these alternatives even though they were explicitly told not to do so. In other words, to mitigate boredom, they changed behavior (Struk et al., 2020). This behavioral pattern might also possibly occur in a homeschooling context. Thus, a learning environment that limits boredom could be achieved by proposing structured and diverse learning material, by using different learning modalities, and by reducing general distractions (e.g., putting distractions such as smartphones out of reach). Further, the difficulty of the learning material should be considered (Eastwood et al., 2012; Westgate and Wilson, 2018). By providing sufficient assistance, adapting the requirements or encouraging schoolers to seek for help, an under/overload can be prevented which in turn decreases the risk for a lack of adherence. Finally, boredom also arises as a consequence of lack of meaning (Van Tilburg and Igou, 2017; Westgate and Wilson, 2018). Thus, a further strategy might be to make homeschooling more meaningful and valuable.
Importantly, even in optimal environments (e.g., structured environment) people can still get bored. This is particularly true for boredom prone schoolers, who tend to experience boredom more frequently. Therefore, it would be crucial to equip schoolers with specific skills to recognize and cope with their boredom. One suggestion is, for e.g., to help schoolers to identify their individual “warning signs” of boredom (and of related constructs such as mind-wandering) and to encourage them to change activity by explicitly choosing the next activity, as for example to explore alternative learning material when bored with the task at hand. More generally, learning to recognize and use boredom in an adaptive manner might be useful for schoolers to redirect their attention to relevant external and/or internal information. This strategy should in turn also make homeschooling less self-control demanding.
There are limitations of the study that should be considered when interpreting the findings. One major limitation is that we used cross-sectional data, thus the temporal ordering of variables in the causal chain of mediation reflects theoretical considerations that do not necessarily correspond to how these effects unfold over time. For this reason, the estimated mediation effect is of correlational nature. Since correlations cannot offer insight into directionality of a relation between variables, it will be important for future research to carefully examine the directionality of the effect. Only then effective interventions can be implemented to improve homeschooling conditions. Furthermore, we worked with a small set of self-report measures, the sample size is also relatively small, and we recruited a convenience sample. Further research is needed to investigate other variables that are known to correlate with the variables measured here, such as self-efficacy or sense of agency (in terms of individual awareness of what one can do). Self-efficacy pertains to subjective expectations of being able to deal with situations that are novel and/or challenging on the basis of one's own competencies (Bandura, 1997). It is thus conceivable that self-efficacy would allow shedding additional light on the link between the perceived difficulty of homeschooling and adherence to homeschooling. Future research would benefit from investigating the generalizability in more detail using other methodologies (such as online experience sampling methods) and other samples, including different socio-cultural contexts.
Because boredom has been associated with numerous negative outcomes (Eastwood et al., 2012), it has a rather negative connotation. However, it has also been linked to positive outcomes such as creativity (Harris, 2000) and pro-social behavior (Van Tilburg and Igou, 2017). Boredom is a powerful motivator for negative and positive behaviors alike (Elpidorou, 2014, 2017). If correctly taken into account, boredom informs schoolers, that they are failing to pay attention and that a change in behavior is needed. One advantage of homeschooling (compared to traditional settings) might be the possible individualization of learning. Homeschoolers are more flexible and can thus more strongly regulate their way of learning if provided with the right support.
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/32rwe/).
Ethics Statement
The study was reviewed and approved by the ethics commission of the Swiss Distance University Institute. Written informed consent from the participants' legal guardian was not required to participate in this online study in accordance with the institutional requirements.
Author Contributions
CM, SP, MB, and WW contributed to conception and design of the study. SP and CM prepared the study and collected the data. CM performed the analyses and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors discussed the results, contributed to the final manuscript and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank all participating schoolers and their parents for their cooperation.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.594256/full#supplementary-material
References
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York, NY: W H Freeman/Times Books/ Henry Holt&Co.
Bearden, L. J., Spencer, W. A., and Moracco, J. C. (1989). A study of high school dropouts. School Couns. 37, 113–120.
Bench, S. W., and Lench, H. C. (2019). Boredom as a seeking state: Boredom prompts the pursuit of novel (even negative) experiences. Emotion 19, 242–254. doi: 10.1037/emo0000433
Bertrams, A., and Dickhäuser, O. (2009). Messung dispositioneller selbstkontroll-kapazität: eine deutsche adaptation der kurzform der self-control scale (SCS-K-D). Diagnostica 55, 2–10. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924.55.1.2
Bieleke, M., Barton, L., and Wolff, W. (2020a). Trajectories of boredom in self-control demanding tasks. PsyArXiv. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/ekqrv
Bieleke, M., Martarelli, C. S., and Wolff, W. (2020b). Boredom makes it difficult, but it helps to have a plan: Investigating adherence to social distancing guidelines during the COVID-19 pandemic. PsyArXiv. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/enzbv
Blume, F., Schmidt, A., Kramer, A. C., Schmiedek, F., and Neubauer, A. B. (2020). Homeschooling during the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic: The role of students' trait self-regulation and task attributes of daily learning tasks for students' daily self-regulation. PsyArXiv. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/tnrdj
Boylan, J., Seli, P., Scholer, A. A., and Danckert, J. (2020). Boredom in the COVID-19 pandemic: Trait boredom proneness, the desire to act, and rule-breaking. Pers. Individ. Diff. 171:110387. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110387
Burn, T. (2020). A Helping Hand: Education Responding to the Coronavirus Pandemic. OECD. Available online at: https://oecdedutoday.com/education-responding-coronavirus-pandemic/
Danckert, J. (2019). Boredom: Managing the Delicate Balance Between Exploration and Exploitation, Vol. 2. ed J. R. Velasco (Springer). doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-26395-9_3
Eastwood, J. D., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., and Smilek, D. (2012). The unengaged mind: defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 482–495. doi: 10.1177/1745691612456044
Elhai, J. D., Vasquez, J. K., Lustgarten, S. D., Levine, J. C., and Hall, B. J. (2017). Proneness to boredom mediates relationships between problematic smartphone use with depression and anxiety severity. Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 36, 707–720. doi: 10.1177/0894439317741087
Elpidorou, A. (2014). The bright side of boredom. Front. Psychol. 5:1245. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01245
Elpidorou, A. (2017). The moral dimensions of boredom: a call for research. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 21, 30–48. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000098
Geana, A., Wilson, R., Daw, N. D., and Cohen, J. (2016). “Boredom, information-seeking and exploration,” in Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 1751–1756. Available online at: https://cogsci.mindmodeling.org/2016/papers/0307/paper0307.pdf
Gomez-Ramirez, J., and Costa, T. (2017). Boredom begets creativity: a solution to the exploitation-exploration trade-off in predictive coding. Bio Systems 162, 168–176. doi: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2017.04.006
Harris, M. B. (2000). Correlates and characteristics of boredom proneness and boredom. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 30, 576–598. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02497.x
Isacescu, J., Struk, A. A., and Danckert, J. (2017). Cognitive and affective predictors of boredom proneness. Cog. Emot. 31, 1741–1748. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1259995
Jolly, J. L., and Matthews, M. S. (2020). The shifting landscape of the homeschooling continuum. Educ. Rev. 72, 269–280. doi: 10.1080/00131911.2018.1552661
Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., and Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 661–679. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12003196
Lindner, C., Nagy, G., and Retelsdorf, J. (2018). The need for self-control in achievement tests: changes in students' state self-control capacity and effort investment. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 21, 1113–1131. doi: 10.1007/s11218-018-9455-9
Martarelli, C. S., Bertrams, A., and Wolff, W. (2020). A personality trait-based network of boredom, spontaneous and deliberate mind-wandering. Assessment. doi: 10.1177/1073191120936336. [Epub ahead of print].
Martarelli, C. S., Feurer, E., Dapp, L. C., and Roebers, C. M. (2018). Profiles of executive functions and social skills in the transition to school: A person-centred approach. Infant Child Dev. 27:2114. doi: 10.1002/icd.2114
Martarelli, C. S., and Wolff, W. (2020). Too bored to bother? Boredom as a potential threat to the efficacy of pandemic containment measures. Humanit. Soc. Sic. Commun. 7:28. doi: 10.1057/s41599-020-0512-6
Milyavskaya, M., Inzlicht, M., Johnson, T., and Larson, M. J. (2019). Reward sensitivity following boredom and cognitive effort: a high-powered neurophysiological investigation. Neuropsychologia 123, 159–168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.03.033
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.Am. 108, 2693–2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108
Mugon, J., Struk, A., and Danckert, J. (2018). A failure to launch: regulatory modes and boredom proneness. Front. Psychol. 9:1126. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01126
Pan, Q., and Zhu, Q. (2018). Development of self-control in early childhood—a growth mixture modeling approach. Cogent Psychol. 5:1544537. doi: 10.1080/23311908.2018.1544537
Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., and Perry, R. P. (2010). Boredom in achievement settings: exploring control–value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. J. Educ. Psychol. 102, 531–549. doi: 10.1037/a0019243
Rosseel, Y., Jorgensen, T. D., Oberski, D., Byrnes, J., Vanbrabant, L., Savalei, V., et al. (2018). lavaan: Latent variable analysis [R package]. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/package=lavaan
Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., and Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: AN integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron 79, 217–240. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.007
Struk, A. A., Carriere, J. S. A., Cheyne, J. A., and Danckert, J. (2017). A short boredom proneness scale: Development and psychometric properties. Assessment 24, 346–359. doi: 10.1177/1073191115609996
Struk, A. A., Scholer, A. A., Danckert, J., and Seli, P. (2020). Rich environments, dull experiences: How environment can exacerbate the effect of constraint on the experience of boredom. Cog. Emot. 34, 1517–1523. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2020.1763919
Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., and Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J. Pers. 72, 271–322. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x
Thorell, L. B., Skoglund, C. B., Peña, A. G., Baeyens, D., Fuermaier, A., Groom, M., et al. (2020). Psychosocial effects of homeschooling during the COVID-19 pandemic: differences between seven European countries and between children with and without mental health conditions. PsyArXiv. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/68pfx
Van Bavel, J. J., Baicker, K., Boggio, P. S., Capraro, V., Cichocka, A., Cikara, M., et al. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 460–471. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z
Van Tilburg, W. A. P., and Igou, E. R. (2017). Boredom begs to differ: differentiation from other negative emotions. Emotion 17, 309–322. doi: 10.1037/emo0000233
Welsh, J. A., Nix, R. L., Blair, C., Bierman, K. L., and Nelson, K. E. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. J. Educ. Psychol. 102, 43–53. doi: 10.1037/a0016738
Westgate, E. C., and Wilson, T. D. (2018). Boring thoughts and bored minds: the MAC model of boredom and cognitive engagement. Psychol. Rev. 125, 689–713. doi: 10.1037/rev0000097
Wolff, W., Bieleke, M., Stähler, J., and Schüler, J. (2020a). Too bored for sports? Adaptive and non-adaptive latent personality profiles for exercice behavior. SportRxiv Preprints. doi: 10.31236/osf.io/tjw89
Wolff, W., and Martarelli, C. S. (2020). Bored into depletion? Towards a tentative integration of perceived self-control exertion and boredom as guiding signals for goal-directed behavior. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15, 1273–1283. doi: 10.1177/1745691620921394
Wolff, W., Martarelli, C. S., Schüler, J., and Bieleke, M. (2020b). High boredom proneness and low trait self-control impair adherence to social distancing guidelines during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:5420. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17155420
Keywords: COVID-19, homeschooling, boredom, self-control, self-regulation
Citation: Martarelli CS, Pacozzi SG, Bieleke M and Wolff W (2021) High Trait Self-Control and Low Boredom Proneness Help COVID-19 Homeschoolers. Front. Psychol. 12:594256. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.594256
Received: 12 August 2020; Accepted: 25 January 2021;
Published: 18 February 2021.
Edited by:
Douglas F. Kauffman, Medical University of the Americas–Nevis, United StatesReviewed by:
James Danckert, University of Waterloo, CanadaValerio Capraro, Middlesex University, United Kingdom
Copyright © 2021 Martarelli, Pacozzi, Bieleke and Wolff. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Corinna S. Martarelli, corinna.martarelli@fernuni.ch