Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 03 June 2021
Sec. Health Psychology

Exploring the Psychological Effects of COVID-19 Home Confinement in China: A Psycho-Linguistic Analysis on Weibo Data Pool

\r\nPeijing Wu,Peijing Wu1,2Nan ZhaoNan Zhao1Sijia Li,Sijia Li1,2Zeyu Liu,Zeyu Liu1,2Yilin Wang,Yilin Wang1,2Tianli LiuTianli Liu3Xiaoqian Liu*Xiaoqian Liu1*Tingshao Zhu,Tingshao Zhu1,2
  • 1CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Beijing, China
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
  • 3Institute of Population Research, Peking University, Beijing, China

Backgrounds: With the rapid spread of COVID-19, strict home confinement has been implemented in most parts of Chinese regions. Millions of people were not allowed to leave their homes except for special reasons. Home confinement plays an essential role in curbing pandemic and promoting preventive behaviors, but it may affect individuals’ mental health as well.

Objects: The objective of this study was to explore the psychological impacts of home confinement.

Materials and Methods: We collected more than 150,360 Weibo messages from 5,370 Chinese active users, and then extracted psycho-linguistic features from these messages. Psycho-linguistic analysis was carried out using the 2 (confinement vs. non-confinement) × 2 (before vs. after confinement) repeated measure analysis of variance (RM ANOVA).

Results: The results showed that the frequency of positive emotion words was remarkably decreased during home confinement [F(1,5368) = 7.926, p = 0.005, η2 = 0.001]. In high-endemic subgroup, home confinement also reduced the frequency of exclusion words [F(1,3445) = 4.518, p = 0.034, η2 = 0.001] and inhibition words [F(1,3445) = 10.154, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.003].

Conclusion: Home confinement caused a decline in the use of positive emotion words. This indicates that home confinement can increase the frequency of negative emotions. The changes of exclusion words and inhibition words in high-endemic areas may be related to the high epidemic threat and the urgent need for social distancing in these areas.

Introduction

At the end of 2019, an outbreak of novel coronavirus (COVID-19) has quickly spread in China (Huang C. et al., 2020). To control the epidemic, home confinement of varying strictness—from checkpoints at building entrances to hard limits on going outdoors—have been implemented for millions of people in China (NewYorkTimes, 2020). The details of such regulations might be slightly different from city to city, but they all limited daily activities of people.

Such quarantine tends to cause negative psychological effects (Brooks et al., 2020), especially in terms of emotion and cognition. The emotional symptoms include moody (Lee et al., 2005), fear (Desclaux et al., 2017; Caleo et al., 2018), anger (Cava et al., 2005; Caleo et al., 2018), anxiety (Bai et al., 2004), and depression (Hawryluck et al., 2004). The cognitive symptoms include confusion (Cava et al., 2005; Pan et al., 2005; Braunack-Mayer et al., 2013; Caleo et al., 2018), numbness (Pan et al., 2005), avoidance and high-risk judgment related to post-traumatic stress disorder (Bai et al., 2004; Reynolds et al., 2008; Wu et al., 2009). Previous studies of medical staff during SARS showed that being quarantined was a predictor of posttraumatic stress (Bai et al., 2004; Wu et al., 2009) and depression symptoms (Liu et al., 2012) in hospital employees. Another study investigated on the psychosocial responses of children and their parents during SARS and H1N1 found that quarantine and isolation could be offensive to a significant portion of children and their parents (Sprang and Silman, 2013).

Based on above evidences, we can infer that large-scale home confinement during COVID-19 is also likely to have a negative psychological impact on public. However, previous researches generally focus on special population at a small sample size, such as patients and medical staff. In addition, although there are precedents for similar activity restrictions, such as in areas of Singapore, Canada and China during severe acute respiratory syndrome (SARS) (Gupta et al., 2005; Hsieh et al., 2005; Ooi et al., 2005). But the difference is that restrictions in these precedents only isolate suspected cases and contacts within a limited range, while COVID-19 home confinement restricted daily activities of millions of normative population, regardless of whether they have been in contact with suspected patients. In order to reduce the risk of such undifferentiated isolation in terms of public mental health, and to provide references for future interventions, it is necessary to understand the potential psychological changes caused by COVID-19 home confinement.

To explore the psychological changes caused by public emergencies, the pretest-posttest research design is the most commonly practiced (Peterson and Seligman, 2003; Li et al., 2020; Su et al., 2020). Psychological states are usually measured by retrospective questionnaires, yet it may not be appropriate to measure the psychological effect of home confinement during COVID-19. Since home confinement was carried out in an emergency condition, it would have been impossible to predict the time of implementation. Hence, we could not possibly conduct any prior measurement. In addition, because home confinement might have been cancelled in cities where the epidemic was rapidly controlled in short time (e.g., only 1 week in Ningbo, Zhejiang), there would have been little time available to conduct any timely survey. For the above reasons, we considered other novel methods to explore the psychological impact of home confinement.

Due to the widespread use of the Internet and rapid growth of virtual environments, individuals spend increasingly more time online, especially on social media. Several studies have shown that public mental states can be effectively identified by analyzing the psycho-linguistic features obtained from Social Network Services (SNS), including Twitter, Weibo, etc. (Golder and Macy, 2011; Zhao et al., 2016; Alizadeh et al., 2019). For instance, Niu et al. (2014) found that psycho-linguistic features have obvious advantages in identifying emotions of Weibo users; Golder et al. (Golder and Macy, 2011) revealed the trend of positive and negative emotions of Twitter users by calculating word frequencies according to the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC); Alizadeh et al. (2019) analyzed the moral aspirations of political extremists on Twitter by word frequencies related to moral foundation. During the COVID-19 period, researchers also used psycho-linguistic features to explore the related psychological effects (Huang F. et al., 2020; Li et al., 2020; Su et al., 2020). Su et al. (2020) used LIWC to examine the impact of COVID-19 lockdown in Wuhan and Lombardy. Li et al. (2020) found some negative psychological effects caused by COVID-19 Epidemic Declaration by LIWC. Huang F. et al. (2020) revealed the roles of fear and collectivism in COVID-19 prevention based on the related psycho-linguistic features. Compared to conventional psychological surveys, psycho-linguistic features on social media have the advantages of traceability and real-time (Bollen et al., 2009; Golder and Macy, 2011), which can be matched with the time period of home confinement in various regions and effectively reflect the psychological changes of users (Tausczik and Pennebaker, 2010; Zhao et al., 2016). Therefore, we aimed to use the psycho-linguistic features on social media as a real-time measurement across large time span and diverse populations for determining the psychological impact of home confinement.

In this study, we aimed to explore the possible mental impacts of home confinement by analyzing the changes in psycho-linguistic features, extracted by the LIWC tool, among Chinese Weibo users. It is hypothetically assumed that home confinement may cause some negative psychological effects, which leads to a significant difference in relevant psycho-linguistic features before and after the confinement. Our findings can lay a foundation for further research on the psychological impacts of quarantine during COVID-19 pandemic, especially data supplements for large samples of a general population. Once the possible psychological effects were clarified, managers could formulate more effective and targeted public mental health policies. For example, if we found that home confinement had led to a decrease in the use of positive emotion words, managers could pay more attention on how to restore the public positive emotion, instead of focusing on public panic or anger. And researchers could further study what factors of home confinement reduced public positive emotion, so as to achieve more targeted intervention suggestions.

Materials and Methods

Data Collection

According to the National Coronary Pneumonia Epidemic Prevention and Control Headquarters’ announcements, 17 cities had implemented home confinement from January 23, 2020 to March 30, 2020. As shown in Table 1, we defined the above 17 cities as home confinement cities, while others as non-home confinement cities.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Home confinement cities and their start and end time.

Next, we obtained samples from the public Weibo data pool composed of 1.16 million users (Li et al., 2014). This database regularly updates Weibo user profile information and the latest Weibo text content through crawler technology. First, we filtered the samples and extracted the valid user data set S according to the following criteria: (i) non-public, non-Chinese, non-commercial accounts; and (ii) published at least ten original Weibo posts from January 11 to February 21, 2020. Given that the cultural background and pandemic status of non-Chinese users may vary from those of Chinese users, we excluded them. Sociodemographic information of the selected participants are presented in Table 2. Privacy protection was strictly conducted, in compliance with the ethical principles listed previously (Kosinski et al., 2015). No participants consent was required during the study process. The research protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (approval number: H15009).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographic characteristics of the selected participants.

After that, we identified the confinement user set Slock in which the location was one of the 17 confinement cities and the non-confinement user set Snon−lock = SSlock. For each confinement city Ci(i = 1.17), we set the 2 weeks before and after home confinement as Tbefore,i and Tafter,i, and screened out the participants of confinement group Ei and non-confinement group Fi according to the following procedures:

i) For each Weibo user in Slock, if its location was Ci and at least one Weibo was posted every day in average between Tbefore,i and Tafter,i, the user was included in the confinement group Ei.

ii) The numbers of male users EM,i and female users EF,i in the confinement group Ei were counted.

iii) The EM,i*2 male users and EF,i*2 female users in Snon–lock who posted at least one Weibo every day in average between Tbefore,i and Tafter,i were randomly selected and included in the non-confinement group Fi.

Through the above process, the ratio of participant numbers between non-confinement and confinement groups reached 2:1. Such setting could increase the statistical power since the number in confinement group was much lesser than another group (Lydersen, 2018). Considering that the durations of home confinement in Taizhou, Ningbo and Yiwu were too short (<2 weeks), the users in these 3 cities were excluded from subsequent analysis. All the above processes were carried out based on the users’ profile information available on Weibo. It shall be emphasized that the users’ location information was also obtained from their registration profile. This might not be the exact geographical location, but it was related to the user. Lastly, for all selected users, we collected their Weibo posts within 2 weeks before and after the start time of home confinement in their own city. A total of 150,360 Weibo posts were collected.

Data Analysis

After eliminating the forwarding Weibo posts, TextMind system 4.01 (a Chinese segmentation and word frequency statistical tool) was used to extract the psycholinguistic features (Gao et al., 2013). This system analyzed the input text in Chinese, and then employed Simplified Chinese LIWC (SCLIWC) dictionary (Zhao et al., 2016) for word frequency statistics.

The SCLIWC dictionary has been proven to detect individuals’ attentional focus, emotionality and thinking styles effectively (Tausczik and Pennebaker, 2010; Zhao et al., 2016). The words have previously been categorized into over 80 linguistic dimensions, including psychological processes (e.g., positive and negative emotion categories, cognitive processes such as the use of causation words), relativity-related words (e.g., time, verb tense, motion, space), and traditional content dimensions (e.g., sex, death, home, occupation) (Pennebaker and Lay, 2002). A recent study has shown that similar quarantine tends to cause negative emotions and cognition effects (Brooks et al., 2020). Hence, the affective processes words and cognitive words (13 categories in total) were selected as the affective and cognitive linguistics features of Weibo users, respectively. Examples on each word category are summarized in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Examples on affective processes words and cognitive words in SCLIWC.

Given that this study was in the background of pandemic and the severity of prior epidemic also greatly influenced people’s mental states (Murray and Schaller, 2012), we divided all the home confinement cities into two subgroups: high-endemic subgroup and low-endemic subgroup. Since Hubei had more than 83% of confirmed cases in China (as of March 1, 2020), all cities in this province were defined as high-endemic areas, while the remaining were low-endemic areas. For all home confinement cities group, high-endemic subgroup and low-endemic subgroup, the analysis was conducted separately. The entire sample filtering and grouping process is shown in Supplementary Figure 1, and the demographic profiles of the selected participants are listed in Table 2.

RM ANOVA was performed on the frequencies of each word using SPSS 24.0 (Statistical Product and Service Solutions), with home confinement status (confinement vs. non-confinement) as the between-subject variable and time (before vs. after confinement) as the within-subject variable. To exclude possible differences in pandemic severity between confinement and non-confinement groups, we only focused on the variables with significant interactions. The significant interaction meant that the difference between groups was inconsistent before and after home confinement, and this difference was mainly caused by home confinement.

Results

Several significant interactions of group (confinement vs. non-confinement) time (before vs. after confinement) were found through RM ANOVA. As these interactions reflected the impacts of home confinement on psycho-linguistic features, Simple Effect Analysis was carried out subsequently.

All Home Confinement Cities

The results of RM ANOVA in the group of all home confinement cities are summarized in Table 4. The group × time interaction was significant for the frequency of positive emotion words (e.g., love, nice, sweet) [F(1,5368) = 7.926, p = 0.005, η2 = 0.001] and inhibition words (e.g., lock, constrain, stop) [F(1,5368) = 10.166, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.002]. Figure 1 reveals the interaction plots for all home confinement cities groups. Further Simple Effect Analysis showed that the frequency of positive emotion words was decreased in the confinement group [F(1) = 16.173, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.003], and was remarkably lower than that in the non-confinement group after home confinement [F(1) = 7.569, p = 0.006, η2 = 0.001]. The frequency of inhibition words was increased in the confinement group [F(1) = 5.066, p = 0.024, η2 = 0.001], and was markedly higher than that in the non-confinement group after home confinement [F(1) = 26.522, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.005].

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The results of RM ANOVA in all home confinement cities.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The interaction plots for words with significant interactions in the group of all home confinement cities. Significant group (confinement vs. non-confinement) * time (before vs. after confinement) interaction was found in the frequency of positive emotion words (A) and Inhibition words (B) by RM ANOVA. “Confinement” represents samples in all 14 cities where home confinement have implemented; “Non-confinement” represents samples that have not experienced home confinement; “Before” represents the data collected from 2 weeks before home confinement, and “After” represents that from 2 weeks after.

High-Endemic Subgroup

The results of RM ANOVA in the subgroup of high-endemic cities are presented in Table 5. The group × time interaction was significant for the frequency of positive emotion words [F(1,3445) = 6.903, p = 0.009, η2 = 0.002], inhibition words [F(1,3445) = 10.154, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.003] and exclusive words (e.g., but, without, exclude) [F(1,3445) = 4.518, p = 0.034, η2 = 0.001]. Figure 2 shows the interaction plots for high-endemic subgroups. Further simple effect analysis revealed that the frequency of positive emotion words was decreased in the confinement group [F(1) = 15.060, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.004], and was remarkably lower than that in the non-confinement group after home confinement [F(1) = 10.337, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.003]. The frequency of inhibition words was increased in the confinement group [F(1) = 2.703, p = 0.100, η2 = 0.002], and was markedly higher than that in the non-confinement group before [F(1) = 7.367, p = 0.007, η2 = 0.013] and after home confinement [F(1) = 44.703, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.013]. The frequency of exclusion words was increased in the confinement group [F(1) = 8.029, p = 0.005, η2 = 0.002], and was apparently higher than that in the non-confinement group before [F(1) = 8.776, p = 0.003, η2 = 0.003] and after home confinement [F(1) = 26.311, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.008].

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. The results of RM ANOVA in high-endemic subgroup.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The interaction plots for words with significant interactions in high-endemic subgroup. Significant group (confinement vs. non-confinement) * time (before vs. after confinement) interaction was found in the frequency of positive emotion words (A), Inhibition words (B), and Exclusion words (C) by RM ANOVA. “Confinement” represents samples in all 14 cities where home confinement have implemented; “Non-confinement” represents samples that have not experienced home confinement; “Before” represents the data collected from 2 weeks before home confinement, and “After” represents that from 2 weeks after.

Low-Endemic Subgroup

Based on the analysis of the low-endemic areas, only positive emotion words had a significant group × time interaction [F(1,1921) = 5.796, p = 0.016, η2 = 0.003]. The results are demonstrated in Table 6. Figure 3 shows the interaction plots for the low endemic subgroup. Further simple effect analysis indicated that the frequency of positive emotion words was decreased in the confinement group [F(1) = 9.374, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.005], and no significant difference was observed between confinement and non-confinement groups before and after home confinement.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. The results of RM ANOVA in low-endemic subgroup.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The interaction plots for words with significant interactions in low-endemic subgroup. Significant group (confinement vs. non-confinement) * time (before vs. after confinement) interaction was found in the frequency of positive emotion words by RM ANOVA. “Confinement” represents samples in all 14 cities where home confinement have implemented; “Non-confinement” represents samples that have not experienced home confinement; “Before” represents the data collected from 2 weeks before home confinement, and “After” represents that from 2 weeks after.

Discussion

This study used Weibo data and psycho-linguistic analysis to explore the psychological impacts of home confinement. Our results found a decline in the frequency of positive emotion words (e.g., love, nice, sweet) after home confinement in all home confinement cities group, high-endemic subgroup and low-endemic subgroup. This indicates that home confinement is associated with an increase in the frequency of negative emotions. Compared with the low-endemic subgroup, the high-endemic subgroup reported more diverse changes such as the increased use of exclusion (e.g., but, without, exclude) and inhibition (e.g., lock, constrain, stop) words.

The Association Between Home Confinement and Decreased Positive Emotions

The frequency of positive emotion words (e.g., love, nice, sweet) decreased after home confinement in all three groups, indicating that home confinement is associated with a decline in people’s positive emotions (Kahn et al., 2007; Golder and Macy, 2011) regardless of high-endemic or low-endemic status. There could be many reasons for the decline in positive emotions. On the one hand, home confinement limited the opportunities to experience various sources of positive emotion, including the access to nature (Johnsen, 2014), social gatherings (House et al., 1988) and outdoor sports (Coon et al., 2011). On the other hand, growing evidence has suggested that people who stay at home for a long time may have a low level of self-satisfaction (Wu et al., 2019), which in turn weakens their happiness and self-efficacy, and ultimately impairs their ability to perceive positive emotions (Bandura, 1977).

It is also worth noting that positive and negative emotions are not entirely negatively correlated (Clark et al., 1989). We found that home confinement was not significantly associated with increased frequencies of fear, anger or any other negative emotions. Moreover, our findings implied that home confinement mainly suppressed the positive emotions of the residents, but did not explicitly induce a specific negative emotion. It seems to be different from previous reports. Considering that previous researches mainly focused on specific sample groups such as patients and medical staff, they might have already borne more pressure from pandemic (Bai et al., 2004; Wu et al., 2009; Brooks et al., 2020). However, our research was primarily targeted at the general populations, especially individuals who had no close contact with COVID-19. Therefore, the negative psychological impact of home confinement might not be so significant. Besides, in the early stages of data collection (after January 23, 2020), public negative emotions, such as anxiety, fear and angry, had occurred at a very high level (Li et al., 2020). Thus, home confinement might not able to further increase the frequency of negative emotions.

The Influence of Home Confinement on Cognition Processing

Inhibition words (e.g., lock, constrain, stop) and exclusion words (e.g., but, without, exclude) both reflect the cognitive processing characteristics of individuals (Zhao et al., 2016). Inhibition words represent the sense of being restricted and constrained (Pennebaker and Lay, 2002). There was no doubt that home confinement would have resulted in restrictions on daily activities such as shopping and outdoor sports. From the perspective of leisure constraints models (Crawford et al., 1991), these restrictions can be regarded as structural barriers. Our results indicated that home confinement also increased individuals’ sense of being constrained. As such, this could be considered as intrapersonal barriers and might further reduce the level of motivation and participation in leisure, thereby affecting their stress management abilities on both physical and psychological illness (Louise, 1995).

Exclusion words represent a measure of cognitive complexity, and the rising of it indicates the increase of cognitive complexity (Tausczik and Pennebaker, 2010). It is obvious that home confinement has introduced great changes in people’s lifestyles, and in this scenario, people need to consume more cognitive resources to deal with various restrictions, thus leading to the increase of cognitive complexity (Franconeri et al., 2013). Prior research has suggested that exclusion words are richer in genuine information than deceptive content such as rumors (Zhou et al., 2004; Chua and Banerjee, 2016). In this study, home confinement led to the increased use of exclusion words, indicating that people in this state are more seeking for genuine information.

Although the extents and type of activity restrictions between high-endemic and low-endemic areas are similar, we did not find a significant interaction for the use of inhibition words and exclusion words in low-endemic areas. This might be attributed to higher epidemic threats and stronger government publicity in high-endemic areas. To control the severe epidemic situation, local governments in high-endemic areas had made more considerable efforts to implement strict isolation procedures, which might amplify people’s sense of restrictions (Kasperson et al., 1988). What’ more, since the residents in high-endemic areas are facing serious disease threats, they are more likely to share some self-protection information (Pyzczynski et al., 2004). Abundant information and complex contents could increase cognitive complexity as well (Van Knippenberg et al., 2015). As a comparison, the frequency of exclusion words in low-endemic subgroup also showed an upward trend, but was not obvious compared to high-endemic subgroup.

General Discussion

In summary, this study examined the psychological impacts of an extreme measure—home confinement in the pandemic outbreak situation. Our results showed that home confinement generally caused a decline in the use of positive emotion words, indicating that home confinement generally can increase the frequency of negative emotions.

A decline in positive emotion could bring some negative effects such as worsening the crisis stress response during the pandemic (Fredrickson et al., 2003) and impairing the performance at the workplace in the short time (Staw et al., 1994). Less long-term positive emotions might result in poorer physical and mental health (Richman et al., 2005; Cohen and Pressman, 2006). Recent findings (Brooks et al., 2020) demonstrated that quarantine could increase a wide range of negative psychological effects. A nationwide survey during COVID-19 highlight the high risk of psychological symptoms related to quarantine including anxiety, depression, insomnia, and acute stress (Wang et al., 2021). Our results complement those studies showing that an internal reason for these negative effects might be a decline in positive emotion.

For the decline of positive emotion, we could advocate corresponding measures to restore and increase the experience of positive emotions, such as natural contacts (Johnsen, 2014), meditation (Fredrickson et al., 2017), music (Croom, 2015), prosocial behavior (Varma et al., 2020), sports (Maher et al., 2021), in-person social interactions with friends (Lades et al., 2020), expressing gratitude and visualizing best possible selves (Sheldon and Lyubomirsky, 2006). Moreover, we could shorten the home confinement duration and encourage suitable outdoor activities. These activities might help to reduce the possibility of negative psychological effects such as depression and anxiety during COVID-19 (Lades et al., 2020).

Besides, our results also showed that home confinement led to the increased use of inhibition and exclusion words. This suggests that home confinement can promote the people’s sense of being constrained and cognitive complexity. People under confinement may feel confused since isolation and inadequate guidelines (Cava et al., 2005; Pan et al., 2005; Braunack-Mayer et al., 2013; Caleo et al., 2018). In response, public health authorities could provide sufficient and clear information as much as possible. Thus, ensuring the public have a good understanding of the disease in question and the reasons for home confinement should be a priority (Brooks et al., 2020).

In addition, our results revealed that the public’s response to home confinement was different in terms of cognition in high-endemic vs. low-endemic areas. These differences should be considered accordingly when developing anti-epidemic polices. To receive the best confinement effect and minimize the cost of confinement, the local authorities should pay more attention to the restrictions in different regions.

In conclusion, this study found that home confinement led to a decline in the use of positive emotion words, and led to an increase of the use of inhibition and exclusion words in high-endemic areas. At the theoretical level, we adopted a longitudinal study to compare the psychological characteristics of a large sample before and after confinement. While other studies only obtained self-reports of participants after isolation. Longitudinal design could help eliminate irrelevant interference factors such as recall errors. At the practical level, other studies often emphasize the importance of public mental health services. Our research put more emphasis on using the public’s own positive emotions to combat potential psychological risks. Such measures could give full play to the subjective initiative of the public and the intervention cost is relatively lower.

Limitations and Future Work

There are some unavoidable limitations in our study. Firstly, there were few home confinements implemented at smaller scales, such as in a single community or even in one building. As the affected population was relatively small, the samples were excluded. Secondly, we could not obtain the health status of all subjects from the network data. However, in general, most of the active Weibo users were healthy individuals. Therefore, our conclusion was applicable to the public. Thirdly, our samples only included the active users of Weibo, and did not cover people who publish less than one post per day on average. Fourthly, the user’s registration might deviate from the user’s actual regional information, which may lead to slightly inaccurate results. Lastly, the emotions mentioned in this study were all explicit emotions. Implicit emotions were difficult to be measured directly from social media texts. Thus, more researches should be carried out in the future.

During the pandemic outbreaks, the public psychological state is an essential factor to consider, and it is important to understand the potential impact of home confinement on psychological state. This would be a starting point in exploring more profound effects, more relevant factors, such as other implicit emotions, and more precise methods that can be applied in future research. For example, we can use natural language processing (NLP) technology to analyze the implicit emotions in web texts. Most advanced methods can obtain 70∼87% accuracy (Alswaidan and Menai, 2020).

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

This research project was approved by the Institute of Psychology of the Chinese Academy of Sciences’ Ethical Committee (project number: H15009). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

NZ and TZ conceived and planned this article. PW and YW carried out the search. TZ collected and provided the data. PW analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. YW collected some information. ZL and SL drafted the discussion part of the manuscript. NZ, TZ, TL, XL, and PW reviewed and edited the writing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded by National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 31700984) and China Social Science Fund (17AZD041 and 16AZD058).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.587308/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Figure 1 | The flow chart of sample filtering and grouping process.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://ccpl.psych.ac.cn/textmind/

References

Alizadeh, M., Weber, I., Cioffi-Revilla, C., Fortunato, S., and Macy, M. (2019). Psychology and morality of political extremists: evidence from Twitter language analysis of alt-right and Antifa. EPJ Data Sci. 8:17. doi: 10.1140/epjds/s13688-019-0193-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alswaidan, N., and Menai, M. (2020). A survey of state-of-the-art approaches for emotion recognition in text. Knowl. Inf. Syst. 16, 2937–2987. doi: 10.1007/s10115-020-01449-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bai, Y., Lin, C.-C., Lin, C.-Y., Chen, J.-Y., Chue, C.-M., and Chou, P. (2004). Survey of stress reactions among health care workers involved with the SARS outbreak. Psychiatr. Serv. 55, 1055–1057. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.55.9.1055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 84:191. doi: 10.1016/0146-6402(78)90002-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bollen, J., Pepe, A., and Mao, H. (2009). “Modeling public mood and emotion: twitter sentiment and socio-economic phenomena,” in Proceedings of the 5th International AAAI Conference on Weblogs and Social Media (ICWSM 2011) Computer Science, Barcelona.

Google Scholar

Braunack-Mayer, A., Tooher, R., Collins, J. E., Street, J. M., and Marshall, H. (2013). Understanding the school community’s response to school closures during the H1N1 2009 influenza pandemic. BMC Public Health 13:344. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, S. K., Webster, R. K., Smith, L. E., Woodland, L., Wessely, S., Greenberg, N., et al. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet 395, 912–920. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caleo, G., Duncombe, J., Jephcott, F., Lokuge, K., Mills, C., Looijen, E., et al. (2018). The factors affecting household transmission dynamics and community compliance with Ebola control measures: a mixed-methods study in a rural village in Sierra Leone. BMC Public Health 18:248. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-5158-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cava, M. A., Fay, K. E., Beanlands, H. J., McCay, E. A., and Wignall, R. (2005). The experience of quarantine for individuals affected by SARS in Toronto. Public Health Nurs. 22, 398–406. doi: 10.1111/j.0737-1209.2005.220504.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chua, A. Y., and Banerjee, S. (2016). “Linguistic predictors of rumor veracity on the internet,” in Proceedings of the International MultiConference of Engineers and Computer Scientists, (Singapore: Nanyang Technological University).

Google Scholar

Clark, L. A., Watson, D., and Leeka, J. (1989). Diurnal variation in the positive affects. Motiv. Emot. 13, 205–234. doi: 10.1007/BF00995536

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., and Pressman, S. D. (2006). Positive Affect and Health. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 122–125. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00420.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coon, J. T., Boddy, K., Stein, K., Whear, R., Barton, J., and Depledge, a. M. H (2011). Does participating in physical activity in outdoor natural environments have a greater effect on physical and mental wellbeing than physical activity indoors? A systematic review. Environ. Sci. Technol. 45, 1761–1772. doi: 10.1021/es102947t

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, D. W., Jackson, E. L., and Godbey, G. (1991). A hierarchical model of leisure constraints. Leis. Sci. 13, 309–320. doi: 10.1080/01490409109513147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croom, A. M. (2015). Music practice and participation for psychological well-being: a review of how music influences positive emotion, engagement, relationships, meaning, and accomplishment. Music. Sci. 19, 44–64. doi: 10.1177/1029864914561709

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desclaux, A., Badji, D., Ndione, A. G., and Sow, K. (2017). Accepted monitoring or endured quarantine? Ebola contacts’ perceptions in Senegal. Soc. Sci. Med. 178, 38–45. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franconeri, S. L., Alvarez, G. A., and Cavanagh, P. (2013). Flexible cognitive resources: competitive content maps for attention and memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 134–141. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L., Boulton, A. J., Firestine, A. M., Van Cappellen, P., Algoe, S. B., Brantley, M. M., et al. (2017). Positive emotion correlates of meditation practice: a comparison of mindfulness meditation and loving-kindness meditation. Mindfulness 8, 1623–1633. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0735-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., and Larkin, G. R. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84:365. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, R., Hao, B., Bai, S., Li, L., Li, A., and Zhu, T. (2013). “Improving user profile with personality traits predicted from social media content,” in Proceedings of the 7th ACM Conference on Recommender Systems, Hong Kong.

Google Scholar

Golder, S. A., and Macy, M. W. (2011). Diurnal and seasonal mood vary with work, sleep, and daylength across diverse cultures. Science 333, 1878–1881. doi: 10.1126/science.1202775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gupta, A. G., Moyer, C. A., and Stern, D. T. (2005). The economic impact of quarantine: SARS in Toronto as a case study. J. Infect. 50, 386–393. doi: 10.1016/j.jinf.2004.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawryluck, L., Gold, W. L., Robinson, S., Pogorski, S., Galea, S., and Styra, R. (2004). SARS control and psychological effects of quarantine, Toronto, Canada. Emerg. Infect. Dis. 10, 1206–12012. doi: 10.3201/eid1007.030703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

House, J., Landis, K., and Umberson, D. (1988). Social relationships and health. Science 241, 540–545. doi: 10.1126/science.3399889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsieh, Y.-H., King, C.-C., Chen, C. W. S., Ho, M.-S., Lee, J.-Y., Liu, F.-C., et al. (2005). Quarantine for SARS, Taiwan. Emerg. Infect. Dis. 11, 278–282. doi: 10.3201/eid1102.040190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, C., Wang, Y., Li, X., Ren, L., Zhao, J., Hu, Y., et al. (2020). Clinical features of patients infected with 2019 novel coronavirus in Wuhan, China. Lancet 395, 497–506. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30183-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, F., Ding, H., Liu, Z., Wu, P., Li, A., and Zhu, T. (2020). How fear and collectivism influence public’s preventive intention towards COVID-19 infection: a study based on big data from the social media. BMC Public Health 20:1707. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-09674-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnsen, S. ÅK. (2014). Using the Natural Environment for Emotion Regulation Conceptual and Empirical Explorations. Ph. D. thesis. Trondheim: Norwegian University of Science and Technology.

Google Scholar

Kahn, J. H., Tobin, R. M., Massey, A. E., and Anderson, J. A. (2007). Measuring emotional expression with the linguistic inquiry and word count. Am. J. Psychol. 120, 263–286. doi: 10.2307/20445398

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasperson, R. E., Renn, O., Slovic, P., Brown, H. S., Emel, J., Goble, R., et al. (1988). The social amplification of risk: a conceptual framework. Risk Anal. 8, 177–187. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.1988.tb01168.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosinski, M., Matz, S. C., Gosling, S. D., Popov, V., and Stillwell, D. (2015). Facebook as a research tool for the social sciences: opportunities, challenges, ethical considerations, and practical guidelines. Am. Psychol. 70, 543–556. doi: 10.1037/a0039210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lades, L. K., Laffan, K., Daly, M., and Delaney, L. (2020). Daily emotional well−being during the COVID−19 pandemic. Br. J. Health Psychol. 25, 902–911. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S., Chan, L. Y., Chau, A. M., Kwok, K. P., and Kleinman, A. (2005). The experience of SARS-related stigma at amoy gardens. Soc. Sci. Med. 61, 2038–2046. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, L., Li, A., Hao, B., Guan, Z., and Zhu, T. (2014). Predicting active users’ personality based on micro-blogging behaviors. PloS One 9:e84997. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0084997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, S., Wang, Y., Xue, J., Zhao, N., and Zhu, T. (2020). The impact of COVID-19 epidemic declaration on psychological consequences: a study on active Weibo users. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:2032. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17062032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, X., Kakade, M., Fuller, C. J., Fan, B., Fang, Y., Kong, J., et al. (2012). Depression after exposure to stressful events: lessons learned from the severe acute respiratory syndrome epidemic. Compr. Psychiatry 53, 15–23. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lydersen, S. (2018). Balanced or imbalanced samples? Tidsskr. Norske Laegeforen. Tidsskr. Prakt. Med. Ny Raekke 138:539. doi: 10.4045/tidsskr.18.0539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Louise, M. (1995). Main and stress-moderating health benefits of leisure. Loisir Et Societe 18, 33–51. doi: 10.1080/07053436.1995.10715489

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maher, J. P., Hevel, D. J., Reifsteck, E. J., and Drollette, E. S. (2021). Physical activity is positively associated with college students’ positive affect regardless of stressful life events during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 52:101826. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, D. R., and Schaller, M. (2012). Threat (s) and conformity deconstructed: perceived threat of infectious disease and its implications for conformist attitudes and behavior. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 42, 180–188. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.863

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

NewYorkTimes (2020). To Tame Coronavirus, Mao-Style Social Control Blankets China. Available online at: https://www.nytimes.com/2020/02/15/business/china-coronavirus-lockdown.html?smid=nytcore-ios-share (accessed March 10, 2021).

Google Scholar

Niu, G. P. M., Wei, O., and Cai, X. (2014). Emotion analysis of chinese microblogs using lexicon-based approach. Comput. Sci. 41, 253–258,289.

Google Scholar

Ooi, P. L., Lim, S., and Chew, S. K. (2005). Use of quarantine in the control of SARS in Singapore. Am. J. Infect. Control. 33, 252–257. doi: 10.1016/j.ajic.2004.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, P. J., Chang, S.-H., and Yu, Y.-Y. (2005). A support group for home-quarantined college students exposed to SARS: learning from practice. J. Spec. Group Work 30, 363–374. doi: 10.1080/01933920500186951

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennebaker, J. W., and Lay, T. C. (2002). Language use and personality during crises: analyses of mayor Rudolph Giuliani’s press conferences. J. Res. Pers. 36, 271–282. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.2002.2349

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, C., and Seligman, M. E. (2003). Character strengths before and after September 11. Psychol. Sci. 14, 381–384. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.24482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pyzczynski, T., Greenberg, J., and Koole, S. L. (2004). “Experimental existential psychology: exploring the human confrontation with reality,” in Handbook of Experimental Existential Psychology. eds J. Greenberg, S. L. Koole, and T. Pyszczynski. (London: The Guilford Press), 3–10.

Google Scholar

Reynolds, D. L., Garay, J., Deamond, S., Moran, M. K., Gold, W., and Styra, R. (2008). Understanding, compliance and psychological impact of the SARS quarantine experience. Epidemiol. Infect. 136, 997–1007. doi: 10.1017/S0950268807009156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richman, L. S., Kubzansky, L., Maselko, J., Kawachi, I., Choo, P., and Bauer, M. (2005). Positive emotion and health: going beyond the negative. Health Psychol. 24, 422. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.24.4.422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheldon, K. M., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2006). How to increase and sustain positive emotion: the effects of expressing gratitude and visualizing best possible selves. J. Posit. Psychol. 1, 73–82. doi: 10.1080/17439760500510676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sprang, G., and Silman, M. (2013). Posttraumatic stress disorder in parents and youth after health-related disasters. Disaster Med. Public Health Prep. 7, 105–110. doi: 10.1017/dmp.2013.22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staw, B. M., Sutton, R. I., and Pelled, L. H. (1994). Employee positive emotion and favorable outcomes at the workplace. Org. Sci. 5, 51–71. doi: 10.2307/2635070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Su, Y., Xue, J., Liu, X., Wu, P., Chen, J., Chen, C., et al. (2020). Examining the impact of COVID-19 lockdown in Wuhan and Lombardy: a psycholinguistic analysis on Weibo and Twitter. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:4552. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17124552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tausczik, Y. R., and Pennebaker, J. W. (2010). The psychological meaning of words: LIWC and computerized text analysis methods. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 29, 24–54. doi: 10.1177/0261927x09351676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Knippenberg, D., Dahlander, L., Haas, M. R., and George, G. (2015). Information, Attention, and Decision Making. Briarcliff Manor, NY: Academy of Management.

Google Scholar

Varma, M. M., Chen, D., Lin, X., Aknin, L., and Hu, X. (2020). Prosocial behavior promotes positive emotion during the COVID-19 pandemic. PsyArxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/vdw2e

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Shi, L., Que, J., Lu, Q., Liu, L., Lu, Z., et al. (2021). The impact of quarantine on mental health status among general population in China during the COVID-19 pandemic. Mol. Psychiatry 1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01019-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, A. F.-W., Catmur, C., Wong, P. W., and Lau, J. Y. (2019). The presence, characteristics and correlates of pathological social withdrawal in Taiwan: an online survey. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 66, 84–92. doi: 10.1177/0020764019882724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, P., Fang, Y., Guan, Z., Fan, B., Kong, J., Yao, Z., et al. (2009). The psychological impact of the SARS epidemic on hospital employees in China: exposure, risk perception, and altruistic acceptance of risk. Can. J. Psychiatr. 54, 302–311. doi: 10.1177/070674370905400504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, N., Jiao, D., Bai, S., and Zhu, T. (2016). Evaluating the validity of simplified Chinese version of LIWC in detecting psychological expressions in short texts on social network services. PloS One 11:e0157947. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, L., Burgoon, J. K., Nunamaker, J. F., and Twitchell, D. (2004). Automating linguistics-based cues for detecting deception in text-based asynchronous computer-mediated communications. Group Decis. Negot. 13, 81–106. doi: 10.1023/B:GRUP.0000011944.62889.6f

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: home confinement, mental health, COVID-19, psycho-linguistic analysis, LIWC, activity restriction

Citation: Wu P, Zhao N, Li S, Liu Z, Wang Y, Liu T, Liu X and Zhu T (2021) Exploring the Psychological Effects of COVID-19 Home Confinement in China: A Psycho-Linguistic Analysis on Weibo Data Pool. Front. Psychol. 12:587308. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.587308

Received: 25 July 2020; Accepted: 10 May 2021;
Published: 03 June 2021.

Edited by:

Changiz Mohiyeddini, Oakland University William Beaumont School of Medicine, United States

Reviewed by:

Catarina Tomé Pires, Autonomous University of Lisbon, Portugal
Ang Li, Beijing Forestry University, China
Rachele Mariani, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Wu, Zhao, Li, Liu, Wang, Liu, Liu and Zhu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xiaoqian Liu, liuxiaoqian@psych.ac.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.