Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 09 March 2021
Sec. Emotion Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Control of Emotions in Challenging Contexts View all 20 articles

Cultivating Affective Resilience: Proof-of-Principle Evidence of Translational Benefits From a Novel Cognitive-Emotional Training Intervention

Sanda Dolcos,
Sanda Dolcos1,2*Yifan Hu,
Yifan Hu1,2*Christian WilliamsChristian Williams1Paul C. Bogdan,Paul C. Bogdan1,2Kelly Hohl,Kelly Hohl1,2Howard BerenbaumHoward Berenbaum1Florin Dolcos,,Florin Dolcos1,2,3
  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, Champaign, IL, United States
  • 2Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, Champaign, IL, United States
  • 3Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, Champaign, IL, United States

Available evidence highlights the importance of emotion regulation (ER) in psychological well-being. However, translation of the beneficial effects of ER from laboratory to real-life remains scarce. Here, we present proof-of-principle evidence from a novel cognitive-emotional training intervention targeting the development of ER skills aimed at increasing resilience against emotional distress. This pilot intervention involved training military veterans over 5–8 weeks in applying two effective ER strategies [Focused Attention (FA) and Cognitive Reappraisal (CR)] to scenarios presenting emotional conflicts (constructed with both external and internal cues). Training was preceded and followed by neuropsychological, personality, and clinical assessments, and resting-state functional MRI data were also collected from a subsample of the participants. Results show enhanced executive function and psychological well-being following training, reflected in increased working memory (WM), post-traumatic growth (PTG), and general self-efficacy (GSE). Brain imaging results showed evidence of diminished bottom-up influences from emotional and perceptual brain regions, along with evidence of normalized functional connectivity in the large-scale functional networks following training. The latter was reflected in increased connectivity among cognitive and emotion control regions and across regions of self-referential and control networks. Overall, our results provide proof-of-concept evidence that resilience and well-being can be learned through ER training, and that training-related improvements manifested in both behavioral change and neuroplasticity can translate into real-life benefits.

Introduction

The increasing prevalence of emotional disturbances, such as anxiety and depression, in the adult population is a major public health concern. It is estimated that by 2030 anxiety and depression will be among the most prevalent causes of disability worldwide. Because of their high prevalence (with over 40 million Americans having anxiety disorders alone), these emotional disturbances are associated with overwhelming long-term costs, which may affect the overall quality of life even decades later (Howard et al., 2014). Complicating the issue, these mental health problems often go untreated because the dominant models of delivering mental health treatment in clinical settings prevent many individuals from accessing such services (Kazdin, 2019). Moreover, deficient emotion regulation (ER), which is one of the core problems in emotional disorders, may persist even after treatment and remission, hence increasing the likelihood of relapse and the persistence of emotional distress (Kanske et al., 2012). Psychological resilience, which involves adaptive emotional responses in the face of adversity, can provide a protective buffer against the harmful effects of stressful events. Resilience is influenced by a combination of internal personal attributes and external factors (family and social networks support). Among the most important personal attributes associated with resilience is the ability to self-regulate emotional responses and adaptively engage cognitive/executive control. Here, we provide proof-of-concept evidence regarding the effectiveness of a novel training alternative that captures the complexity of ER in everyday life. Our training program is grounded in the emotion regulation choice framework (Sheppes et al., 2014) and is based on a multidimensional approach involving behavioral, personality, clinical, and brain imaging assessments.

Self-regulation influences the ability to control thoughts, emotions, and actions to achieve a desired outcome, and encompasses cognitive, emotional, neural, and behavioral levels (Mischel et al., 1989; Blair and Diamond, 2008). Deficient regulation of emotion (or emotion dysregulation) is one of the core problems in emotional disturbances, such as anxiety and depression, and may persist after therapeutic treatment and remission (Kanske et al., 2012). Self-regulation capacities indexing differences in vulnerability to anxiety and stress are related to both psychological and neural factors, highlighting the need to develop self-regulatory solutions based on an integrative understanding of psychological factors and their interactions, along with an understanding of the associated neural mechanisms. Psychological factors include both cognitive control [working memory (WM), inhibitory control] and ER skills (Naragon-Gainey et al., 2017), as also demonstrated by our own work (Hu and Dolcos, 2017; Moore et al., 2018). Neural factors include network-level (Power et al., 2011) targets for cognitive control (“cognitive control networks”), involving dorsolateral prefrontal cortical (dlPFC) and lateral parietal (LP) regions, and emotion/ER networks (“salience/survival network”), involving basic emotion processing regions, such as the amygdala (AMY), and regions of emotion integration and regulation, such as the ventrolateral PFC (vlPFC), medial PFC (mPFC), and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; Hopfinger et al., 2000; Seeley et al., 2007; Dosenbach et al., 2008). The role of these networks in self-regulation has been identified based on both functional and structural neuroimaging studies. For instance, we have recently shown increased activity in the cognitive control network and reduced activity in regions of the salience network, when engaging specific ER strategies (Denkova et al., 2015; Iordan et al., 2019; Dolcos et al., 2020a). Also, increased gray matter volume in cognitive control regions provides protection against symptoms of anxiety (Dolcos et al., 2016; Hu and Dolcos, 2017; Moore et al., 2018).

Important to consider in this context are possible links between psychological and neural aspects in self-regulation, which highlight the importance of flexible adaptive behavior (Naragon-Gainey et al., 2017). For instance, increased focus on distressing thoughts and memories observed in affective disorders may be linked to an inability to flexibly switch attentional focus between internal and external environments, which may be the main cause for getting “stuck in the rut” (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991; Cooney et al., 2010; Holtzheimer and Mayberg, 2011). This is consistent with evidence pointing to alterations in the default or resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) of the large-scale brain networks linked to cognitive/executive and emotional dysfunctions, with increased coupling among regions on the default mode network (DMN) and functional decoupling of DMN from regions of the frontoparietal control network (FPCN; Hopfinger et al., 2000; Raichle et al., 2001; Fox et al., 2005; Dosenbach et al., 2008; Kaiser et al., 2015).

Recent cognitive neuroscience research offers some initial promising evidence that ER may be enhanced through cognitive training (Denny and Ochsner, 2014), although such evidence is limited. Cognitive training and interventions that train specific psychological abilities, such as mindfulness-based attention and multitasking performance, can induce changes in brain structure and function (Denny and Ochsner, 2014; Verghese et al., 2016; Valk et al., 2017; Cohen and Ochsner, 2018; Dolcos et al., 2020c). Evidence from these interventions complements clinical studies showing volume reductions in specific brain regions following traumatic life events (Sekiguchi et al., 2015), and provide evidence for possible enhancements induced by customized cognitive training. However, most of the work on ER training is limited because it has focused on practicing one specific strategy (Sekiguchi et al., 2015; Cohen and Ochsner, 2018; Dolcos et al., 2020c). In our view, such a “one size fits all” approach minimizes the complexity of ER, which inherently involves interactions of person, situation, and ER strategy factors (Aldao et al., 2015).

Our view is consistent with the emotion regulation choice framework, which proposes that ER involves two major cognitive stages, an attentional selection stage and a semantic meaning stage (Sheppes et al., 2014). The initial attentional stage consists of an early disengagement from emotional information before it undergoes elaborated processing. This is typically achieved through attentional deployment strategies, such as focused attention (FA), which involve disengaging attention from the emotional information before it is represented in working memory by focusing on neutral thoughts or details (Van Dillen and Koole, 2007). The subsequent semantic meaning stage involves later engagement with the emotional information that passes the early attentional selection stage (Sheppes et al., 2014). The most common semantic strategy is cognitive reappraisal (CR), which involves engaging with and modifying the meaning of emotional information through semantic processing (e.g., Gross, 2002). These early vs. later strategies have differential benefits. Specifically, blocking emotional information early, through attentional deployment, before it gathers force, allows modulation of high-intensity emotional information, by engaging relatively simple cognitive processes, whereas the elaborated semantic processing that occurs during CR allows processing, evaluating, and remembering emotional information, which are crucial for long-term goals and for adaptation (Sheppes et al., 2014). Psychological resilience and well-being require flexible adaptation of ER strategies to fit differing situational demands (Gross, 2007; Kashdan and Rottenberg, 2010; Watkins, 2011). Therefore, it is important to examine interactive effects of regulation strategies that are most beneficial for a given person in a given situation.

The following additional concepts are also central to consider in this context: self-efficacy (SE), cognitive flexibility (CF), and working memory (WM). The concept of SE, originating in the Social Cognitive Theory (SCT; Bandura, 1986), refers to individuals’ beliefs about their own competence to exert control over events that matter (Bandura, 1997). Such beliefs influence cognitive, affective, motivational, and decisional processes that support individuals in achieving their goals and are crucial determinants of initiating and maintaining changes in behavior (Bandura, 1986). Individuals with high SE engage in more effortful, persistent, and resilient coping efforts, which may enable them to identify important opportunities within stressful circumstances and promote individual growth (Bandura, 1997, 2001). Importantly, recent evidence identifying SE as a mechanism influencing the effects of stress on mental health, along with suggestions regarding the malleability and transfer of SE beliefs across functional domains, point to the importance of considering SE as a key concept in interventions aimed at improving mental health (Bandura, 1986; Maciejewski et al., 2000; Zinken et al., 2008; Schönfeld et al., 2019).

Regarding CF, given the complexity and shifting nature of contextual demands in everyday life, individuals may need to flexibly engage multiple regulation strategies, both within and across emotional episodes (ER flexibility; Kashdan and Rottenberg, 2010; Aldao et al., 2015; Ford et al., 2019). A rich repertoire of ER strategies, along with their flexible implementation linked to current contextual demands, have been associated with enhanced adaptation and better coping (Cheng, 2001; Bonanno et al., 2004; Kashdan and Rottenberg, 2010; Bonanno and Burton, 2013; Aldao et al., 2015; Koole et al., 2015; Levy-Gigi et al., 2016). Inflexibility, on the other hand, characterized by rigid attempts to control psychological reactions to discomfort, has been associated with increased distress and detrimental effects on self-efficacy (Gamez et al., 2014; Levin et al., 2014; Jeffords et al., 2018; Tavakoli et al., 2019).

Flexible engagement of ER strategies is also an important predictor of post-traumatic growth (PTG). PTG theory suggests that growth occurs by developing regulation strategies that encourage constructive thinking and allow individuals to engage with trauma-related emotions and memories (Tedeschi and Calhoun, 2004). Reappraisal, a strategy focused on engaging with and changing the meaning of the emotional content, has been consistently associated with PTG, but recent ER research supports a more nuanced, context-dependent view. For instance, reappraisal seems to be preferred when dealing with low-intensity traumatic events, and attention focus/distraction is preferred for coping with high-intensity distressing emotions (Sheppes et al., 2014; Orejuela-Dávila et al., 2019). Moreover, the use of reappraisal or distraction at different phases in the trauma recovery may have different effects, reflected in increased PTG or reduced post-traumatic stress (Levy-Gigi et al., 2016).

Finally, WM also plays a critical role in the ability to successfully engage ER and reduce symptoms of distress (Schmeichel and Demaree, 2010). Defined as the capacity-limited resource that temporally maintains and manipulates information in the service of higher functions, WM is an executive function that plays a key role in the regulation of cognitive and emotional processes at both early and later stages of processing (Baddeley, 2003). Evidence linking WM to the early stage control of visual attention shows that WM capacity contributed to the ability to focus one’s eyes away from a salient visual stimulus, and predicted the ability to ignore the “unattended” message in a dichotic-listening task (Conway et al., 2001; Kane et al., 2001). Moreover, WM capacity has been linked to the ability to engage later-stage ER strategies, such as reappraisal and suppression (Schmeichel et al., 2008; McRae et al., 2012). Finally, increased WM capacity is essential in reducing the detrimental impact of apprehensive thoughts, which tend to be “permanent residents” in the WM of anxious individuals (Eysenck et al., 2007).

The main goal of the present study was to investigate the effectiveness of a comprehensive cognitive-emotional training program aimed at developing healthy and flexible ER skills to increase resilience and well-being and improve executive function. This training program builds upon evidence, including from our own research, regarding cognitive and emotional control and their link to the associated neural mechanisms, and capitalizes on evidence regarding the effectiveness of two ER strategies in reducing emotional distress: focused attention (FA) and cognitive reappraisal (CR; Ochsner and Gross, 2005). This combination of strategies is based on evidence regarding their effectiveness in both healthy functioning and in clinical conditions, which allows for optimal adaptive responses when facing real-life emotional challenges (Kanske et al., 2012; Denkova et al., 2015; Iordan et al., 2019; Dolcos et al., 2020a,b). For instance, FA can be quickly deployed when individuals may unexpectedly encounter highly emotional stimuli (earlier stages of emotion processing) and CR can draw on a combination of cognitive control processes (later stages of emotion processing) to change one’s emotional appraisals and responses (Gross, 1998). Also, both strategies can be applied to external (percepts) and to internal (memories, thoughts) stimuli, as well as to developing problem-solving skills. Hence, these two aspects (i.e., external vs. internal) were both trained using a picture processing task and a writing task, respectively. Of particular relevance is also evidence regarding their effectiveness in influencing emotional memories, which is a topic less explored in the ER literature. For instance, we recently demonstrated the effectiveness of FA during both encoding of memories for emotional pictures and retrieval of emotional autobiographical memories (Denkova et al., 2015; Iordan et al., 2019; Dolcos et al., 2020a).

The effectiveness of the training was assessed using neuropsychological, personality, and clinical assessments, along with measures of rsFC, before and after the training. We tested the following hypotheses regarding the neurobehavioral effects of the present ER intervention: (1) behavioral improvements following training would be reflected in measures of both cognitive/executive and affective domains, and possibly in measures indexing more general abilities, such as self-efficacy, and positive psychological growth; (2) regarding brain imaging, we expected normalization of the rsFC in the large-scale functional networks following training, possibly reflected in decreased connectivity among DMN regions and increased connectivity between DMN and FPCN regions. We also explored evidence for diminished bottom-up influences, possibly linked to decreased rsFC of perceptual regions and AMY, as well as possible changes in the connectivity between basic emotion processing regions (AMY) and emotion control (PFC) regions.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Nineteen military veterans (with a deployment assignment to Iraq and/or Afghanistan in the preceding 5 years) enrolled in post-secondary education at the time of the study (mean age = 30.9; SD = 7.9; 95% males, 79% White/European American, 11% Black/African American, 5% Asian/Asian-American, and 5% Other) were recruited to participate in the present pilot intervention. Participants were assigned to one of two training programs: (1) a cognitive-emotional regulation training (CERT) program (N = 9); or (2) a psychosocial training (PSYCT) program (N = 10), ensuring equal proportions of participants with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) – i.e., scores 33 or above on the PTSD Checklist – PCL-5 in each condition (Weathers et al., 2013). Branch of service was also considered when assigning participants to the two programs. All participants provided written informed consent under a protocol approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.

General Procedures

The intervention took place over 5–8 weeks, and participants in both conditions received equal amount of training/therapy per week (90 min), over two 45-min sessions for the CERT intervention and all in one session for the PSYCT intervention. The CERT participants had individual sessions led by an advanced doctoral student in cognitive neuroscience, where they learned how to apply the two ER strategies (i.e., FA and CR) to hypothetical scenarios (see Figure 1 below) in computerized tasks, and the PSYCT participants had group sessions with an advanced doctoral candidate in clinical psychology. All participants completed a set of self-report measures and a battery of cognitive tasks before and after the training. Ten participants (five from each group) also received structural and resting-state functional brain scans before and after the intervention.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of the Intervention Approach. Big-picture details (left-side panel) and specific details regarding the implementation of ER training in the CERT group (right-side panel).

Training/Intervention Procedures

CERT Group

Participants in this group were individually guided by the researcher to perform computer-based tasks that simulated situations that required ER (Figure 1). Specifically, two types of situations were involved: external situations, where emotional challenges would arise from negative events in the surrounding environment, and internal situations, where emotional challenges would come from intruding negative autobiographical memories. The external ER was simulated using a picture-viewing task, where participants were instructed to apply ER strategies while viewing emotionally disturbing images. The internal ER was simulated using a timed writing task, where they were instructed to write about pre-identified emotional episodes from their autobiographical memories, in ways consistent with ER strategies. The autobiographical memories were identified using established procedures that have been effectively employed in our previous research to collect emotional autobiographical memories (Denkova et al., 2011, 2012, 2013a,b, 2015; Iordan et al., 2019). In the present study, participants were prompted with cues of a wide range of life events and were instructed to select and provide short descriptions of 12 most negative and 12 most positive events that reminded them of specific, unique, and personal events that they would like to work on during the training. Training primarily focused on the identified negative events. Due to the potential sensitive nature of past experiences of our subject population, a highly personalized approach was adopted, where participants were encouraged to identify six unpleasant episodes that they were comfortable working with on a regular basis, throughout the duration of the training. Because of the highly individualized nature of these memories, participants varied in the way they rated the emotional intensity associated with their own unpleasant memories, but overall they rated them as intense (mean intensity/arousal = 5.57; SD = 1.35) on a seven-point Likert scale (1 = “Not at all,” 7 = “Very much”). In each session, participants were guided to confront the presented emotional challenges, and practiced the application of both FA and CR to reduce the emotional impact created by the tasks.

In order to facilitate mastery over the strategies and encourage transfer effects to occur, participants were trained to switch between FA and CR, between the two types of tasks (involving internal thoughts and memories and external stimuli) and three temporal dimensions (present, past memories, and future worries). Several gradients were built into the session schedules. The training started out by familiarizing participants with one strategy at a time in a session, focusing on guiding participants to apply FA or CR to one internal and one external event selected by the participant. As the training advanced, each session posed increasingly complex situational demands, by guiding participants to first apply the same strategy to different events and eventually deploy different strategies on different events within a session, thereby achieving an increasing level of flexibility. Flexibility training culminated with sessions that engaged both strategies to work with worries for expected future events.

PSYCT Group

This intervention was developed by two of the authors (CW and HB), who are clinical psychologists and one of them (CW) is a military veteran. The Control intervention focused on the provision of skills (e.g., goal setting, problem solving) that were considered likely to be useful to military student veterans. This intervention emphasized skills that student veterans had likely already acquired in the military (e.g., time management) and explored how the skillset could be translated to an academic setting. Skills were presented and practiced in a group format, with participants given assignments to practice the skills between sessions. The intervention included elements of several evidence-based psychological treatments that were considered likely to be useful to military student veterans. Specifically, the intervention included elements of problem-solving therapy (Nezu, 2004), acceptance and commitment therapy (Hayes et al., 2006), cognitive therapy for depression (Beck, 1979), social skills training (Bellack et al., 1981), and behavioral activation treatment (Cuijpers et al., 2007). The treatment was provided in a group format (with group sizes ranging from 4 to 8), once per week for 90 min. Each week focused on a different aspect of psychoeducation and/or skill development (e.g., identifying values and cognitive distortions, clarifying goals, expressing anger, and recognizing thoughts, sensations, and behaviors associated with emotions).

Symptom, Personality, and Neuropsychological Assessments

Posttraumatic Stress Disorder status was assessed using the PCL-5 (Weathers et al., 2013), which consists of 20 items that present challenges associated with a stressful experience (e.g., “Repeated, disturbing dreams of the stressful experience?”). Participants are instructed to report how much they are bothered by these challenges using a five-point scale (0 = “Not at all”; 4 = “Extremely”). An overall PCL-5 score is computed by summing the item ratings.

Anhedonic Depression was assessed using an abbreviated version of the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire – Anhedonic Depression Scale (MASQ-AD; Bredemeier et al., 2010), which contains eight items asking participants how often they feel various positive and negative experiences (e.g., “Felt really bored”; “Felt unattractive”). Participants responded using a five-point scale, corresponding to how frequently they have experienced a variety of different symptoms during the past week (1 = “Not at all”; 5 = “Extremely”). Item ratings were summed to calculate the final MASQ-AD score. Some versions of this questionnaire ask participants how often they experience thoughts of suicide, but this question was omitted from our presentation due to IRB restrictions.

Trait Worry was assessed using an abbreviated version of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer et al., 1990; Kertz et al., 2014), which consists of eight items, each presenting a statement concerning how often participants worry (e.g., “I do not tend to worry about things”). To each item, participants responded using a five-point scale, describing how applicable the statement was to them (1 = “Not at all typical of me”; 5 = “Very typical of me”). Item ratings were summed to calculate the final PSWQ scores.

Trait Affect was assessed using the Positive and Negative Affective Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988), which includes a list of 20 adjective descriptors of 10 positive (e.g., “interested,” “enthusiastic”) and 10 negative (e.g., “irritable,” “upset”) affects. Items were rated on a five-point scale (1 = “Very slightly or not at all”; 5 = “Extremely”) according to the extent to which “[the person] feels this way over a longer period of time.” This version of the PANAS was supplemented by adding three positive affect words (“pleased,” “cheerful,” and “happy”) and five negative affect words (“frustrated,” “down,” “anxious,” “grouchy,” and “sad”), for a total of 28 items (Boden et al., 2012). Positive and negative affect scores were summed for affect totals.

Post-Traumatic Growth was assessed using the PTG Inventory (PTGI; Tedeschi and Calhoun, 1996), which consists of 21 items that assess changes in peoples’ lives following a crisis or disaster. Each item presents participants a statement describing a potentially positive change in one’s mental state linked to the crisis (e.g., “I can better appreciate each day”; “I changed my priorities about what is important in life”). Participants rated the extent that they believed the statement applied to them, using a six-point scale (0 = “I did not experience this change as a result of my crisis”; 5 = “I experienced this change to a very great degree as a result of my crisis”). An overall PTGI score was created by summing the item ratings.

General Self-Efficacy was assessed using the General Self-Efficacy scale (GSE; Schwarzer and Jerusalem, 1995), which consists of 10 items assessing participants’ optimism and self-belief concerning difficult tasks and overcoming adversity. Participants were presented statements addressing how they tend to solve problems (e.g., “When I am confronted with a problem, I can usually find several solutions”) and were instructed to respond using a four-point scale corresponding to how much they believe the statement applies to them (1 = “Not at all true”; 4 = “Exactly true”). An overall GSE score was tallied by summing the values for each item.

Emotional Approach Coping was assessed using the Emotional Approach Coping scale (EAC; Stanton et al., 1994), which is an eight-item questionnaire designed to measure emotional coping. It contains two subscales, one on emotional processing (EP), addressing the extent that participants process their emotions in a healthy manner (e.g., “I acknowledge my emotions”), and a second subscale on emotional expression (EE), addressing whether participants are comfortable expressing their emotions (e.g., “I let my feelings come out freely”). For questions on each subscale, participants indicate how often they engage their emotions in these manners (1 = “I usually do not do this at all”; 4 = “I usually do this a lot”). Final EP and EE scores are calculated by summing the item ratings associated with each subscale. A total EAC score, reflecting the ability to acknowledge, understand, and express emotions, is also calculated, with lower scores representing poorer emotional coping.

Working memory was assessed using two versions of the Two-Back Working Memory task, one focusing on the spatial locations of the letter stimuli (Two-Back Space) and the other focusing on the identity of the letter stimuli (Two-Back Letter; Bredemeier and Berenbaum, 2013). For each version, participants were presented with a series of letter stimuli appearing at different spatial locations on the computer screen, one at a time, and responded via keyboard. Participants were instructed to maintain the two prior stimuli in their working memory. For the spatial version of the task, participants indicated whether the current stimulus was in the same or different location as the one presented two trials ago. For the letter version, participants responded to whether the current stimulus was the same as or different from the one presented two trials ago. Each stimulus was presented for 500 ms (with a 2,000 ms intertrial interval), and each participant completed five blocks of 20 stimuli for both space and letter tasks. Working memory performance was assessed as the percent accuracy and the average response time (RT) to correct trials. The first two trials were excluded from analyses, as they lack prior stimuli for comparison.

Attention and Executive Functions were measured using the Stroop test (Stroop, 1935). In this task, participants named the color of words while ignoring the content of the words (i.e., color words). The words used were all color words (e.g., red, blue, yellow, and green). The test takes advantage of individuals’ abilities to read words more quickly and automatically than naming colors.

Brain Imaging Data Acquisition

MRI scanning was conducted on a 3T Siemens PRISMA scanner. Sagittal localizer and 3-D MPRAGE anatomical images were first obtained (TR = 2,300 ms; TE = 2.32 ms; FOV = 230 × 230 mm; volume size = 192 slices; voxel size = 0.9 × 0.9 × 0.9 mm3). Functional images consisted of a series of axial images acquired using an echoplanar sequence (TR = 2,000 ms; TE = 25 ms; FOV = 230 × 230 mm; volume size = 38 slices; voxel size = 2.5 × 2.5 × 3 mm3, number of volumes = 298), while participants stayed eyes-open.

Data Analyses

Behavioral Data Analyses

The effects of interventions on cognitive and affective well-being were assessed by measuring participants’ pre- vs. post-intervention scores on the neuropsychological and questionnaire assessments. For each assessment, the simple main effects of the CERT and PSYCT training were first assessed by submitting participants’ scores to paired t-tests, done separately for each training group. Next, to compare the pre vs. post differences between the two groups, two-way mixed-ANOVAs were performed, with Time (Pre vs. Post) as the within-subject factor, and Group (CERT vs. PSYCT) as the across-subject factor. All the behavioral data were analyzed using the SPSS software (IBM Corp. 2017. version 25.0).

Brain Imaging Data Analyses

Preprocessing

Functional images collected at both timepoints (i.e., pre- and post-intervention) were despiked using 3dDespike in AFNI (Cox, 1996), before being submitted to preprocessing in SPM12,1 where they first underwent slice timing and two-pass realignment. The resulting motion parameters were later used to calculate scan-to-scan head displacement used for denoising. Co-registration was done in two steps: first between functional and anatomical images within each session, then between all images collected pre- and post- intervention. To take advantage of the repeated scans, the Longitudinal Registration toolbox (Ashburner and Ridgway, 2013) in SPM12 was used to first create mid-point average anatomical images for each participant, which were then segmented and submitted to groupwise Dartel template creation (Ashburner, 2007). Using this Dartel template, functional images were resampled to the MNI space on an isotropic 3 mm grid combining transformations estimated at previous steps in a single interpolation. Lastly, images were smoothed with a 6 mm FWHM Gaussian filter. Notably, smoothed data were used to define voxel time series at each voxel, whereas unsmoothed data were used to generate tissue type regressors used during denoising as well as to define seed timeseries for the chosen Regions of Interest (ROIs). After preprocessing, images were processed through denoising steps to further remove noises and artifacts, including (1) demeaning and detrending across each session, (2) nuisance regressions, which used a combination of motion regressors (six realignment parameters and their first-order derivatives), aCompCor (Behzadi et al., 2007) regressors (signals from top five principal components generated from each of the tissue maps of white matter and cortico-spinal fluids, along with their first-order derivatives), and main condition effects (computed by convolving images in a session with a canonical hemodynamic response function to further remove simple session-related co-activation confounds), (3) a simultaneous (Hallquist et al., 2013) bandpass filter of (0.008, 0.09) Hz, and (4) scrubbing based on framewise displacement (Power et al., 2011) with a threshold of 0.5 mm. Denoising steps were implemented in the CONN toolbox (v18b; Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012). Denoised images were visually inspected to ensure the effectiveness of the procedures by observing the normality of the functional connectivity distributions, the relative independence between functional connectivity values and nodal distances, and that there were no substantial differences between the two experimental groups.

Functional Connectivity Analysis

A seed-based approach was implemented to examine the effects of interventions on the rsFC, based on clear a priory interest in regions involved in bottom-up emotion processing (i.e., amygdala, AMY) and top-down cognitive/emotional control processing, as follows: dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC), ventrolateral PFC (vlPFC), and medial PFC (mPFC; Moore et al., 2018). Two complementary atlases were used to define the masks of the seeds: the anatomically-based FSL Harvard-Oxford atlas (Frazier et al., 2005; Desikan et al., 2006; Makris et al., 2006; Goldstein et al., 2007) was used to define the seed mask for the subcortical structure, AMY, and the CONN network atlas (derived from ICA analyses of 497 subjects in a dataset from the Human Connectome Project) was used to define the seed masks for the cortical regions, with the lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) node in the Frontoparietal network roughly corresponding to the dlPFC, the inferior frontal gyrus node in the Language network roughly corresponding to the vlPFC, and the Default Mode network node in the medial PFC, located anterior and ventral to the rostral ACC, roughly corresponding to the mPFC.

Seed timeseries were extracted from the unsmoothed data aggregated across all voxels within each seed ROI, and voxel timeseries were extracted from the smoothed data at each voxel in the rest of the brain. Functional connectivity values were calculated as the Pearson correlation coefficients between the selected ROI seeds and voxels in the rest of the brain, which were then Fisher-transformed into z-scores to allow subsequent statistical testing. At the second level, analyses focused on investigating Group × Time interaction effects, where differential functional connectivity changes were identified in the two groups as a result of the intervention, as well as a main effect of Time, where functional connectivity changes were identified across both groups. To this end, a general linear model was constructed, with Time being the within-subject factor and Group being the between-subject factor, each with two levels. Interaction effects were tested using the contrast [CERT(Post-Pre) − PSYCT(Post-Pre)], and the main effects contrast [CERT(Post-Pre) + PSYCT(Post-Pre)]. Unless otherwise specified, statistical significance was set at a voxel-level threshold of p < 0.005 (uncorrected) combined with a cluster-level threshold of p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected). These analyses were performed in the CONN toolbox (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012).

Results

Evidence of Enhanced Well-Being and Executive Function Following ER Training

Table 1 summarizes the means and SDs of behavioral measures. As hypothesized, we identified behavioral patterns of increased abilities associated with the CERT training within domains relevant to adaptive cognitive and emotional processing. Specifically, the CERT group showed significant pre vs. post increases in GSE [t(8) = 2.910, p = 0.020, d = 0.970; Figure 2]. Additionally, participation in the CERT group promoted positive growth-focused mindsets with regard to participants’ traumatic events [PTGI: t(8) = 1.994, p = 0.041, one-tailed, d = 0.665]. While both changes support the efficacy of CERT training, only the benefits in GSE were found to be specific to the CERT group. This was demonstrated by a two-way mixed-design ANOVA examining the effect of Time (Pre vs. Post) and Training Group (CERT vs. PSYCT), which revealed a significant Time × Training Group interaction effect [F(1,17) = 7.196, p = 0.016, ηp2 = 0.297], further supporting the effectiveness of our training in increasing self-efficacy and positive psychological change.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics linked to each intervention.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Increased General Self-Efficacy (GSE) following Cognitive – Emotional Regulation Training (CERT). Paired t-tests revealed a significant pre- to post-training increase in the GSE score that was specific to the CERT group (N = 9). This was confirmed by a two-way ANOVA Time (Pre vs. Post) × Group (PSYCT vs. CERT) interaction (N = 19). *significant at p < 0.05 (two-tailed); ns, non-significant.

Second, also as hypothesized, participants in the CERT group also showed post-training improvements in executive function (Figure 3), and these increases were observed in both Two-Back Letter and Space WM tasks. These findings were confirmed by paired samples t-tests for both the Two-Back Letter and Two-Back Space tasks, which both showed a decrease in reaction time in the CERT group [Letter: t(8) = −3.172, p = 0.013, d = −1.06; Space: t(8) = −2.535, p = 0.035, d = −0.845]. Although a two-way mixed-design ANOVA did not identify significant Time × Training Group interactions for either the Letter [F(1,17) = 1.38, p = 0.257, ηp2 = 0.077] or Space [F(1,18) = 2.62, p = 0.122, ηp2 = 0.146] tasks, the effect of training on WM was significant only in the CERT group, but not in the PSYCT group [Letter: t(9) = 1.279, p = 0.233, d = −0.404; Space: t(9) = 0.650, p = 0.532, d = −0.206]. These results indicate that improvements in executive domains were exclusive to the CERT training group. No other significant changes linked to the interventions were identified (all ps > 0.05).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Increased Working Memory Performance following CERT Intervention. Figure shows the results of the Two-Back Letter task, and the same patterns were also observed for the Space task (not shown). Paired t-tests of pre- and post-training reaction times (RT) within the Two-Back WM tasks revealed significant improvements (faster time), which were exclusive to the CERT group. *significant at p < 0.05 two-tailed; ns, non-significant.

Changes in Resting-State Functional Connectivity Following ER Training

Further supporting the effectiveness of the CERT intervention, brain imaging results showed significant changes in the rsFC (Table 2), overall, supporting the idea that the our ER training facilitated functional decoupling of bottom-up emotion and perception processing regions from regions of the DMN, along with enhanced functional coupling among top-down cognitive control regions and between regions of default-mode and control networks. First, for the subcortical seed regions, the AMY showed overall decreased rsFC with cortical regions involved in higher-level cognition and lower-level perception, following training (Figure 4). Specifically, across both groups, there was decreased rsFC between the left AMY seed and clusters in the cortical midline regions, including the medial frontal (mPFC/ACC) and parietal cortices. Interestingly, in the CERT group, the left AMY showed larger decrease in the rsFC with a cluster in the left visual cortex.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Brain regions showing changes in functional connectivity following training.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Decreased amygdala (AMY) rsFC following Training. Subjects across both CERT and PSYCT groups showed reductions in left AMY-medial prefrontal frontal cortex (mPFC) and left AMY-RSC/mPC rsFC following training (double lines); similar reduced rsFC was also identified for the right AMY (not shown), at a lower threshold (see Table 2). Interaction effects additionally revealed larger post-training reduction in rsFC between the left AMY seed and the left OPC, in the CERT group (single line; see also Table 2). Blue nodes and edges reflect decreases in rsFC. Black node outline indicates the seed region. mPFC, Medial Prefrontal Cortex; ACC, Anterior Cingulate Cortex; AMY, Amygdala; RSC, Retrosplenial Cortex; mPC, Medial Parietal Cortex; OPC; Occipito-parietal Cortex; and L/R, Left/Right Hemispheres.

Second, for the cognitive control cortical regions, the left dlPFC showed decreased rsFC with a cluster in the left occipital pole, which was seen across both CERT and PSYCT groups. However, the left dlPFC showed larger increase in rsFC with a cluster in the cingulate/paracingulate cortex as well as with a cluster in the right posterior segment of the inferior frontal cortex (IFC), in the CERT group (Figure 5, left panel). Moreover, increased rsFC was also found between the right dlPFC and a cluster in the posterior part of the left middle frontal cortex, which was also larger, in the CERT group. Regarding the vlPFC seeds, there was increased rsFC between the right vlPFC seed and a cluster in the posterior segment of the right IFC, as well as clusters in the bilateral occipito-temporal cortex (OTC), all of which were larger in the CERT group. Finally, regarding the mPFC seed, there was increased rsFC with clusters in the right lateral parietal cortex (LPC), across both groups, and with a cluster in the left LPC, which was stronger in the CERT group (Figure 5, right panel).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Changes in the rsFC of Control Regions following Training. Left panel: altered dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) rsFC following training. Subjects across both CERT and PSYCT groups showed reductions in rsFC between the left dlPFC seed and areas of the left visual cortex, following training (blue double line). CERT training was also associated with stronger increases in the rsFC between the left dlPFC seed and the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), and between the left dlPFC seed and a posterior portion of the right inferior frontal cortex (red solid lines). Additionally, the CERT training was also associated with larger increases in the rsFC between the right dlPFC seed and areas of the right visual cortex, and between the right dlPFC seed and a posterior area of the left middle frontal gyrus (red solid lines). Right panel: increased ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC) and mPFC rsFC following training. The CERT training was associated with stronger increases in the rsFC between the right vlPFC seed and a posterior area of the inferior frontal cortex (IFC; pIFC), as well as between the right vlPFC seed and the bilateral occipito-temporal cortex (OTC). Regarding the mPFC, there was increased post-training rsFC between the mPFC seed and the right LPC across both CERT and PSYCT groups (red double line), and the CERT training was also associated with larger increases in the rsFC between the mPFC seed and the left LPC (red solid line). Blue reflects decreases in functional connectivity, red reflects increases, and their combination (left dlPFC node) reflects both. Black node outlines indicate the seed regions. dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; MFC, middle frontal cortex; vlPFC, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; IFC, inferior frontal cortex; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; OC, occipital cortex; OTC, occipito-temporal cortex; LPC, lateral parietal cortex; and L/R, left/right hemispheres.

Overall, these findings pinpoint training-dependent changes in bottom-up as well as top-down processes that are less driven by the emotional salience and basic perceptual processing, along with enhanced in-tune responses among cognitive and emotion control regions and across self-referential and cognitive control networks.

Discussion

This report presents proof-of-principle evidence for the effectiveness of a novel cognitive-emotional training intervention targeting the acquisition of ER skills aimed at increasing resilience against emotional distress in military veterans. There were two main novel results: (1) behavioral results showed evidence for enhanced psychological well-being and executive function following training, reflected in increased GSE, PTG, and WM; (2) brain imaging results showed evidence of diminished bottom-up influences from emotional and perceptual brain regions, along with evidence of normalized rsFC in the large-scale functional networks following training, reflected in increased connectivity among cognitive and emotion control regions and across DMN and FPCN networks. These findings are discussed below.

Enhanced Well-Being and Executive Function Following ER Training

First, CERT, but not PSYCT, was associated with increased GSE. GSE is not constrained to specific types of tasks or situations, but rather reflects more general beliefs in one’s competence to manage a broad range of tasks and challenges (Luszczynska et al., 2005). Therefore, successful performance of a range of behaviors and activities in a variety of situations is expected to build such a generalized sense of efficacy (Schwarzer and Luszczynska, 2007). The enhanced GSE as a result of engaging in the complex CERT is in line with previous intervention programs that have shown an efficacy-enhancing impact of a range of mastery experiences (Schunk, 1989; Bandura, 1997). Increased GSE is a very desirable outcome, given that SE is seen as the most crucial and proximal predictor of behavior (Bandura, 1997, 2002). SE has a self-regulatory function in dealing with stress and negative affect, being linked to better psychological adjustment, lower distress, fewer symptoms of burnout, and fewer symptoms of depression over time, along with reduced social anxiety, depression, and externalizing symptoms and better posttraumatic recovery (Brouwers and Tomic, 2000; Benight and Harper, 2002; Benight and Bandura, 2004; Bisschop et al., 2004; Gallagher et al., 2011, 2020; Singh and Bussey, 2011). It is assumed that SE impacts the appraisal and interpretation of stressful situations (transactional stress theory) and exerts beneficial effects through constructive regulation of motivational, affective, and decisional processes (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984; Bandura, 1997; Simmen-Janevska et al., 2012). This leads individuals with higher levels of self-efficacy to consider anxiety and stress symptoms as more controllable and temporary (Cervone, 2000; Leganger et al., 2000). We expect that, with a larger sample, the increased self-efficacy in our CERT group will also be associated with multiple benefits on well-being.

Second, both CERT and PSYCT were associated with increased PTG. One essential component of PTG is the ability to manage distressing emotions elicited by the traumatic event. The PTG model proposed by Tedeschi and Calhoun (2004) suggests that the process of rebuilding disrupted beliefs involves significant cognitive processing that allows engagement with trauma-related emotions and memories. Recent research examining the relation between ER and PTG suggests that strategies that involve engagement with the emotional stimuli, such as reappraisal, might influence PTG by helping individuals extract meaning from their traumatic experiences (Larsen and Berenbaum, 2015). Whereas reappraisal seems to be the choice for dealing with low-intensity situations, attentional deployment strategies are less cognitively demanding and seem to be preferred in high-intensity situations in individuals who had experienced a recent traumatic event (e.g., in the last 6 months, Strauss et al., 2016; Orejuela-Dávila et al., 2019). Both of our training programs involved significant cognitive processing. CERT, which offered our participants the flexibility to choose between FA and CR, allowed both engagement with diverse negative stimuli (pictures, memories, and worries) and disengagement of attention from the negative aspects of these stimuli, and facilitated PTG. The beneficial effect of ER training on PTG is in line with available evidence showing a positive correlation between self-reported engagement of adaptive ER strategies and PTG in undergraduate students (Thomas et al., 2019).

Third, participants in the CERT group also showed post-training improvements in WM, and these increases were observed in both Letter and Space tasks, while the PSYCT group participants did not show such increases. Individuals with emotional disorders such as anxiety and depression show a bias toward processing of negative emotional information (threat, sadness, etc.), leading to excessive rumination on past negative events and worry about the uncertainty of the future (for a review, see Mogg and Bradley, 2018). Our brain systems have a limited capacity, and thus when people get stuck on negative (and task-irrelevant) information, they have fewer resources to invest to complete the demands of the tasks at hand, shift attention, and process information efficiently (Derakshan and Eysenck, 2009). This gives rise to patterns of cognitive inflexibility (for a review, see Stange et al., 2017). When executive functions of WM become inefficient and rigid, people are more likely to experience interference and find it difficult to achieve their goals efficiently (Berggren and Derakshan, 2013).

Recent research suggests a strong link between WM, attentional control, and cognitive reappraisal (Shipstead et al., 2014). In fact, some authors argue that WM capacity refers specifically to attention control (Kane et al., 2001). Successful WM needs efficient use of attentional control to hold information temporarily and to manipulate the content in order to execute ER tasks, which require overriding habitual responses. Moreover, evidence from cross-sectional and training studies have established that WM ability and ER are connected, and that this connection is likely mediated by attention control (Schmeichel et al., 2008; McRae et al., 2012; Schweizer et al., 2013). The two ER strategies trained in the CERT group involve a number of mental operations, such as keeping in mind the specific ER strategy, monitoring/resolving the conflict between habitual and targeted reactions, selection among possible alternatives, and the modulation of behavior that, repeated over the course of the training, have helped participants improve their WM (Ochsner et al., 2012).

Finally, it should also be noted that an important aspect of the present training linked to the engagement of memory processes is not only related to WM per se, as discussed above, but also related to “working with memory.” This is because our ER training also affected the way participants encoded and retrieved emotional episodic memories. Indeed, there is evidence that both ER strategies affect emotional memory during both stages (Denkova et al., 2015; Dolcos et al., 2020a), with the overall tendency for reduction of the impact of emotion on memory (but see Dillon et al., 2007). In particular, by focusing away from the most emotional aspects of external stimuli, the engagement of FA is associated with reduced recollection of memory for emotional pictures (Dolcos et al., 2020a). Similarly, FA is also effective in decreasing the impact of recollected emotional autobiographical memories, both when retrieved in isolation and when retrieved as internal emotional distraction during an ongoing cognitive task (Denkova et al., 2015; Iordan et al., 2019). Hence, it is reasonable to expect that training participants to use ER strategies with external and internal stimuli have longer-lasting effects on the encoding and retrieval of memories for emotional events.

Changes in Resting-State Functional Connectivity Following ER Training

The brain imaging findings from our pilot investigation provide further evidence for the effectiveness of the intervention in the CERT group. First, reduced functional connectivity between AMY and midline cortical structures (both frontal and parietal) is consistent with the idea of normalized bottom-up emotional influences on activity in DMN brain regions (Zilverstand et al., 2017). DMN has been linked to a range of self-referential processing (internal thought, memory retrieval, future planning, and emotion regulation), and hence reduced rsFC from a basic emotion processing region suggests diminished influences that would emotionally color the affective state of participants when turning their focus on the internal environment, which is typically associated with a negative emotional bias in emotional disturbances (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991; Raichle et al., 2001; Fox et al., 2005; Schacter et al., 2007; Cooney et al., 2010; Denkova et al., 2015). Interestingly, these effects were observed across the participants from CERT and PSYCT groups, which suggests similar mechanisms of change in the expected direction. However, further supporting diminished default interactions between the AMY and perceptual areas, the reduced AMY-visual cortex rsFC was larger in the CERT group. This finding is consistent with the differences in training between the CERT and PSYCT groups. Whereas the PSYCT training primarily took the form of interpersonal communications about higher-level ideas (e.g., goals, values, and skills), the CERT training emphasized cognitive mechanisms (e.g., FA), including processing of visual stimuli, in combination with advanced cognitive processing (i.e., CR), meant to diminish the impact of emotional stimulation at different stages of emotion processing. Hence, reduced coupling between regions involved in bottom-up emotion signaling specifically observed in the CERT group suggests that this training equips participants with skills that reduce the impact of bottom-up emotional influences. This is particularly important in preparing them to respond quickly in emotionally intense situations, where more effortful emotion regulation might be difficult to deploy immediately (Sheppes et al., 2014; Orejuela-Dávila et al., 2019).

Turning to brain regions involved in cognitive/emotional control, reduced rsFC of the left dlPFC with the visual cortex is also consistent with diminished bottom-up influences from perceptual brain regions, a change that was also observed across the CERT and PSYCT groups. The overall greater increased connectivity of dlPFC ROIs, bilaterally, with vlPFC and dACC areas following the CERT intervention suggests strengthened default cross-talk among areas associated with cognitive/executive and affective control regions promoted by our ER training. This is important, given the central role of the dlPFC as part of the FPCN, which is involved in interfacing between focusing on internal and external stimulation, together with brain regions (vlPFC and ACC) circumscribed by other networks (salience and cingulo-opercular; Fuster, 1997; Smith and Jonides, 1999; Hopfinger et al., 2000; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Seeley et al., 2007; Dosenbach et al., 2008). This allows flexible behavior involving adaptive switches between paying attention to external stimulation and being aware of our internal states, needs, thoughts, and memories (Sridharan et al., 2008; Zabelina and Andrews-Hanna, 2016). Finally, increased rsFC connectivity among subregions of the right IFC (anterior and posterior), along with stronger increased connectivity between mPFC and lateral parietal cortical areas, in the CERT group, are also consistent with enhanced functional coupling among control brain regions and normalized cross-network interactions, respectively, promoted by the CERT training.

Interestingly, our expected increased connectivity between the AMY and ER brain regions was not confirmed. Our expectation was based on task-related evidence of increased functional coupling between basic emotion processing (AMY) and emotion control (lateral and medial PFC) regions, possibly indexing the need to regulate (Dolcos et al., 2006; Denkova et al., 2015). This is consistent with the idea that such coupling is necessary for the latter regions to exert control on the AMY response, when facing external or internal emotional challenges (Dolcos et al., 2006; Denkova et al., 2015). However, it may be the case that such couplings may only be transiently increased by current challenges and not necessarily also implemented in longer-term changes reflected by measures of rsFC. Further research is needed to clarify this matter.

Caveats

A caveat of the current study is the limited sample, as there were only 19 participants in total. Future research should further confirm the proof-of-principle results of this pilot study in larger samples. Despite the smaller sample size, our findings showed strong improvements in post-traumatic growth, self-efficacy, and working memory, although there were no significant decreases in anxiety and depression as a result of the training. It is possible that changes in our primary outcomes of psychological distress have not been detected at the behavioral level due to the relatively small sample sizes of the groups in our pilot study. Nevertheless, some of the factors of behavior change, such as self-efficacy, might be more sensitive to the particular type of training offered by our interventions, and they may act as buffering mechanisms that contribute to the protection against symptoms of distress. Moreover, changes in the rsFC following both CERT and PSYCT training indicate a normalization of bottom-up emotional influences on activity in self-referential processing regions, suggesting that both types of training helped participants experience diminished negative affect when focusing on internal thoughts and worries. Second, the combination of FA and CR in the CERT was the most effective proof-of-concept strategy for testing our approach grounded in interactive person-situation-strategy processes and the emotion regulation choice framework (Sheppes et al., 2014). However, their relative contribution to the observed effects is not clear. Although it is reasonable to expect that FA contributed to improved WM and CR to the enhanced PTG, future research using multiple control groups is needed to disentangle the unique contribution of each of these strategies.

Conclusion

Reduced ability to control emotional responses is a major marker of affective disturbances. Despite the small sample, the present pilot study provides proof-of-principle evidence for a sustainable cognitive emotion-regulation training intervention that goes beyond costly traditional models of therapeutic treatment by targeting the development of healthy and flexible ER skills in a sample of military veterans. Using a combination of behavioral and brain imaging methods, this study showed enhanced executive function and psychological well-being following training, reflected in increased working memory, post-traumatic growth, and general self-efficacy. Moreover, brain imaging results showed evidence of diminished bottom-up influences from emotional and perceptual brain regions, along with evidence of normalized functional connectivity in the large-scale functional networks following training, reflected in increased connectivity among cognitive and emotion control regions and across regions of self-referential and control networks. Overall, our results provide proof-of-concept evidence that resilience and well-being can be learned through ER training, and that training-related improvements manifested in both behavioral change and neuroplasticity can translate into real-life benefits.

Data Availability Statement

The data sets analyzed for the current report are available upon requests made to the corresponding authors, pending approval from participants and the Institutional Review Board (IRB).

Ethics Statement

The study was approved by the IRB office at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, and all participants provided written informed consent. The procedures used in this study adhere to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

SD and FD designed the study, with input from YH, CW, and HB. YH and CW collected the data. SD, FD, and YH planned the analytical approach, with input from HB. YH performed the analyses, with help from PB and KH and input from FD and SD. SD and FD wrote the first draft of the manuscript, and then revised it based on contributions from YH, PB, KH, and CW, and feedback from HB. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by a seed grant from the Chez Family Foundation Center for Wounded Veterans in Higher Education, at the University of Illinois.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was conducted in part at the Biomedical Imaging Center of the Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign (UIUC-BI-BIC).

Footnotes

References

Aldao, A., Sheppes, G., and Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation flexibility. Cogn. Ther. Res. 39, 263–278. doi: 10.1007/s10608-014-9662-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashburner, J. T. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. NeuroImage 38, 95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashburner, J. T., and Ridgway, G. R. (2013). Symmetric diffeomorphic modeling of longitudinal structural MRI. Front. Neurosci. 6:197. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 829–839. doi: 10.1038/nrn1201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman and company.

Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: an agentic perspective. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 1–26. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (2002). Social cognitive theory in cultural context. J. Appl. Psychol. 51, 269–290. doi: 10.1111/1464-0597.00092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York, NY: Guilford press.

Google Scholar

Behzadi, Y., Restom, K., Liau, J., and Liu, T. T. (2007). A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. NeuroImage 37, 90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellack, A. S., Hersen, M., and Himmelhoch, J. (1981). Social skills training compared with pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy in the treatment of unipolar depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 138, 1562–1567. doi: 10.1176/ajp.138.12.1562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benight, C. C., and Bandura, A. (2004). Social cognitive theory of posttraumatic recovery: the role of perceived self-efficacy. Behav. Res. Ther. 42, 1129–1148. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benight, C. C., and Harper, M. L. (2002). Coping self-efficacy perceptions as a mediator between acute stress response and long-term distress following natural disasters. J. Trauma. Stress. 15, 177–186. doi: 10.1023/A:1015295025950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berggren, N., and Derakshan, N. (2013). Attentional control deficits in trait anxiety: why you see them and why you don’t. Biol. Psychol. 92, 440–446. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bisschop, M. I., Kriegsman, D., Beekman, A., and Deeg, D. (2004). Chronic diseases and depression: the modifying role of psychosocial resources. Soc. Sci. Med. 59, 721–733. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.11.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C., and Diamond, A. (2008). Biological processes in prevention and intervention: the promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Dev. Psychopathol. 20, 899–911. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boden, M. T., Berenbaum, H., and Topper, M. (2012). Intuition, affect, and peculiar beliefs. Personal. Individ. Differ. 52, 845–848. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonanno, G. A., and Burton, C. L. (2013). Regulatory flexibility: an individual differences perspective on coping and emotion regulation. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 591–612. doi: 10.1177/1745691613504116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal, M., and Coifman, K. (2004). The importance of being flexible: the ability to both enhance and suppress emotional expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychol. Sci. 15, 482–487. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00705.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bredemeier, K., and Berenbaum, H. (2013). Cross-sectional and longitudinal relations between working memory performance and worry. J. Exp. Psychopathol. 4, 420–434. doi: 10.5127/jep.032212

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bredemeier, K., Spielberg, J. M., Silton, R. L., Berenbaum, H., Heller, W., and Miller, G. A. (2010). Screening for depressive disorders using the mood and anxiety symptoms questionnaire anhedonic depression scale: a receiver-operating characteristic analysis. Psychol. Assess. 22, 702–710. doi: 10.1037/a0019915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brouwers, A., and Tomic, W. (2000). A longitudinal study of teacher burnout and perceived self-efficacy in classroom management. Teach. Teach. Educ. 16, 239–253. doi: 10.1016/S0742-051X(99)00057-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cervone, D. (2000). Thinking about self-efficacy. Behav. Modif. 24, 30–56. doi: 10.1177/0145445500241002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, C. (2001). Assessing coping flexibility in real-life and laboratory settings: a multimethod approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80:814. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.5.814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, N., and Ochsner, K. N. (2018). From surviving to thriving in the face of threats: the emerging science of emotion regulation training. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 24, 143–155. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., and Bunting, M. F. (2001). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: the importance of working memory capacity. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 8, 331–335. doi: 10.3758/BF03196169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooney, R. E., Joormann, J., Eugene, F., Dennis, E. L., and Gotlib, I. H. (2010). Neural correlates of rumination in depression. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 470–478. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.4.470

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuijpers, P., Van Straten, A., and Warmerdam, L. (2007). Behavioral activation treatments of depression: a meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 27, 318–326. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denkova, E., Chakrabarty, T., Dolcos, S., and Dolcos, F. (2011). Brain imaging investigation of the neural correlates of emotional autobiographical recollection. J. Vis. Exp. 54:2396. doi: 10.3791/2396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denkova, E., Dolcos, S., and Dolcos, F. (2012). Reliving emotional personal memories: affective biases linked to personality and sex-related differences. Emotion 12, 515–528. doi: 10.1037/a0026809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denkova, E., Dolcos, S., and Dolcos, F. (2013a). The effect of retrieval focus and emotional valence on the inferior frontal cortex activity during autobiographical recollection. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:192. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denkova, E., Dolcos, S., and Dolcos, F. (2013b). The effect of retrieval focus and emotional valence on the medial temporal lobe activity during autobiographical recollection. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:109. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denkova, E., Dolcos, S., and Dolcos, F. (2015). Neural correlates of 'distracting' from emotion during autobiographical recollection. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 219–230. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denny, B. T., and Ochsner, K. N. (2014). Behavioral effects of longitudinal training in cognitive reappraisal. Emotion 14, 425–433. doi: 10.1037/a0035276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derakshan, N., and Eysenck, M. W. (2009). Anxiety, processing efficiency, and cognitive performance: new developments from attentional control theory. Eur. Psychol. 14, 168–176. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.14.2.168

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desikan, R. S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., et al. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage 31, 968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dillon, D. G., Ritchey, M., Johnson, B. D., and LaBar, K. S. (2007). Dissociable effects of conscious emotion regulation strategies on explicit and implicit memory. Emotion 7, 354–365. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolcos, S., Hu, Y. F., Iordan, A. D., Moore, M., and Dolcos, F. (2016). Optimism and the brain: trait optimism mediates the protective role of the orbitofrontal cortex gray matter volume against anxiety. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 263–271. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolcos, F., Katsumi, Y., Bogdan, P., Shen, C., Jun, S., Buetti, S., et al. (2020a). The impact of focused attention on subsequent emotional recollection: a functional MRI investigation. Neuropsychologia 138:107338. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolcos, F., Katsumi, Y., Chen, A., Jun, S., Bogdan, P., Heller, W., et al. (2020b). The impact of focused attention on emotional experience: a functional MRI investigation. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 20, 1011–1026. doi: 10.3758/s13415-020-00816-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolcos, F., Katsumi, Y., Moore, M., Berggren, N., de Gelder, B., Derakshan, N., et al. (2020c). Neural correlates of emotion-attention interactions: from perception, learning, and memory to social cognition, individual differences, and training interventions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 108, 559–601. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolcos, F., Kragel, P., Wang, L., and McCarthy, G. (2006). Role of the inferior frontal cortex in coping with distracting emotions. Neuroreport 17, 1591–1594. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000236860.24081.be

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dosenbach, N. U., Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Schlaggar, B. L., and Petersen, S. E. (2008). A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., and Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion 7, 336–353. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ford, B. Q., Gross, J. J., and Gruber, J. (2019). Broadening our field of view: the role of emotion polyregulation. Emot. Rev. 11, 197–208. doi: 10.1177/1754073919850314

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., and Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 102, 9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frazier, J. A., Chiu, S., Breeze, J. L., Makris, N., Lange, N., Kennedy, D. N., et al. (2005). Structural brain magnetic resonance imaging of limbic and thalamic volumes in pediatric bipolar disorder. Am. J. Psychiatr. 162, 1256–1265. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.7.1256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuster, J. M. (1997). Network memory. Trends Neurosci. 20, 451–459. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01128-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, M. W., Long, L. J., and Phillips, C. A. (2020). Hope, optimism, self-efficacy, and posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analytic review of the protective effects of positive expectancies. J. Clin. Psychol. 76, 329–355. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, D., Ni Mhaolain, A., Crosby, L., Ryan, D., Lacey, L., Coen, R. F., et al. (2011). Self-efficacy for managing dementia may protect against burden and depression in Alzheimer's caregivers. Aging Ment. Health 15, 663–670. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2011.562179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gamez, W., Chmielewski, M., Kotov, R., Ruggero, C., Suzuki, N., and Watson, D. (2014). The brief experiential avoidance questionnaire: development and initial validation. Psychol. Assess. 26, 35–45. doi: 10.1037/a0034473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, J. M., Seidman, L. J., Makris, N., Ahern, T., O’Brien, L. M., Caviness, V. S. Jr., et al. (2007). Hypothalamic abnormalities in schizophrenia: sex effects and genetic vulnerability. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 935–945. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology 39, 281–291. doi: 10.1017/S0048577201393198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2007). Handbook of emotion regulation. 1st Edn. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Hallquist, M. N., Hwang, K., and Luna, B. (2013). The nuisance of nuisance regression: spectral misspecification in a common approach to resting-state fMRI preprocessing reintroduces noise and obscures functional connectivity. NeuroImage 82, 208–225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., and Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: model, processes and outcomes. Behav. Res. Ther. 44, 1–25. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holtzheimer, P. E., and Mayberg, H. S. (2011). Stuck in a rut: rethinking depression and its treatment. Trends Neurosci. 34, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2010.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., and Mangun, G. R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 284–291. doi: 10.1038/72999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, A. L., Galambos, N. L., and Krahn, H. J. (2014). Forecasting life and career satisfaction in midlife from young adult depressive symptoms. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 35, 51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2013.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, Y., and Dolcos, S. (2017). Trait anxiety mediates the link between inferior frontal cortex volume and negative affective bias in healthy adults. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 775–782. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx0008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iordan, A. D., Dolcos, S., and Dolcos, F. (2019). Brain activity and network interactions in the impact of internal emotional distraction. Cereb. Cortex 29, 2607–2623. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeffords, J. R., Bayly, B. L., Bumpus, M. F., and Hill, L. G. (2018). Investigating the relationship between university students’ psychological flexibility and college self-efficacy. J. Coll. Stud. Ret. 22:1521025117751071. doi: 10.1177/1521025117751071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaiser, R. H., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Wager, T. D., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2015). Large-scale network dysfunction in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity. JAMA Psychiat. 72, 603–611. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R., and Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130, 169–183. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanske, P., Heissler, J., Schonfelder, S., and Wessa, M. (2012). Neural correlates of emotion regulation deficits in remitted depression: the influence of regulation strategy, habitual regulation use, and emotional valence. NeuroImage 61, 686–693. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kashdan, T. B., and Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 865–878. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kazdin, A. E. (2019). Annual research review: expanding mental health services through novel models of intervention delivery. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 60, 455–472. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kertz, S. J., Lee, J., and Bjorgvinsson, T. (2014). Psychometric properties of abbreviated and ultra-brief versions of the Penn State worry questionnaire. Psychol. Assess. 26, 1146–1154. doi: 10.1037/a0037251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koole, S. L., Schwager, S., and Rothermund, K. (2015). Resilience is more about being flexible than about staying positive. Behav. Brain Sci. 38:e109. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X14001599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsen, S. E., and Berenbaum, H. (2015). Are specific emotion regulation strategies differentially associated with posttraumatic growth versus stress? J. Aggress. Maltreat. Trauma 24, 794–808. doi: 10.1080/10926771.2015.1062451

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York, NY: Springer publishing company.

Google Scholar

Leganger, A., Kraft, P., and Røysamb, E. (2000). Perceived self-efficacy in health behaviour research: conceptualisation, measurement and correlates. Psychol. Health 15, 51–69. doi: 10.1080/08870440008400288

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, M. E., MacLane, C., Daflos, S., Seeley, J., Hayes, S. C., Biglan, A., et al. (2014). Examining psychological inflexibility as a transdiagnostic process across psychological disorders. J. Contextual Behav. Sci. 3, 155–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2014.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy-Gigi, E., Bonanno, G. A., Shapiro, A. R., Richter-Levin, G., Kéri, S., and Sheppes, G. (2016). Emotion regulatory flexibility sheds light on the elusive relationship between repeated traumatic exposure and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 4, 28–39. doi: 10.1177/2167702615577783

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luszczynska, A., Scholz, U., and Schwarzer, R. (2005). The general self-efficacy scale: multicultural validation studies. Aust. J. Psychol. 139, 439–457. doi: 10.3200/JRLP.139.5.439-457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maciejewski, P. K., Prigerson, H. G., and Mazure, C. M. (2000). Self-efficacy as a mediator between stressful life events and depressive symptoms: differences based on history of prior depression. Br. J. Psychiatry 176, 373–378. doi: 10.1192/bjp.176.4.373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makris, N., Goldstein, J. M., Kennedy, D., Hodge, S. M., Caviness, V. S., Faraone, S. V., et al. (2006). Decreased volume of left and total anterior insular lobule in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 83, 155–171. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McRae, K., Ciesielski, B., and Gross, J. J. (2012). Unpacking cognitive reappraisal: goals, tactics, and outcomes. Emotion 12, 250–255. doi: 10.1037/a0026351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., and Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the penn state worry questionnaire. Behav. Res. Ther. 28, 487–495. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., and Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science 244, 933–938. doi: 10.1126/science.2658056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogg, K., and Bradley, B. P. (2018). Anxiety and threat-related attention: cognitive-motivational framework and treatment. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 225–240. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, M., Culpepper, S., Phan, K. L., Strauman, T. J., Dolcos, F., and Dolcos, S. (2018). Neurobehavioral mechanisms of resilience against emotional distress: an integrative brain-personality-symptom approach using structural equation modeling. Personal. Neurosci. 1:e8. doi: 10.1017/pen.2018.11

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naragon-Gainey, K., McMahon, T. P., and Chacko, T. P. (2017). The structure of common emotion regulation strategies: a meta-analytic examination. Psychol. Bull. 143, 384–427. doi: 10.1037/bul0000093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nezu, A. M. (2004). Problem solving and behavior therapy revisited. Behav. Ther. 35, 1–33. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(04)80002-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 100, 569–582. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., and Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., Silvers, J. A., and Buhle, J. T. (2012). Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: a synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1251, E1–E24. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06751.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orejuela-Dávila, A. I., Levens, S. M., Sagui-Henson, S. J., Tedeschi, R. G., and Sheppes, G. (2019). The relation between emotion regulation choice and posttraumatic growth. Cognit. Emot. 33, 1709–1717. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2019.1592117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Power, J. D., Cohen, A. L., Nelson, S. M., Wig, G. S., Barnes, K. A., Church, J. A., et al. (2011). Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron 72, 665–678. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 98, 676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schacter, D., Addis, D., and Buckner, R. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 657–661. doi: 10.1038/nrn2213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmeichel, B. J., and Demaree, H. A. (2010). Working memory capacity and spontaneous emotion regulation: high capacity predicts self-enhancement in response to negative feedback. Emotion 10, 739–744. doi: 10.1037/a0019355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmeichel, B. J., Volokhov, R. N., and Demaree, H. A. (2008). Working memory capacity and the self-regulation of emotional expression and experience. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95:1526. doi: 10.1037/a0013345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schönfeld, P., Brailovskaia, J., Zhang, X. C., and Margraf, J. (2019). Self-efficacy as a mechanism linking daily stress to mental health in students: a three-wave cross-lagged study. Psychol. Rep. 122, 2074–2095. doi: 10.1177/0033294118787496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schunk, D. H. (1989). “Self-efficacy and cognitive skill learning” in Research on motivation in education, goals and cognitions. Vol. 3. eds. C. Ames and R. Ames (New York, NY: Academic Press).

Google Scholar

Schwarzer, R., and Jerusalem, M. (1995). “Generalized self-efficacy scale” in Measures in health psychology: A user’s portfolio. Causal and control beliefs. eds. J. Weinman, S. Wright, and M. Johnston (Windsor, UK: NFER-NELSON), 35–37.

Google Scholar

Schwarzer, R., and Luszczynska, A. (2007). Self-efficacy. Health behavior constructs: theory, measurement, and research. National Cancer Institute. Available at: https://cancercontrol.cancer.gov/brp/research/constructs/self-efficacy (Accessed February 2, 2021).

Google Scholar

Schweizer, S., Grahn, J., Hampshire, A., Mobbs, D., and Dalgleish, T. (2013). Training the emotional brain: improving affective control through emotional working memory training. J. Neurosci. 33, 5301–5311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2593-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sekiguchi, A., Kotozaki, Y., Sugiura, M., Nouchi, R., Takeuchi, H., Hanawa, S., et al. (2015). Resilience after 3/11: structural brain changes 1 year after the Japanese earthquake. Mol. Psychiatry 20, 552–554. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.28

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., Radu, P., Blechert, J., and Gross, J. J. (2014). Emotion regulation choice: a conceptual framework and supporting evidence. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 163–181. doi: 10.1037/a0030831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shipstead, Z., Lindsey, D. R., Marshall, R. L., and Engle, R. W. (2014). The mechanisms of working memory capacity: primary memory, secondary memory, and attention control. J. Mem. Lang. 72, 116–141. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2014.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmen-Janevska, K., Brandstätter, V., and Maercker, A. (2012). The overlooked relationship between motivational abilities and posttraumatic stress: a review. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 3:18560. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v3i0.18560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, P., and Bussey, K. (2011). Peer victimization and psychological maladjustment: the mediating role of coping self-efficacy. J. Res. Adolesc. 21, 420–433. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00680.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E. E., and Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science 283, 1657–1661. doi: 10.1126/science.283.5408.1657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sridharan, D., Levitin, D. J., and Menon, V. (2008). A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 105, 12569–12574. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800005105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stange, J. P., Alloy, L. B., and Fresco, D. M. (2017). Inflexibility as a vulnerability to depression: a systematic qualitative review. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 24, 245–276. doi: 10.1111/cpsp.12201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanton, A. L., Danoff-Burg, S., Cameron, C. L., and Ellis, A. P. (1994). Coping through emotional approach: problems of conceptualization and confounding. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66, 350–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.2.350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauss, G. P., Ossenfort, K. L., and Whearty, K. M. (2016). Reappraisal and distraction emotion regulation strategies are associated with distinct patterns of visual attention and differing levels of cognitive demand. PLoS One 11:e0162290. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0162290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tavakoli, N., Broyles, A., Reid, E. K., Sandoval, J. R., and Correa-Fernández, V. (2019). Psychological inflexibility as it relates to stress, worry, generalized anxiety, and somatization in an ethnically diverse sample of college students. J. Contextual Behav. Sci. 11, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2018.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tedeschi, R. G., and Calhoun, L. G. (1996). The posttraumatic growth inventory: measuring the positive legacy of trauma. J. Trauma. Stress. 9, 455–471. doi: 10.1002/jts.2490090305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tedeschi, R. G., and Calhoun, L. G. (2004). Posttraumatic growth: conceptual foundations and empirical evidence. Psychol. Inq. 15, 1–18. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1501_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, E. A., Hamrick, L. A., Owens, G. P., and Tekie, Y. T. (2019). Posttraumatic growth among undergraduates: contributions from adaptive cognitive emotion regulation and emotional intelligence. Traumatology 26, 68–73. doi: 10.1037/trm0000203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valk, S. L., Bernhardt, B. C., Trautwein, F. M., Bockler, A., Kanske, P., Guizard, N., et al. (2017). Structural plasticity of the social brain: differential change after socio-affective and cognitive mental training. Sci. Adv. 3:e1700489. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1700489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dillen, L. F., and Koole, S. L. (2007). Clearing the mind: a working memory model of distraction from negative mood. Emotion 7, 715–723. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verghese, A., Garner, K. G., Mattingley, J. B., and Dux, P. E. (2016). Prefrontal cortex structure predicts training-induced improvements in multitasking performance. J. Neurosci. 36, 2638–2645. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3410-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, E. (2011). Dysregulation in level of goal and action identification across psychological disorders. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 260–278. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weathers, F. W., Litz, B. T., Keane, T. M., Palmieri, P. A., Marx, B. P., and Schnurr, P. P. (2013). The PTSD checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5). Available at: www.ptsd.va.gov (Accessed February 2, 2021).

Google Scholar

Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., and Nieto-Castanon, A. (2012). Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2, 125–141. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zabelina, D. L., and Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Dynamic network interactions supporting internally-oriented cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 40, 86–93. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zilverstand, A., Parvaz, M. A., and Goldstein, R. Z. (2017). Neuroimaging cognitive reappraisal in clinical populations to define neural targets for enhancing emotion regulation. A systematic review. NeuroImage 151, 105–116. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zinken, K. M., Cradock, S., and Skinner, T. C. (2008). Analysis system for self-efficacy training (ASSET): assessing treatment fidelity of self-management interventions. Patient Educ. Couns. 72, 186–193. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2008.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: emotion regulation training, transfer effects of intervention, affective resilience, emotional well-being, emotional memory recollection, emotion-cognition interactions, large-scale functional brain networks, resting state fMRI

Citation: Dolcos S, Hu Y, Williams C, Bogdan PC, Hohl K, Berenbaum H and Dolcos F (2021) Cultivating Affective Resilience: Proof-of-Principle Evidence of Translational Benefits From a Novel Cognitive-Emotional Training Intervention. Front. Psychol. 12:585536. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.585536

Received: 20 July 2020; Accepted: 25 January 2021;
Published: 09 March 2021.

Edited by:

Maurizio Codispoti, University of Bologna, Italy

Reviewed by:

Steven Grant Greening, University of Manitoba, Canada
Klaus Mathiak, RWTH Aachen University, Germany

Copyright © 2021 Dolcos, Hu, Williams, Bogdan, Hohl, Berenbaum and Dolcos. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sanda Dolcos, c2RvbGNvc0BpbGxpbm9pcy5lZHU=; Yifan Hu, eWlmYW5odTJAaWxsaW5vaXMuZWR1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.