Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 07 October 2021
Sec. Eating Behavior

Differences Between Omnivores and Vegetarians in Personality Profiles, Values, and Empathy: A Systematic Review

\nSophie HollerSophie Holler1Holger Cramer,,Holger Cramer2,3,4Daniela LiebscherDaniela Liebscher1Michael Jeitler,Michael Jeitler1,5Dania SchumannDania Schumann1Vijayendra MurthyVijayendra Murthy3Andreas Michalsen,Andreas Michalsen1,5Christian S. Kessler,
Christian S. Kessler1,5*
  • 1Institute of Social Medicine, Epidemiology and Health Economics, Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Corporate Member of Freie Universität Berlin and Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  • 2Department of Internal and Integrative Medicine, Evang. Kliniken Essen-Mitte, Faculty of Medicine, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany
  • 3Australian Research Centre in Complementary and Integrative Medicine, University of Technology Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 4National Centre for Naturopathic Medicine, Southern Cross University, Lismore, NSW, Australia
  • 5Department of Internal and Complementary Medicine, Immanuel Hospital Berlin, Berlin, Germany

Numerous medical studies have documented vegetarian diets as having various health benefits. Studies have also compared vegetarians with other dietary groups from a socio-psychological perspective. The objective of this review is to investigate the differences between vegetarians and omnivores in terms of their personality profiles, values, and empathy skills. A search was conducted across three electronic databases. Non-randomized, observational, cross-sectional, and cohort studies were eligible. Outcomes provided information about the differences between the above-mentioned dietary groups regarding their personality profiles, values, and empathy skills. A shortened version of the Newcastle–Ottawa Scale was used to assess the risk of bias for the included studies. Of the 2,513 different studies found, 25 (total number of participants n = 23,589) were ultimately included. These studies indicate that vegetarians significantly differ from omnivores in their personalities, values, and ability to be empathetic. Omnivorism is associated with an increased orientation toward social dominance, greater right-wing authoritarianism, and, in line with this, a stronger tendency to be prejudiced. Vegetarianism is associated with greater openness and empathy. The values of vegetarians are based more on universalism, hedonism, stimulation, and self-direction, whereas the values of omnivores are based more on the idea of power. To answer a narrowly defined and clear question, issues such as animal ethics, animal rights, and environmental protection are not considered in this review. The findings of this review, showing marked differences in personality correlating to the choice of diet and the increasing influence of plant-based diets on a global level, indicate that further studies about vegetarianism are warranted.

Background

As a form of nutrition, vegetarianism describes the partial or complete omission of various animal products. There are different subgroups (e.g., ovo-lacto vegetarianism and veganism) with partially inconsistent definitions (Beardsworth and Keil, 1991, 1992; Ruby, 2012; Rothgerber, 2015b). Often, and in this review, vegetarianism is defined as the abandonment of all meat and seafood products without exception (Craig and Mangels, 2009). There are other nutritional forms that occupy an intermediate position between omnivorism and vegetarianism. Thus, numerous people describe themselves as flexitarians (those who reduce their consumption of meat with the quality of food playing an important role) (Dagevos and Voordouw, 2013), semi-vegetarians (those who exclude red meat), or pescatarians (those who exclude meat and meat products but eat fish and seafood) (Deutsche Gesellschaft für Ernährung e.V, 2013).

In recent years, and especially in Western countries, vegetarianism has gained increasing widespread attention in medical, ecological, political, and other contexts (Key et al., 2006; Baron, 2013; Orlich et al., 2013; Leitzmann, 2014; Bündnis 90/Die Grünen, 2017; Christoffer et al., 2017; Schweizerische Vereinigung für Vegetarismus, 2017). As such, the proportion of vegetarian people is growing noticeably (APF-VVSQ, 2010; Vegetarierbund Deutschland, 2017; Šimčikas, 2018; Vegetarian Society, 2018). Currently, there are ~7.3 million vegetarians in the US (3.2% of the population) (Vegetarian Times, 2008). A more recent source indicates that ~5% of Americans are vegetarians (Newport, 2012; Statista, 2020). In Germany, there are ~8 million (10% of the German population) vegetarians (Vegetarierbund Deutschland, 2017).

The reasons for adopting a vegetarian diet in the Western world are especially related to health, ethics, and morality (Janda and Trocchia, 2001; Fox and Ward, 2008; Bobic et al., 2012; Ruby, 2012; Hoffman et al., 2013; De Backer and Hudders, 2014). In contrast, the motivations for following a vegetarian way of life in so-called “newly industrialized countries” are, above all, based on religion and are culturally rooted (Preece, 2009; Ruby et al., 2013). In India for example, approximately 20–42% of the population follow a vegetarian diet (Yadav and Kumar, 2006; Ruby et al., 2013; Vegetarierbund Deutschland, 2017). A decisive reason for this is Hinduism, the faith to which ~81% of the population belongs (Albrecht et al., 2017).

Many studies have dealt with these different motivational issues (Mathieu and Dorard, 2016; Rosenfeld and Burrow, 2017b), and the medical examinations of various types of diets that have been conducted in many studies indicate that a well-planned vegetarian diet offers a diverse range of health benefits (Craig and Mangels, 2009; Craig, 2010; Baron, 2013; Orlich et al., 2013; Dinu et al., 2017).

Considering the different motives for adopting a vegetarian diet, vegetarianism is not just an eating behavior, but often part of a way of life. A critical reflection on our eating and consumption behaviors and their associated global problems may cause behavioral changes (e.g., becoming a vegetarian) that could contribute to the solving of current global challenges in terms of the climate crisis, habitat destruction, and others (ProVeg International, 2021). Thus, comparisons of different dietary groups could be relevant to find ways to reduce global meat and other animal-based product consumption. However, these changes can be in conflict with culturally conditioned, long-standing lifestyles that often go hand in hand with certain levels of social prosperity. This results in a potential for social conflict. For example, vegetarians and vegans are still often described as a dominant, intolerant, totalitarian, missionary, and militant (Taufen, 2011; Grau, 2014; Gross, 2017; Herries, 2017; Sotscheck, 2018). In contrast, after a comparatively short research span in this scientific field, the impression arises that vegetarians seem to be more empathetic and less interested in social hierarchical structures and seem to have more altruistic values compared to omnivores (Mitte and Kämpfe-Hargrave, 2007; Filippi et al., 2010; Ruby, 2012; Rothgerber, 2015a). For these reasons, it is important to examine the current evidence in detail and to conduct a comprehensive search within the framework of this systematic review.

This article systematically reviews previous studies that compared omnivores, flexitarians, and semi-vegetarians with vegetarians (including vegans) by rating the differences in their personality profiles, values, and empathy skills. For this purpose, all studies that met the inclusion criteria listed in section Eligibility Criteria and had target outcomes that raised personality traits, value concepts, and/or the ability to be empathetic were considered for inclusion.

Methods

Eligibility Criteria

The present review was conducted according to the guidelines for meta-analyses and systematic reviews of observational studies known as “MOOSE” (meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology; 35). Studies were included in this review based on the following criteria:

a) Types of studies: quantitative, epidemiological, observational studies with a cross-sectional design, and cohort studies were eligible. There were no restrictions on language or year of publication.

b) Types of participants: Studies with adult participants were included (no children or adolescents). Additionally, studies needed to compare individuals following an omnivore, flexitarian, or semi-vegetarian diet with individuals following a vegetarian (ovo-lacto vegetarian or vegan) diet. A vegetarian diet was defined as the absence of the consumption of meat, meat products, fish, and seafood. There were no restrictions regarding the time-period over which participants followed the stated diet.

c) Types of outcomes: Studies were eligible if they compared one (or more) of the above-mentioned dietary group(s) with a vegetarian group and assessed at least one of the following: personality characteristics, value patterns, and/or empathetic ability. For this review, personality was defined according to Carver and Scheier (2011) as “a dynamic organization, inside the person, of psychophysical systems that create the person's characteristic pattern of behavior, thoughts, and feelings.” Personality characteristics were defined as continuous variables that differ between persons and stay, more or less, stable over longer periods of time. Other characteristics that can be manipulated by situational factors or changed through therapeutic interventions, such as food neophobia (Pliner and Hobden, 1992), were not defined as personality traits in this review, although this is discussed controversially. Values were defined as “(a) concepts or beliefs, (b) about desirable end states or behaviors, (c) that transcend specific situations, (d) guide selection or evaluation of behavior and events, and (e) are ordered by relative importance” (Schwartz and Bilsky, 1987). Definitions were agreed upon by all researchers.

Qualitative papers, commentaries, and other reviews were ineligible.

Search Methods

The last update of the complete study search took place on April 19, 2021, with no restrictions on publication year. The search was conducted in three electronic databases (PubMed, Scopus, and PsycINFO).

The search was constructed around the following search terms and is exemplarily shown for the PubMed search:

#1 (“Diet, Vegetarian”[Mesh] OR Vegetarian*[Title/Abstract] OR Vegan*[title/Abstract] OR “meat-free”[Title/Abstract])

#2 (“Empathy”[Mesh] OR Empath*[Title/Abstract] OR Compassion*[Title/Abstract])

#3 (“Personality”[Mesh] OR Personality[Title/Abstract] OR“Big Five”[Title/Abstract]OR Trait*[Title/Abstract]OR openness[Title/Abstract] OR conscientiousness[Title/Abstract]OR extraversion[Title/Abstract]OR introversion[Title/Abstract]OR agreeableness[Title/Abstract]OR neuroticism[Title/Abstract])

#4 (Value*[Title/Abstract]OR “Ethics”[Mesh] OR “Morals”[Mesh] OR “Ethic*”[Title/Abstract]OR “Moral*”[Title/Abstract])

#5 (#2 OR #3 OR #4)

#6 (#1 AND #5)

#7 (animals[Mesh] NOT humans[Mesh])

#8 (#6 NOT #7)

This search strategy was adapted for each database.

Data Extraction and Management

First, all the studies from the searches were classified by the first author of this review as either thematically appropriate or not by reading titles and abstracts. To improve the quality of the screening stage, 5% of all titles and abstracts were randomly screened by a second author for independent agreement purposes, where there was complete agreement. Thereafter, the thematically appropriate studies were either included according to their full texts or excluded due to the characteristics of their participants, outcomes, or design. Potentially eligible studies were examined independently by three authors and decisions concerning the inclusion or exclusion of studies were made collaboratively.

Risk of Bias in Individual Studies

The risk of bias was assessed by two authors independently, using the Newcastle–Ottawa scale (NOS) for case-control studies (Wells et al., 2014). The absence of an exposure factor or a disease in the inherent question for this review and, accordingly, in the included studies, necessitated the use of a shortened variant of the NOS. Thus, the category “exposure” and the subcategory “definition of controls” (in the category “selection”) were excluded. The following categories were examined: “selection” [with the subcategories: “adequate case definition,” “representativeness of the cases,” and “selection of controls” (in each subcategory, a maximum of one star could be awarded)] and “comparability” [with the subcategory “comparability of cases and controls based on the design or analysis” (with a maximum of two stars to rating]) (ClinicalTrials.gov, 2017).

As an exemplary explanation of the procedure and the requirements for the distribution of stars, a transparent presentation of the methods of the individual studies and adequate definitions were emphasized. “Representativeness” was rated with a star if the appropriate nutrition groups were recruited adequately. Stars for the “comparability” of nutritional groups were given when disruptive factors such as age and sex were examined.

Finally, a maximum of five stars could be achieved. A pre-existing rating system was used to classify the studies into corresponding quality grades (Penson et al., 2012). It was adjusted due to the reduction of the maximum number of achievable stars from nine to five stars (*) as follows:

a) Good quality (5 to 3*): 2 or 3 stars in the selection AND 1 or 2 stars in the comparability domain

b) Fair quality (4 to 2*): 1 or 2 stars in the selection AND 1 or 2 stars in the comparability domain

c) Poor quality (1 to 0*): 0 or 1 star in selection OR 0 stars in the comparability domain

No studies were excluded from the review due to a high risk of bias.

Results

Search

The search was conducted over four separate periods of time. A time filter (the date of the previous search until the day of the occurring search) was applied to the three update searches. Thus, it was ensured that the status of the study was current. The search generated a total of 3,321 records, of which 808 were duplicates and were excluded (Figure 1). After investigating titles and abstracts, the remaining 157 records were assessed for eligibility by studying their full texts. One hundred and thirty-two records were excluded based on not meeting all the above-mentioned eligibility criteria (55 by study participants, 62 by outcomes, and 15 by design). Finally, 25 appropriate studies were included in the review, published between 1978 and 2021. Of these 25 studies, six contained two suitable sub-studies each. The latter are partly presented separately in the presentation of results (then mentioned explicitly). If these sub-studies are considered individually, a total of 31 studies were included in this review.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart of results of the search.

Study Characteristics

The characteristics of each study and its participants, outcome measures, and results, are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the included studies.

Setting and Participant Characteristics

Twenty-five observational studies, with a total of 23,589 participants, were included in this review. All studies obtained information about the diets of participants through self-reporting. Of these, a total of 17,403 participants were omnivores (including paleo, gluten-free, weight loss restricted, and occasional omnivores, omnivore fat/cholesterol-avoiders), 4,117 were vegetarians (vegans included), 427 were flexitarians, and 1,484 were semi-vegetarians or pescatarians. Of the remaining 158 participants, the numerical distribution to the different nutritional groups was not apparent to the researchers (Allen et al., 2000: sub-study 1). Sample sizes ranged from 72 (Dhar et al., 2008) to 6,422 (Forestell and Nezlek, 2018), with a median of 386 (336, respectively, when considering each of the two sub-studies of six studies independently (Allen et al., 2000; Lindeman and Sirelius, 2001; Ruby et al., 2013; Piazza et al., 2015; Sariyska et al., 2019; Tan et al., 2021). The mean age of participants ranged from 18.8 (Nezlek et al., 2018) to 51.84 years (Pfeiler and Egloff, 2018), with a median of 30.9 years (31.04 years, respectively), when considering each of the two sub-studies of six studies independently (Allen et al., 2000; Lindeman and Sirelius, 2001; Ruby et al., 2013; Piazza et al., 2015; Sariyska et al., 2019; Tan et al., 2021). Between 52.25 (Ruby et al., 2013) and 100% (Lindeman and Sirelius, 2001; Forestell et al., 2012) of the participants were female, with a median of 68.1%. When considering each of the two sub-studies of six studies independently (Allen et al., 2000; Lindeman and Sirelius, 2001; Ruby et al., 2013; Piazza et al., 2015; Sariyska et al., 2019; Tan et al., 2021), the percentage of women was between 51 (Allen et al., 2000) and 100% (Lindeman and Sirelius, 2001; Forestell et al., 2012), respectively, with a median of 67.7%. In the study conducted by Dhar et al. (2008), no information on gender distribution was provided. Information on the ethnicity of the participants or population groups differed widely and was often not specified. Most of the studies were conducted in industrialized countries (often in the USA). The participants in two of the studies were partially or completely of Indian origin (Dhar et al., 2008; Ruby et al., 2013).

Outcome Measures

Personality characteristics were assessed in nine studies. Values were also assessed in nine studies or, respectively, in 10, when considering sub-studies independently. There was only one study that directly assessed empathy.

Personality

Three studies collected data about right-wing authoritarianism (RWA), namely, Allen et al. (2000) and Ruby et al. (2013). These three studies used the “Right-Wing Authoritarianism Scale” (Altemeyer, 1981), which Veser et al. (2015) collected via Five studies (Allen et al., 2000; Bilewicz et al., 2011; Piazza et al., 2015; Veser et al., 2015; Hopwood and Bleidorn, 2020, short form) investigated social dominance orientation (SDO) using the “Social Dominance Orientation Scale” (Pratto et al., 1994). Furthermore, studies assessed the tendency to be prejudiced through the “Motivation for Prejudice-Free Behavior Scale” (Banse and Gawronski, 2003; Veser et al., 2015), misanthropy (Rothgerber, 2015b), social desirability (Sims, 1978), self-esteem (Nezlek et al., 2018), self-efficacy (Cruwys et al., 2020), and self-control via the “Brief Self-Control Scale” (Tangney et al., 2004; Cruwys et al., 2020), and strictness (Rosenfeld and Burrow, 2018), absolutism, and guilt over violating one's diet (Rothgerber, 2015a). The “Big Five” personality traits (openness to experience, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism) and their aspects were, partly or completely, measured using different questionnaires in seven different studies (Forestell et al., 2012; Forestell and Nezlek, 2018; Kessler et al., 2018; Pfeiler and Egloff, 2018; Cruwys et al., 2020; Hopwood and Bleidorn, 2020; Tan et al., 2021). Forestell et al. (2012) measured variety-seeking behavior. Pfeiler and Egloff (2018) measured trust, patience, impulsivity, risk aversion, and an optimistic attitude about the future. Food neophobia via the “Food Neophobia Scale” (Pliner and Hobden, 1992) and pathogen and moral disgust via the “Three-Domain Disgust Scale” (Tybur et al., 2009) were measured by Cliceri et al. (2018). Seeking, play, care, fear, anger, sadness, spirituality, Machiavellianism, non-pathological narcissism, and non-pathological psychopathy were assessed with the “Short Dark Triad Scale” (Jones and Paulhus, 2014) by Sariyska et al. (2019). Hopwood and Bleidorn (2020) investigated diverse antisocial personality features by measuring the “Big Five” characteristics (see above), SDO (see above), negative affectivity, detachment, psychoticism, antagonism, disinhibition, callousness, self-centeredness, entitlement, and indifference. Lastly, daily affect, depressogenic thinking, and self-focused attention are contents of the study of Nezlek et al. (2018), with emotional eating for the study of Cruwys et al. (2020).

Values

Allen et al. (2000) used the “Rokeach value survey” (Rokeach, 1973). The importance of respect for status and the avoidance of human suffering was assessed by De Backer and Hudders (2015). Cruwys et al. (2020) examined purity, fairness, authority, care, and loyalty (Davies et al., 2014). Both studies used the moral foundation questionnaire of Graham et al. (2011). Purity, fairness, and authority (in addition to universalism and harm), inspired by the five moral foundations of Graham et al. (2009), were also assessed by Ruby et al. (2013) via the “Portrait Value Questionnaire” (Schwartz et al., 2001). Kessler et al. (2018) investigated various values using the “Portraits Value Questionnaire” by Schmidt et al. (2007). Dhar et al. (2008) collected data on ought-to-be and existing values from a cohort of teachers and physicians using the “checklist of values” (Dhar, 1996). Kalof et al. (1999) assessed altruistic and traditional values by means of the “Schwartz value survey” (Schwartz, 1992). The “Schwartz value survey” was also used by Lindeman and Sirelius (2001) to create food choice ideologies. The same study also investigated humanism and normativism using “Tomkins' polarity scale” (De St. Aubin, 1996). Piazza et al. (2015) tested pride and moral self-regard (related to the consumption and use of animal products), and Rothgerber (2015b) assessed ethical idealism through the “ethics position questionnaire” (Forsyth, 1980). The study conducted by Sims (1978) provided information about food-related value orientations, while Pfeiler and Egloff (2018) examined political attitudes. In the study of Trethewey and Jackson (2019), personal health was assessed using a self-designed questionnaire. Daily life satisfaction and the presence of meaning in life were investigated by Nezlek et al. (2018), while current life satisfaction and satisfaction with health were studied by Pfeiler and Egloff (2018). Lastly, Kessler et al. (2018) used the WHO Quality of Life-BREF (World Health Organization, 2014).

Empathy

A total of four studies dealt with the topic of empathy. Preylo and Arikawa (2008) and Cliceri et al. (2018) investigated the ability to feel empathy by examining perspective-taking, fantasy, empathetic concern, and personal distress or disease using the “Interpersonal Reactivity Index” (IRI; Davis, 1983). Kessler et al. (2018) applied the short form of the “Empathizing Scale” (Samson and Huber, 2010), and Hopwood and Bleidorn (2020) examined the lack of empathy via the “Five Factor Narcissism Inventory” (Glover et al., 2012).

Outcomes

In the following analysis, only differences between the dietary groups are reported.

Personality

All three studies that examined RWA found that omnivorism was associated with significantly greater RWA compared to vegetarianism (Allen et al., 2000; Ruby et al., 2013; Veser et al., 2015). All five studies that measured SDO came to a consistent conclusion that omnivorism was associated with a significantly greater SDO than vegetarianism (Allen et al., 2000; Bilewicz et al., 2011; Piazza et al., 2015; Veser et al., 2015; Hopwood and Bleidorn, 2020). In line with these findings, the study conducted by Veser et al. (2015) indicated a significantly higher tendency to be prejudiced among omnivores than among vegetarians. The study by Hopwood and Bleidorn (2020) suggested that, when compared to vegetarians, omnivores may be more callous, self-centered, entitled, indifferent, less agreeable, and had higher scores in agentic values. Appropriately, Sariyska et al. (2019), in their second sub-study, found that Machiavellianism, non-pathological narcissism, and non-pathological psychopathy (here: no significance after controlling for sex) were significantly higher in omnivores than in vegetarians. In two studies (Rothgerber, 2015a,b), vegetarians were not compared to omnivores but to flexitarians who vary between a vegetarian and omnivorous diet. Interestingly, flexitarians score significantly lower on misanthropy in comparison to vegetarians (Rothgerber, 2015b). Furthermore, flexitarians scored significantly lower on absolutism (in strictly following their diet), violated their diets more often, and felt less ethically associated guilt when doing so than vegetarians (Rothgerber, 2015a). Cruwys et al. (2020) primarily investigated factors relating to the adherence to one's diet and found that vegans who were part of the dietary group least likely to state the facilitator of conscientiousness and a lack of willpower as a barrier to adherence, did not base their diet on an individual context. Furthermore, the most frequently stated that they felt no barriers at all, followed by the vegetarian group. A lack of willpower as a barrier to adherence was mentioned second mostly by the paleo group and most often by weight loss dieters along with mood or emotion. Inconvenience as a barrier was reported most frequently by people on a gluten-free diet. Rosenfeld and Burrow (2018) found vegetarians scoring significantly higher in strictness than omnivores. According to the study conducted by Forestell et al. (2012), vegetarians were significantly more open to new experiences and variety-seeking than omnivores. Pfeiler and Egloff (2018) found indications for vegetarians being significantly more open, higher in trust (here: no significance after controlling for socio-demographic variables), and lower in consciousness in comparison to omnivores. In the study of Forestell and Nezlek (2018), vegetarians also appeared to be significantly more open than semi-vegetarians and omnivores, with semi-vegetarians lying in between. Openness was also examined by Tan et al. (2021), with vegans and vegetarians achieving significantly higher scores than omnivores. In addition, this study found vegans and vegetarians scoring significantly higher on intellect and compassion than omnivores. Vegetarians showed a significantly lower pathogen disgust toward infectious agents and a (not significant) lower moral disgust toward antisocial activities than omnivores and flexitarians in the study conducted by Cliceri et al. (2018). Forestell and Nezlek (2018) found omnivores being significantly less neurotic than vegetarians and semi-vegetarians. Compared to omnivores and semi-vegetarians, vegetarians had lower self-esteem, psychological adjustment, stronger negative moods, more negative social events, and more pronounced thoughts about themselves in the study of Nezlek et al. (2018). Sariyska et al. (2019) revealed that vegetarians had significantly higher scores in care, sadness, and spirituality and lower scores in play (here: no significance after Bonferroni correction). Kessler et al. (2018) found no significant differences in personality between the dietary groups among medical professionals.

Values

The outcomes of the reviewed studies were classified by applying the “10 motivational types of values” and by summarizing these values into the following motivational values (Schwartz, 2012):

• Self-transcendence: Universalism, benevolence.

• Conservation: Conformity, tradition, security.

• Self-enhancement: Power, achievement, hedonism.

• Openness to change: Hedonism, stimulation, self-direction.

The findings of the studies were integrated into this model to allow them to be structured according to content. Findings with a p < 0.05 were evaluated as significant and are listed below.

Universalism (Self-transcendence). In comparison to omnivorism, vegetarianism was associated with a significantly greater emphasis on universalism (Ruby et al., 2013, among Euro-Americans, Lindeman and Sirelius, 2001), fairness (Ruby et al., 2013, among Indians and Euro-Americans), social justice, equality, and peace (Allen et al., 2000). Compared to flexitarianism, vegetarianism was associated with a significantly greater belief in the importance of the avoidance of human suffering (De Backer and Hudders, 2015). In comparison to veganism, flexitarianism, and ovo-lacto vegetarianism were associated with a significantly lower emphasis on idealism (Rothgerber, 2015b). In comparison to vegetarianism, omnivorism was associated with a significantly greater emphasis on equity (Allen et al., 2000). Compared to vegetarians, omnivores experienced less moral self-regard (Piazza et al., 2015, related to their consumption and use of animal products). In comparison to omnivores, vegetarians expressed a significantly stronger preference for (food-related) ethics, but omnivores expressed a significantly stronger preference for (food-related) economics and the social and psychological uses of food. The “social-psychological uses of food value-orientation was the most negatively (and ethics most positively) associated with vegetarianism” (Sims, 1978; Filippi et al., 2010).

Benevolence (Self-transcendence). Compared to vegetarians, omnivores stated benevolence as an “ought-to-be” value more frequently (Dhar et al., 2008). Vegetarianism was associated with a significantly greater emphasis on love, while omnivorism was associated with a significantly greater emphasis on responsibility (Allen et al., 2000). Altruistic values were significantly more prevalent among vegetarians than among omnivores (Kalof et al., 1999).

Conformity (Conservation). In comparison to vegetarianism, omnivorism was associated with a significantly greater emphasis on self-control (Allen et al., 2000). Compared to omnivores, vegetarians cited discipline as an existing value more frequently (Dhar et al., 2008). Vegetarians advocated the value of avoiding harm significantly more than omnivores (Ruby et al., 2013, among Indians).

Tradition (Conservation). Vegetarianism was significantly negatively correlated with traditional values (Kalof et al., 1999). The study of Pfeiler and Egloff (2018) found vegetarians scoring significantly lower in conservativism and significantly higher in interest in politics than omnivores. In comparison to omnivores, vegetarians adhered significantly stronger to the food-related value orientation of religion (Sims, 1978).

Security (Conservation). In comparison to omnivores, vegetarians (and vegans) displayed a significantly stronger preference for (food-related) health (Sims, 1978) and personal-health values (Trethewey and Jackson, 2019). Compared to vegetarians, omnivores had a significantly stronger preference for (food-related) familism (Sims, 1978).

Power (Self-enhancement). Compared to vegetarianism, omnivorism was associated with a greater emphasis on social power (Allen et al., 2000). In comparison to flexitarianism, omnivorism was associated with a greater emphasis on the importance of respect for status (De Backer and Hudders, 2015). Among Indians, vegetarians advocated the value of authority significantly more than omnivores; but among Euro-Americans, vegetarians emphasized the values of authority less than omnivores (Ruby et al., 2013). Compared to omnivores, vegetarians stated wealth as an “ought-to-be” value more frequently (Dhar et al., 2008).

Achievement (Self-enhancement). Omnivores experienced less pride (related to their consumption and use of animal products) compared to vegetarians (Piazza et al., 2015). In comparison to omnivorism, vegetarianism was associated with a greater emphasis on growth and intellectualism and a lower emphasis on logic (Allen et al., 2000). Compared to omnivores, vegetarians held a significantly stronger preference for (food-related) education (Sims, 1978).

Hedonism (Self-enhancement and openness to change). Vegetarianism was associated with a greater emphasis on happiness compared to omnivorism (Allen et al., 2000).

Stimulation (Openness to change). Vegetarianism was associated with a greater emphasis on stimulation (Lindeman and Sirelius, 2001) and excitement compared to omnivorism (Allen et al., 2000).

Self-Direction (Openness to change). In comparison to omnivorism, vegetarianism was associated with a greater emphasis on self-direction (Lindeman and Sirelius, 2001).

The study conducted by Lindeman and Sirelius (2001) grouped different values into food choice ideologies. The findings of the authors demonstrated that the ecological ideology (including universalism, stimulation, and self-direction) was positively associated with vegetarianism and a humanistic view of the world. Cruwys et al. (2020) investigated factors relating to the adherence to one's diet, particularly for vegans who were the dietary group most likely and vegetarians who were second most likely to state the adherence facilitators of ethical or moral concerns and identity. Furthermore, vegetarians least frequently stated health as a facilitator. The paleo group most likely reported enjoyment and second most likely reported health as a facilitator. Health as a barrier was reported most frequently by people on a gluten-free diet. Kessler et al. (2018) found no significant differences in values and/or quality of life between the dietary groups among medical professionals. Lower daily life satisfaction with a marginal significance was reported by vegetarians in comparison to omnivores and semi-vegetarians in Nezlek et al. (2018), but no significant differences were found for current life satisfaction and satisfaction with health in Pfeiler and Egloff (2018).

Empathy

The study conducted by Preylo and Arikawa (2008) found that, compared to omnivores, vegetarians scored significantly higher on the subscales for fantasy, personal distress, empathetic concern, and perspective-taking, with the last two being the strongest predictors of vegetarianism. In the study of Cliceri et al. (2018), vegetarians showed significantly higher scores in perspective-taking and (not significantly) in empathic concern, also in comparison to omnivores. Among flexitarians, the values for these two outcomes lay between those of omnivores and vegetarians. De Backer and Hudders (2015) stated that “harm or care focuses on motives to relieve suffering, closely related to empathy” and found that vegetarians put more value on care and empathy compared to flexitarians (Funke, 2003; De Backer and Hudders, 2015). The study of Ruby et al. (2013) found that Indian vegetarians valued the ethic of harm avoidance to a significantly greater degree than Indian omnivores. The indication for omnivores being less empathetic than vegetarians was provided by Hopwood and Bleidorn (2020), while the study by Kessler et al. (2018) found no significant differences in the ability to be empathetic between the dietary groups among medical professionals.

Risk of Bias in Individual Studies

The risks of bias in the individual studies are presented in Table 2. While 13 studies were given four stars (Kalof et al., 1999; Lindeman and Sirelius, 2001; Preylo and Arikawa, 2008; Forestell et al., 2012; De Backer and Hudders, 2015; Veser et al., 2015; Cliceri et al., 2018; Forestell and Nezlek, 2018; Kessler et al., 2018; Pfeiler and Egloff, 2018; Sariyska et al., 2019; Tan et al., 2021), seven studies were given three stars (Allen et al., 2000; Bilewicz et al., 2011; Rothgerber, 2015a; Nezlek et al., 2018; Cruwys et al., 2020; Tan et al., 2021). Seven studies were given two stars (Sims, 1978; Lindeman and Sirelius, 2001; Ruby et al., 2013; Piazza et al., 2015; Rothgerber, 2015b; Rosenfeld and Burrow, 2018), and four studies were given one star (Dhar et al., 2008; Piazza et al., 2015; Trethewey and Jackson, 2019; Hopwood and Bleidorn, 2020). Sub-studies were considered individually. Since the diet of a participant can only be recorded by means of self-reporting in survey-based research, none of the studies could be given five stars (under the subcategory “is the case definition adequate?”). Notably, most of the studies had significantly larger proportions of female participants. Overall, this is associated with a non-negligible risk of bias. In 7 of the 31 included studies (including sub-studies), there was no control for gender (Dhar et al., 2008; Piazza et al., 2015; Rothgerber, 2015a; Rosenfeld and Burrow, 2018; Trethewey and Jackson, 2019; Hopwood and Bleidorn, 2020). Using the NOS, the remaining 24 studies received one star (*) for their consideration of gender distribution in the category “comparability.” Sub-studies were considered individually. According to the quality classes identified in section Risk of Bias in Individual Studies, 19 studies were classified as good, 8 studies as fair, and 4 studies as poor quality. In the case of classification intersections, the study in question was assigned to a higher quality level.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Newcastle-Ottawa scale [76] for case-control studies (* = high quality choice).

Discussion and Conclusions

Summary of Evidence

The results of this systematic review indicate that omnivorism is associated with a greater SDO and RWA when compared to vegetarianism. It was found that omnivorism is associated overall with less openness to new experiences and variety-seeking and with a higher tendency to be prejudiced than vegetarianism. Furthermore, it is indicated that the values of vegetarians are based more on hedonism, universalism, stimulation, and self-direction when compared to omnivores, with the last three concepts leading to a stronger ecological ideology, which is positively associated with a humanistic view of the world. Compared to vegetarians, it cannot be ruled out that the values of omnivores are based more on social power (at least among Western countries) as indicated in the study of Allen et al. (2000). Power, as one of the 10 basic values mentioned in section Values, can be described as its central motivational goal in the sense of “social status and prestige, control or dominance over people and resources” (Schwartz, 2012). Matching indications were found by Hopwood and Bleidorn (2020) and Sariyska et al. (2019). In comparison to omnivores, vegetarians tend to be more neurotic, be lower in self-esteem, and have stronger negative moods (Forestell and Nezlek, 2018; Nezlek et al., 2018). There was no clear trend among the dietary groups regarding values based on achievement, security, conformity, benevolence, or tradition. The review showed a further tendency among vegetarians toward higher empathy than those who follow a diet consisting of meat and meat-based products.

Concordance With Prior Reviews

The findings of this review are partially in line with those of prior reviews that compare dietary habits. In a systematic review by Ruby (2012), diverse findings related to the Western world were presented. Compared to vegetarians, omnivores were found to be more conservative, believed more in traditional values, and revealed a stronger preference for RWA, social hierarchies, and social domination. Compared to omnivores, vegetarians were more liberal and believed more strongly in altruistic values (e.g., environmental protection, equality, and social justice). Vegetarians rejected hierarchical structures, authoritarianism, and violence more frequently and displayed more human-directed empathy than omnivores. In an earlier review, Wilson and Allen (2007) found that vegetarians were less anti-social, more open to new experiences, and ranked emotion as more important than omnivores. Our review also indicates that meat consumption (and, accordingly, omnivorism) might be associated with greater power, inequality, prejudice, hierarchy (e.g., RWA and SDO), and dominance measures and with fewer pro-environmental attitudes. Moreover, Wilson and Allen (2007) present the outcome of a self-conducted study (excluded from this systematic review because of the differentiated consideration of strong, moderate, and weak omnivores) with similar results. Here, strong omnivorism is associated with greater SDO, RWA, and authority measures than vegetarianism.

External and Internal Validity

The studies included in this review were largely conducted in industrialized Western countries (North America, Europe, Australia, and Oceania), except for two studies (Dhar et al., 2008; Ruby et al., 2013) that were (completely or partially) of Indian origin and, therefore, subject to significant religious influence. For this reason, the results of these two studies are of limited use for the question at hand as religiosity can greatly influence the personality and values of people. Moreover, one of the Indian studies was rated as having poor methodological quality. All the participants of the study were adults, and all the studies included vegetarians.

Twenty-three studies included omnivores, and five studies included flexitarians. All studies obtained information about the diets of participants through self-reporting, which appears to be an adequate means of obtaining such information. Another way of collecting the diets of individuals would only be possible in a limited, supervised, and documented framework (e.g., during an in-patient stay). To the extent that the individual studies made it comprehensible, the classification of the participants into various nutritional groups was consistent with the definitions of different nutritional forms (described in section Background). Studies that used inconsistent definitions were excluded. The transferability of the findings is limited by the moderate-to-high risk of bias in some of the studies. The comparability of the included studies was adequate, with all but five sub-studies (at least partially) meeting the appropriate assessment criteria in this category. Finally, it is noteworthy that 21 of the 31 (sub-) studies met them completely.

Limitations

Since the topic of vegetarianism on the one hand and personality, on the other hand, involves a broad spectrum of facets, the selected outcomes might only represent a small part of the full picture. There are many interesting, but also very extensive, aspects of the research fields of nutrition, psychology, and psychiatry. Therefore, and to answer a narrowly defined and clear question, we deliberately omitted the topics listed below. The following limitations of this review must be considered, namely, only studies dealing with healthy people were considered and no outcomes that have primarily pathological relevance were included e.g., depression, appearance dissatisfaction, and eating disorders in Lindeman and Stark (1999). Personality was defined as the totality of the personal, characteristic, and individual qualities of a person. Only the individual values of a person were relevant for this work, not social values and norms (e.g., the link between meat and masculinity in Timeo and Suitner, 2017). Additionally, it should be noted again that, in most of the included studies, the proportion of women was higher than that of men. Therefore, the results for collectives with significantly higher numbers of men could be different. Allen et al. (2000) found men having higher scores on the “vegan-omnivore-scale” (i.e., higher consumption of meat). De Backer and Hudders (2015) displayed flexitarian men compared to omnivore men believing more in the importance of respect for status and authority but found reverse findings for women. In addition, Rothgerber (2015b) found females scoring higher on idealism and lower on misanthropy than men. In the study of Forestell and Nezlek (2018), vegetarians and semi-vegetarians showed a greater tendency toward neuroticism than omnivores only among male participants, whereas women were more neurotic than men in general. The relevance of the above-mentioned differences between the dietary groups, taking the generally higher proportion of women in the studies participants and among vegetarians in general (as already shown in many past studies) into account, remains unclear. Consequently, it is possible that the differences in the measured variables can be explained by gender distribution rather than by genuine differences between the dietary groups. Thus, the general widespread socio-psychological differences between genders play an important role in the evaluation of nutritional studies.

Even though the authors are aware that the topics of animal ethics, animal rights, and environmental protection have a clear relation to the question, corresponding study results (e.g., Lund et al., 2016) were deliberately not considered in this review, as these topics are complex and should be discussed elsewhere. Parallel to this, studies that investigated the motivation for a diet (e.g., Kim et al., 1999; De Boer et al., 2017) were excluded. Additionally, the assessable risk of bias was limited, as not all the criteria of the NOS (Wells et al., 2014) could be applied. Particularly, not all of the categories were assessed, especially the category “exposure,” which could not be considered. Therefore, there were fewer maximum achievable stars, and an adjustment of the rating was needed. Of 31 studies (sub-studies included), four studies were poor and eight studies were of fair quality, representing a further limitation of this review. In addition, the pre-existing rating system used to classify the studies into quality grades was exemplified for the evaluation of the NOS for cohort studies and not for case-control studies (used in this review) (Allen et al., 2000). In addition, the search was carried out only via the three mentioned databases without carrying out additional steps to reduce the risk of publication bias (e.g., hand search for suitable studies, search for gray literature, or contact with societies or individual authors). Furthermore, the missing pre-registration of the review and the absence of a calculation of the inter-coder reliability within the framework of the various steps in the elaboration of this review could be considered as minor methodological weaknesses of this work. Slight discrepancies among the three authors involved in the phase of study selection were resolved unanimously via consensus discussions, but are a potential limitation of this review.

Implications for Further Research and Conclusions

This systematic review indicates that vegetarians differ from omnivores in their personalities, values, and ability to be empathetic. In view of this, and given the increasing influence of a vegetarian diet on an individual and global level, the subject of vegetarianism merits further research. For example, an interesting aspect could also be to further investigate perceptions of others on vegans, vegetarians, and/or meat reducers to compare the extent to which these are consistent with results such as those found in this review. Patel and Buckland (2021) found that meat reducers were perceived as positive overall by others in terms of their social representations and personality traits, which, in some cases, are more positive than vegetarians and habitual meat-eaters. “These results confirm that […] [meat reducers] are an appropriate referent group for use in future social influence-based interventions aiming to reduce meat intake” (Patel and Buckland, 2021). Also, a critical science-based reflection on our way of eating, especially with regard to its global impact on climate, environment, habitats, and humankind, seems very necessary, with references to the current global crises. For example, factory farming and industrial fishing in the context of a quickly growing global demand for animal products have manifold negative effects on human and planetary health: the emission of climate-damaging greenhouse gases (Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change, 2014; Food Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, 2017), the high pollution of the environment with pollutants (Umweltbundesamt, 2015; Food Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, 2018), and the deforestation of (rain-) forests for, among other things, the cultivation of animal feed (Fleischatlas, 2021), to name just a few aspects. Thus, sustainable forms of nutrition should receive more consideration as one of the major contributions to solve these global problems (Willett et al., 2019; Benton et al., 2021). According to a few of the findings, flexitarians seem to occupy an intermediary position between omnivores and vegetarians. For example, Forestell et al. (2012) found vegetarians being more open to new experiences and variety-seeking than flexitarians. It is apparent that the numerical values of flexitarians lie between those of vegetarians and omnivores, albeit much closer to the direction of omnivores. Furthermore, there are indications that ovo-lacto vegetarians often occupy an intermediary position between flexitarians and vegans. For example, flexitarians felt less ethical guilt than ovo-lacto vegetarians, while the latter displayed less ethical guilt than vegans (Rothgerber, 2015a). Additionally, flexitarians and ovo-lacto vegetarians scored significantly lower on idealism than vegans, with no differences between flexitarians and ovo-lacto vegetarians (Rothgerber, 2015b). In contrast, the study of Bilewicz et al. (2011) could not find any clear differences between the ovo-lacto vegetarian and the vegan group. Further, RWA and SDO were positively correlated with the “vegan-omnivore scale” used in the study of Allen et al. (2000). This means that greater vegan identification was associated with lower RWA and SDO that, accordingly, increased with greater omnivore identification. Similar results were presented by Veser et al. (2015). Considering these findings, vegans may be a promising field of research as they may produce even more pronounced outcomes on parameters. By considering the findings of the study of Kessler et al. (2016), one reason for this could be the differences in the motivation for adopting a diet. Furthermore, this study found that vegans (compared to ovo-lacto vegetarians) scored lower on neuroticism and higher on openness and empathy. In terms of their values, vegans scored higher on self-determination and universalism and lower on power, achievement, safety, conformity, and tradition (Kessler et al., 2016). It would be interesting to find out if these distributional tendencies can be confirmed in the framework of other new studies.

In addition, differences between genders and their possible influence on the collected target variables should be given even more attention, as they offer great potential for bias. To examine the relationship of measured outcomes with dietary patterns more precisely, it may be useful to consider the duration of the followed diets more often in future projects. For a better assessment of the risks of bias of the individual studies, it would be very useful to develop an appropriate measurement instrument. The NOS is well-established but not yet adapted for survey-based reviews without any exposure factor.

An investigation on alternative eating habits reveals multiple discrepancies regarding the terminology of the dietary groupings. There are different and confusing definitions of the same diet. The terminology of the various nutritional groups is misleading and worthy of reconsideration. For example, the division between flexitarians and semi-vegetarians is an issue that requires further clarification. In the absence of clear definitions, it is likely that a higher number of people define themselves as vegetarians, which could complicate studies on this topic. Another, albeit cost-intensive method of objectifying the diet status is the measurement of biomarkers in, e.g., blood or urine. This provides one option for increasing the validity of future studies. In general, the transparency of the research process of the individual studies and reviews like the present one could be improved by pre-registering them in the future.

Author's Note

The lead author affirms that this manuscript is an honest, accurate, and transparent account of the study being reported. The reporting of this work is compliant with PRISMA guidelines. The lead author affirms that no important aspects of the study have been omitted and that any discrepancies from the study as planned have been explained.

Author Contributions

SH drafted the manuscript, performed the data extraction, and editing. CK and HC supported the manuscript draft and the data evaluation. MJ, VM, AM, and DS participated in editing the manuscript. The assessment of the risk of bias was done by SH and DL. All authors revised the manuscript critically and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Albiero, P., Ingoglia, S., and Alida, L. C. (2006). Contributo all'adattamento italiano dell'Interpersonal Reactivity Index di Davis. TPM. 13, 107–125.

Google Scholar

Albrecht, B., Aubel, H., Baratta, M., Ell, R., Engler, P., Günther, L., et al. (2017). Der Neue Fischer Weltalmanach 2018 - Zahlen Daten Fakten, ed GmbH SFV (Frankfurt am Main: S. Fischer Verlag GmbH), 736.

Allen, M. W., Wilson, M., Ng, S. H., and Dunne, M. (2000). Values and beliefs of vegetarians and omnivores. J Soc Psychol. 140, 405–422. doi: 10.1080/00224540009600481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altemeyer, B. (1981). Right-Wing Authoritarianism. Winnipeg: University of Manitoba Press.

Google Scholar

Amazon Mechanical Turk (2017). Mechanical Turk is a Marketplace for Work: Amazon.com. Available online at: www.mturk.com (accessed July 30, 2021).

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Google Scholar

APF-VVSQ (2010). A Pound of Flesh. A Survey of 1202 Australians About Whether They're Vegetarian or Vegan and What Their Attitudes to Animals Are. Available online at: https://www.scribd.com/document/26880337/APF-VVSQ (accessed July 30, 2021).

Banse, R., and Gawronski, B. (2003). Die Skala Motivation zu vorurteilsfreiem Verhalten: Psychometrische Eigenschaften und Validität. Diagnostica. 49, 4–13. doi: 10.1026//0012-1924.49.1.4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron, R. B. (2013). Should we all be vegetarians? JAMA Internal Medicine. 173,1238–1239. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.6972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beardsworth, A., and Keil, T. (1992). The vegetarian option: varieties, conversions, motives and careers. Sociol. Rev. 40, 253–293.

Google Scholar

Beardsworth, A. D., and Keil, E. T. (1991). Vegetarianism, veganism, and meat avoidance: recent trends and findings. Br. Food J. 93, 19–24. doi: 10.1108/00070709110135231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects. New York, NY: Harper & Row.

Google Scholar

Benton, T. G., Bieg, C., Harwatt, H., Pudasaini, R., and Wellesley, L. (2021). Food System Impacts on Biodiversity Loss. Three Levers for Food System Transformation in Support of Nature. London: Chatham House.

Google Scholar

Bilewicz, M., Imhoff, R., and Drogosz, M. (2011). The humanity of what we eat: conceptions of human uniqueness among vegetarians and omnivores. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 41, 201–219. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.766

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bobic, J., Cvijetic, S., Baric, I. C., and Satalic, Z. (2012). Personality traits, motivation and bone health in vegetarians. Colleg. Antropol. 36, 795–800.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bündnis 90/Die Grünen (2017). Zukunft wird aus Mut gemacht - Bundestagswahlprogramm 2017. Kapitel D.V.1. – Nachhaltige Konsumentscheidungen ermöglichen. Available online at: https://www.gruene.de/fileadmin/user_upload/Dokumente/BUENDNIS_90_DIE_GRUENEN_Bundestagswahlprogramm_2017_barrierefrei.pdf (accessed July 30, 2021).

Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., and Scheier, M. F. (2011). Perspectives on Personality. Pearson education.

Christoffer, L., Unger, W., and Aumüller, J. (2017). Vebu - Vegan-Trend: Daten und Fakten zum Veggie-Boom: Vegetarierbund Deutschland. Available online at: https://vebu.de/veggie-fakten/entwicklung-in-zahlen/vegan-trend-fakten-zum-veggie-boom/ (accessed July 30, 2021).

Cliceri, D., Spinelli, S., Dinnella, C., Prescott, J., and Monteleone, E. (2018). The influence of psychological traits, beliefs and taste responsiveness on implicit attitudes toward plant-and animal-based dishes among vegetarians, flexitarians and omnivores. Food Qual. Prefer. 68, 276–291. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.03.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ClinicalTrials.gov (2017). U.S. National Library of Medicine. Available online at: https://clinicaltrials.gov/

Google Scholar

Costa, P. T., and McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory. FL: Psychological Assessment Resources Odessa.

Google Scholar

Craig, W. J. (2010). Nutrition concerns and health effects of vegetarian diets. Nutr. Clin. Pract. 25, 613–620. doi: 10.1177/0884533610385707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, W. J., and Mangels, A. R. (2009). Position of the American Dietetic Association: vegetarian diets. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 109, 1266–1282. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2009.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowne, D. P., and Marlowe, D. (1950). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. J. Consult. Psychol. 24, 349–354. doi: 10.1037/h0047358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Cruwys, T., Norwood, R., Chachay, V. S., Ntontis, E., and Sheffield, J. (2020). “An Important Part of Who I am”: the predictors of dietary adherence among weight-loss, vegetarian, vegan, paleo, and gluten-free dietary groups. Nutrients. 12:970. doi: 10.3390/nu12040970

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dagevos, H., and Voordouw, J. (2013). Sustainability and meat consumption: is reduction realistic? Sustainability 9, 60–69. doi: 10.1080/15487733.2013.11908115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, C. L., Sibley, C. G., and Liu, J. H. (2014). Confirmatory factor analysis of the moral foundations questionnaire independent scale validation in a New Zealand sample. Soc. Psychol. 45, 431–436. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, K. L., Panksepp, J., and Normansell, L. (2003). The affective neuroscience personality scales: normative data and implications. Neuropsychoanalysis. 5, 57–69. doi: 10.1080/15294145.2003.10773410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. H. (1983). The effects of dispositional empathy on emotional reactions and helping: A multidimensional approach. J. Pers. 51, 167–184.

Google Scholar

De Backer, C. J., and Hudders, L. (2014). From meatless Mondays to meatless Sundays: motivations for meat reduction among vegetarians and semi-vegetarians who mildly or significantly reduce their meat intake. Ecol. Food Nutr. 53, 639–657. doi: 10.1080/03670244.2014.896797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Backer, C. J. S., and Hudders, L. (2015). Meat morals: relationship between meat consumption consumer attitudes towards human and animal welfare and moral behavior. Meat Sci. 99, 68–74. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2014.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Boer, J., Schosler, H., and Aiking, H. (2017). Towards a reduced meat diet: mindset and motivation of young vegetarians, low, medium and high meat-eaters. Appetite 113, 387–397. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De St. Aubin, E. (1996). Personal ideology polarity: its emotional foundation and its manifestation in individual value systems, religiosity, political orientation, and assumptions concerning human nature. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 152–165.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Deutsche Gesellschaft für Ernährung e.V (2013). Flexitarier — die flexiblen Vegetarier. DGEinfo. 10, 146–148.

DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., and Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the big five. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93:880. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhar, U. (1996). Manual for the Values Inventory. Indore: PIMR.

Dhar, U., Parashar, S., and Tiwari, T. (2008). Profession and dietary habits as determinants of perceived and expected values: an empirical study. J. Human Values. 14, 181–190. doi: 10.1177/097168580801400208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinu, M., Abbate, R., Gensini, G. F., Casini, A., and Sofi, F. (2017). Vegetarian, vegan diets and multiple health outcomes: a systematic review with meta-analysis of observational studies. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr. 57, 3640–3649. doi: 10.1080/10408398.2016.1138447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., and Sunde, U. (2008). Representative trust and reciprocity: prevalence and determinants. Econ. Inquiry. 46, 81–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1465-7295.2007.00082.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., Sunde, U., Schupp, J., and Wagner, G. G. (2011). Individual risk attitudes: measurement, determinants and behavioral consequences. J. Euro. Econ. Assoc. 9, 522–550. doi: 10.1111/j.1542-4774.2011.01015.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donnellan, M. B., Oswald, F. L., Baird, B. M., and Lucas, R. E. (2006). The mini-IPIP scales: Tiny-yet-e effective measures of the Big Five factors of personality. Psychol. Assess. 18, 192–203. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.2.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman Barrett, L., and Russell, J. A. (1998). Independence and bipolarity in the structure of current affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 967–984.

Google Scholar

Filippi, M., Riccitelli, G., Falini, A., Di Salle, F., Vuilleumier, P., Comi, G., et al. (2010). The brain functional networks associated to human and animal suffering differ among omnivores, vegetarians and vegans. PLoS ONE. 5:e10847. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleischatlas (2021). Daten und Fakten über Tiere als Nahrungsmittel. Berlin: Heinrich-Böll-Stiftung.

Food Agriculture Organization of the United Nations (2017). Global Livestock Environmental Assessment Model (GLEAM) – Results. Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations. Available online at: http://www.fao.org/gleam/results/en/

Google Scholar

Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations (2018). The State of Food Security and Nutrition in the World 2018. Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations.

Google Scholar

Forestell, C. A., and Nezlek, J. B. (2018). Vegetarianism, depression, and the five factor model of personality. Ecol. Food Nutr. 57, 246–259. doi: 10.1080/03670244.2018.1455675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forestell, C. A., Spaeth, A. M., and Kane, S. A. (2012). To eat or not to eat red meat. A closer look at the relationship between restrained eating and vegetarianism in college females. Appetite 58, 319–325. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forsyth, D. R. (1980). A taxonomy of ethical ideologies. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39, 175–184.

Google Scholar

Fox, N., and Ward, K. (2008). Health, ethics and environment: a qualitative study of vegetarian motivations. Appetite 50, 422–429. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funke, F. (2003). Die dimensionale Struktur von Autoritarismus [The dimensional structure of authoritarianism] (Dissertation). Jena: Friedrich-Schiller-Universität.

Google Scholar

Gerlitz, J.-Y., and Schupp, J. (2005). Zur Erhebung der Big-Five-basierten Persönlichkeitsmerkmale im SOEP. DIW Research Notes, 4. Available online at: https://www.diw.de/de/diw_01.c.449451.de/publikationen/research_notes/2005_0004/zur_erhebung_der_big-five-basierten_persoenlichkeitsmerkmale_im_soep.html (accessed July 30, 2021).

Google Scholar

Glover, N., Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Crego, C., and Widiger, T. A. (2012). The five-factor narcissism inventory: A five-factor measure of narcissistic personality traits. J. Personal. Assess. 94, 500–512. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2012.670680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberg, L. R. (1999). “Personality Psychology in Europe,” in A Broad-Bandwidth, Public-Domain, Personality Inventory Measuring the Lower-Level Facets of Several Five-Factor Models, eds I. Mervielde, F. Deary, D. Fruyt, and F. Ostendorf (Tilburg: Tilburg University Press), 7–28.

Google Scholar

Graham, J., Haidt, J., and Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. J. Personal. Social Psychol. 96, 1029–1046. doi: 10.1037/a0015141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., and Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. J. Pers. Social Psychol. 101, 366–385. doi: 10.1037/a0021847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grau, A. (2014). Veganer sind moralische Totalitaristen. Available online at: https://www.cicero.de/kultur/veggie-kult-gemuese-buergertum-veganer-sind-moralische-totalitaristen/57512 (accessed July 30, 2021).

Gross, G. (2017). Warum Veganer und Vegetarier so Polarisieren. Available online at: https://www.welt.de/vermischtes/article162311133/Warum-Veganer-und-Vegetarier-so-polarisieren.html (accessed July 30, 2021).

Hahn, E., Gottschling, J., and Spinath, F. M. (2012). Short measurements of personality: Validity and reliability of the GSOEP Big Five Inventory (BFI-S). J. Res. Pers. 46, 355–359. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.03.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herries, C. (2017). The Rise of the Intolerant: Vegans. Part I of a Series: What Vegans, Islamic Radicalism, and Politically Correct Comedy Have in Common. Available online at: https://medium.com/muddledminds/the-rise-of-the-intolerant-case-study-1-vegans-3a5397d71a1e (accessed July 30, 2021).

Hoeppner, B. B., Kelly, J. F., Urbanoski, K. A., and Slymaker, V. (2011). Comparative utility of a single-item vs. multiple-item measure of self-efficacy in predicting relapse among young adults. J. Subst. Abuse Treat. 41, 305–312. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2011.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, S. R., Stallings, S. F., Bessinger, R. C., and Brooks, G. T. (2013). Differences between health and ethical vegetarians. Strength of conviction, nutrition knowledge, dietary restriction, and duration of adherence. Appetite 65, 139–144. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopwood, C. J., and Bleidorn, W. (2020). Antisocial personality traits transcend species. Personal. Disord. 18, 1205–1240. doi: 10.1037/per0000463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (2014). Climate Change 2014. Mitigation of Climate Change: Working Group III Contribution to the Fifth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Janda, S., and Trocchia, P. J. (2001). Vegetarianism: toward a greater understanding. Psychol. Market. 18, 1205–1240. doi: 10.1002/mar.1050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., and Kentle, R. L. (1991). The Big Five Inventory— Versions 4a and 54. Berkeley, CA: University of California at Berkeley; Institute of Personality and Social Research.

Jones, D. N., and Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Introducing the short dark triad (SD3) a brief measure of dark personality traits. Assessment 21, 28–41. doi: 10.1177/1073191113514105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47, 263–291. doi: 10.2307/1914185

CrossRef Full Text

Kalof, L., Dietz, T., Stern, P. C., and Guagnano, G. A. (1999). Social psychological and structural influences on vegetarian beliefs. Rural Sociol. 64, 500–511. doi: 10.1111/j.1549-0831.1999.tb00364.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kashdan, T. B., and Nezlek, J. B. (2012). Whether, when, and how is spirituality related to wellbeing? Moving beyond single occasion questionnaires to understanding daily process. Pers. Social Psychol. Bull. 38, 1523–1535. doi: 10.1177/0146167212454549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, C. S., Holler, S., Joy, S., Dhruva, A., Michalsen, A., Dobos, G., et al. (2016). Personality profiles, values and empathy: differences between lactoovo-vegetarians and vegans. Forschende Komplementarmedizin 23, 95–102. doi: 10.1159/000445369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, C. S., Michalsen, A., Holler, S., Murthy, V. S., and Cramer, H. (2018). How empathic are vegan medical professionals compared to others? Leads from a paper–pencil-survey. Euro. J. Clin. Nutr. 72, 780–784. doi: 10.1038/s41430-017-0007-

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Key, T. J., Appleby, P. N., and Rosell, M. S. (2006). Health effects of vegetarian and vegan diets. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 65, 35–41. doi: 10.1079/PNS2005481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, E. H., Schroeder, K. M., Houser, R. F. Jr., and Dwyer, J. T. (1999). Two small surveys, 25 years apart, investigating motivations of dietary choice in 2 groups of vegetarians in the Boston area. J. Am. Diet. Soc. 99, 598–601. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(99)00147-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, F. R., John, D., Lüdtke, O., Schupp, J., and Wagner, G. G. (2011). Short assessment of the Big Five: robust across survey methods except telephone interviewing. Behav. Res. Methods. 43, 548–567. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0066-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laureati, M., Spinelli, S., Monteleone, E., Prescott, J., et al. (2018). Associations between food neophobia and responsiveness to “warning” chemosensory sensations in food products in a large population sample. Food Qual. Pref. 68, 113–124. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.02.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leitzmann, C. (2014). Vegetarian nutrition: past, present, future. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 100, 496S−502S. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.071365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindeman, M., and Sirelius, M. (2001). Food choice ideologies: the modern manifestations of normative and humanist views of the world. Appetite 37, 175–184. doi: 10.1006/appe.2001.0437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindeman, M., and Stark, K. (1999). Pleasure, pursuit of health or negotiation of identity? Personality correlates of food choice motives among young and middle-aged women. Appetite 33, 141–161. doi: 10.1006/appe.1999.0241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lund, T. B., McKeegan, D. E. F., Cribbin, C., and Sandøe, P. (2016). Animal ethics profiling of vegetarians, vegans and meat-eaters. Anthrozoos 29, 89–106. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2015.1083192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynam, D. R., Gaughan, E. T., Miller, J. D., Miller, D. J., Mullins-Sweatt, S., and Widiger, T. A. (2011). Assessing the basic traits associated with psychopathy: development and validation of the Elemental Psychopathy Assessment. Psychol. Assess. 23, 108–124. doi: 10.1037/a0021146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maples-Keller, J. L., Williamson, R. L., Sleep, C. E., Carter, N. T., Campbell, W. K., and Miller, J. D. (2019). Using item response theory to develop a 60-item representation of the NEO PI–R using the international personality item pool: development of the IPIP–NEO−6. J. Pers. Assess. 101, 4–15. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2017.1381968

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathieu, S., and Dorard, G. (2016). Vegetarianism and veganism lifestyle: motivation and psychological dimensions associated with selective diet. Presse Med. 45, 726–733. doi: 10.1016/j.lpm.2016.06.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R., and Costa, P. T. (2004). A contemplated revision of the NEO Five-Factor Inventory. Pers. Ind. Diff. 36, 587–596. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00118-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitte, K., and Kämpfe-Hargrave, N. (2007). Vegetarierstudie der Universität Jena. Available online at: www.vegetarierstudie.uni/jena.de (accessed July 30, 2021).

Newport, F. (2012). In U.S., 5% Consider Themselves Vegetarians. Available online at: http://www.gallup.com/poll/156215/consider-themselves-vegetarians.aspx (accessed July 30, 2021).

Nezlek, J. B., Forestell, C. A., and Newman, D. B. (2018). Relationships between vegetarian dietary habits and daily well-being. Ecol. Food Nutr. 57, 425–438. doi: 10.1080/03670244.2018.1536657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oishi, S., Diener, E., Choi, D. W., Kim-Prieto, C., and Choi, I. (2007). The dynamics of daily events and well-being across cultures: when less is more. J. Pers. Social Psychol. 93:685. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.4.685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orlich, M. J., Singh, P. N., Sabat,é, J., Jaceldo-Siegl, K., Fan, J., Knutsen, S., et al. (2013). Vegetarian dietary patterns and mortality in Adventist Health Study 2. JAMA Intern. Med. 173, 1230–1238. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.6473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, V., and Buckland, N. J. (2021). Perceptions about meat reducers: Results from two UK studies exploring personality impressions and perceived group membership. Food Qual. Preference 93:104289. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104289

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penson, D. F., Krishnaswami, S., Jules, A., Seroogy, J. C., and McPheeters, M. L. (2012). Evaluation and Treatment of Cryptorchidism. Newcastle-Ottawa Quality Assessment Form for Cohort Studies. Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (US)

Pfeiler, T. M., and Egloff, B. (2018). Examining the “Veggie” personality: results from a representative German sample. Appetite 120, 246–255. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piazza, J., Ruby, M. B., Loughnan, S., Luong, M., Kulik, J., Watkins, H. M., et al. (2015). Rationalizing meat consumption. The 4Ns. Appetite 91, 114–128. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pliner, P., and Hobden, K. (1992). Development of a scale to measure the trait of food neophobia in humans. Appetite 19, 105–120. doi: 10.1016/0195-6663(92)90014-W

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., and Malle, B. F. (1994). Social dominance orientation: a personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 741–763. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.741

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preece, R. (2009). Sins of the Flesh: A History of Ethical Vegetarian Thought. Vancouver: UBC Press.

Google Scholar

Preylo, B. D., and Arikawa, H. (2008). Comparison of vegetarians and non-vegetarians on pet attitude and empathy. Anthrozoos. 21, 387–395. doi: 10.2752/175303708X371654

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ProVeg International (2021). Wie sich die industrielle Tierhaltung auf den Klimawandel auswirkt. Available online at: https://proveg.com/de/5-pros/pro-umwelt/wie-sich-die-industrielle-tierhaltung-auf-den-klimawandel-auswirkt/ (accessed July 30, 2021).

Reuter, M., Panksepp, J., Davis, K., and Montag, C. (2017). Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) - Deutsche Version. Göttingen: Hogrefe-Verlag.

Google Scholar

Rokeach, M. (1973). The Nature of Human Values. New York, NY: Free Press.

Google Scholar

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Rosenfeld, D. L., and Burrow, A. L. (2017a). The unified model of vegetarian identity: a conceptual framework for understanding plant-based food choices. Appetite 112, 78–95. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenfeld, D. L., and Burrow, A. L. (2017b). Vegetarian on purpose: understanding the motivations of plant-based dieters. Appetite 116, 456–463. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenfeld, D. L., and Burrow, A. L. (2018). Development and validation of the Dietarian Identity Questionnaire: assessing self-perceptions of animal-product consumption. Appetite 127, 182–194. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothgerber, H. (2015a). Can you have your meat and eat it too? Conscientious omnivores, vegetarians, and adherence to diet. Appetite 84, 196–203. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothgerber, H. (2015b). Underlying differences between conscientious omnivores and vegetarians in the evaluation of meat and animals. Appetite 87, 251–258. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.12.206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruby, M. B. (2012). Vegetarianism. A blossoming field of study. Appetite 58, 141–50. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruby, M. B., Heine, S. J., Kamble, S., Cheng, T. K., and Waddar, M. (2013). Compassion and contamination. Cultural differences in vegetarianism. Appetite 71, 340–348. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samson, A. C., and Huber, O. W. (2010). Short German versions of empathizing and systemizing self-assessment scales. Swiss J. Psychol. 69, 239–244. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185/a000028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sariyska, R., Markett, S., Lachmann, B., and Montag, C. (2019). What does our personality say about our dietary choices? Insights on the associations between dietary habits, primary emotional systems and the Dark Triad of personality. Front. Psychol. 10:2591. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schimmack, U., Krause, P., Wagner, G. G., and Schupp, J. (2009). Stability and change of wellbeing: an experimentally enhanced Latent State-Trait-Error Analysis. Soc. Indic. Res. 95, 19–31. doi: 10.1007/s11205-009-9443-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, P., Bamberg, S., Davidov, E., Herrmann, J., and Schwartz, S. H. (2007). Die Messung von Werten mit dem “Portraits Value Questionnaire”. Zeitschrift Sozialpsychologie. 38, 261–275. doi: 10.1024/0044-3514.38.4.261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schupp, J., Richter, D., Goebel, J., Kroh, M., Schröder, C., Liebau, E., et al. (2016). SOEP Innovationssample (SOEP-IS), Daten der Jahre 1998-2014. Berlin: DIW Berlin. doi: 10.5684/soep.is.2014

CrossRef Full Text

Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 25, 1–65.

Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. H. (2012). An overview of the Schwartz theory of basic values. Online Read. Psychol. Culture. 2:11. doi: 10.9707/2307-0919.1116

CrossRef Full Text

Schwartz, S. H., and Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward a universal psychological structure of human values. J. Pers. Social Psychol. 53, 550–562. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.3.550

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. H., Melech, G., Lehmann, A., Burgess, S., Harris, M., and Owens, V. (2001). Extending the cross-cultural validity of the theory of basic human values with a different method of measurement. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 32, 519–542. doi: 10.1177/0022022101032005001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schweizerische Vereinigung für Vegetarismus (2017). Ökologische Folgen des Fleischkonsums. Available online at: http://www.vegetarismus.ch/pdf/b05.pdf (accessed July 30, 2021).

Šimčikas, S. (2018). Is the Percentage of Vegetarians and Vegans in the U.S. Increasing? Available online at: https://animalcharityevaluators.org/blog/is-the-percentage-of-vegetarians-and-vegans-in-the-u-s-increasing/#summary (accessed July 30, 2021).

Sims, L. S. (1978). Food-related value-orientations, attitudes, and beliefs of vegetarians and non-vegetarians. Ecol. Food Nutr. 7, 23–35. doi: 10.1080/03670244.1978.9990508

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sindermann, C., Sariyska, R., Lachmann, B., Brand, M., and Montag, C. (2018). Associations between the dark triad of personality and unspecified/specific forms of Internet-use disorder. J. Behav. Addict. 7, 985–992.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sotscheck, R. (2018). Aufgehübschter Dreck für Veganer - Veganismus ist ein Kult reicher Mittelschichtskinder, die einen missionieren wollen – jetzt auch noch auf der Grünen Insel! Available online at: https://taz.de/Die-Wahrheit/!5489455/ (accessed July 30, 2021).

Statista (2020). Halten Sie sich an eine oder mehrere der folgenden Ernährungsweisen? Available online at: https://de.statista.com/prognosen/1000398/usa-verbreitete-ernaehrungsweisen (accessed July 30, 2021).

SurveyMonkey (2017). Available online at: https://www.surveymonkey.de/ (accessed July 30, 2021).

Tan, N. P., Conner, T. S., Sun, H., Loughnan, S., and Smillie, L. D. (2021). Who gives a veg? Relations between personality and Vegetarianism/Veganism. Appetite 163:105195. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., and Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J. Pers. 72, 271–324. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taufen, A. (2011). An Open Letter to Militant Vegans. Available online at: https://www.westword.com/restaurants/an-open-letter-to-militant-vegans-5739899 (accessed July 30, 2021).

Timeo, S., and Suitner, C. (2017). Eating meat makes you sexy: conformity to dietary gender norms and attractiveness. Psychol. Men Mascul. 19, 418–429. doi: 10.1037/men0000119

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trapnell, P. D., and Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: distinguishing rumination from reflection. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 284–304.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Trethewey, E., and Jackson, M. (2019). Values and cognitive mechanisms: comparing the predictive factors of Australian meat intake. Appetite 142:104386. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trommsdorff, G. (1994). “Zukunft als Teil individueller Handlungsorientierungen,” in Erwartungen an die Zukunft. Zeithorizonte und Wertewandel in der sozialwissenschaftlichen Diskussion, eds E. Holst, J. P. Rinderspacher, and J. Schupp (Campus: Frankfurt a. M.), 45–76.

Google Scholar

Tybur, M. J., Liebeman, D., and Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating and morality: individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 103–122. doi: 10.1037/a0015474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Umweltbundesamt (2015). Umweltprobleme der Landwirtschaft. Deutschland: Dessau-Roßlau.

Van Trijp, H. C. M., and Steenkamp, J.-B. E. M. (1992). Consumers' variety seeking tendency with respect to foods: measurement and managerial implications. Eur. Rev. Agr. Econ. 19, 181–195.

Google Scholar

Vegetarian Society (2018). Veggie Living Fact Sheets: Statistics. Available online at: https://www.vegsoc.org/page.aspx?pid=755 (accessed July 30, 2021).

Vegetarian Times (2008). Vegetarianism In America. Available online at: https://www.vegetariantimes.com/uncategorized/vegetarianism-in-america (accessed July 30, 2021).

Vegetarierbund Deutschland (2017). Anzahl der Veganer und Vegetarier in Deutschland. Available online at: https://vebu.de/veggie-fakten/entwicklung-in-zahlen/anzahl-veganer-und-vegetarier-in-deutschland/ (accessed July 30, 2021).

Veser, P., Taylor, K., and Singer, S. (2015). Diet, authoritarianism, social dominance orientation, and predisposition to prejudice Results of a German survey. British Food Journal. 117, 1949–1960. doi: 10.1108/BFJ-12-2014-0409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vischer, T., Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., Schupp, J., Sunde, U., et al. (2013). Validating an ultra-short survey measure of patience. Econ. Lett. 120, 142–145. doi: 10.1016/j.econlet.2013.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Collani, G. (2002). Das Konstrukt der Sozialen Dominanzorientierung als generalisierte Einstellung: eine Replikation. Zeitschrift für Politische Psychologie. 10, 262–283.

Wagner, G. G., Frick, J. R., and Schupp, J. (2007). The German socio-economic panel study (SOEP) - evolution, scope and enhancements (July 1, 2007). SOEP Paper No. 1. Berlin: DIW Berlin. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.1028709

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wardle, J. (1987). Eating style: a validation study of the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire in normal subjects and women with eating disorders. J. Psychosom. Res. 31, 161–169.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Wells, G. A., Shea, B., O'Connell, D., Peterson, J., Welch, V., Losos, M., et al. (2014). The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for Assessing the Quality of Nonrandomised Studies in Meta-Analyses. Available online at: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp (accessed July 30, 2021).

Google Scholar

Willett, W., Rockstrom, J., Loken, B., Springmann, M., Lang, T., Vermeulen, S., et al. (2019). Food in the anthropocene: the EAT-Lancet commission on healthy diets from sustainable food systems. Lancet. 393, 447–492. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)31788-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, M. S., and Allen, M. W. (2007). Social Psychological Motivations and Foundations of Dietary Preference, ed L. Brown (New York, NY), 65–82.

World Health Organization (2014). Management of Substance Abuse. WHO Quality of Life-BREF (WHOQOL-BREF). Available online at: www.who.int/substance_abuse/research_tools/whoqolbref/en (accessed July 30, 2021).

Wuensch, K. L., Jenkins, K. W., and Poteat, G. M. (2002). Misanthropy, idealism and attitudes towards animals. Anthrozoos. 15, 139–149. doi: 10.2752/089279302786992621

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yadav, Y., and Kumar, S. (2006). The Food Habits of a Nation. Available online at: http://www.thehindu.com/todays-paper/the-food-habits-of-a-nation/article3089973.ece.9 (accessed July 30, 2021).

Keywords: vegetarian, plant-based, omnivore, diet, personality, values, empathy

Citation: Holler S, Cramer H, Liebscher D, Jeitler M, Schumann D, Murthy V, Michalsen A and Kessler CS (2021) Differences Between Omnivores and Vegetarians in Personality Profiles, Values, and Empathy: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 12:579700. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.579700

Received: 03 July 2020; Accepted: 31 August 2021;
Published: 07 October 2021.

Edited by:

Nicholas T. Bello, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, United States

Reviewed by:

Tamlin S. Conner, University of Otago, New Zealand
Nicola Joy Buckland, The University of Sheffield, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2021 Holler, Cramer, Liebscher, Jeitler, Schumann, Murthy, Michalsen and Kessler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Christian S. Kessler, Y2hyaXN0aWFuLmtlc3NsZXImI3gwMDA0MDtjaGFyaXRlLmRl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.