Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 13 August 2021
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19): Psychoeducational Variables Involved in the Health Emergency View all 30 articles

A Path Analysis Model of Protection and Risk Factors for University Academic Stress: Analysis and Psychoeducational Implications for the COVID-19 Emergency

  • 1School of Education and Psychology, University of Navarra, Pamplona, Spain
  • 2School of Psychology, University of Almería, Almería, Spain

The aim of this research was to empirically validate hypothesized predictive relationships of protection and risk factors for experiencing academic stress. A synthesis of models—the presage–process–product model; the studying, learning and performing under stress competency model; and self- vs. external-regulatory theory—underlies the investigation and is important for assessment and guidance in stress situations within the university context. Over the course of an academic year, a sample of 564 Spanish university students voluntarily completed validated questionnaires, in an online format, on several psychological variables connected to academic stress. Correlational analysis and the path analysis model, within an ex post facto design, were used to build empirical models of the presage–process–product factors that constitute protection or risk factors in academic stress. Two statistically acceptable models appeared: one with protection factors and another with risk factors in predicting and preventing academic stress at a university. These results support the need for psychology units at university that have a preventive, health and education focus, going beyond the merely clinical. Focus on an individual is insufficient, given that there are also contextual factors that predispose academic stress. Discussion, conclusions, and implications for assessment and intervention in academic stress in university students and teachers, within the present COVID-19 crisis, are offered.

Introduction

Human beings require learning experiences in order to restructure their knowledge and their ways of interacting with reality; today, COVID-19 has become such an experience—unusual, unexpected, but common among us all. In the field of healthcare, it is an object of analysis and learning. It is obvious that COVID-19 has all the components of a health and medical-biological emergency, just as what was declared by the WHO. The configuration, functionality, and structure of this fast-spreading biological entity are not yet clearly understood. Knowledge has been lacking about its primary care, through conventional pharmacological prevention (vaccines), and its secondary or tertiary care (pharmacological treatment, ventilators, etc.). As a consequence, the disease is now pandemic and growing by geometric progression.

To evaluate and intervene in behavioral variables (psychoeducational and psychosocial) is the business of psychology as a behavioral science. It is time to recognize that many health-related issues have both a medical-biological component and a psychosocial component (behavioral, personal, and contextual). We must learn that medicine, biology, and psychology should work together on epidemiological and health-related issues from an integrated, biopsychosocial model (de la Fuente, 2020a; Frazier, 2020).

In the context of today, educational psychology—as a specialized branch of scientific knowledge in psychology—can contribute its own elements and models in the realm of academic stress. This article thus has a 3-fold aim: (1) present a conceptual–synthesis model or heuristic based on previous conceptual models that have provided evidence (Slavin, 2019); (2) empirically demonstrate the hypothesized relationships in university students; (3) present implications and proposals for intervention to help educational psychologists evaluate and advise students, teachers, and university institutions during the COVID-19 health crisis. An example of this purpose is the Research Topic in which the present research report is included (de la Fuente et al., 2021a).

In a complementary fashion, this exploratory study seeks to offer an empirical and conceptual synthesis of different theoretical models in the line of research that analyzes variables connected with stress behavior in the university context. A few partial contributions have been put forward to date, which we seek to integrate into this final model or heuristic.

The 3P Model for Analysis Within the COVID-19 Health Emergency

The 3P (presage–process–product) model, or theory of Biggs of student approaches to learning (SAL) (Biggs, 1970a, 1985, 1993, 1999), is an essential conceptual heuristic for addressing the prevention of academic stress, particularly in the context of the COVID-19 health crisis. It presents a systemic view of university teaching and learning processes and has become one of the seminal models that are most prevalent in the literature (Barattucci et al., 2017; Ginns et al., 2018; Kember et al., 2020):

1) Presage variables. Evidence has demonstrated the existence of different presage variables of university learning and academic achievement. Biggs himself proposed influence coming from personality factors (Biggs, 1970a) and factors of the faculty context (Biggs, 1970b) as precursors to the learning approach of the students at a university (Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2007; Ginns et al., 2018). Positive psychology has recently contributed new elements for consideration, such as positivity as dispositional optimism (Caprara and Steca, 2005; Caprara et al., 2006, 2009, 2010, 2011; Alessandri et al., 2012). Relationships have also appeared between personality and academic confidence (Sander and de la Fuente, 2020).

2) Process variables. The core research, using this model, has historically focused on learning approaches as an essential process variable (Biggs, 1972, 1973, 1976, 1978, 1985, 1987). Classical cognitive research established associations and predictive relationships with cognitive variables like learning strategies, metacognitive processes, and self-regulated learning (Heikkilä and Lonka, 2006; de la Fuente et al., 2008). This paradigm has evolved toward the study of emotional and affective factors in our day, establishing relationships between learning approaches and several variables of this type (Trigwell and Ashwin, 2003; Trigwell, 2006, 2012; Trigwell et al., 2012, 2013).

3) Product variables. Finally, the product variable is understood to be achievement or satisfaction, and previous research showed a predictive relationship between learning approaches and achievement (Karagiannopoulou et al., 2018).

The 3P model can serve as a general heuristic for evaluating complex realities, such as that of COVID-19 in the university context. However, despite the wide range of evidence and research on this topic, an essential aspect of reality has not been thoroughly analyzed, namely, the high level of environmental and personal stress that exists in this context. This limitation has given rise to other complementary heuristics that address elements of the model that can be improved. Such is the case of the SLPS Competency Model (de la Fuente, 2015a).

Competence in Studying, Learning, and Performing Under Stress as a Model for Analysis Within the COVID-19 Health Emergency

The (Original) SLPS Competency Model (V.1)

The educational psychology model of competence in studying, learning, and performing under stress (SLPS) (de la Fuente, 2015a) is based conceptually on the Gagné instructional model (Gagné, 1985), taking into account three levels of learning that are required to be competent. It focuses on the process variables of the 3P model (Biggs, 1993), since it establishes behaviors that make up appropriate repertories for dealing with academic stress situations. Given the current COVID-19 crisis, it seems reasonable that this model can be useful to evaluate and intervene with university students who so require. See Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The competency model of studying, learning, and performing under stress, SLPS original (de la Fuente, 2015a).

The learning approaches variable was considered a meta-learning variable by Biggs himself (1985). Both the theory of learning approaches (Biggs, 1993; Asikainen and Gijbels, 2017) and its assessment instrument (Biggs et al., 2001) have become established internationally. Recent research has consistently shown that deep approach is associated with better learning and achievement at a university, while surface approach is associated with poorer university learning and achievement (Cetin, 2015; Asikainen and Gijbels, 2017). Learning approach has recently been related to coping strategies and resilience, with deep approach related to problem-focused strategies and high resilience, and surface approach to emotion-focused strategies and low resilience (de la Fuente et al., 2017a; Banerjee et al., 2019). Relationships have recently been established between learning approach (deep vs. surface) and achievement emotions (positive vs. negative), respectively (de la Fuente et al., 2020e).

Self-regulation, as a meta-behavioral variable, has also been related to a number of variables: in positive association with type of coping strategies used (de la Fuente, 2020a,b), positive achievement emotions (de la Fuente et al., 2020c), academic behavioral confidence (de la Fuente et al., 2020f), and deep learning approach (de la Fuente et al., 2021c), and in negative association with procrastination (Garzón-Umerenkova et al., 2018).

Coping strategies, as a meta-affective variable, have also shown a relation to the states of engagement burnout (de la Fuente et al., 2015a). Moreover, a recently proposed relationship model, including achievement emotions, emotion- vs. problem-focused coping strategies, and ultimate state of engagement burnout, has also acted as a potential 2-fold mechanism in positive vs. negative perfectionism (de la Fuente et al., 2020b). Coping strategies have also been related to the self-regulation characteristics of students (Amate-Romera and de la Fuente, 2021).

Resilience also has been studied as a meta-motivational variable, mediating between personality characteristics and perceived stress (de la Fuente et al., 2021f). Other studies examine its predictive value for coping strategies and the motivational states of engagement burnout (de la Fuente et al., 2021d).

Achievement emotions likewise have been studied widely in recent research, with much important evidence. Positive and negative relationships have been verified in different stress situations according to the source of the stress triggers (related to class, study time, or testing) (de la Fuente et al., 2020c).

Regarding academic behavioral confidence (Sander and Sanders, 2006, 2009; Sander, 2009; Sander et al., 2011), prior research showed its positive relationship to deep learning approach and to academic achievement (de la Fuente et al., 2013). More recently, a relationship to positive achievement emotions has also been found (Sander and de la Fuente, 2020).

The (Adapted and Integrated) SLPS Competency Model as a Buffering Variable When Facing Academic Stress (V.2)

The competency model for studying, learning, and performing under stress, SLPS (the adapted and integrated model; de la Fuente, 2021) assumes that, if a university student has an adequate level of the learning behaviors that make up this competency, these behaviors will act as protective factors or buffers against stress. The student will be able to adequately cope with academic stress situations and ultimately have fewer learning problems and stress symptoms (de la Fuente, 2015b,c). However, there are also risk factors that can predispose a greater experience of academic stress.

Despite the goodness of this model, it still underplays contextual factors (the design and development of teaching) that can also carry weight as protection or risk factors in experiencing stress. For this reason, contributions from SRL vs. ERL theory have also been taken into account (de la Fuente, 2017). Figure 1 shows a graphic representation of this adapted model in the context of the former models.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Competency model for studying, learning, and performing under stress, SLPS: an adapted and integrated model, v.2. (de la Fuente, 2021), with variables of this study. L, learning process variables; T, teaching process variables; C, conceptual factors; P, procedural factors; A, attitudinal factors.

SRL vs. ERL Theory in the COVID-19 Health Emergency

The SRL vs. ERL Model as a Heuristic for Analyzing Stress Factors in the University Teaching and Learning Process

The theoretical model entitled SRL vs. ERL theory (de la Fuente, 2017; de la Fuente et al., 2019a) seeks to straightforwardly identify the possible combinations of internal and external regulations that can occur in any university teaching–learning process. Basically, students are assumed to have different levels of behavioral self-regulation (high, medium, and low); they take on a given learning process from different starting points (with regulating, non-regulating, or dysregulating behaviors). In the same degree, teachers can show diverse teaching behavior in regard to external regulation (high, medium, and low); their teaching behaviors affect learning in ways that can be regulatory, non-regulatory, or dysregulatory (Pachón-Basallo et al., 2021). These two typologies in students and teachers are then combined in the teaching-learning process, giving rise to multiple interactions. Recent research has presented a heuristic that organizes the different possible interactions and has also tested their effects, with consistent evidence in relation to learning approaches and academic achievement (de la Fuente et al., 2020d), and to the factors and symptoms of stress (de la Fuente et al., 2020f).

Prior evidence reported on (1) the effect of the levels of self-regulation of students (high, medium, and low) on their learning behaviors and on emotional resources that they engage in during university learning; (2) the effect of the level of regulatory teaching (high, medium, and low) on learning behaviors and on the emotional resources that students engage in during university learning; (3) the combined effect of the different possible interactions between student and teacher regulatory levels, representing these as a consistent, increasing or decreasing linear function, according to the variable analyzed. Thus, the combination of different levels of regulation—from the lowest and most dysregulatory to the highest and most regulatory—has been found to determine the positive and negative achievement emotions of university students, as well as emotion- or problem-focused coping strategies (de la Fuente et al., 2019b, 2020a,b). This combination, moreover, has been found to determine learning approaches, perceived satisfaction, and personal achievement, as well as academic behavioral confidence and procrastination (de la Fuente et al., 2020b,c). In turn, characteristics of the teaching process determine the factors and symptoms of the stress of the students when learning (de la Fuente et al., 2021a,c,e,f). In summary, the stress reactions of university students depend on personal factors and also on contextual factors or the type of teaching process deployed.

This theoretical model proposes effective or regulatory teaching as a buffering factor against academic stress. Insofar as the teaching process is regulatory or effective, it will minimize the effect of stress during the learning process, particularly in this exceptional context of COVID-19. The teaching process during this period should be regular, clear, and predictable. Any radical, disorienting changes or adjustments during this period will not contribute to a buffering effect against stress but will become stress triggers (Barattucci, 2017; de la Fuente et al., 2017b). This schema has also been applied to psychoeducational behavior analysis in the COVID-19 health emergency (de la Fuente et al., 2021b).

The three preceding models have been conceptually merged in an updated, integrated version (de la Fuente, 2021; V.2), taking into account variables from the teaching process, which were not present in the original model (de la Fuente, 2015a). See Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. A structural predictive model of protective factors against academic stress (Model 2). CONSCIENT, conscientiousness; POSITOTAL, Positivity; DEEP, deep approach; CONFIDENCE, academic behavioral confidence; SR, self-regulation; PROBLCOP, problem-focused coping; POSITEMOT, positive achievement emotions; RT, regulatory teaching; STRESSTEACH, stress factors of teaching process; STRESSLEARN, stress factors of learning process; STRESSTOT, symptoms of stress.

Aims and Hypotheses

Based on the conceptual synthesis presented, the aim of this research was to empirically analyze the hypothesized relationship between personal characteristics of students (presage variables); their competency for learning under stress, as a protective or buffering variable against stress (a process variable); and their learning difficulties and final stress levels (a product variable) in order to establish implications for evaluation and intervention in a situation with high academic stress, such as the COVID-19 health emergency. Based on previous research and the existing evidence, the following relationships were hypothesized in situations of academic stress:

Hypothesis 1. There will be a structural predictive relationship, protecting against academic stress, that comprises the following: (1) presage factors, including the personality component of conscientiousness and its associated positivity; (2) process factors with a buffering effect, that are part of the competence for coping with stress (meta-cognitive, meta-emotional, meta-motivational and meta-behavioral, and attitudinal variables); (3) product factors: a low level of learning-related or academic stress as final dependent variables of the prediction.

Hypothesis 2. The relationship established in hypothesis 1 will be modulated positively by factors of the teaching context: (1) Presage factors: high regulatory teaching; (2) process factors: low stress factors from difficulties in the teaching process: (3) product factors: a low level of learning-related or academic stress as final dependent variables of the prediction.

Hypothesis 3. There will be a structural predictive relationship of vulnerability to academic stress that comprises: (1) presage factors, including the personality component of neuroticism and its associated lack of positivity; (2) process factors that pertain to a lack of competence for coping with academic stress (lack of meta-cognitive, meta-emotional, meta-motivational and meta-behavioral, and attitudinal variables); (3) product factors: a high level of learning-related or academic stress as final dependent variables of the prediction.

Hypothesis 4. The relationship established in Hypotheses 3 will be modulated negatively by factors of the teaching context: (1) Presage factors: low regulatory teaching; (2) process factors: high stress factors from difficulties in the teaching process: (3) product factors: a high level of learning-related or academic stress as final dependent variables of the prediction.

Method

Participants

The participants were 564 students enrolled in Psychology and Primary Education degrees at two Spanish universities. Their ages ranged from 19 to 25, with a mean age of 22.35 (σX = 7.1) years. Students age 26 and older were excluded. About 80.3% were women and 19.7% were men. Sampling was incidental and not probabilistic. The students from 20 different academic subjects completed the inventories. An incidental, non-randomized study design was used. Each Guidance Department of the universities invited participation from teachers, and the teachers invited their students to participate on an anonymous, voluntary basis. Each course (subject) was considered one specific teaching–learning process. The students completed the questionnaires online for one subject over one academic year. Only the students who voluntarily wished to participate did so.

Instruments

Presage Factors

Conscientiousness and neuroticism (personal factors) were assessed, using the big five questionnaire BFQ-N (del Barrio et al., 2006) based on Barbaranelli et al. (2003), and adapted for young university students (de la Fuente, 2014a). Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) reproduced a five-factor structure corresponding to the big five model. The results showed adequate psychometric properties and acceptable fit indices. The second-order confirmatory model showed a good fit [chi-square = 38.273; degrees of freedom (20–15) = 5; p < 0.001; NFI = 0.939; RFI = 0.917; IFI = 0.947; TLI = 0.937, CFI = 0.946; RMSEA = 065; HOELTER = 2,453 (p < 0.05) and 617 (p < 0.01)]. The total scale also showed good internal consistency [alpha = 0.956; Part 1 = 0.932, Part 2 = 0.832; Spearman–Brown = 0.962; Guttman = 0.932].

Positivity (personal factor). Measured by the Escala de Positividad (positivity scale) (Caprara et al., 2012). This scale contains 10 items on a five-point Likert response scale. The Spanish validation data for our sample produced acceptable values [chi-square = 308.992; degrees of freedom (44–20) = 20; p < 0.001; NFI = 0.901; RFI = 0.894; IFI = 0.912 TLI = 0.923, CFI = 0.916; RMSEA = 0.085; HOELTER = 260 (p < 0.05) and 291 (p < 0.01)]. The total scale also showed good internal consistency (alpha = 0.893; Part 1 = 0.832, Part 2 = 0.813; Spearman–Brown = 0.862; Guttman = 0.832).

Process Factors

Learning Variables

Learning approaches (a meta-cognitive factor) were measured by the revised two-factor study process questionnaire, R-SPQ-2F (Biggs et al., 2001). Twenty items measure two dimensions: deep learning approach (e.g., “I find that, at times, studying gives me a feeling of deep personal satisfaction”) and surface learning approach (e.g., “My aim is to pass the course while doing as little work as possible”). Students answer the items on a five-point Likert scale from 1 (“rarely true of me”) to 5 (“always true of me”). The R-SPQ-2F was translated into Spanish, adapted for cultural differences, independently back-translated, and further modified where needed. Using a Spanish sample, Justicia et al. (2008) showed a confirmatory factor structure similar to that of Biggs et al. (2001)—a first-order structure of two factors. These authors also reported acceptable reliability coefficients. In this study, confirmatory factor analysis produced a second factor structure with two factors (chi-square = 2,645.77; df = 169, CFI = 0.95, GFI = 0.91, AGFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.07). In the present study, Cronbach alpha reliability coefficients were acceptable (Deep α = 0.81; Surface α = 0.77) and similar to what the original authors found (omega index = 0.85).

Self-regulation behavior (a meta-behavior factor). This variable was measured, using the short self-regulation questionnaire (SSRQ) (Miller and Brown, 1991). The Spanish adaptation was previously validated in Spanish samples (Pichardo et al., 2014, 2018), showing acceptable validity and reliability, with values similar to the English version. Four factors (goal setting-planning, perseverance, decision-making, and learning from mistakes) were measured by a total of 17 items (all with saturations >0.40). The confirmatory factor structure is consistent (chi-square = 250.83, df = 112, CFI = 0.90, GFI = 0.92, AGFI = 0.90, RMSEA =0.05). Internal consistency was acceptable for the questionnaire total (α = 0.86) and for three factors: goal setting-planning (α = 0.79), decision-making (α = 0.72), and learning from mistakes (α = 0.72). The perseverance factor showed low internal consistency (α = 0.63); omega index = 0.75.

Procrastination (a negative meta-behavior variable). For this variable, we used a validated Spanish version of the procrastination assessment scale-students (PASS) (Garzón-Umerenkova and Gil-Flores, 2017). The original scale by Solomon and Rothblum (1984) consists of 44 items under two sections. The first section uses 18 items to assess procrastination frequency. Our study made use only of the second section, items 19 to 44, which investigates the cognitive-behavioral reasons for procrastination. On the five-point answer scale, 1 means “It does not reflect my motives whatsoever,” 3 means “It reflects my motives to an extent,” and 5 means “It reflects my motives completely.” In its validation for Spain, a language adjustment was made, and adequate reliability values were obtained (Cronbach's alpha from 0.71 to 0.82; omega index = 0.76). The confirmatory model showed a good fit [chi-square = 944,633; degrees of freedom (350–85) = 265; p < 0.001; NFI = 0.921; RFI = 0.915; IFI = 0.936; TLI = 0.926, CFI = 0.932; RMSEA = 0.032; HOELTER = 533 (p < 0.05) and 565 (p < 0.01)].

Coping strategies (meta-emotional factor). The coping strategies scale (EEC) was used in its original version (Sandín and Chorot, 2003), as validated for university students (de la Fuente, 2014b). The Lazarus and Folkman questionnaire (1984) and coping assessment studies by Moos and Billings (1982) were foundational to this scale, constructed according to theoretical-rational criteria. The original instrument contained 90 items. The validation produced a first-order structure with 64 items and a second-order structure with 10 factors and two dimensions, both of them were significant. The dimensions showed adequate fit values [Chi-square = 878.750; degrees of freedom (77–34) = 43, p < 0.001; NFI = 0.901; RFI = 0.945; IFI = 0.903, TLI = 0.951, CFI = 0.903]. The measures confirming reliability were Cronbach alpha values of 0.93 (complete scale), 0.93 (first half) and 0.90 (second half), Spearman-Brown of 0.84 and Guttman 0.80, Omega index = 0.86. Eleven factors and two dimensions make up the questionnaire: (1) Dimension: emotion-focused coping: F1. Fantasy distraction; F6. Help for action; F8. Preparing for the worst; F9. Venting and emotional isolation; F11. Resigned acceptance; and (2) Dimension: problem-focused coping: F2. Help seeking and family counsel; F5. Self-instructions; F10. Positive reappraisal and firmness; F12. Communication of feelings and social support; F13. Seeking alternative reinforcement.

Resilience (a meta-motivational factor) was measured, using the CD-RISC scale (Connor and Davidson, 2003) in its validated Spanish version (Manzano-García and Ayala-Calvo, 2013). Adequate reliability and validity values were obtained in Spanish samples, and a five-factor structure: F1: Persistence/tenacity and a strong sense of self-efficacy (tenacity); F2: Emotional and cognitive control under pressure (stress); F3: Adaptability/ability to bounce back (change); F4: Perception of control (control), and F5: Spirituality. The confirmatory model showed a good fit [chi-square = 1,619,170; degrees of freedom (350–85) = 265; p < 0.001; NFI = 0.929; RFI = 0.948; IFI = 0.922; TLI = 0.908, CFI = 0.920; RMSEA = 0.063; HOELTER = 240 (p < 0.05) and 254 (p < 0.01); Cronbach alpha = 0.88; omega index = 0.85)].

Learning related emotions (an attitudinal factor) were measured by the achievement emotions questionnaire (AEQ) (Pekrun et al., 2002; Perry et al., 2005), with scales for nine different emotions (enjoyment, hope, pride, relief, anger, anxiety, hopelessness, shame, and boredom), measured along two axes. The nine different emotions include emotions occurring during activity (enjoyment, boredom, and anger), prospective outcome emotions (hope, anxiety, and hopelessness), and retrospective outcome emotions (pride, relief, and shame). The two axes address valence, whether positive or negative emotions, and activation, where emotions can be either activating or deactivating. The four resulting quadrants are able to classify the emotions as either: (1) positive activating: enjoyment, hope, and pride; (2) positive deactivating: relief; (3) negative activating: anger, anxiety, and shame; or (4) negative deactivating: hopelessness and boredom. In this sample, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) reproduced a structure that corresponds to the AEQ model:

1) Achievement emotions pertaining to class (Paoloni, 2015). The results showed adequate psychometric properties and acceptable fit indices. The confirmatory model showed a good fit [chi-square = 843.028; degrees of freedom (44–25) = 19; p < 0.001; NFI = 0.954; RFI = 0.967; IFI = 0.953; TLI = 0.958, CFI = 0.971; RMSEA = 0.081; HOELTER = 156 (p < 0.05) and 158 (p < 0.01). Internal consistency for the total scale was good (Alpha = 0.904; Part 1 = 0.803, Part 2 = 0.853; Spearman–Brown = 0.903 and 853; Guttman = 0.862; omega index = 0.84). Sample items include item 1 (I get excited about going to class =; item 36 (I get bored); item 75 (I feel so hopeless—all my energy is depleted).

2) Achievement emotions pertaining to study (de la Fuente, 2015b). The results showed adequate psychometric properties and acceptable fit indices. The confirmatory model showed a good fit [chi-square = 729,890; degrees of freedom (44–25) = 19; p < 0.001; NFI = 0.964; RFI = 0.957; IFI = 0.973; TLI = 0.978, CFI = 0.971; RMSEA = 0.080; HOELTER = 165 (p < 0.05) and 178 (p < 0.01)]. The total scale also showed good internal consistency (alpha = 0.939; Part 1 = 0.880, Part 2 = 0.864; Spearman–Brown = 0.913 and 884; Guttman = 0.903; omega index = 0.87). Sample items include item 90 (I get angry when I have to study); item 113 (My sense of confidence motivates me); item 144 (I'm proud of myself).

3) Achievement emotions pertaining to testing (de la Fuente, 2015c). The results showed adequate psychometric properties and acceptable fit indices. The confirmatory model showed a good fit [chi-square = 376,658; degrees of freedom (44–25) = 19; p < 0.001; NFI = 0.978; RFI = 0.969; IFI = 0.983; TLI = 0.978, CFI = 0.963; RMSEA = 0.080; HOELTER = 169 (p < 0.05) and 188 (p < 0.01). Internal consistency for the total scale was good [alpha = 0.913; Part 1 = 0.870, Part 2 = 0.864; Spearman–Brown = 0.824 and 0.869; Guttman = 0.868; omega index = 0.88]. Sample items include item 170 (Before the exam, I feel nervous and uneasy); item 181 (I enjoy taking the exam); item 224 (I am very satisfied with myself).

Academic behavioral confidence (an attitudinal factor) was measured by the academic behavioral confidence scale (Sander and Sanders, 2006, 2009) in its validated Spanish version (Sander et al., 2011). The ABC scale was developed from and tentatively positioned against the established constructs of self-concept and self-efficacy. This psychometric scale is a self-report for undergraduate students from Spain and the UK, assessing their anticipated study-related behaviors (in a program assumed to consist largely of lecture-based courses). Four subscales comprise the total ABC scale and draw out crucially distinct aspects of the academic behavior of students: grades, studying, verbalizing, and attendance (Sander, 2009). Students respond to a question stem (“How confident are you that you will be able to.”) for items, such as “.manage your workload to meet coursework deadlines” and “.write in an appropriate academic style.” Answers are given on a five-point scale (1 = “not at all confident,” 5 = “very confident”). The higher the score, the greater the confidence of the students in using effective study skills or behaviors. Prior studies yielded a four-factor model (confidence in grade achievement, studying, attending class, and discussing course material) with adequate reliability and validity (Sander and Sanders, 2009). The confirmatory model showed a good fit [chi-square = 767,516; degrees of freedom (152–54) = 98; a probability level = 0.000; NFI = 0.969; RFI = 0.962; IFI = 0.973; TLI = 0.967, CFI = 0.973; RMSEA = 0.073; HOELTER = 203 (p < 0.05) and 222 (p < 0.01)]. Internal consistency for the total scale was good [alpha = 0.952; Part 1 = 0.932, Part 2 = 0.872; Spearman–Brown = 0.961; Guttman = 0.935; omega index = 0.87].

Engagement (a motivational factor). For this variable, we used a validated Spanish version of the Utrecht Work Engagement-Burnout Scale (Schaufeli et al., 2002; Schaufeli and Bakker, 2003). The psychometric properties were satisfactory with a sample of students from Spain. The model obtained good fit indices, showing a second-order structure of three factors: vigor, dedication, and absorption. Also verified were scale unidimensionality and metric invariance in the samples assessed (chi square = 792,526, df = 74, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.954, TLI = 0.976, IFI = 0.954, TLI = 0.979, and CFI = 0.923; RMSEA = 0.083; HOELTER = 153, p < 0.05; 170 p < 0.01). The Cronbach alpha for this sample was 0.900 (14 items); 0.856 (7 items) and 0.786 (7 items) for the two parts, respectively; omega index = 0.85.

Burnout (a motivational factor). The validated Spanish version of the engagement-burnout scale (Schaufeli et al., 2002) was used. The psychometric properties with a sample of students from Spain were satisfactory. Good fit indices were obtained, showing a second-order structure of three factors: exhaustion or depletion, cynicism, and lack of effectiveness. Also verified were scale unidimensionality and metric invariance in the samples assessed [chi square = 767.885, df = 87, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.956, TLI = 0.964, IFI = 0.951, TLI = 0.951, and CFI = 0.953; RMSEA = 0.071; HOELTER = 224, p < 0.05; 246 p < 0.01]. The Cronbach alpha for this sample was 0.874 (15 items); 0.853 (8 items) and 0.793 (7 items) for the two parts, respectively; omega index = 0.88.

Teaching Variables

Regulatory teaching (a meta-instructional variable). The student version of the assessment of the teaching-learning process (ATLP) (de la Fuente et al., 2012) was used to evaluate how students perceive the teaching process. The scale that addresses regulatory teaching constitutes Dimension 1 of the confirmatory model. The ATLP-D1 contains 29 items with a five-factor structure: specific regulatory teaching, regulatory assessment, preparation for learning, satisfaction with the teaching, and general regulatory teaching. Having been previously validated in university students (de la Fuente et al., 2012), the scale shows a factor structure with adequate fit indices (chi-square = 590.626; df = 48, p <0.001, CF1 = 0.838, TLI = 0.839, NFI = 0.850, NNFI = 0.867; RMSEA = 0.068). Internal consistency is also adequate (ATLP D1: a = 0.83; specific regulatory teaching, a = 0.897; regulatory assessment, a = 0.883; preparation for learning, a = 0.849; satisfaction with the teaching, a = 0.883, and general regulatory teaching, a = 0.883); omega index = 0.80.

Factors of stress. Cuestionario de Estrés Académico (CEA) [Academic factors of a stress questionnaire] (Cabanach et al., 2008, 2016). In order to analyze the internal structure of the scale, we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) of the whole set of data from our sample and thus verified the second-level structure. The default model has a good fit [chi-square = 66,457, df = 13, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.935, TLI = 0.961, IFI = 0.947, RFI = 0.965, and NFI = 0.947; RMSEA = 0.057; HOELTER = 0.430 (p < 0.05) and 0.532 (p < 0.01)]. The proposed model contains 53 items with a seven-factor structure and two dimensions, where one factor differs from the original version. The resulting factors were (1) stress in learning dimension: task overload (Factor 2), difficulty performance control (F3), social climate (Factor 5), and test anxiety (Factor 7); (2) Stress in teaching dimension: methodology difficulties (Factor 1), public interventions (Factor 4); content lacks value (Factor 6). Overall reliability = 0.961; part 1 = 0.932, part 2 = 0.946; omega index = 0.88.

Product Factors

Effects of Stress. Stress response questionnaire (CRE) (Cabanach et al., 2007). We found adequate psychometric properties for this scale in this sample of Spanish students. The confirmatory structural model of the CRE has the following dimensions [Chi-square = 846.503; Degrees of freedom (275–76) = 199, p < 0.001; NFI = 0.952; RFI = 0.965; IFI = 0.953): F1. Burnout; F2. Sleep difficulties; F3. Irritability; F4. Negative thoughts; F5. Agitation. Scale unidimensionality and metric invariance in the samples were confirmed [RMSEA = 0.046; CFI.922 and TLI 0.901; HOELTER = 431 (p < 0.05) and 459 (p < 0.01)]. Cronbach's alpha was 0.920, part 1 = 0.874 and part 2 = 0.863; omega index = 0.90.

Procedure

The students were informed about the research, and the volunteers completed the online self-informed consent on the e-Coping with Stress Platform (de la Fuente, 2015b) [http://www.inetas.net]. The questionnaires were then completed outside of normal class hours. They were asked to complete the questionnaires over one semester, during the period of September 2019 to February 2020. They completed one of the questionnaires each weekend in the order in which they were presented in the description; each questionnaire was completed one time during a 13-week period. A Certificate of Participation in R & D Project (10 h) was awarded, acknowledging the number of participation hours.

The platform organized the data anonymously, assigning a number to each user, whereby the different completed inventories were associated accordingly. The R and D Project was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Navarra (Ref. 2018.170).

Data Analysis

An ex post facto, transversal design of linear analysis was used to test the hypotheses.

Preliminary Analyses: Normality Assumptions

First, we explored the quality of the data by testing for outliers and missing cases. We tested for univariate outliers by calculating the typical scores for each variable, considering cases with Z scores outside the +/−3 range to be potentially atypical cases (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2001a,b). In addition, the Mahalanobis distance (D2) was used to detect atypical combinations of variables (atypical multivariate cases), a statistical measure of multidimensional distance of an individual from the centroid or mean of the given observations (Lohr, 1999). This procedure detects significant distances from the typical combinations or centroids of a set of variables. The literature suggests removing univariate and multivariate outliers, or reassigning them the nearest extreme score (Weston and Gore, 2006). The procedure was carried out, using SPSS (v.26, IBM, Armonk, NY, USA), which includes a specific routine for missing values analysis that determines the magnitude of missing values and whether they are presented in a systematic or random manner.

Linear Association

We conducted bivariate correlational analyses (Pearson, two-tailed) with the total factor scores for the construct of the model. IBM-SPSS v. 25 was used for both analyses.

Path Analysis of Exploratory Prediction

Exploratory predictive hypotheses were tested, using path analysis with a mediational model, for multiple measurements (Ato and Vallejo, 2011). For each hypothesis posed, we tested a different empirical model of path analysis (Byrne, 2016). The first two models related to the predictive analysis of protective factors (buffers) against academic stress, while the second two focused on the prediction of risk factors for academic stress. Models 2 and 4 were selected, because they fulfilled the statistical parameters and responded empirically to the proposed integrative model. We assessed the model fit by first examining the ratio of chi-square to degrees of freedom, then the comparative fit index (CFI), normed fit index (NFI), incremental fit index (IFI), and relative fit index (RFI). All fit measures of the incremental model were above the suggested limit of 0.90 (Bentler, 1990): Comparative fit index (CFI), incremental fit index (IFI), normed fit index (NFI), relative fit index (RFI), and Tucker–Lewis index (TLI). We replicated the results of the original scale. The value of the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) was 0.084, less than the warning value of 0.09 (Vázquez et al., 2006). We also used the Hoelter index to determine the adequacy of the sample size. AMOS (v.22) was used for these analyses. Keith (2006) proposed the following beta coefficients as research benchmarks for direct effects: < 0.05 is considered too small to be meaningful, above.05 is small but meaningful, above.10 is moderate, and above.25 is large. For indirect effects, we used the definition of an indirect effect as the product of two effects; using Keith's benchmarks above, we propose a small indirect effect =0.003, moderate =0.01, and large =0.06, values that are significant in the sphere of education.

Results

Preliminary Analyses: Normality Assumptions

The descriptive and normality results showed the fit required for using linear analyses with the variables of the sample. Regarding asymmetry and kurtosis, in most cases, the obtained values were <0.500. As for the Kolmogorov–Smirnoff test, the distribution of values was not significantly different from a normal distribution. See Table 1.

Correlations

Protective Factors Against Academic Stress

The personal (presage) factors of conscientiousness and positivity were found in statistically significant association with different constituent factors of the competency for studying, learning, and performing under stress (process). These characteristic factors of the SLPS competency were also significantly associated among themselves, namely, deep approach, self-regulation, problem-focused coping, resilience, positive achievement emotions, engagement, and academic behavioral confidence. These factors, in turn, were negatively associated with stress responses (product), defined as stress factors of the learning process, and stress symptoms. In complementary fashion, Regulatory teaching (presage) was associated positively with certain constituent factors of the SLPS competency and also negatively with stress factors of the teaching process (process). See Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Distribution and normalization statistics of the sample (n = 564).

Risk Factors of Academic Stress

The personal factor neuroticism had a statistically significant, negative association with positivity (a presage factor). Also, neuroticism had a statistically significant, positive association with different factors representing a lack of the SLPS competency for learning under stress (process), such as procrastination, burnout, and negative achievement emotions. Factors showing a lack of SLPS competency were also positively associated with one another, such as surface approach, emotion-focused coping, negative achievement emotions, and burnout, and were negatively associated with protective factors, such as academic behavioral confidence, self-regulation, and resilience. Such risk factors were, in turn, positively associated with stress responses (product), defined as stress factors of the learning process and stress symptoms. In a complementary fashion, regulatory teaching (presage) was associated positively with certain constituent factors of the SLPS competency and also negatively with stress factors of the teaching process (process). See Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Bivariate correlations between protective factors against academic stress in this research (n = 564).

Path Analysis Predictive Relationships

The four exploratory models that were tested fulfilled the statistical parameters required for the empirical fit (see Table 3). From these four models, models 2 and 4 were selected. Despite having somewhat less significance, they showed a better fit to the theoretical model on which this research is based. Model 1 shows statistics of the stress protection factors that refer exclusively to the learning process. Model 2 shows the statistics when teaching process variables are also included; for this reason, it is more powerful, and the statistical values are better fitted. Model 3 refers to risk factors for stress that are triggered by the learning process, while Model 4 incorporates stress factors from the teaching process as well, also showing better a statistical fit (see Table 4). We, therefore, present below the specific, predictive statistical values from Models 2 and 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Bivariate correlations between risk factors of academic stress in this research (n = 564).

The Model of Protective Factors Against Academic Stress (Model 2)

Direct Effects of the Protective Factors

The predictive structural Model 2 demonstrated many predictive relationships pertaining to students. Two personal factors (presage) were found to protect against stress: conscientiousness and positivity. The former significantly predicted the latter (B = 0.376). Both were significant predictors of different levels of the SLPS competency components (presage). Conscientiousness, as an “executive” personality variable, appeared as a significant predictor of meta-cognitive variables (deep approach; B = 0.190), meta-behavioral variables (self-regulation; B = 0.438), meta-emotional variables (problem-focused coping; B = 0.110), meta-motivational variables (resilience; B = 0.090), and meta-affective variables (academic behavioral confidence; B = 0.180), and also of emotional variables (positive emotions; B = 0.247). Positivity, as a personal psychological variable, appeared as a significant predictor of emotional variables (positive emotions; B = 0.177).

The relationships between the factors of the SLPS competency model (process) were also very significant. Thus, deep learning predicted academic behavioral confidence (B = 0.111). Academic behavioral confidence was predicted by self-regulation (B = 0.121) and engagement (B = 0.120). Self-Regulation predicted academic behavioral confidence (B = 0.177), resilience (B = 0.186), engagement and positive emotions (B = 0.465). One especially important predictive effect was to the predictive power of positive emotions with respect to deep approach (B = 0.450), academic behavioral confidence (B = 0.228), self-regulation (beta = 0.219), problem-focused coping (B = 0.280), and resilience (B = 0.118).

In the analysis of teaching process factors, the positive predictive value of regulatory teaching (RT) was demonstrated in regard to positivity (B = 0.11), academic behavioral confidence (B = 0.068), engagement (B = 0.207), and positive emotions (B = 134). Inversely, regulatory teaching negatively predicted the stress in teaching factor (B = −0.115), which, in turn, negatively predicted positive emotions (B = −0.127), and positively predicted stress in learning (B = 0.589).

Different relations were also shown in regard to the prediction of stress symptoms, such as stress during learning and stress symptoms (product). Some factors were significant negative predictors of stress in learning, such as self-regulation (B = −0.171), and positive emotions (B = −0.175), but others were positive predictors, like problem-focused coping (B = 0.069) and, above all, stress in teaching (B =0.589). Finally, stress symptoms were negatively predicted by personal factors like self-regulation (B = −0.167) and resilience (B = −0.143), but also by stress in learning (B = 0.543), a factor that was already shown to be predicted by stress in teaching.

Indirect Effects of the Protective Factors

The indirect effects showed that some variables had adequate values as mediating predictors. Thus, the variables conscientiousness and positivity (presage factors) showed a positive effect on nearly all the variables belonging to the SLPS competency—conscientiousness having greater predictive strength—and a significant negative predictive effect on the two variables of experiencing academic stress (B = −0.211 and B = −0.119, respectively).

Second, a group of factors belonging to the SLPS competency (process factors) showed positive, predictive indirect effects among themselves (self-regulation, problem-focused coping, engagement, and positive emotions), and negative effects on academic stress (B = −0.003; B = −0.119; B = −0.037; B = −0.032; B = −0.070). Likewise, regulatory teaching had a positive indirect effect on most variables of the SLPS competency (confidence, B = 0.104; self-regulation, B = 0.078; problem-focused coping, B = 0.101; engagement, B = 0.018; positive emotions, B = 0.139; resilience, B = 0.097).

Finally, most of the SLPS variables had a negative indirect predictive effect on academic stress (confidence, B = −0.003; self-regulation, B = −0.019; engagement, B = −0.032; positive emotions, B = −0.037; resilience, B = 0.097), as did regulatory teaching (B = −0.066). However, positive indirect prediction factors did appear (problem-focused coping, B = 0.101; stress in teaching, B = 0.323). See Table 5 and Figure 1.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Statistical parameters of structural models.

Combination Factors

Many total effects were combined effects of direct and indirect prediction effects. Table 5 presents the total, direct, and indirect effects (full and partial mediation effects) of protective factors of students against academic stress. Observe the predictive value of both personal characteristics and teaching process characteristics.

A Model of Risk Factors in Academic Stress (Model 4)

Direct Effects of the Risk Factors

This model gave evidence of two personal factors with predictive weight (presage factors). Neuroticism had a negative predictive value for positivity (B = −0.304). Moreover, neuroticism showed a positive predictive value for several risk factors that characterize a lack of SLPS competency, such as surface approach (B = 0.208), procrastination (B = 0.331), burnout (B = 0.102) and negative emotions (B = 0.180). It also showed a negative predictive value on protective factors like academic behavioral confidence (B = −0.231) and self-regulation (B = −0.188).

When analyzing the risk factors belonging to the SLPS competency model, we confirmed significant predictive relationships between them. Surface approach negatively predicted academic behavioral confidence (B = −0.164) and self-regulation (B = −0.266), and positively predicted negative emotions (B = 0.243). Procrastination negatively predicted self-regulation (B = −0.129) and positively predicted emotion-focused coping (B = 0.170), burnout (B =0.208) and negative emotions (B = 0.237). Also, emotion-focused coping and burnout predicted negative emotions (B = 0.160; B = 0.196, respectively).

As for context factors, regulatory teaching appeared as a protective factor that, in addition to positively predicting protective factors like positivity (B = 0.220), academic behavioral confidence (B = 0.144) and self-regulation (B = 0.107), it negatively predicted risk factors like procrastination (B = −0.111), burnout (B = −0.116) and stress in teaching (B = −0.100). However, the risk factor stress in teaching positively predicted stress in learning (B = 0.391).

Finally, the risk factors of the SLPS competency predicted experiences of academic stress. Negative emotions predicted stress in learning (B = 0.163). Procrastination (B = 0.185) and stress in teaching (B = 0.391), as risk factors, positively predicted stress symptoms. Stress in learning positively predicted stress symptoms (B =0.487). By contrast, protective factors like self-regulation (B = −0.142) and resilience (B = −0.131) negatively predicted stress experiences.

Indirect Effects of the Risk Factors

Neuroticism, as a personal risk factor (presage), showed numerous indirect effects that positively predicted risk factors of the SLPS competency, such as emotion-focused coping (B = 0.057), burnout (B = 0.231) and negative emotions (B = 0.239). In addition, its indirect effects negatively predicted protective factors like academic behavioral confidence (B = −0.238), self-regulation (B = −0.189) and resilience (B = −0.267).

Regarding risk factors of the SLPS competency (process factors), certain risk factors (surface approach and procrastination) showed the indirect effect of positively predicting other risk factors (emotion-focused coping, burnout, and negative emotions). The opposite occurred with personal protective factors (academic behavioral confidence and self-regulation) and contextual protective factors (regulatory teaching), where risk factors were negatively predicted (burnout and negative emotions).

Finally, there was an indirect effect that positively predicted stress symptoms (product factor): from neuroticism (a presage factor), surface approach, procrastination, burnout, negative emotions, and stress in teaching (process factors). Protective factors like positivity (presage factor), academic behavioral confidence, self-regulation, resilience, and regulatory teaching (process factors) appeared as negative predictors of stress symptoms (a product factor). See Table 6 and Figure 3.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Total, indirect, and direct effects of stress protection factors in this study, and 95% bootstrap confidence intervals (CI).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. A structural predictive model of risk factors for academic stress (Model 4). POSITOTAL, positivity; SURFACE, surface approach; CONFIDENCE, academic behavioral confidence; SR, self-regulation; PROCRRT, procrastination; EMOTCOP, emotion-focused coping; NEGATEMOT, negative achievement emotions; RT, regulatory teaching; STRESSTEACH, stress factors of teaching process; STRESSLEARN, stress factors of learning process; STRESSTOT, symptoms of stress; CI, confidence interval. Bootstrapping sample size = 430.

Combination Factors

In this model, many total effects were also combined effects of direct and indirect prediction effects. Table 7 presents the total, direct, and indirect effects (full and partial mediation effects) of risk factors of students for academic stress. Observe the predictive value of both personal characteristics and teaching process characteristics.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Total, indirect, and direct effects of stress risk factors in this study, and 95% bootstrap confidence intervals (CI).

Discussion

In general, the results support the established hypotheses in various aspects. Regarding Hypothesis 1, certain personality factors (conscientiousness and positivity) demonstrated their protective function against stress by positively and directly predicting the behaviors that make up the SLPS competency, and negatively and indirectly predicting stress levels. This result concurs with previous research that asserted the factors of conscientiousness and positivity as personal factors that protect against stress (Caprara and Steca, 2005; Caprara et al., 2017; Greene et al., 2020) and as predictors of adequate learning processes (Biggs, 1970b). Previous evidence showed a positive, partially predictive relationship between different factors of the SLPS competency and the variables of deep approach, academic behavioral confidence, problem-focused coping (Leszko et al., 2020), positive emotions, and resilience (de la Fuente et al., 2017a, 2019b).

It is of great interest that behaviors that are inherent in the SLPS competency, according to its model (de la Fuente, 2015a), prove to be associated with and to predict each other, forming clusters of protective (buffering) and risk factors. This has revealed the existence of a group of protective factors against stress, such as the deep approach, which has a positive linear relationship with academic behavioral confidence, self-regulation, positive achievement emotions, engagement, and resilience, in line with what was reported in previous research (Quoidbach et al., 2010; Artuch-Garde et al., 2017; de la Fuente et al., 2021f). This reflects a clear associative relationship between the metacognitive, meta-behavioral, meta-motivational, emotional, and attitudinal variables of learning, when learning takes place under conditions of university academic stress. These variables have been traditionally separated in their effects and their analyses—just as what is espoused by the SLPS competency model. Finally, the above variables were found to negatively predict, both directly and indirectly, the factors of stress in learning and stress symptoms in a manner consistent with the previous evidence (Alias et al., 2020).

Hypothesis 2 has also been confirmed since regulatory or effective teaching directly predicted personal positivity, as well as various protective factors of the SLPS competency, such as academic behavioral confidence, engagement, and achievement emotions, as seen in previous research (Baeten et al., 2010). But it is also interesting to note that such teaching also negatively predicted the factor stress in teaching, which would lead to a direct negative prediction of stress in learning, also established by previous research (de la Fuente and Justicia, 2003). In other words, regulatory teaching would indirectly and inversely predict the factor stress in learning and, consequently, stress symptoms. This result is consistent with previous research that also established causal factors of stress in the teaching process, and it provides empirical support for the SRL vs. ERL theory (de la Fuente et al., 2020a,c,d,f).

The assumptions of Hypothesis 3 were also empirically supported by our results. The personal factor neuroticism was confirmed as a personal risk factor since it minimizes positivity (Greene et al., 2020), as well as protective factors of the SLPS competency, such as academic behavioral confidence, self-regulation, and resilience (McDonnell and Semkovska, 2020). Neuroticism is also a positive predictor of the risk factors analyzed here, such as surface approach, procrastination, burnout, and negative achievement emotions, as supported by abundant prior evidence (Chen et al., 2020; Yang et al., 2020). In addition, a positive linear relationship between risk factors of the SLPS competency has been demonstrated. Thus, surface approach was shown to be connected through association and prediction to a lack of academic behavioral confidence and self-regulation (de la Fuente et al., 2013) and to the use of emotion-focused coping strategies, burnout, negative achievement emotions, and a lack of resilience (de la Fuente et al., 2017b). Therefore, this positive linear connection between meta-cognitive, meta-affective, meta-motivational, emotional, and attitudinal risk factors could be considered a cluster of risk for experiencing academic stress, since all of them positively predict stress in learning and stress symptoms. Despite these results, some authors have defended the potential of stress during university learning (Rudland et al., 2019).

Finally, regarding Hypothesis 4, regulatory or effective teaching also appeared as a negative predictor of risk factors for the SLPS competency, such as procrastination (Brando-Garrido et al., 2020) and burnout in a direct manner but also indirectly through the stress in teaching factor, which also predicted negative achievement emotions and stress in learning. These results are consistent with an integrated vision of teaching and learning processes in the analysis of academic phenomena at a university (Prosser and Trigwell, 1999; Rosário et al., 2013). Based on previous research and the results of the present study, the SLPS competency includes certain student factors, both presage and process, that protect against academic stress, while other factors constitute risks for experiencing stress. Moreover, these factors coexist with presage and process factors of teaching, which also directly predispose and predict the experience of stress (Biggs and Tang, 2007; Kember et al., 2020; de la Fuente et al., 2021e). This evidence is highly important for university policies of prevention, assessment, and psychoeducational intervention in the emotional health and well-being of students during the COVID-19 period.

Several research limitations should be mentioned. On one hand, the sample was limited and may have a selection bias due to the arbitrary selection procedure. On the other hand, the sample was taken at a time prior to the health emergency. The model, then, remains to be validated in situ. Unfortunately, the short impact period of the COVID-19 event does not yet allow for a process evaluation. Another methodological limitation when extrapolating consequences from this research study is the fact that the data were not collected over a long period, with a longitudinal design, but were collected under a short, cross-sectional design (the real duration of the university subject). The results from paths that generated a better fit of the selected model cannot be interpreted in causal terms, given that verification, using a longer, longitudinal design, would be required. Another limitation is the molar level of analysis used in this research (de la Fuente et al., 2019b), distant from the biological processes of microanalysis. In the future, the analysis of these relationships should be contextualized within this current moment of a health emergency, and the variable of positive vs. negative emotional reactivity should be explored (Becerra et al., 2017), as being particularly important in this competency.

Implications for Actions During the COVID-19 Emergency

Current events are forcing us to make broad behavioral adjustments in the organization of our personal life, family life, and academic life for the weeks ahead. In order to make these adjustments smoothly, we need to keep in mind different behavioral principles and strategies. For example (de la Fuente, 2020b):

1) Presage factors:

Students: It is important to know the characteristics of students to be able to detect which students have protective factors (and so reinforce them) and which are more likely to have personal risk factors (and so be able to intervene). The aim is to keep students from falling into a vicious circle of risk factors during the COVID-19 episode due to their low levels of SLPS competency.

Teachers: Based on previous evaluations, we need to identify which teaching processes incorporate protective factors against stress (regulatory design) or, instead, involve risk factors in stress (non-regulatory or dysregulatory design). In the former case, these teaching processes should be reinforced and fine-tuned to the new situation, without big changes. In the second case, factors and adjustments that are most dysregulatory toward the learning process must be identified (e.g., drastic changes in content, methodologies, timing, and assessment). These must be avoided or corrected (de la Fuente et al., 2020e).

2) Process factors: Certain intervention strategies are suggested for maximizing the stress-buffering effect of the SLPS competency. Example:

Students: Self-Regulation and Self-Regulated Learning

1) While homebound, stay close to your usual schedule:

· Circadian rhythms and personal habits go far in helping to maintain a sequence of actions to self-regulate and to not lose motivation.

· Give yourself daily doses of positive emotions and rewarding experiences while sheltering at home. It is very important to keep a positive emotional outlook. Distress (diffuse negative emotionality and discouragement) can be triggered by abrupt changes in the daily rhythm of one, or by a sense of uncertainty and loss of behavioral control.

2) Self-regulate your own behavior during this period:

· Every day, plan objectives, schedules, and actions, being flexible but also systematic.

· Exercise control over your own behavior. Force yourself to continue working and also to stop and take leisure time (a substitute for outside activities). Tell yourself that you are doing the right thing. Use different relaxation techniques to decrease any anxiety.

· It is not a good time to take on serious, complex issues in your life situation, because this may cause even greater stress and loss of situational control. If it is truly necessary, make small, gradual adjustments.

·Take advantage to catch up on pending matters, whether personal, family-related or academic tasks. This is a gift of time.

· Evaluate your behavior at the end of the day and redefine your objectives (family related, personal, and academic) for the next few days.

Teachers: Self-Regulation and External Regulation of Students

1) In the subjects you teach, maintain a regulatory approach that gives your students a perception of control and continuity:

· Keep your usual hours of contact with the students, using appropriate technology. Direct online classes allow you to continue with the subject and lessen anxiety about the students.

· Make every adjustment you can so that all participants perceive normality and a sense of control. It is best to keep up the regular pace of the subject while making adjustments that the situation requires. It is not a good time to make big, unexpected changes.

· If needed, adjust your assessment activities and system during this period. Make students aware that the new situation means new behavioral challenges, including the chance for them to practice online teamwork from home.

2) Apply external regulation to help students in their learning process:

· If you have not already done so, this is a good time to convert all learning resources to online formats and encourage students to learn autonomously from home.

· Plan regular, general messages and aids for your students so they feel that the teaching-learning process continues with some normality.

· Offer personalized online tutoring for students who need it. It is especially important to keep direct contact with the student representative in each class in order to be informed of any possible problems or help that the students are needing.

· Regularly reevaluate whether students need adjustments to the material, assignments, etc.

· Pay attention to the emotional state and expectations of your students. Convey calm and assurance with your own behavior. Your students see themselves reflected in you and your demeanor when interacting with them. Become a mentor that supports the process, also on an emotional level.

3) Product factors: For students with risk factors or vulnerability to stress, in addition to the steps mentioned above, specific emotional regulation techniques like mindfulness (de la Fuente et al., 2018) or emotional refocusing (Quoidbach et al., 2010) should be worked on, given their effectiveness in the short and long term.

Conclusion

Empirical models of university academic stress can be useful for: (1) detecting university students who may be at risk during health emergencies like COVID-19; (2) designing psychoeducational learning support systems for students who are experiencing stress in this situation; (3) promoting teaching strategies that protect against academic stress in this context. If we have preventive models of this academic phenomenon, it will be easier to prepare ourselves sooner for emergencies like the one we are currently experiencing. It is very important that certain behavioral repertories be implemented, and so act as psychological vaccines for coping with stress and improving well-being at a university. This is done through psychoeducational programs that improve the competence of students and teachers before the symptoms of academic stress set in (especially in large-scale events like COVID-19) (Coulson, 2019; Young et al., 2020). The services of our University Guidance and Psychology Departments should be an essential, irreplaceable tool for accomplishing this task. The analysis heuristic presented here could be used by Applied Psychology Units at universities for evaluating and intervening in processes of academic stress.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by The Ethics Committee of the University of Navarra (ref. 2018.170). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JdF: conceptual design, data analysis, writing the article, and R&D Project management.

Funding

R&D Project PGC2018-094672-B-I00, University of Navarra (Ministry of Science and Education, Spain), UAL18 SEJ-DO31-A-FEDER (University of Almería, Spain), and the European Social Fund.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Alessandri, G., Caprara, G. V., and Tisak, J. (2012). The unique contribution of positive orientation to optimal functioning: Further explorations. Eur. Psychol. 17, 44–54. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alias, N. F., Mustafa, S. M. S., and Hamzah, L. M. (2020). The symptoms of stress as perceived by University Students. Int. J. Soc. Sci. Res. 2, 139–149. doi: 10.21101/cejph.a3873

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amate-Romera, J., and de la Fuente, J. (2021). Relationships between test anxiety, self-regulation and strategies for coping with stress, in professional examination candidates. Ann. Psychol. 37, 276–286. doi: 10.6018/analesps.411131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Artuch-Garde, R., González-Torres Md, C., de la Fuente, J., Vera, M. M., Fernández-Cabezas, M., and López-García, M. (2017). Relationship between resilience and self-regulation: a study of spanish youth at risk of social exclusion. Front. Psychol. 8:612. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asikainen, H., and Gijbels, D. (2017). Do students develop towards more deep approaches to learning during studies? A systematic review on the development of students' deep and surface approaches to learning in higher education. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 29, 205–234. doi: 10.1007/s10648-017-9406-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ato, M., and Vallejo, G. (2011). Los efectos de terceras variables en la investigación psicológica [The effects of third variables in psychological research]. Ann. Psychol. 27, 550–561. Available online at: http://revistas.um.es/analesps

Google Scholar

Baeten, M., Kyndt, E., Struyven, K., and Dochy, F. (2010). Using student-centred learning environments to stimulate deep approaches to learning: factors encouraging or discouraging their effectiveness. Educ.Res. Rev. 5, 243–260. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2010.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banerjee, Y., Akhras, A., Khamis, A. H., Alsheikh-Ali, A., and Davis, D. (2019). Investigating the relationship between resilience, stress-coping strategies, and learning approaches to predict academic performancein undergraduate medical students: protocol for a proof-of-concept study. JMIR Res. Protoc. 8:e14677. doi: 10.2196/14677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barattucci, M. (2017). Approach to study as an indicator of the quality of teaching and of learning environment: the contribution of john biggs. J. e-Learn. Knowledge Soc. 13, 77–88. doi: 10.20368/1971-8829/1311

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barattucci, M., Pagliaro, S., Cafagna, D., and Bosetto, D. (2017). An examination of theapplicability of Biggs' 3P learning process model to Italian university. J. e-Learn. Knowledge Soc. 13, 163–180.

Google Scholar

Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., Rabasca, A., and Pastorelli, C. (2003). A questionnaire for measuring the Big Five in late childhood. Pers. Individ. Diff. 34, 645–664. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00051-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becerra, R., Preece, D., Campitelli, G., and Scott-Pillow, G. (2017). The assessment of emotional reactivity across negative and positive emotions: development and validation of the perth emotional reactivity scale (PERS). Assessment 26, 868–879. doi: 10.1177/1073191117694455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol. Bull. 107:238.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Biggs, J., and Tang, C. (2007). Teaching for Quality Learning at University, Buckingham: Society for Research into Higher Education and Open University Press.

Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B. (1970a). Faculty patterns in study behaviour. Austr. J. Psychol. 22, 161–174.

Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B. (1970b). Personality correlates of certain dimensions of study behaviour. Austr. J. Psychol. 22, 287–297.

Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B. (1972). Study behaviour and matriculation performance in two school populations. Austr. J. Educ. 16, 187–204.

Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B. (1973). Study behaviour and performance in objective and essay formats. Austr. J. Educ 17, 157–167.

Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B. (1976). Dimension of study behaviour: Another look at ATI. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 46, 68–80.

Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B. (1978). Individual and group differences in study processes. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 48, 266–279.

Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B. (1985). The role of meta-learning in study process. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 55, 185–212.

Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B. (1987). Study Process Questionnaire Manual. Hawthorn, VIC: Australian Council for Educational Research.

Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B. (1993). From theory to practice: A cognitive systems approach. Higher Educ. Res. Dev. 12, 73–85.

Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B. (1999). Teaching for Quality Learning at university. Buckingham: Open University Press.

Google Scholar

Biggs, J. B., Kember, D., and Leung, Y. P. (2001). The revised two-factor study process questionnaire: R-SPQ-2F. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 71, 133–149. doi: 10.1348/000709901158433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brando-Garrido, C., Montes-Hidalgo, J., Limonero, J. T., Gómez-Romero, M. J., and Tomás-Sábado, J. (2020). Relationship of academic procrastination with perceived competence, coping, self-esteem and self-efficacy in nursing students. Enfermería Clíni. 30, 398–403. doi: 10.1016/j.enfcle.2019.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M. (2016). Structural Equation Modeling With AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Cabanach, R. G., Rodríguez, S., Valle, A., Piñeiro, I., and Millán, P. G. (2008). Metas académicas y vulnerabilidad al estrés en contextos académicos. Aula Abierta 36, 3–16.

Google Scholar

Cabanach, R. G., Rodríguez, S., Valle, A., Piñeiro, I., and Millán, P. G. (2007). Metas académicas y vulnerabilidad al estrés en contextos académicos [Academic goals and vulnerability to stress in academic contexts]. Aula Abierta 36, 3–16.

Google Scholar

Cabanach, R. G., Souto-Gestal, A., and Franco, V. (2016). Escala de Estresores Académicos para la evaluación de los estresores académicos en estudiantes universitarios. Rev. Iberoamericana Psicol. Salud 7, 41–50.

Google Scholar

Caprara, G. V., Alessandri, G., Eisenberg, N., Kupfer, A., Steca, P., Caprara, M. G., et al. (2012). The positivity scale. Psychol. Assess. 24, 701–712. doi: 10.1037/a0026681

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caprara, G. V., Alessandri, G., Trommsdorff, G., Heikamp, T., Yamaguchi, S., and Suzuki, F. (2011). Positive orientation across three cultures. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 43, 77–83. doi: 10.1177/0022022111422257

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caprara, G. V., Eisenberg, N., and Alessandri, G. (2017). Positivity: The dispositional basis of happiness. J. Happiness Stud. 18, 353–371. doi: 10.1037/a0025626

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caprara, G. V., Fagnani, C., Alessandri, G., Steca, P., Gigantesco, A., Sforza, L. L. C., et al. (2009). Human optimal functioning: The genetics of positive orientation towards self, life, and the future. Behav. Gene. 39, 277–284. doi: 10.1007/s10519-009-9267-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caprara, G. V., and Steca, P. (2005). Affective and social self-regulatory efficacy beliefs as determinants of positive thinking and happiness. Eur. Psychol. 10, 275–286. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.10.4.275

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caprara, G. V., Steca, P., Alessandri, G., Abela, J. R., and McWhinnie, C. M. (2010). Positive orientation: Explorations on what is common to life satisfaction, self-esteem, and optimism. Epidemiol. Psichiatria Soc. 19, 63–71. doi: 10.1017/S1121189X00001615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caprara, G. V., Steca, P., Gerbino, M., Paciello, M., and Vecchio, G. M. (2006). Looking for adolescents' well-being: Self-efficacy beliefs as determinants of positive thinking and happiness. Epidemiol. Psichiatria Soc. 15, 30–43. doi: 10.1017/s1121189x00002013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cetin, B. (2015). Academic motivation and approaches to learning in predicting college students' academic achievement: findings from Turkish and US Samples. J. College Teach. Learn. 12:1411150. doi: 10.19030/tlc.v12i2.9200

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Furnham, A., and Lewis, M. (2007). Personality andapproaches to learning predict preference for different teaching methods. Learn. Individ. Differ. 17, 241–250. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2006.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., Pu, J., Shi, W., and Zhou, Y. (2020). The impact of neuroticism on symptoms of anxiety and depression in elderly adults: The mediating role of rumination. Curr. Psychol. 39, 42–50. doi: 10.1007/s12144-017-9740-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connor, K. M., and Davidson, J. R. (2003). Development of a new resilience scale: the Connor-Davidson resilience scale (CD-RISC). Depress Anxiety 18, 76–82. doi: 10.1002/da.10113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coulson, F. (2019). Learning From At-Risk College Students Who Participate in Flow Activities. Doctoral Dissertation. ProQuest Number:27671119, Minneapolis, MN, United States.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J. (2014a). Five Scale, BFQ-U. Adaptation for Young University Students. Almería: University of Almería.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J. (2014b). Confirmatory Structural Equation Model of Coping Strategies Scale (EEC). Almería: University of Almería.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J. (2015a). Competency for Studying, Learning and Performing Under Stress: Self-Help Guide for University Students, Graduates and Professional Examination Candidates. Almería: Education & Psychology I+D+I: e-publishing R&D&I Series.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J. (2015b). Learning-Related Emotions Questionnaire. Almería: University of Almería.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J. (2015c). Test-Related Emotions Questionnaire. Almería: University of Almería.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J. (2017). Theory of self- vs. externally- regulated learning TM: fundamentals, evidence, and applicability. Front. Psychol. 8:1675. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J. (2020a). The Healthcare, Medical-Biological and Behavioral Emergency of COVID-19. Available online at: https://www.inetas.net/stress/seccion.php?ididioma=1&idseccion=2&idproyecto=1

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J. (2020b). Behavioral Strategies for Self-Regulation and Regulating Students During This Time of Online Academic Work From Home. Available online at: https://www.inetas.net/stress/seccion.php?ididioma=1&idseccion=2&idproyecto=1

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J. (2021). Competency for Studying, Learning and Performing Under Stress: Adapted and Integrated model (V.2). Pamplona: University of Navarra.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Amate, J., González-Torres, M. C., Artuch, R., García-Torrecillas, J. M., and Fadda, S. (2020a). Effects of levels of self-regulation and regulatory teaching on strategies for coping with academic stress in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 11:22. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Fernández-Cabezas, M., Cambil, M., Vera, M. M., González-Torres, M. C., and Artuch-Garde, R. (2017a). Linear relationship between resilience, learning approaches, and coping strategies to predict achievement in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 8:1039. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., García-Torrecillas, J. M., and Rodríguez-Vargas, S. (2015a). “The relationships between coping strategies, test anxiety and burnout-engagement behavior in university undergraduates,” in Coping Strategies and Health, ed M. Bachman (New York, NY: Nova Science Publisher).

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., González-Torres, M. C., Artuch-Garde, R., Vera, M. M., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., and Peralta-Sánchez, F. J. (2021f). Resilience as a buffering variable between the big five components and factors and symptoms of academic stress at university. Front. Psychiatry 12:600240. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.600240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., González-Torres, M. C., Aznárez-Sanado, M., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Peralta-Sánchez, F. J., and Vera, M. M. (2019a). Implications of unconnected micro, molecular, and molar level research in psychology: the case of executive functions, self-regulation, and external regulation. Front. Psychol. 10:1919. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., and Justicia, F. (2003). Regulación de la enseñanza para la autorregulación del aprendizaje en la Universidad [Regulation of teaching for self-regulation of learning at the University]. Aula Abierta 82, 161–171.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Kauffman, D. F., Dempsey, M. S., and Kauffman, Y. (2021a). Coronavirus disease (COVID-19): psychoeducational variables involved in the health emergency. Front. Psychol. Retrieved from: https://www.frontiersin.org/research-topics/13709/coronavirus-disease-covid-19-psychoeducational-variables-involved-in-the-health-emergency (accessed April 21, 2015).

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Kauffman, D. F., Dempsy, M. S., and Kauffman, Y. (2021b). Analysis and psychoeducational implications of the behavior factor during the COVID-19 emergency. Front. Psychol. 12:613881. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.613881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Lahortiga-Ramos, F., Laspra-Solís, C., Maestro-Martín, C., Alustiza, I., Aubá, E., et al. (2020b). A structural equation model of achievement emotions, coping strategies and engagement-burnout in undergraduate students: a possible underlying mechanism in facets of perfectionism. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:2106. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17062106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., López, M., Zapata, L., Sollinas, G., and Fadda, S. (2015b). “Improving mental Health through an online self-assessment and self-help e-Utility in university students,” in Progress in Education, ed R. V. Nata (New York, NY: Nova Publisher).

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Mañas, I., Franco, C., Cangas, A. J., and Soriano, E. (2018). Differential effect of level of self-regulation and mindfulness training on coping strategies used by university students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15:2230. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15102230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Peralta-Sánchez, F. J., Garzón-Umerenkova, A., Vera, M. M., and Paoloni, P. (2019b). Applying the SRL vs ERL theory to the knowledge of achievement emotions in undergraduate university students. Front. Psychol. 10:2070. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Pachón-Basallo, M., Santos, F. H., Peralta-Sánchez, F. J., González-Torres, M. C., Artuch-Garde, R., et al. (2021e). How has the COVID-19 crisis affected the academic stress of university students? the role of teachers and students. Front. Psychol. 12:626340. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.626340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Paoloni, P. V., Vera-Martínez, M. M., and Garzón-Umerenkova, A. (2020c). Effect of levels of self-regulation and situational stress on achievement emotions in undergraduate students: class, study and testing. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020:4293. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17124293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Peralta-Sánchez, F. J., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Sander, P., Garzón-Umerenkova, A., and Zapata, L. (2020d). Effects of self-regulation vs. external regulation on the factors and symptoms of academic stress in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 11:1773. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Peralta-Sánchez, F. J., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Santos, F. H., Fadda, S., and Gaeta-González, L. G. (2020e). Do learning approaches set the stage for emotional well-being in college students? Sustainability 12:6984. doi: 10.3390/su12176984

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Pichardo, M. C., Justicia, F., and Berbén, A. (2008). Learning approaches, self-regulation and achievement in three European universities. Psicothema 20, 705–711.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Sander, P., Garzón-Umerenkova, A., Vera-Martínez, M. M., Fadda, S., and Gaetha, M. L. (2021c). Self-regulation and regulatory teaching as determinants of academic behavioral confidence and procrastination in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 12:602904. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.602904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Sander, P., Kauffman, D. F., and Yilmaz Soylu, M. (2020f). Differential effects of self- vs. external-regulation on learning approaches, academic achievement, and satisfaction in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 11:543884. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.543884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Sander, P., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Vera, M. M., Garzón, A., and Fadda, S. (2017b). Combined effect of levels in personal self-regulation and regulatory teaching on meta-cognitive, on meta-motivational, and on academic achievement variables in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 8:232. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Sander, P., and Putwain, D. (2013). Relationship between undergraduate student confidence, approach to learning and academic performance: the role of gender. Rev. Psicodidáct. 18, 375–393. doi: 10.1387/RevPsicodidact.7078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Santos, F. H., Garzón-Umerenkova, A., Fadda, S., Solinas, G., and Pignata, S. (2021d). Cross-sectional study of resilience, positivity and coping strategies as predictors of engagement-burnout in undergraduate students: implications for prevention and treatment in mental well-being. Front. Psychiatry12:596453. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.596453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Zapata, L., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Cardelle-Elawar, M., Sander, P., Justicia, F., et al. (2012). Regulatory teaching and self-regulated learning in college students: confirmatory validation study of the IATLP scales. Electr. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 10, 839–866. doi: 10.25115/ejrep.v10i27.1511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Zapata, L., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Sander, P., and Cardelle-Elawar, M. (2015c). The role of personal self-regulation and regulatory teaching to predict motivational-affective variables, achievement, and satisfaction: a structural model. Front. Psychol. 6:399. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

del Barrio, V., Carrasco, M. Á., and Holgado, F. P. (2006). Factor structure invariance in the Children's Big Five Questionnaire. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 22, 158–167. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.22.3.158

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frazier, L. D. (2020). The past, present, and future of the biopsychosocial model: A review of The Biopsychosocial Model of Health and Disease: New philosophical and scientific developments by Derek Bolton and Grant Gillett. New Ideas Psychol. 57:100755. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2019.100755

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagné, R. M. (1985). Conditions of Learning (4th ed.). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

Google Scholar

Garzón-Umerenkova, A., de la Fuente, J., Amate, J., Paoloni, P. V., Fadda, S., and Pérez, J. F. (2018). A linear empirical model of self-regulation on flourishing, health, procrastination, and achievement, among university students. Front. Psychol. 9:536. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garzón-Umerenkova, A., and Gil-Flores, J. (2017). El papel de la procrastinación académica como factor de la deserción universitaria. Rev. Complutense Educ. 28, 307–324. doi: 10.5209/rev_RCED.2017.v28.n1.49682

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ginns, P., Martin, A. J., and Papworth, B. (2018). Student learning in Australian high schools: Contrasting personological and contextual variables in a longitudinal structural model. Learn. Individ. Diff. 64, 83–93. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2018.03.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, R. E., Cowan, H. R., and McAdams, D. P. (2020). Personality and coping in life challenge narratives. J. Res. Personal. 86:103960. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2020.103960

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heikkilä, A., and Lonka, K. (2006). Studying in higher education: students' approaches to learning, self-regulation, and cognitive strategies. Stud. Higher Educ. 31, 99–117. doi: 10.1080/03075070500392433

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Justicia, F., Pichardo, M. C., Cano, F., Berbén, A. B., and de la Fuente, J. (2008). The revised two-factor study process questionnaire (RSPQ-2F): Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses at item level. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 23, 355–372. doi: 10.1007/BF03173004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karagiannopoulou, E., Milienos, F. S., and Athanasopoulos, V. (2018). Associations between defense styles, approaches to learning, and achievement among university students. Front. Educ. 3:53. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2018.00053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keith, T. Z. (2006). Multiple Regression and Beyond. Boston, MA: Pearson Education, Inc.

Google Scholar

Kember, D., Webster, B. J., and Chan, W. S. C. (2020). Refocusing the 3P model to incorporate a learning and teaching environment and graduate attributes. Educ. Psychol. 5, 592–607. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2020.1732304

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leszko, M., Iwański, R., and Jarzebińska, A. (2020). The Relationship between Personality Traits and Coping Styles among First-Time and Recurrent Prisoners in Poland. Front. Psychol. 10:2969. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lohr, S. (1999). Sampling: Design and Analysis. Boston, MA: Cengage Learning.

Google Scholar

Manzano-García, G., and Ayala-Calvo, J. C. (2013). New perspectives: towards an integration of the concept “burnout” and its explanatory models. Anal. Psicol. 29, 800–809. doi: 10.6018/analesps.29.3.145241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonnell, S., and Semkovska, M. (2020). Resilience as mediator between extraversion, neuroticism, and depressive symptoms in university students. J. Posit. Psychol. Wellbeing 4, 26–40.

Google Scholar

Miller, W. R., and Brown, J. M. (1991). Self-Regulation as a Conceptual Basis for the Prevention and Treatment of Addictive Behaviors. Self-Control and the Addictive Behaviors. Maxwell MacMillan Publishing.

Google Scholar

Moos, R. H., and Billings, A. G. (1982). Children of alcoholics during the recovery process: alcoholic and matched control families. Addict. Behav. 7, 155–163. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(82)90040-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pachón-Basallo, M., de la Fuente, J., and Gonzáles-Torres, M. C. (2021). Regulation/non-regulation/dys-regulation of health behavior, psychological reactance, and health of universityundergraduate students. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:3793. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18073793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paoloni, P.V. (2015). Class-Related Emotions Questionnaire. Spanish version, University of RÍo Cuarto, CONICET (Argentina).

Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., and Perry, R. P. (2002). Academic emotions in students' self-regulated learning and achievement: a program of qualitative and quantitative research. Educ. Psychol. 37, 91–105. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3702_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, R. P., Hladkyj, S., Pekrun, R. H., Clifton, R. A., and Chipperfield, J. G. (2005). Perceived academic control and failure in college students: A three-year study of scholastic attainment. Res. High. Educ. 46, 535–569.

Google Scholar

Pichardo, C., Justicia, F., de la Fuente, J., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., and Berbén, A. B. (2014). Factor structure of the self-regulation questionnaire (SRQ) at Spanish Universities. Spanish J. Psychol. 17:E62. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2014.63

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pichardo, M. C., Cano, F., Garzón-Umerenkova, A., de la Fuente, J., Peralta-Sánchez, F. J., and Amate-Romera, J. (2018). Self-regulation questionnaire (SRQ) in Spanish adolescents: factor structure and rasch analysis. Front. Psychol. 9:1370. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prosser, M., and Trigwell, K. (1999). Understanding Learning and Teaching. London: Society for Research into Higher Education and Open University Press.

Google Scholar

Quoidbach, J., Berry, E. V., Hansenne, M., and Mikolajczak, M. (2010). Positive emotion regulation and well-being: Comparing the impact of eight savoring and dampening strategies. Personal. Indiv. Differ. 49, 368–373. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosário, P. J. C., Núñez, P. J., Ferrando, M. O., Paiva, A., Lourenço, R., and Cerezo, A. (2013). The relationship between approaches to teaching and approaches to studying: a two-level structural equation model for biology achievement in high school. Metacogn. Learn. 8, 47–77. doi: 10.1007/s11409-013-9095-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudland, J. R., Golding, C., and Wilkinson, T. J. (2019). The stress paradox: How stress can be good for learning. Med. Educ. 54, 40–45. doi: 10.1111/medu.13830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sander, P. (2009). Current developments in measuring academic behavioural confidence. Psychol. Teach. Rev. 15, 32–44.

Google Scholar

Sander, P., and de la Fuente, J. (2020). Modelling students' academic confidence, personality and academic emotions. Curr. Psychol. 1–12. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-00957-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sander, P., de la Fuente, J., Stevenson, K., and Jones, T. (2011). The validation of the academic behavioural confidence scale with spanish psychology student. Psychol. Learn. Teach. 10, 11–24. doi: 10.2304/plat.2011.10.1.11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sander, P., and Sanders, L. (2006). Understanding academic confidence. Psych. Teach. Rev. 12, 29–39.

Google Scholar

Sander, P., and Sanders, L. (2009). Measuring academic behavioural confidence: The ABC scale revisited. Stud. Higher Educ. 34, 19–35. doi: 10.1080/03075070802457058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandín, B., and Chorot, P. (2003). Cuestionario de Afrontamiento del Estrés (CAE): desarrollo y validación preliminar. Rev. Psicopatol. Psicol. Clín. 8, 39–53. doi: 10.5944/rppc.vol.8.num.1.2003.3941

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B., and Bakker, A. B. (2003). UWES - Utrecht Job Commitment Scale: Test Manual. Utrech: Department of Psychology, Utrecht University.

Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B., Salanova, M., González-Romá, V., and Bakker, A. B. (2002). The measurement of engagement and burnout: A two sample confirmatory factor analytic approach. J. Happiness Stud. 3, 71–92. doi: 10.1023/A:1015630930326

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slavin, R. E. (2019). How evidence-based reform will transform research and practice in education. Educ. Psychol. 55, 21–31. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2019.1611432

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solomon, L. J., and Rothblum, E. D. (1984). Academic procrastination: frequency and cognitive-behavioral correlates. J. Counsel. Psychol. 31:503. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.31.4.503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2001a). Principal components and factor analysis. Using Multivar. Stat. 4, 582–633.

Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2001b). Cleaning up your act: Screening data prior to analysis. Using Multivariate Statisticts 5, 61–116.

Google Scholar

Trigwell, K. (2006). “An analysis of the relations between learning and teaching approaches,” in Lifelong Learning: Concepts and Contexts, eds P. Sutherland and J. Crowther (London: Routledge).

Google Scholar

Trigwell, K. (2012). “Scholarship of teaching and learning,” in University Teaching in Focus: A Learning-Centred Approach, eds L. Hunt and D. Chalmers (Camberwell VIC: Acer Press).

Google Scholar

Trigwell, K., and Ashwin, P. (2003). Undergraduate Students' Experience of Learning at the University of Oxford. Oxford: Oxford Learning Institute.

Google Scholar

Trigwell, K., Ashwin, P., and Millan, E. S. (2013). Evoked prior learning experience and approach to learning as predictors of academic achievement. Br. J. Educ. Psych. 83, 363–378. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.2012.02066.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trigwell, K., Ellis, R. A., and Han, F. (2012). Relations between students' approaches to learning, experienced emotions and outcomes of learning. Stud. High. Educ. 37, 811–824. doi: 10.1080/03075079.2010.549220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vázquez, C., Hervás, G., and Ho, S. (2006). Intervenciones clínicas basadas en la psicología positiva: fundamentos y aplicaciones. Psicol. Conduct. 14, 401–432.

Google Scholar

Weston, R., and Gore, P. A. A. (2006). Brief guide to structural equation modeling. Couns. Psychol. 34, 719–751. doi: 10.1177/0011000006286345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., Mao, Y., Niu, Y., Wei, D., Wang, X., and Qiu, J. (2020). Individual differences in neuroticism personality trait in emotion regulation. J. Affect. Disord. 265, 468–474.doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.01.086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, N. D., Mumby, M. A., and Smolinski, J. A. (2020). Maximizing Mental Health Services: Proven Practices That Promote Emotional Well-being. Wilmington, NC: Vernon-Press.

Google Scholar

Keywords: academic stress, protection and risk factors, 3P model, SLPS competency model, SRL vs. ERL theory, university students, COVID-19

Citation: de la Fuente J (2021) A Path Analysis Model of Protection and Risk Factors for University Academic Stress: Analysis and Psychoeducational Implications for the COVID-19 Emergency. Front. Psychol. 12:562372. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.562372

Received: 15 May 2020; Accepted: 15 June 2021;
Published: 13 August 2021.

Edited by:

Ana Lucia Pereira, Universidade Estadual de Ponta Grossa, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Zahra Hosseinkhani, Qazvin University of Medical Sciences, Iran
Rafael García-Ros, University of Valencia, Spain
Javier Fiz Pérez, European University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2021 de la Fuente. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jesús de la Fuente, jdlfuente@unav.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.