- School of Psychology, Education and Sport Sciences (FPCEE Blanquerna), Ramon Llull University, Barcelona, Spain
Knowing and measuring the psychosocial reactions of people to the coronavirus crisis could be useful for predicting citizen responsibility and psychological well-being in the general population. In this research, we present the COVID Reaction Scales (COVID-RS), a new tool that can measure and quantify the psychopathological reactions of the population to the COVID-19 crisis. The sample consisted of 667 subjects. Explorative and confirmative factor analyses were applied to examine the validity and reliability of the COVID-RS. Five dimensions were extracted that predicted 35.08% of the variance of the psychopathological reactions: (1) disorganized behaviors, (2) avoidant behaviors, (3) maladaptive information consumption, (4) herd behaviors and (5) loneliness. The results indicated that social quarantine induces and increases psychopathological reactions. However, emotional loneliness is reduced for each person with whom the respective subject lives during the quarantine. Finally, we can conclude that the COVID-RS has satisfactory validity and reliability. Measuring dysfunctional reactions to COVID-19 can enable the prediction of citizen responsibility.
Introduction
Most of the studies that related the coronavirus crisis to mental health focused on determining the psychopathological impact of the social quarantines enacted in Western and Asian countries (see Parmet and Sinha, 2020; Venkatesh and Edirappuli, 2020). In general, during the early phases of the pandemic, numerous publications noted significant increases in levels of anxiety and depression, as well as a growing trend of irrational behaviors in the general population (e.g., Ahorsu et al., 2020; Brooks et al., 2020; Lee, 2020; López and Rodó, 2020; Shanafelt et al., 2020). Once the international social quarantining measures were lifted (i.e., reopening of borders between the countries of the European Union), the social and psychiatric consequences of this crisis became more complex to analyze (e.g., Escolà-Gascón et al., 2020; Frías et al., 2020). The main reason is that there are still no behavioral and psychosocial markers that allow effective decisions to be made to prevent the spread of the coronavirus and safeguard the quality of life of the population. As a demonstration of this problem, many scientific articles that gave solutions regarding how to solve this crisis were retracted (see Yeo-Teh and Tang, 2020).
The results of this study offer new statistically valid and consistent psychometric measures to examine the processes of psychosocial adaptation and dysfunctional management of the general population in the face of this international crisis. Specifically, we offer the development of a new scale that aims to characterize and quantify the psychopathological reactions of the general population in response to the coronavirus crisis. This new scale is called the COVID Reaction Scales (COVID-RS).
The consequences of the COVID-19 crisis can be summarized hypothetically in three dimensions (in addition to the medical-health dimension): (1) Changes in social behavior (e.g., Armitage and Nellums, 2020; Bavel et al., 2020); (2) Changes in consumption of information (e.g., Innerarity and Colomina, 2020a,b; Masip et al., 2020); and (3) Socioeconomic changes (e.g., Bonaccorsi et al., 2020; Nicola et al., 2020). All these characteristics can be defined in many ways, but in this research, they will be examined from a psychological and clinical perspective (see De Sousa et al., 2020).
The first dimension refers to the various beliefs or conceptions about lifestyle, socialization behaviors and the quality of mental health of people (see Lau et al., 2005; Chan et al., 2020). For example, Zhang and Ma (2020) reported that more than 50% of the Chinese population felt panic and horror at the possibility of contracting COVID-19 (included also generalized anxiety). Likewise, symptoms related to posttraumatic stress were identified (e.g., Boyraz and Legros, 2020; Horesh and Brown, 2020; Liang et al., 2020). Irrational behaviors were also observed, associated with stocking up on food and with dietary changes that many people made during confinement, most notably eating high-calorie foods (see Mattioli et al., 2020). Likewise, a sharp increase in compulsive buying of hygienic products (especially toilet paper, which was sold out in most supermarkets) was reported (see Pagano et al., 2020; Zhou et al., 2020). This type of behavior is related to herd behavior and the pseudoscientific beliefs that the general population have developed in response to the uncertainty surrounding the COVID-19 pandemic (see Escolà-Gascón et al., 2020). Some authors ask whether these behaviors can be explained by the generalized panic and collective fear that the population has perceived in the face of the coronavirus crisis (see Khan et al., 2020).
The second, referring to changes in information consumption, can be characterized during the first months of the pandemic as (1) accelerated digitization. This concept means that communication and social interaction were massively digitized (see Innerarity and Colomina, 2020a). (2) Disintermediation. This concept refers to the disappearance of media outlets that facilitated the understanding of technical information (see García-Morales, 2020). (3) Infodemic, which is an overabundance of COVID-19-related information (see Innerarity and Colomina, 2020b). According to Andreu-Sánchez and Martín-Pascual (2020), one of the consequences of disintermediation is the indiscriminate appearance of hoaxes or “fake news” about the coronavirus since many local media acted as filters that prevented disinformation. Currently, it is the direct consumer of the information who must filter and screen which news he or she decides to believe and which not (see Aleixandre-Benavent et al., 2020). The problem is that not everyone has sufficient skill and knowledge to effectively screen information (see Pulido et al., 2020a). In fact, Pulido et al. (2020b) observed that fake news is “tweeted” or disseminated more on social networks than scientifically-based information. This can have very negative effects on how the population reacts to the pandemic, which could lead to failed preventive health measures against the advancing virus. For example, Escolà-Gascón et al. (2020) found that pseudoscientific beliefs and positive psychotic symptoms had increased significantly after a social quarantine of 57 days (the study was conducted with a Spanish population; bear in mind that the duration of the quarantine varies according to the legislation and the situation of each country). Determining the social consequences of collective psychosis related to the consumption of information is something that is still in the process of analysis, and more results based on scientific evidence are required to reach a conclusive conclusion (see Van Rheenen et al., 2020).
Third, socioeconomic changes represent the most difficult factor to operationalize in psychological terms. This dimension may be best characterized by the records of the regularization of labor promoted by some governments, such as border closures and the suspension of certain social and leisure activities (i.e., restaurants, hotels, sports centers, etc.), the granting of economic in the United States and deferring tax payments or offering tax relief in the United Kingdom and Spain (see Boletín Oficial del Estado, 2020; Deloitte Insights, 2020; Government UK, 2020; Nicola et al., 2020). Actually, a few years ago, Barbisch et al. (2015) had already reflected on the viability of social quarantine by comparing previous mutations of the SARS virus with ebolavirus and pointed out that it could have economic consequences that in the medium or long term would not be sustainable for governments. Regarding people who kept their jobs by teleworking, scientific evidence suggests that fatigue and mental exhaustion are the main psychological consequences of increased perceived work stress (e.g., Tavares et al., 2020).
These three factors are directly related to the psychological well-being of the population and the decisions that each person makes regarding how to react to this crisis (e.g., Escolà-Gascón et al., 2020). In reality, psychological decisions and psychopathological reactions were not variables taken into account in the mathematical models that were developed to predict the epidemiological behavior of coronavirus transmission (see Ivorra et al., 2020). The lack of experimental and valid data concerning the psychopathological impact of this crisis questions the effectiveness of these mathematical models to predict the medical and psychosocial consequences derived from the COVID-19 (see Yeo-Teh and Tang, 2020).
The definition of psychopathological reactions in this study are based on the attachment theory developed by Ainsworth and Bowlby (1991). This theory argues that humans face the daily problems of adult life based on learning and the affective bond developed from childhood. Thus, the concept of “reaction” should be understood in this study as the predominant coping style in each subject, determined by their prior relationships and learning. These styles can be psychopathological when affective relationships are learned and developed in a dysfunctional way. Ainsworth and Bowlby (1991) call this dysfunctional quality insecure attachment. Subjects who have an insecure attachment tend to have a negative view of themselves, self-describe and remain in a state of defensive anxiety on a regular basis (read Camps-Pons et al., 2014). Likewise, insecure attachment can be classified into three coping styles: avoidant, dependent-ambivalent, and disorganized. In this research, we will focus on the avoidant (with anxious and paranoid characteristics) and disorganized (with schizoid and schizotypal characteristics) styles. Avoidant attachment is characterized by the presence of social anxiety, attitudes of distrust in social relationships, and feelings of vulnerability. In contrast, disorganized attachment is characterized by the presence of irrational beliefs, impersonal or cold social relationships, and relentless negative thinking. Therefore, the term “psychopathological reactions” refers to coping styles that meet the characteristics of avoidant and disorganized profiles (e.g., Wang et al., 2020). These coping styles acquire much emphasis when international crises or natural catastrophes occur, so they represent an essential object of study (see Sung et al., 2020; Tian et al., 2020).
Finally, the concept of loneliness or levels of loneliness is defined in this investigation as established by de Jong-Gierveld and Kamphuis (1985). This conception is characterized by understanding loneliness based on two main psychological parameters: the lack of emotional support and the subjective suffering that each individual perceives when they are psychologically alone (see also Trejnowska et al., 2020). More concretely, in a pandemic context, loneliness is also defined as the fear of losing social supports or being physically alone, as well as increased anxiety due to the uncertainty regarding what the individual must personally endure (see Hwang et al., 2020).
Materials and Methods
Description of the Sample
A total of 667 participants from the general population participated (30.9% were men and 69.1% were women). All of them were of legal age (mean = 32.46; standard deviation = 10.373). A total of 34.5% resided in the community of Catalonia, 28% in Madrid, 19.8% in Castilla-La Mancha and 17.7% resided in Andalusia. All participants were asked the number of people they had lived with during the 57 days of confinement (mean = 2.07; standard deviation = 1.486). Given that the coronavirus had impacted differently in each of the regions, sociodemographic data were collected concerning the educational level, the presence of psychiatric history, and the economy vs. health dilemma. They were also asked if they had contracted the coronavirus disease. Table 1 classifies the four previous variables according to the autonomous community in which each subject resides.
The sociodemographic information was obtained in a self-reported manner, and the subjects signed written informed consent as voluntary authorization to participate in this research.
Instruments Used
De Jong-Gierveld Loneliness Scale (DJGLS)
The DJGLS is a questionnaire consisting of 11 items that examine the perceived loneliness of the subject according to the social deprivation theoretical model developed by Peplau and Perlman (1982). The items are statements that express different situations and desires for social contact with other people. All of them were written by de Jong-Gierveld and Kamphuis (1985). The answers are coded as follows: “yes” = 2 points, “more or less” = 1 point and “No” = 0 points. It should be noted that items 1, 2, 4, 7, 8, and 11 must be scored inversely, so that “yes” = 0 points, “more or less” = 1 point and “No” = 2 points. All the answers are added together, and the total result will be the direct score of the perceived levels of loneliness. In this study, the Spanish adaptation was developed by Buz et al. (2014). The validity and reliability of the scores of this scale were excellent in their original version, but the Spanish version showed a better internal consistency index than the initial scale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.91).
COVID Reaction Scales (COVID-RS)
This scale was developed by Álex Escolà-Gascón and aimed to measure the psychopathological reactions and the way each subject copes with the coronavirus crisis. It consists of 31 items expressed in the form of statements. The responses are scored according to the Likert model, which ranges from 0 (which means “completely disagree”) to 4 (which means “totally agree”). The items are grouped into five dimensions contrasted and validated in this report: (1) avoidant behaviors (AB); (2) disorganized behaviors (DB); (3) Maladaptive information consumption (MI); (4) Loneliness (LO); and (5) Herd behavior (HB). The development process of the items and the clinical contents that each scale evaluates are described in the procedures section (see Table 2). The reliability and validity of the COVID-RS were analyzed in this study.
Table 2. Description of the theoretical framework related to the coping styles and COVID-RS questionnaire development.
Procedures
This research follows an ex post facto or correlational methodological design. The procedure can be classified into two large blocks: the procedure related to the development of the COVID-RS questionnaire and the procedure related to sampling.
Development of COVID Reaction Scales (COVID-RS) Items
The items were written taking into account 4 sources of information: (1) the theories related to coping and attachment styles (see Ainsworth and Bowlby, 1991); (2) the statistical evidence describing the changes in information consumption during the COVID-19 crisis (e.g., Pulido et al., 2020b); (3) the loneliness model proposed by de Jong-Gierveld and Kamphuis (1985); and (4) the empirical evidence regarding the most common pathological behaviors during the first social quarantine (see Escolà-Gascón et al., 2020). Table 2 summarizes the clinical indicators of the COVID-RS to specify more clearly the relationship between each construct and item.
In total, 31 items were written in the form of statements or phrases. All of them were reviewed and approved by the research team of this report. Although coding the responses is the same for all items, the COVID-RS was designed to take into account two application contexts: The first 15 items were written to be answered in the current context, and from a more general perspective, they are written in the present tense. The rest of the items are written in the present perfect because they intend to integrate the psychological consequences and possible metric biases derived from the first mass confinement that was experienced in the European Union (e.g., Brooks et al., 2020). This study tests the validity and reliability of the 31 items of the COVID-RS.
Development of Sampling
The sample was obtained through the online application and distribution of the two questionnaires specified in the previous section. Google Forms was used to digitize the items and responses. The massive online application of the tests on social networks and WhatsApp began on July 22 and ended on August 04, 2020. The first raw data matrix obtained was cleaned and because 27 of the participants were minors, these cases were eliminated from the original matrix. There were no blank responses, and no missing values were identified. Once the matrix was refined, 667 final subjects remained, which are the responses analyzed in this report. All participants checked the acceptance box before responding to the scales.
Ethics Statement
The Committee of Ethical Guarantees of Ramon Llull University, (Barcelona, Spain) reviewed, favorably evaluated, and approved this research. Likewise, the procedures of this study adhere to the Spanish Government Data Protection Act 15/1999 and the Declaration of Helsinki of 1975, revised in 2013.
Data Analysis
The data were processed with the JAMOVI open-access statistical program (see The Jamovi Project, 2020). First, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was applied. The factors were extracted by parallel analysis and the unweighted least squares method (see Reise et al., 2000). The Promax rotation was applied. From the solution obtained in the EFA, the structural equations were applied adjusting a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) model. The parameters were estimated using the maximum likelihood method, and the respective fit indices provided by the AMOS program (an extension of SPSS 25 specialized in structural equations) were applied. According to Kline (2013) and Abad et al. (2015) the following adjustment indices and thresholds were used: root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA, threshold ≤0.05); adjusted goodness of fit index (AGFI, threshold ≥0.9); parsimony ratio (PRATIO, threshold ≥0.9); parsimony adjustment to the comparative fit index (PCFI, threshold ≥0.8); comparative fit index (CFI, threshold ≥0.95); Tucker-Lewis coefficient (TLI, threshold ≥0.95); and incremental fit index (IFI, threshold ≥0.95).
Given that this program allows obtaining the Bayes information criterion (BIC), Akaike information criterion (AIC) and consistent Akaike information criterion (CAIC) indices, which indicate the degree of misfit in the model, the Mismatch Reduction Ratio (MRR) was estimated following the deviance expression developed by Pardo and Ruiz (2015):
where
−2LL0 is the deviation from the null model,
−2LL1 is the deviation from the proposed theoretical model,
MRR is the Mismatch Reduction Ratio,
AIC0 is the AIC index corresponding to the null model and
AIC1 is the AIC index corresponding to the theoretical model.
The reliability of the COVID-RS was calculated from the internal consistency indices based on Cronbach’s alpha. Given that the items are ordinal variables, the omega coefficient by McDonald (1999) is:
where λj is the factor loading of item j,
is the communality of item j, and
ψ is the unique variance.
According to Abad et al. (2015), the threshold used to interpret omega coefficient and Cronbach’s alpha coefficient is 0.6. The results of this coefficient above this value indicate acceptable internal consistency values. However, for the BIC, AIC, and CAIC indices, there are no specific thresholds values and for this reason the MRR index is used (check this information in Pardo and Ruiz, 2015).
Results
Exploratory Factor Analysis
The EFA of all the items of the COVID-RS is presented in Tables 3, 4.
A total of 5 factors were extracted that together explained 35.08% of the variance of the data. The first factor was composed of items 1, 3, 5, 6, 9, 10, 14, and 15. Taking into account the content of the items (see Table 2), it was called Disorganized behaviors (DB). The second consisted of items 2, 4, 7, 8, 11, 12, and 13. The content of the items referred to Avoidant behaviors (AB). The third group included items 16, 19, 21, 25, 26, and 27, and called Maladaptive information consumption (MI). The fourth grouped items 17, 18, 22, 23, and 24, the content of which indicated that it should be called Herd behavior (HB). The last factor had items 28, 29, 30, and 31 and was called Loneliness (LO). These factors were used for fitting the confirmatory model presented below.
Confirmatory Factor Analysis
Taking advantage of the results of the EFA, it was then checked whether it was possible to extract new latent variables using a second-order analysis. The content of the items (see Table 2) and the theoretical framework suggested that HB and DB could form a higher-order factor related to dissociation. Similarly, AB and DB have in common that their items are related to mistrust (insecure coping style). If we take into account that MI also includes attributes of anxiety, then AB, DB, and MI could form a new higher order factor related to symptoms of anxiety. This logic allowed fitting the confirmatory model of Figure 1.
Figure 1. Theoretical model of the COVID-RS scale, showing covariance and standardized regression coefficients. P-values were not included for each coefficient because all of them were significant <0.05.
The latent variables LA (Lack of awareness) and CAI (Coronavirus Anxiety Impact Index) were defined. Both factors predicted between 43.8 and 57.6% of the variance of the first-order factors extracted in the first EFA.
Table 5 shows the fit indices of the null model (independent) and those of the theoretical model related to the COVID-RS. The table also includes the Mismatch Reduction Ratio (MRR).
Table 5. Model fit indices of the theoretical model (see Figure 1).
Although the Chi Square statistic has yielded a significance critical level, it should be noted that it is highly sensitive to the sample size, so it becomes inconsistent at the statistical level (see Gorsuch, 1983). Instead, the analysis of the comparative fit indices is recommended, which show values greater than 0.95. Likewise, the RMSEA (root mean square error of approximation), AGFI (adjusted goodness of fit index), and PRATIO (parsimony ratio) indices also showed acceptable and satisfactory values that approve model fit. The estimation of the MRR indicated that the model manages to reduce the misfit between 79 and 85%.
These analyses allow us to conclude that the COVID-RS is a valid questionnaire for examining the psychopathological reactions of the general population to the coronavirus crisis.
Reliability Analysis
Tables 6–8 present the descriptive statistics associated with the items of both the COVID-RS and of the DJGLS.
The descriptive statistics of the scales of both tests and the Cronbach’s alpha and McDonald’s omega reliability coefficients were obtained by summing the responses. This information is presented in Table 9.
Table 9. Descriptive statistics for all dimensions of the COVID-RS and de Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale. Reliability coefficients are also included.
In general, the results obtained satisfactorily highlight the reliability of the scores of both the COVID-RS and DJGLS scales. However, the reliability coefficients of the LA factor were the lowest.
Analysis of Perceived Loneliness
The correlations between the LO, DJGLS scale and, the number of people with whom each subject had lived during the periods of confinement (hereinafter NPPL) were calculated. Table 10 shows the correlation matrix.
Table 10. Correlation matrix between loneliness scales (LO and DJGLS) and number of people the participant lived with during the social confinement.
The simple linear regression of the NPPL and LO indicates that, for every person with whom each participant lives, the levels of loneliness are reduced by 1.1 points (within the LO metric, which ranges between 0 and 16). The value 1.1 is the unstandardized regression coefficient or β1. The model constant (β0) was 10.398. In total, the NPPL variable explains 18% of the reduction in levels of solitude.
Discussion
The main objective of this study was to facilitate the validity and reliability of new statistical measures concerning the psychopathological reactions of the population amid the COVID-19 crisis. Analyses using structural equations and internal consistency coefficients revealed that the COVID-RS provides valid and reliable scores to measure the psychopathological reactions of the population to this crisis.
Interpretation and Speculation on the Results
On the one hand, the indices obtained in the factorial analyses (both in their exploratory format and in the model of Figure 1) suggest that the reactions of the population identified in the scientific literature (see Ahorsu et al., 2020; Brooks et al., 2020; Lee, 2020; López and Rodó, 2020; Shanafelt et al., 2020) can be measured validly and reliably in 5 general dimensions: disorganized behaviors (DB), avoidant behaviors (AB), maladaptive information consumption (MI), herd behaviors (HB) and loneliness (LO). This allows for 2 general interpretations:
(1) The presence of the MI dimension supports the results and conclusions obtained in some studies that warn of the social danger of infodemia, disinformation, and the acceleration of digital media. What measures have governments or public organizations applied to control the quality of information about the coronavirus is something that has not been scientifically evaluated (e.g., Escolà-Gascón et al., 2020). However, taking into account the parameters of Figure 1, it cannot be denied that the dysfunctional consumption of COVID-19 information is a psychological reaction that negatively affects the mental health of people. This is because the coronavirus anxiety impact index (CAI) can predict up to 43.82% of dysfunctional information consumption (R2 ≈ 0.6622 = 0.438). Although this measure based on R2 is an approximate estimate, it is evidence that shows the strength of the relationship between anxiety and the consumption of COVID-19 information. Therefore, it is necessary to provide the general population with digital and psychological resources to promote the correct use of information.
(2) The HB dimension coincides with other studies that warned of the irrational behavior of the population amid the uncertainty related to the COVID-19 crisis (e.g., Pagano et al., 2020; Zhou et al., 2020). Interestingly, the Lack of awareness (LA) index negatively predicted the Herd behaviors (HB) dimension (−0.707). This result is inconsistent with the herd behavior theory since it is precisely the dissociation or disconnection with reality that leads to irrational behaviors that are not logically explained. This negative regression coefficient does not coincide with some studies that positively related herd behaviors with panic behaviors and lack of awareness (e.g., Saglietto et al., 2020). On the one hand, considering the content of the items, this result supports the possibility that HB also measures obsessive-compulsive behaviors, which are positively correlated with cognitive self-consciousness (e.g., Cohen and Calamari, 2004). Then, cognitive self-consciousness would be a mediating variable that could explain the effects of LA on HB. On the other hand, the negative correlation −0.99 between CAI and LA indicates clearly that both indices measure the same construct (anxiety reactions) but from two opposite poles according to the level of consciousness (see Öhman, 2008): LA refers to anxious reactions with low levels of consciousness and CAI is related to anxious reactions with high levels of consciousness. This hypothesis would imply that HB would be positively correlated with CAI. This last logic and classification coincides with the contemporary literature on the psychological evidence identified on coronavirus (e.g., Wang et al., 2020). However, it is recommended in future research to validate the COVID-RS model by including the cognitive self-consciousness variable as a mediator and by estimating an extra parameter that predicts the effects of CAI on HB. Likewise, LA and CAI are hypothetical latent factors. This means that in future studies the concurrent and predictive validity of these two factors should be analyzed with other previously validated anxiety scales.
Finally, the correlation matrix of Table 10 suggests that LO and DJGLS do not measure the same type of loneliness. Like the LA and CAI indices, it is possible that both scales measure different facets of the “loneliness” construct. Analyzing the items of the LO scale, it can be concluded that their contents express the desire for emotional connection and the illusion of sharing leisure time with other people. In contrast, the items of the DJGLS scale focus on the evaluation of social desire but also include 6 items that examine the lack of emotional support. In this sense, it is completely understandable for a person to miss and look forward to being reunited with their loved ones (concept of loneliness measured in LO) and at the same time feel loved and emotionally supported (loneliness evaluated on the DJGLS scale). Therefore, when using the LO scale, it should be taken into account that it is a kind of loneliness based on social and affective desire but not on the lack of psychological support (social deprivation). This argument justifies why the correlation between both scales is so low. Based on these results and if in the future the population should be confined again, the following health/psychological recommendation can be offered: loneliness is less dysfunctional if the subject lives with at least 2 more people. Therefore, it seems advisable to develop confinement situations where people can live with two other people so that deteriorating mental health is not so harmful to people.
Possible Limitations
The limitations of this research are focused on methodological, theoretical, and sampling aspects.
First, the methodological limitations are mainly found in the reliability coefficients of the LA and CAI indices. Although the omega coefficients of both factors reach the minimum acceptance range, they are still low values (see McDonald, 1999). Something similar occurs with the LO scale. How to mathematically manipulate these scales to improve their reliability is something that in psychometric terms is not salvageable with the data of this research. However, based on the negative correlation observed between LO and NPPL, as an alternative to this limitation, it is proposed to include the following mathematical transformation to try to optimize the LO scale scores:
The expression wNPPL is the number of people with whom the participant lived during confinement. ∑nLO is the sum of the responses of the items belonging to the LO scale. LOmax. is the maximum score of the LO scale, which in this case would be 16.
Although this formula is intended to be a more effective alternative than the total sum of the responses of the LO items, it should be statistically tested before being used to make clinical decisions. For this, it is proposed to use a new sample (if possible a clinical sample) and to replicate the internal structure of the COVID-RS questionnaire. Likewise, as a complement to this methodological limitation, we should highlight the lack of tests regarding the concurrent, convergent, and discriminant validities. These psychometric properties should be examined in future analyses.
Second, at the conceptual level, it should be noted that the items of the AB and DB scales do not directly measure coping styles; they measure coping styles adapted to the current context of the COVID-19 pandemic. In reality, they reinforce or contextualize the theories of Ainsworth and Bowlby (1991). Therefore, these scales should not be used as direct or explicit measures of coping styles. Along the same lines, there are certain difficulties in interpreting the factors CAI, LA, and LO. Although the results of the structural equations and correlations suggest that CAI represents fear due to excess activation or anxiety, LA represents fear due to the absence of insight, and LO represents loneliness understood in terms of desires to reunite, new models of structural equations would be necessary to validate its theoretical structure. More specifically and as already suggested, new models should be analyzed to test how the presence of a third factor that groups CAI and LA in the same construct influences the fit and the relationship between these variables.
Finally, the sample used was not recruited using probabilistic procedures, so its representativeness is questionable outside the autonomous communities or regions not included in the analysis. This representativeness is also highly questionable if one takes into account that the subjects come from the general population and not from the clinical-psychiatric population. Thus, new psychometric analyses of the COVID-RS would be necessary in a sample of patients with a formal diagnosis. Likewise, an analysis of the invariance of the COVID-RS scores could be performed including vulnerable groups of the population (i.e., COVID-19 survivors, elderly and medical patients with a risk profile).
Main Conclusions
The main conclusions that can be deduced from the results and discussion are summarized in the following points:
(1) The COVID Reaction Scales (COVID-RS) is a valid and reliable psychometric test to examine the psychopathological reactions of the population to the coronavirus crisis. The COVID-RS scores can be used as decision criteria to predict how the population will react to government and health measures against the spread of COVID-19. However, before using the COVID-RS for this last purpose, the predictive validity of this scale should be examined. These measures could also be included in the mathematical models that predict the contagion curve for coronavirus.
(2) The psychopathological reactions of the population to the coronavirus crisis can be classified according to the attachment style theory proposed by Ainsworth and Bowlby (1991). Specifically, the structural equations identified two of these styles: avoidant and disorganized. These styles do not provide the population with functional tools for the psychological management of preventative health regulations against coronavirus.
(3) In health and psychological terms, there are reasons and statistical evidence that quarantine states do not harm the mental health and emotional loneliness of the subject when they are in the company of loved ones or family members. Specifically, emotional loneliness is reduced by 1.1 points on the LO scale for each person with whom the respective subject lives during the quarantine.
In general, the COVID-RS scale can be used as a valid and reliable tool for psychological and epidemiological measurement of the reactions of people regarding to their way of coping with the consequences derived from the coronavirus crisis. These measurements can be useful to make effective political and health decisions to confront the COVID-19 crisis successfully.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Committee of Ethical Guarantees of Ramon Llull University, (Barcelona, Spain). Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.
Author Contributions
ÁE-G conceived and planned the study, collected the sample, performed the statistical analyses and wrote the manuscript in consultation with F-XM and JR. JG supervised the project. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.607064/full#supplementary-material
References
Abad, F. J., Olea, J., Ponsoda, V., and García, C. (2015). MEDICIÓN en Ciencias Sociales y de la Salud. Madrid: Editorial Síntesis.
Ahorsu, D., Lin, C., Imani, V., Saffari, M., Griffiths, M., and Pakpour, A. (2020). The fear of COVID-19 scale: development and initial validation. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 27, 1–7. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00270-8
Ainsworth, M. S., and Bowlby, J. (1991). An ethological approach to personality development. Am. Psychol. 46, 333–341. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.46.4.333
Aleixandre-Benavent, R., Castelló-Cogollos, L., and Valderrama-Zurián, J. C. (2020). Información y comunicación durante los primeros meses de Covid-19. Prof. Inf. 29:e290408.
Andreu-Sánchez, C., and Martín-Pascual, M. A. (2020). Fake images of the SARS-CoV-2 coronavirus in the communication of information at the beginning of the first Covid-19 pandemic. Prof. Inf. 29:e290309.
Armitage, R., and Nellums, L. (2020). COVID-19 and the consequences of isolating the elderly. Lancet Public Health 5:e256. doi: 10.1016/s2468-2667(20)30061-x
Barbisch, D., Koenig, K., and Shih, F. (2015). Is there a case for quarantine? perspectives from SARS to Ebola. Disaster Med. Public Health Prep. 9, 547–553. doi: 10.1017/dmp.2015.38
Bavel, J., Baicker, K., Boggio, P., Capraro, V., Cichocka, A., Cikara, M., et al. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat. Human Behav. 4, 460–471. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z
Boletín Oficial del Estado (2020). Real Decreto 463/2020, de 14 de Marzo, por el que se Declara el Estado de Alarma para la Gestión de la Situación de crisis Sanitaria Ocasionada por el COVID-19. Available online at: https://www.boe.es/diario_boe/txt.php?id=BOE-A-2020-3692 (accessed March 14, 2020)
Bonaccorsi, G., Pierri, F., Cinelli, M., Flori, A., Galeazzi, A., Porcelli, F., et al. (2020). Economic and social consequences of human mobility restrictions under COVID-19. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 117, 15530–15535. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2007658117
Boyraz, G., and Legros, D. (2020). Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) and traumatic stress: probable risk factors and correlates of posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Loss Trauma 25, 503–522. doi: 10.1080/15325024.2020.1763556
Brooks, S., Webster, R., Smith, L., Woodland, L., Wessely, S., Greenberg, N., et al. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet 395, 912–920. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(20)30460-8
Buz, J., Urchaga, D., and Polo, M. E. (2014). Factor structure of the de jong gierveld loneliness scale in spanish elderly adults. Anal. Psicol. 30, 588–596. doi: 10.6018/analesps.30.2.148371
Camps-Pons, S., Castillo-Garayoa, J. A., and Cifre, I. (2014). Attachment and psychopathology in formerly maltreated adolescents and young adults: clinical implications. Clínica y Salud 25, 67–74. doi: 10.5093/cl2014a6
Chan, J., Yuan, S., Kok, K., To, K., Chu, H., Yang, J., et al. (2020). A familial cluster of pneumonia associated with the 2019 novel coronavirus indicating person-to-person transmission: a study of a family cluster. Lancet 395, 514–523. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(20)30154-9
Cohen, R., and Calamari, J. (2004). Thought-focused attention and obsessive–compulsive symptoms: an evaluation of cognitive self-consciousness in a nonclinical sample. Cogn. Ther. Res. 28, 457–471. doi: 10.1023/b:cotr.0000045558.75538.ff
de Jong-Gierveld, J., and Kamphuis, F. H. (1985). The development of a rasch-type loneliness scale. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 9, 289–299. doi: 10.1177/014662168500900307
De Sousa, A., Mohandas, E., and Javed, A. (2020). Psychological interventions during COVID-19: challenges for low and middle income countries. Asian J. Psychiatr. 51:102128. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102128
Deloitte Insights (2020). COVID-19 Potential Implications for the Banking and Capital Markets Sector. Available online at: https://www2.deloitte.com/global/en/insights/economy/covid-19/banking-and-capital-markets-impact-covid-19.html (accessed July 20, 2020).
Escolà-Gascón, Á, Marín, F., Rusiñol, J., and Gallifa, J. (2020). Pseudoscientific beliefs and psychopathological risks increase after COVID-19 social quarantine. Glob. Health 16:78. doi: 10.1186/s12992-020-00603-1
Frías, A., Navarro, S., Palma, C., Farriols, N., Aliaga, F., Solves, L., et al. (2020). Clinical course and predictors in patients with borderline personality disorder during the COVID-19 outbreak: a 2.5-month naturalistic exploratory study in Spain. Psychiatr. Res. 292:113306. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113306
García-Morales, V. (2020). Donde habitan las mentiras: libertades de expresión e información en tiempos de odio e hiperinformación. Rev. CIDOB D’afers Int. 124, 25–48. doi: 10.24241/rcai.2020.124.1.25
Government UK (2020). Support for those affected by COVID-19. -19. https://www.gov.uk/find-coronavirus-support
Horesh, D., and Brown, A. D. (2020). Traumatic stress in the age of COVID-19: a call to close critical gaps and adapt to new realities. Psychol. Trauma 12, 331–335. doi: 10.1037/tra0000592
Hwang, T., Rabheru, K., Peisah, C., Reichman, W., and Ikeda, M. (2020). Loneliness and social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int. Psychogeriatr 32, 1–4. doi: 10.1017/s1041610220000988
Innerarity, D., and Colomina, C. (2020a). Introducción: desinformación y poder, la crisis de los intermediarios. Rev. CIDOB D’afers Int. 124, 7–10. doi: 10.24241/rcai.2020.124.1.7
Innerarity, D., and Colomina, C. (2020b). La verdad en las democracias algorítmicas. Rev. CIDOB D’afers Int. 124, 11–24. doi: 10.24241/rcai.2020.124.1.11
Ivorra, B., Ferrández, M., Vela-Pérez, M., and Ramos, A. (2020). Mathematical modeling of the spread of the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) taking into account the undetected infections. Commun. Nonlinear Sci. Numer. Simul. 88:105303. doi: 10.1016/j.cnsns.2020.105303
Khan, S., Siddique, R., Li, H., Ali, A., Shereen, M., Bashir, N., et al. (2020). Impact of coronavirus outbreak on psychological health. J. Global Health 10:010331. doi: 10.7189/jogh.10.010331
Kline, P. (ed.). (2013). Handbook of Psychological Testing. 2nd Edn, 752. London: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9781315812274
Lau, J., Yang, X., Pang, E., Tsui, H., Wong, E., and Wing, Y. (2005). SARS-related perceptions in Hong Kong. Emerg. Infect. Dis. 11, 417–424. doi: 10.3201/eid1103.040675
Lee, S. (2020). Coronavirus anxiety scale: a brief mental health screener for COVID-19 related anxiety. Death Stud. 44, 393–401. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2020.1748481
Liang, L., Gao, T., Ren, H., Cao, R., Qin, Z., Hu, Y., et al. (2020). Post-traumatic stress disorder and psychological distress in Chinese youths following the COVID-19 emergency. J. Health Psychol. 25, 1164–1175. doi: 10.1177/1359105320937057
López, L., and Rodó, X. (2020). The end of social confinement and COVID-19 re-emergence risk. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 746–755. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0908-8
Masip, P., Aran-Ramspott, S., Ruiz-Caballero, C., Suau, J., Almenar, E., and Puertas-Graell, D. (2020). Consumo informativo y cobertura mediática durante el confinamiento por el Covid-19: sobreinformación, sesgo ideológico y sensacionalismo. Prof. Inf. 29:e290312. doi: 10.3145/epi.2020.may.12
Mattioli, A., Sciomer, S., Cocchi, C., Maffei, S., and Gallina, S. (2020). Quarantine during COVID-19 outbreak: changes in diet and physical activity increase the risk of cardiovascular disease. Nutr. Metab. Cardiovasc. Dis. 30, 1409–1417. doi: 10.1016/j.numecd.2020.05.020
McDonald, R. P. (1999). Factor Analysis and Related Methods. New Jersey, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Nicola, M., Alsafi, Z., Sohrabi, C., Kerwan, A., Al-Jabir, A., Iosifidis, C., et al. (2020). The socio-economic implications of the coronavirus pandemic (COVID-19): a review. Intl. J. Surg. 78, 185–193. doi: 10.1016/j.ijsu.2020.04.018
Öhman, A. (2008). “Fear and anxiety: overlaps and dissociations,” in Handbook of emotions, eds M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, and L. F. Barrett (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 709–728.
Pagano, M., Hess, J., Tsang, H., Staley, E., Gernsheimer, T., Sen, N., et al. (2020). Prepare to adapt: blood supply and transfusion support during the first 2 weeks of the 2019 novel coronavirus (COVID -19) pandemic affecting Washington state. Transfusion 60, 908–911. doi: 10.1111/trf.15789
Pardo, A., and Ruiz, M. A. (2015). Análisis de Datos en Ciencias Sociales y de la Salud III. Madrid: Editorial síntesis.
Parmet, W., and Sinha, M. (2020). Covid-19 — the law and limits of quarantine. N. Engl. J. Med. 382:e28. doi: 10.1056/nejmp2004211
Peplau, L. A., and Perlman, D. (1982). “Perspectives on loneliness,” in in Loneliness: A Sourcebook of Current Theory, Research and Therapy, eds L. A. Peplau and Y. D. Perlman (New Jersey, NJ: John Wiley).
Pulido, C., Ruiz-Eugenio, L., Redondo-Sama, G., and Villarejo-Carballido, B. (2020a). A new application of social impact in social media for overcoming fake news in health. Intl. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:2430. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17072430
Pulido, C., Villarejo-Carballido, B., Redondo-Sama, G., and Gómez, A. (2020b). COVID-19 infodemic: more retweets for science-based information on coronavirus than for false information. Intl. Soc. 35, 377–392. doi: 10.1177/0268580920914755
Reise, S. P., Waller, N. G., and Comrey, A. L. (2000). Factor analysis and scale revision. Psychol. Assess. 12, 287–297. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.12.3.287
Saglietto, A., D’Ascenzo, F., Zoccai, G. B., and De Ferrari, G. M. (2020). COVID-19 in Europe: the Italian lesson. Lancet 395, 1110–1111. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30690-5
Shanafelt, T., Ripp, J., and Trockel, M. (2020). Understanding and addressing sources of anxiety among health care professionals during the COVID-19 pandemic. JAMA 323, 2133–2134. doi: 10.1001/jama.2020.5893
Sung, Y., Nam, T., and Hwang, M. (2020). Attachment style, stressful events, and Internet gaming addiction in Korean university students. Pers. Individ. Differ. 154:109724. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.109724
Tavares, F., Santos, E., Diogo, A., and Ratten, V. (2020). Teleworking in portuguese communities during the COVID-19 pandemic. Global Econ. doi: 10.1108/jec-06-2020-0113 Online ahead of print
The Jamovi Project (2020). Jamovi. (Version 1.0) [Computer Software]. Available online at: https://www.jamovi.org (accessed April 19, 2020)
Tian, Y., Chen, J., and Wu, X. (2020). Parental attachment, coping, and psychological adjustment among adolescents following an earthquake: a longitudinal study. Anxiety Stress Coping 33, 429–439. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2020.1746769
Trejnowska, A., Goodall, K., Rush, R., Ellison, M., and McVittie, C. (2020). The relationship between adult attachment and coping with brain tumour: the mediating role of social support. Psycho Oncol. 29, 729–736. doi: 10.1002/pon.5325
Van Rheenen, T., Meyer, D., Neill, E., Phillipou, A., Tan, E., Toh, W., et al. (2020). Mental health status of individuals with a mood-disorder during the COVID-19 pandemic in Australia: initial results from the COLLATE project. J. Affect. Disord. 275, 69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.06.037
Venkatesh, A., and Edirappuli, S. (2020). Social distancing in covid-19: what are the mental health implications? Br. Med. J. 369:1379. doi: 10.1136/bmj.m1379
Wang, H., Xia, Q., Xiong, Z., Li, Z., Xiang, W., Yuan, Y., et al. (2020). The psychological distress and coping styles in the early stages of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic in the general mainland Chinese population: a web-based survey. PLoS One 15:e0233410. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0233410
Yeo-Teh, N., and Tang, B. (2020). An alarming retraction rate for scientific publications on Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19). Account. Res. 1–7. doi: 10.1080/08989621.2020.1782203 Online ahead of print
Zhang, Y., and Ma, Z. (2020). Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic on mental health and quality of life among local residents in liaoning province, China: a cross-sectional study. Intl. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:2381. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17072381
Keywords: COVID-19, SARS-CoV-2, coronavirus, post-pandemic, coping styles
Citation: Escolà-Gascón Á, Marín F-X, Rusiñol J and Gallifa J (2020) Measuring Psychosocial Reactions to COVID-19: The COVID Reaction Scales (COVID-RS) as a New Assessment Tool. Front. Psychol. 11:607064. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.607064
Received: 17 September 2020; Accepted: 30 October 2020;
Published: 19 November 2020.
Edited by:
Sai-Fu Fung, City University of Hong Kong, Hong KongReviewed by:
Francesco Giancamilli, Sapienza University of Rome, ItalyIvan Herrera-Peco, Alfonso X El Sabio University, Spain
Copyright © 2020 Escolà-Gascón, Marín, Rusiñol and Gallifa. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Álex Escolà-Gascón, alexeg@blanquerna.url.edu