Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 21 December 2020
Sec. Perception Science
This article is part of the Research Topic An Integrated Model of the Olfactory System Through Phylogenetic, Evolutionary, and Clinical Aspects: From Invertebrates to Humans, Through Sensory, Perceptual, and Cognitive Olfactometry Studies. View all 9 articles

French Vividness of Olfactory Imagery Questionnaire: A Potential Tool for Diagnosing Olfactory Loss by Assessing Olfactory Imagery?

  • 1Université de Lorraine, INSERM, IADI, Nancy, France
  • 2Université de Lorraine, DevAH, Nancy, France
  • 3CIC 1433 Innovation Technologique, INSERM, Université de Lorraine, CHRU Nancy, Nancy, France
  • 4Université de Lorraine, InnoCIM, ENSAIA, Myrissi, Nancy, France

Several studies have shown a significant relationship between smelling and olfactory imagery abilities. The primary aim of the present study was to validate a French version of the Vividness of Olfactory Imagery Questionnaire (VOIQ). The secondary aim was to investigate its capability to differentiate individuals with smell loss from healthy individuals. After having elaborated a French translation of the VOIQ (fVOIQ), we evaluated olfactory imagery abilities of 387 French participants who anonymously self-completed the fVOIQ: 121 pathologic individuals (hyposmic and anosmic), 244 normosmic individuals (healthy non-expert), and 22 fragrance experts. Significant split-half reliability as expressed by Spearman correlation coefficients for the global sample, as well as for each group separately, indicated the excellent internal consistency of the fVOIQ. Moreover, results revealed a significant effect of the smelling ability group on fVOIQ score, suggesting that daily olfactory stimulation is fundamental to maintaining the ability to create a vivid image and that severe loss of smell may result in progressive impairment of olfactory imagery. Our fVOIQ and the original English version seemingly have similarly high benefit in differentiating experts and normosmic individuals based on their olfactory imagery ability. Moreover, the fVOIQ seems capable of differentiating individuals with loss of smell from healthy individuals. These findings demonstrate the reliability and validity of the fVOIQ, and its capability to differentiate individuals’ smelling ability according to their olfactory imagery ability.

Introduction

Sensory imagery refers to the volitional mental simulation of sensory experiences and is known to be feasible in every sensory modality (Andrade et al., 2012). Several studies have shown that recall of sensory information is possible thanks to modality-specific neural networks, which overlap with those of their respective sensory modalities (Farah, 1988; Kosslyn et al., 2001). Consequently, there are forms of coherence between real perception and sensory imagery. For example, consistencies can be observed between perceived and imaged stimuli. In the case of olfaction, the pleasantness and intensity of a smell are preserved when imaged (Carrasco and Ridout, 1993; Sugiyama et al., 2006). However, of all senses, olfactory imagery seems to be particularly challenging (Brower, 1947; Lindauer, 1969). The lesser importance of olfaction in everyday life and the unawareness of having formed an image are potential reasons for the difficulty of olfactory imagery (Stevenson and Case, 2005). Therefore, wider interindividual variability can be observed during olfactory imagery tasks, than in any other sensory modality.

Numerous factors influencing olfactory imagery vividness have been studied. Flohr et al. (2014) studied the effect of smell loss on the ability to create an image. This study demonstrated that olfactory loss was an important factor in olfactory imagery ability. Regular exposure to olfactive stimuli would be key in forming an image. Therefore, many pathologies resulting in smell loss could impact imagery, such as sinonasal polyposis, or coronavirus (Lechien et al., 2020). Therefore, the assessment of olfactory imagery ability in regard to its relationship with real odor perception could provide help for the early assessment of smell loss. A specific tool capable of assessing olfactory imagery ability is thus needed.

Several authors have worked on creating questionnaires assessing sensorial imagery (Betts, 1910; Sheehan, 1967; Marks, 1973; White et al., 1978), though few specifically assess olfactory imagery. Questionnaires based on Betts’ Questionnaire upon Mental Imagery, which was initially developed for assessing 150 items pertaining to all sensory modalities, are time-consuming, which can induce a bias when they are completed. Gilbert et al. (1998) developed the Vividness of Olfactory Imagery Questionnaire (VOIQ) adapted from the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (VVIQ—Marks, 1973). Specific to the assessment of olfactory imagery, the VOIQ offers a contextual approach to the odors that are to be imagined. The total of 16 items makes the VOIQ a reasonably short questionnaire, which can be completed within 5–10 min. Its simplicity makes it possible for individuals to complete on their own. Gilbert et al. (1998) showed that the VOIQ allows differentiation between the olfactive imagery abilities of fragrance experts and healthy non-expert individuals. Therefore, the regular exposure to olfactory stimuli may be a key to form a vivid image (Royet et al., 2013). Moreover, Flohr et al. (2014) including 16 patients with severe smell loss suggested that olfactory loss might be an important factor in imagery abilities. However, these findings need to be confirmed with larger samples. Currently, the VOIQ is adapted to English-speaking populations. Therefore, the development of a French version of this questionnaire is of great interest.

The primary aim of the present study was to validate a French version of the VOIQ (fVOIQ). The secondary aim was to investigate its capability to differentiate individuals with smell loss from healthy individuals. To accomplish this, we compared olfactory imagery abilities of fragrance expert, normosmic (healthy non-expert), and pathologic (hyposmic and anosmic) individual samples. We hypothesized that experts would have a better olfactory imagery ability as compared to healthy non-expert and pathologic individuals. Similarly, we expected better olfactory imagery ability for healthy non-expert individuals as compared to those who were pathologic.

Materials and Methods

Participants

387 participants volunteered for this study (see Table 1). They were recruited by either a web-based (web) or a paper & pencil (p&p)-administered questionnaire. All participants were asked to anonymously self-report their gender, age group, and to categorize their ability to perceive smells orthonasally a priori into one of four groups: (1) anosmic (either born unable to smell—congenital anosmic—or having developed the inability), (2) hyposmic, (3) normosmic, and (4) expert (i.e., professional fragrance experts). All participants who were recruited by the paper & pencil questionnaire declared themselves as normosmic. Each participant was then classified (according to their declaration) into one of four groups: one pathologic group (n = 121), which included hyposmic and anosmic participants, two normosmic groups (np&p = 21 and nweb = 223), and one expert group (n = 22).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participants’ characteristics according to questionnaire completion condition: web-based or paper and pencil.

Instrument

The original English version of the VOIQ was first transcribed into French with the help of collaborators fluent in English and native in French. The translation was then presented to a professional translator, fluent in French, and native in English, who back translated the questionnaire to English and made corrections by comparing it to the original questionnaire. In addition, French healthy internal collaborators were also asked to note whether the questionnaire translation was clearly understandable. If it was not, they had to specify which item was not understood. Thus, amendments to the French version were progressively made until both parties fully agreed on the final translated document.

Procedure

In the manner of the original version of VOIQ, the fVOIQ is composed of 16 items split into 4 situations for which 4 specific smells are presented (see Table 2). Each participant was instructed to read each sentence and imagine the considered smell. They were then asked to rate the vividness of the imagined smell on a scale from 1 to 5 where each score corresponds to the rating scale presented in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. VOIQ and fVOIQ rating scales and 16 items of questionnaires split into four situations for which four specific smells.

As previously indicated, either the web or pen and paper version of the fVOIQ was self-administered. Regardless of the form of administration, participants were not asked to provide any identifying data.

Data Analysis

Raw data for each participant (gender, age interval, orthonasal smell perception ability, and fVOIQ global score) were stored in a specific database. The mean fVOIQ score was calculated for each participant (sum of answers/16 items). A low mean score (close to 1) indicates a good ability to imagine smells, and vice versa.

All statistical analyses were performed using R version 3.6.3. The threshold for statistical significance was α = 0.05. fVOIQ score distribution for all four groups differed from the normal distribution (Shapiro–Wilk test, ps < 0.001). Therefore, quantitative data were expressed as medians (Med) associated with interquartile range (IQR) and qualitative data by their frequencies.

In order to verify the reliability of the fVOIQ, the internal consistency of the questionnaire was assessed. Split-half reliability of the fVOIQ was tested by Spearman correlations between average scores on odd items and even items for each participant. The Mann–Whitney U test was used to compare fVOIQ scores between groups (web-based or paper & pencil administered questionnaire). A Kruskal–Wallis non-parametric ANOVA was used to study group effect (pathologic, normosmic and expert) on olfactory imagery ability, i.e., their median fVOIQ score. Tests were adjusted for multiple comparisons according to Bonferroni–Holm correction.

Results

There were no significant differences in fVOIQ scores between web-based and paper & pencil conditions (Medp&p = 2.69 ± 0.69; Medweb = 2.31 ± 1.25; Z = −1.09; p > 0.05). Therefore, all normosmic participants were grouped for further analyses, reducing the number of groups to three: one pathologic group (n = 121), one normosmic group (n = 244), and one expert group (n = 22).

Internal Reliability of the fVOIQ

Split-half reliability as expressed by Spearman correlation coefficients was statistically significant for the global sample, as well as for each group separately (Figure 1), thus verifying the internal consistency of the fVOIQ.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Split-half reliability expressed by Spearman correlation coefficients for global population and each group (pathologic, normosmic, and expert). ***p < 0.001.

Olfactory Ability Differentiation by fVOIQ Score

The Kruskal–Wallis non-parametric ANOVA showed a significant effect of group on the median fVOIQ score [H(2, N = 387) = 211.6; p < 0.001]. As illustrated in Figure 2A, the score for the pathologic group (Medpat = 4.69; IQR = 1.25) was significantly higher than both normosmic and expert groups (Mednor = 2.38, IQR = 1.13 and Medexp = 2.00, IQR = 0.63; corrected ps < 0.001). The score for the normosmic group was also significantly higher than that of the expert group (corrected p < 0.05).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. fVOIQ scores in each group, i.e., pathologic, normosmic, and expert (A) and for each type of anosmia (B). *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

The differentiation power of the fVOIQ was further tested in the pathologic group, by comparing congenital anosmic participants, to those who had developed anosmia (Figure 2B). Comparison was performed with a Mann–Whitney U test for independent samples. Results revealed that congenital anosmic participants (Med = 5.00; IQR = 0.06) had significantly higher fVOIQ scores than participants with developed anosmia (Med = 4.69; IQR = 1.38; Z = −2.79; p < 0.01).

Discussion

The primary aim of this study was to validate a questionnaire allowing to assess olfactory imagery ability in French-speakers. The internal consistency of our fVOIQ was high on the global population, and in each group. In line with our hypothesis, the fVOIQ corroborated previous findings using Gilbert et al.’s (1998) original English version, that is to say, better olfactory imagery ability in fragrance experts, than normosmic individuals. These findings suggest that regular exposure to strong olfactory stimuli is a key to form an olfactory representation (Bensafi and Rouby, 2007; Royet et al., 2013). This was supported by the results of the fragrance experts in our study, although this effect could be underestimated by a recruiting effect in normosmic volunteers as suggested by Arshamian et al. (2011). Indeed, these authors proposed that this recruiting method may have appealed to individuals with high odor interest, and therefore higher olfactory imagery abilities. Therefore, this study has demonstrated that the fVOIQ is reliable and a valid tool for assessing olfactory imagery ability in French populations.

Our results showed a wide variation in normosmic individuals, allowing us to distinguish good odor imagers, from poorer imagers (Bensafi et al., 2005). In addition to this, and despite the differences in the number of participants in each group, our findings suggest a potential benefit of the fVOIQ in discriminating individuals with smell impairments. Indeed, participants with loss of smell had significantly higher fVOIQ scores than fragrance experts and healthy non-experts, meaning they had more difficulty imagining odors. Therefore, more so than regular strong odor exposure, natural and daily olfactory stimulation is fundamental to maintaining the ability to create a vivid image. The relationship between smelling and olfactory imagery abilities is thus further substantiated. Moreover, the significant difference between congenital anosmic and participants with developed anosmia reveals that the fVOIQ provides the ability to differentiate these two populations. This finding suggests that severe loss of smell results in progressive impairment of olfactory imagery ability. Further studies are needed to determine whether the fVOIQ is a consistent tool to evaluate the severity level of olfactory impairment in individuals with smell loss.

Lastly, it is important to note that contextualized olfactory imagery vividness depends on culture (Stevenson and Boakes, 2003), which is why all participants in this study were French. However, the questionnaire used in this study may not apply to all French-speaking populations, as some items or situations could not be relatable to individuals of certain countries with other cultures.

A perspective of application of the fVOIQ could also be the diagnosing of olfactory impairments. Indeed, according to the European Rhinologic Society, patients today with a sudden loss of smell should be considered as potentially COVID-19 positive. A significant proportion of COVID-19 patients (20–60%) experience loss of smell or taste (Mazzatenta et al., 2020). Recent findings suggest that loss of smell as a result of COVID-19 could be caused by alteration of the posterior gyrus rectus and olfactory bulbs (Politi et al., 2020). This symptom can predict infection to this coronavirus (Lechien et al., 2020), as it appears prior to other symptoms such as coughing or fever. In this context, measuring one’s olfactory ability could be a way of diagnosing COVID-19 early in many individuals. However, contagion risks are high, and protective medical equipment and detection procedures are time intensive and costly. The present study allows us to suggest that the assessment of one’s olfactory imagery abilities could provide a self-administered preliminary diagnosis of COVID-19, maintaining social distancing and limiting financial cost. Likewise, the original VOIQ for English-speaking populations (representing over 2 billion people) could be a relevant tool as it is quick, free, non-invasive, and self-administered (independently of the administration method) and safe in the COVID-19 context.

Conclusion

The fVOIQ appears to have high reliability and validity, when compared to the data of the original English VOIQ. It can therefore be used in a French population for assessing olfactory imagery ability and could also differentiate individuals with smell loss from healthy individuals.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

GH and MJ conceived, designed the study, conducted the experiments, collected, and processed the data. LF, GH, and HC analyzed the data. LF, HC, MJ, and GH wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank C. Bourassa for their editing English assistance.

References

Andrade, J., May, J., and Kavanagh, D. (2012). Sensory imagery in craving: from cognitive psychology to new treatments for addiction. J. Exp. Psychopathol. 3, 127–145. doi: 10.5127/jep.024611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arshamian, A., Willander, J., and Larsson, M. (2011). Olfactory awareness is positively associated to odour memory. J. Cogn. Psychol. 23, 220–226. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2011.483226

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bensafi, M., Pouliot, S., and Sobel, N. (2005). Odorant-specific patterns of sniffing during imagery distinguish ‘bad’ and ‘good’ olfactory imagers. Chem. Senses 30, 521–529. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bji045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bensafi, M., and Rouby, C. (2007). Individual differences in odor imaging ability reflect differences in olfactory and emotional perception. Chem. Senses 32, 237–244. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjl051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Betts, G. H. (1910). The distribution and functions of mental imagery. Psychol. Bull. 7, 351–352. doi: 10.1037/h0064277

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brower, D. (1947). The experimental study of imagery: Ii. The relative predominance of various imagery modalities. J. Gen. Psychol. 37, 199–200. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1947.9918152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrasco, M., and Ridout, J. B. (1993). Olfactory perception and olfactory imagery: a multidimensional analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 19, 287–301. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.19.2.287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farah, M. J. (1988). Is visual imagery really visual? Overlooked evidence from neuropsychology. Psychol. Rev. 95, 307–317. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.95.3.307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flohr, E. L. R., Arshamian, A., Wieser, M. J., Hummel, C., Larsson, M., Mühlberger, A., et al. (2014). The fate of the inner nose: odor imagery in patients with olfactory loss. Neuroscience 268, 118–127. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, A. N., Crouch, M., and Kemp, S. E. (1998). Olfactory and visual mental imagery. J. Ment. Imag. 22, 137–146.

Google Scholar

Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., and Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 635–642. doi: 10.1038/35090055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lechien, J. R., Chiesa-Estomba, C. M., De Siati, D. R., Horoi, M., Le Bon, S. D., Rodriguez, A., et al. (2020). Olfactory and gustatory dysfunctions as a clinical presentation of mild-to-moderate forms of the coronavirus disease (COVID-19): a multicenter European study. Eur. Arch. Oto Rhino Laryngol. 277, 2251–2261. doi: 10.1007/s00405-020-05965-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindauer, M. S. (1969). Imagery and sensory modality. Percept. Mot. Skills 29, 203–215. doi: 10.2466/pms.1969.29.1.203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marks, D. F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. Br. J. Psychol. 64, 17–24. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1973.tb01322.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazzatenta, A., Neri, G., D’Ardes, D., Marinari, S., Porreca, E., Cipollone, F., et al. (2020). Smell and taste in severe CoViD-19: self-reported vs testing. Front. Med. 7:589409. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2020.589409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Politi, L. S., Salsano, E., and Grimaldi, M. (2020). Magnetic resonance imaging alteration of the brain in a patient with coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) and anosmia. JAMA Neurol. 77, 1028–1029. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2020.2125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Royet, J.-P., Delon-Martin, C., and Plailly, J. (2013). Odor mental imagery in non-experts in odors: a paradox? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:87. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheehan, P. W. (1967). A shortened form of Betts’ questionnaire upon mental imagery. J. Clin. Psychol. 23, 386–389. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(196707)23:3<386::aid-jclp2270230328>3.0.co;2-s

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevenson, R. J., and Boakes, R. A. (2003). A mnemonic theory of odor perception. Psychol. Rev. 110, 340–364. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevenson, R. J., and Case, T. I. (2005). Olfactory imagery: a review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 244–264. doi: 10.3758/BF03196369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugiyama, H., Ayabe-Kanamura, S., and Kikuchi, T. (2006). Are olfactory images sensory in nature? Perception 35, 1699–1708. doi: 10.1068/p5453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, K. D., Ashton, R., and Law, H. (1978). The measurement of imagery vividness: effects of format and order on the betts’ questionnaire upon mental imagery. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 10, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/h0081537

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: olfactory imagery, olfactory ability, smell loss, anosmic, odor expert, normosmic, French questionnaire

Citation: Fantin L, Ceyte H, Ramdane-Cherif Z, Jacquot M and Hossu G (2020) French Vividness of Olfactory Imagery Questionnaire: A Potential Tool for Diagnosing Olfactory Loss by Assessing Olfactory Imagery? Front. Psychol. 11:606667. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.606667

Received: 15 September 2020; Accepted: 17 November 2020;
Published: 21 December 2020.

Edited by:

Sara Invitto, University of Salento, Italy

Reviewed by:

Alfredo Campos, University of Santiago de Compostela, Spain
Antonella Di Pizio, Technical University of Munich, Germany

Copyright © 2020 Fantin, Ceyte, Ramdane-Cherif, Jacquot and Hossu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gabriela Hossu, Zy5ob3NzdUBjaHJ1LW5hbmN5LmZy

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.