Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 11 December 2020
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Social Belongingness and Well-Being: International Perspectives View all 31 articles

Perceived Social Support and Its Effects on Changes in the Affective and Eudaimonic Well-Being of Chilean University Students

Rubia Cobo-Rendn
Rubia Cobo-Rendón1*Yaranay Lpez-AnguloYaranay López-Angulo2María Victoria Prez-VillalobosMaría Victoria Pérez-Villalobos3Alejandro Díaz-MujicaAlejandro Díaz-Mujica3
  • 1Laboratorio de Investigación e Innovación educativa Dirección de Docencia, Universidad de Concepción, Concepción, Chile
  • 2Facultad de Ciencias Sociales y Comunicaciones, Universidad Santo Tomas, Concepción, Chile
  • 3Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Departamento de Psicología, Universidad de Concepción, Concepción, Chile

The beginning of university life can be a stressful event for students. The close social relationships that they can experience can have positive effects on their well-being. The objective of this paper is to estimate the effect of perceived social support on the changes of the hedonic and eudaimonic well-being of Chilean university students during the transition from the first to the second academic year. Overall, 205 students participated (63.90% men and 36.09% women) with an average age of 19.14 years (SD = 1.73), evaluated during their first academic year (2017) and the succeeding one (2018). For the evaluation of perceived social support, the Spanish version of the Perceived Social Support Questionnaire “MSPSS” was used, and PERMA-profiler was used to measure hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Changes through the time of hedonic well-being and social support and the correlations between the variables were analyzed. Changes in the perception of social support were analyzed according to four categories of hedonic well-being. The prediction of social support for eudaimonic well-being was evaluated. Results indicated that the perception of students’ social support did not change over time. Statistically significant differences were found in hedonic well-being scores in the two measurements, being significantly higher in the first measurement than in the second one. More than 50% of the participants presented a positive balance of affections. The perception of social support is associated with the two types of well-being. Students who had a high balance of affections had a greater perception of general social support than the groups of positive evolution of affections and a low balance of affections. In the case of the friends and family support dimensions, the perception in the high-balance group of affections concerning the low-scale group is greater. Improving the perception of social support increases the eudaimonic well-being of university students. The perception of support that students had during the beginning of their university life benefits their general well-being, which contributes to their mental health.

Introduction

There is currently an interest in the study of mental health issues in Higher Education. Research in different contexts describes the importance of mental health in university students (Wörfel et al., 2016; Leung, 2017), with well-being being an important aspect in students’ academic success (Langford et al., 2014).

The concept of well-being can be evaluated from the hedonic and eudaimonic approach. Hedonic well-being is defined as the presence of positive moods, the absence of negative moods (affective components), and life satisfaction (cognitive component) related to life assessment (Diener et al., 1999). Affective balance, part of this type of well-being, is defined as the ability to balance the emotions associated with life experiences. It is an affective element resulting from the estimation of the differences between the presence of positive and negative emotions experienced by each person over time (Diener, 2000; Diener et al., 2010, 2015).

Affective well-being can be defined as frequent experiences of pleasant moods, such as joy, and infrequent experiences of negative affect or unpleasant moods, such as anger and fear (Diener et al., 2010). This type of well-being is measured by the frequency and intensity of positive and negative emotions. Leaving aside the idea of a single construct of bipolar affect (pleasant affect versus unpleasant affect), the affective component considers evaluations of both, positive and negative affective states. That is, the presence of a positive affect is not the same as the absence of a negative affect (Diener et al., 2017; Weismayer, 2020). An important aspect to consider is that positive life events are closely related to pleasant emotions, whereas negative life events are accompanied by unpleasant emotions (Diener, 2000). Due to this, affective well-being has been associated with better health and longevity (Diener and Chan, 2011). Regarding temporality, the presence of various moods in cognitive evaluation is the result of the experiences presented in people’s lives (Diener et al., 1999). Several studies indicate a greater presence of positive than negative affectivity in university students and the general population (Contreras et al., 2010; Diener et al., 2010; Zubieta et al., 2012; Barrantes-Brais and Ureña-Bonilla, 2015). Correlations among positive affectivity and academic performance, hope, curiosity, enthusiasm, and perseverance were found in the literature review regarding Higher Educational Contexts (Oriol, 2016; Zhang and Chen, 2018).

Eudaimonic well-being is defined as the cognitive assessment of the development of skills and virtues necessary for optimal psychological development (Keyes et al., 2002; Ryff, 2014). It is related to the search for optimal personal development or human flourishing (Waterman, 2008), with the meaning of life, personal growth, autonomy, positive relationships with others, self-esteem, and cognitive flexibility (Ryff and Singer, 1996; Ryff, 2014; Malkoç and Mutlu, 2019).

One of the most widely accepted theoretical approaches was the one presented by Carol Ryff, who defines psychological well-being from six dimensions: self-acceptance: positive self-evaluations, positive relationships: satisfactory and authentic interpersonal relationships, autonomy: sense of self-determination and independence, mastery of the environment: ability to effectively manage one’s life and the surrounding world, personal growth: sense of continuous personal development and life purpose, which is considered as the sense of self-direction, and persistence in fulfilling vitally important goals (Ryff, 1989). In longitudinal terms, the well-being profiles that are persistently high or low are considered as predictors of chronic diseases and functional deterioration concerning health subjective terms (Ryff et al., 2015). Additionally, there is consensus in considering the relevance of close, deep, personal sense social relationships in well-being and other mental health indexes (Feeney and Collins, 2015; Poots and Cassidy, 2020).

People with close social relationships tend to report higher levels of well-being and flourishing (Diener and Seligman, 2002; Myers, 2015; Diener et al., 2018). Perceived social support affects the way people perceive themselves and the world around them. A meta-analysis indicates that not having a network of meaningful relationships in life is more predictive of mortality than other lifestyle behaviors, such as smoking or physical activity (Holt-Lunstad and Smith, 2012).

Social support is the perception of being cared for by others and having a reliable network to turn to when needed, in everyday situations or specific moments of crisis (Taylor, 2011). It can be perceived from three sources: family, friends, and significant others (Zimet et al., 1988). Social support is also referred to as the frequency of support actions that are provided by others (Santini et al., 2015); which is why, it can be understood as the subjective feeling of being supported (Santini et al., 2015). Additionally, the type of support can be (1) emotional, (2) instrumental, (3) evaluative, and (4) informative (Sarason et al., 1990).

Overall, perceived social support is a significant predictor of life satisfaction and negative affect (Siedlecki et al., 2014; Kostak et al., 2019; Shensa et al., 2020). Specifically, emotional support has important benefits in mental health, so many studies focus on the relationship between depression and perceived social support (Kleiman and Riskind 2013; Santini et al., 2015; Kostak et al., 2019; Shensa et al., 2020). Perceived social support and social bonds are positively related to mental and physical health (Cohen and Janicki-Deverts, 2009; Umberson and Karas Montez, 2010). Research points to a positive association between perceived social support and psychological well-being, which allows it to be seen as a valuable protective mechanism that can improve psychological well-being by maintaining positive emotional feelings and mitigating stress (Chu et al., 2010; Thoits, 2011; Liu et al., 2014). The different facets of hedonic well-being (life satisfaction, positive and negative affects) can be predicted by different aspects of perceived social support (Siedlecki et al., 2014). Also, perceived social support is seen as mediating the relationship between stress and well-being (Poots and Cassidy, 2020).

Research on predictors of academic success points to perceived social support as one of the facilitating factors of adaptation to university life and protective of challenging situations imposed by the university (Friedlander et al., 2007; Bahar, 2010; Rahat and İlhan, 2016). Since the theory of self-determination, the need to belong to a group is relevant for healthy psychological development and human flourishing (Ryan and Deci, 2000; Tomás and Gutiérrez, 2019); however, little has been studied about the effects of perceived social support over time.

Due to affective well-being implying the presence of positive and negative affects, with no exclusion of both previous components (Diener et al., 2017; Weismayer, 2020), it is important to investigate how the perception of social support that was acquired during the first year of the university experience can affect the students’ well-being. Although research has linked perceived social support to well-being measures, some researchers have found negative or no consequences of perceived social support on well-being (Lepore et al., 2008; Lakey et al., 2010). Differences in outcomes can be derived from the ways in which perceived social support and well-being are conceptualized and operationalized (Siedlecki et al., 2014), and research on these issues has also focused on cross-sectional measures. For this reason, it is necessary to evaluate how the student’s perception of social support affects his/her well-being, considering the process that he/she undergoes during university entry and the empirical evidence that first-year students have the lowest levels of well-being (Brandy et al., 2015; van Der Zanden et al., 2018). Thus, this paper aims at estimating the effect of perceived social support on changes in affective and eudaimonic well-being in university students.

To respond to this objective, the following hypotheses are raised. First, the perception of social support and affective well-being in participating university students varies over time (H1). Second, there is a positive relationship between the perception of social support with affective and eudaimonic well-being (H2). Third, perceptions of social support predict changes over time in affective well-being and eudaimonic well-being in university students (H3).

Materials and Methods

The research design used corresponds to a comparative-prospective study, and at a temporal level, it is a longitudinal panel research (Ato et al., 2013).

Participants

Overall, 131 (63.90%) were men, and 74 (36.09%) were women. The average age was M = 19.14 years, SD = 1,730. Follow-up was achieved in only 26.3%, and there were cases of some careers where it was not possible to perform follow-up applications due to problems of access to student groups due to academic activity stoppages, impacting on the percentage of students analyzed in Q2.

The distribution of participants who were followed up by degree courses is as follows: 56.09% (n = 115) belonged to Engineering and Basic Sciences, 18.04% (n = 37) to Architecture, 14.14% (n = 29) to Social Sciences and Humanities, and 11.70% (n = 24) to Pedagogy or Education. The selection was through a non-probability sample for convenience, considering the first-year students of the participating careers.

Instruments

Perceived Social Support

The Perceived Social Support Questionnaire “MSPSS” (Zimet et al., 1988), specifically the Spanish version by Ortíz and Baeza (2011), was used for the evaluation of perceived social support. The purpose of this questionnaire is to assess people’s perception of social support from relevant sources. It is designed as a self-report and is composed of 12 items distributed in three dimensions: family (four items), e.g., “My family gives me the help and emotional support I need”; friends (four items), e.g., “I can talk about my problems with my friends”; and other significant ones (four items), e.g., “When I have difficulties I have someone to support me.” This version uses a Likert response scale and ranges from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree. The higher the score, the higher the perception of social support. In this study, confirmatory factor analysis corroborated the distribution of three related factors [X2(51) = 123,269, p < 0.05; root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.084; Confirmatory Fit Index (CFI) = 0.935; Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) = 0.916]. The reliability was from ω = 0.89 for the family dimension, from ω = 0.91 for the friend’s dimension, and from ω = 0.89 for the other significant dimensions in the first measurement.

Emotional and Eudaimonic Well-Being

The PERMA-Profiler (Butler and Kern, 2016) was used for the well-being evaluation. This instrument consists of 23 items with a Likert response format where 1 = lowest score and 7 = highest score. It is made up of five main dimensions (positive emotions, commitment, positive relationships, purpose, and achievement) and three contrast dimensions (negative emotions, health perception, and one item for loneliness). For this study, the dimensions were reorganized to evaluate the elements of affective well-being (positive and negative affect) and eudaism separately. The coincidence of the dimensions proposed by the PERMA-Profiler, to the concepts to the theory of psychological well-being, such as the meaning of life, autonomy, and positive relations with others, allows the identification and measurement of this variable (Ryff and Singer, 1996; Ryff, 2014).

The eudaimonic well-being was measured with 12 items that correspond to: the dimension of positive relationships with others (three items), evaluates to what extent the person receives and gives support to others, their satisfaction, and the feeling of being appreciated, e.g., how often do you feel loved; the meaning or purpose of life (three items), involves the feelings of leading a life with intention, meaning, and value, e.g., in general, how often do you feel you are following a meaningful direction in your life; the competence or achievement dimension (three items), assesses how often the person advances in their goals, takes responsibility, and feels able to complete their daily responsibilities, e.g., how often are you able to handle your responsibilities?; and the commitment dimension (three items), assesses the presence of vigor, absorption, and dedication, e.g., how often do you feel absorbed in what you are doing. The average of the dimensions forms an indicator of psychological well-being with a range of responses from 1 to 7 points. Other studies report adequate psychometric properties in university students and Latin American samples (Butler and Kern, 2016; Pastrana and Salazar-Piñeros, 2016; Lima-Castro et al., 2017). To confirm these dimensions, a confirmatory factorial analysis was performed; an adequate model fit was obtained [X2(48) = 91,748, p < 0.05; RMSEA = 0.067; CFI = 0.947; TLI = 0.927]. The reliability of the scale ranged from ω = 0.89 in the first measurement.

Emotional well-being was measured with six items from the same questionnaire, of which three measure positive emotions of satisfaction, joy, and optimism, e.g., in general, how often do you feel happy, and the remaining three measure negative emotions, such as anxiety, sadness, and anger, e.g., in general, how often do you feel angry? Each emotion has a range of responses between 1 and 7 points. The subtraction between positive and negative emotions is used as an indicator of the scale of affect. A confirmatory factor analysis was performed to confirm these dimensions, obtaining an adequate model fit [X2(8) = 16,677, p < 0.05; RMSEA = 0.073; CFI = 0.973; TLI = 0.949]. The reliability for positive emotions was ω = 0.84 and negative ω = 0.67 in the first measurement.

Procedure

The present research was endorsed by the Ethics Committee of the participating university, and the ethical criteria for research with human beings were corroborated. During the applications, participants were informed of the characteristics of the study, the right to voluntary participation, and the handling of data privacy. For the collection of data, contact was made with the authorities of the faculties (Deans, Department Directors, and Heads of Studies), who indicated the courses where the applications would be made. Participants were selected through a non-probabilistic sampling for convenience. The questionnaires were massively applied in the classrooms during March 2017 (Q1). The follow-up was carried out during April and June 2018 (Q2) in the same careers that were applied in Q1. The difference was that the classrooms where the applications were made corresponded to the courses of second-year students, since 1 year had passed.

For the follow-up (Q2), the data identifying the participants who answered the instruments in the classrooms during Q2 were compared with the database obtained in Q1. Three identification criteria were used (first and last names, ID card, and career). Based on these criteria, follow-up cases were digitally identified, and also, a manual check of the data set was performed.

Analysis Plan

The MPLUS software was used to carry out the confirmatory factor analyses of the scales. The SPSS Windows version 21 software was used for data analysis descriptive (means, standard deviations, frequencies, percentages) to describe the characteristics of the sample and the main variables of the study. SPSS was also used to analyze changes over time in emotional well-being and the perception of social support (H1), with Student’s t-test of related samples. Pearson correlations were made to estimate the relationships of perception of social support, affective well-being, and eudaimony (H2). To respond to H3, participants were initially classified into four categories of affective well-being according to the balance of affects scores obtained in the two measurements, with possible combinations being made for high and low balance of affects cases. Subsequently, factor-type variance analyses (ANOVAs) were performed to evaluate the prediction of perceived social support in the types of affective and eudaimonic well-being (H3).

Results

The general objective of this study was to estimate the effect of perceived social support on changes in the emotional and eudaimonic well-being of university students. Table 1 describes the scores obtained for the dimensions of perceived social support and types of well-being.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Scores on social support, affective, and eudaimonic well-being.

When performing the means comparison analysis to confirm whether the perception of social support and the subjective well-being vary over time (H1), it was found that the social support dimensions did not present statistically significant changes over time. Concerning the affective well-being, statistically significant differences were found in the scores of T1 and T2 being significantly higher in the scores presented at the beginning of the academic year (M = 4.06, SD = 5.54) than in the second year (M = 2.60, SD = 5.84).

To confirm the hypothesis (H2), which describes positive relations of the perception of social support with the types of well-being, Pearson’s correlation estimates are described in Table 2. The results indicate a higher level of association of T1 perceived social support with the eudaimonic and affective well-being in T1 than with the scores obtained in T2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Pearson’s correlation of the dimensions of perception of social support with affective and eudaimonic well-being in university students.

When analyzing the correlations, at a cross-sectional level, the results confirm positive and significant correlations between the dimensions of perceived social support with affective well-being and eudaimonic well-being (T1). The magnitude of the correlations of the perception of social support was higher with the eudaimonic well-being in the T1 and T2 measurements. A greater relationship was found between the dimensions of the perception of social support (T1) and the eudaimonic well-being (T2) than with the affective well-being (T2); meanwhile, there is no statistically significant correlation in the dimension of the perception of social support of friends (T1) with the affective well-being (T2). At a general level, there is more congruence between the dimensions of perceived social support and eudaimonia well-being in all its measurements.

Changes in the Perception of General Social Support According to the Evolution of Emotional and Eudaimonia Well-Being

To assess the prediction of social support in the change of students’ well-being (H3), four categories of well-being change were established (see Table 3). In this case, the group of students with “low well-being stable” refers to the group of students who presented low scores (less than 3pts) in the two measures. The group “negative change in well-being” in the first measurement presented high scores (higher than 3pts) and in the second one low scores. The category called “positive evolution of well-being” includes the group of students who presented low scores in the first measurement and in the second one high scores; finally, the category “stable high well-being” includes the students who presented high scores during the two measurements. In the case of affective well-being, the results presented in Table 3 indicate that more than 50% of the participants present a positive affective balance. In the case of eudaimonic well-being, 38% present high scores in the second measurement.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Change in the balance of well-being during the beginning of the university career.

ANOVA Factorials were carried out to respond to this hypothesis. When analyzing the differences in the perception of social support by categories of the evolution of affective well-being, statistically significant differences are presented in the scores of the perception of social support in the analyzed affectivity groups F(3,187) = 4.264, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.064 (see Figure 1). Additionally, statistically significant inter-subject effects are presented F(3,187) = 10.187, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.140.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Differences in the perception of general social support by categories of evolution of affective and eudaimonic well-being.

Multiple comparisons obtained from Bonferroni’s post-hoc tests indicate that students in the high affective well-being category maintain similar scores of high perception of general social support during the two measurements (M = 17.72, SD = 2.88 and M = 17.42, SD = 2.89, respectively). Similarly, it is observed that the averages in the categories of students of positive evolution of affective well-being (M = 14.98, SD = 3.47 and M = 15.80, SD = 3.47) and low stable affective well-being (M = 14.69; SD = 3.14 and M = 15.06; SD = 3.00) had at the beginning of the first year a perception of social support different from the one they presented in the second academic year).

In the case of the perception of social support by categories of the evolution of eudaimonic well-being, statistically significant differences are presented F(3,187) = 4.264, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.064 (see Figure 1). Additionally, there are statistically significant inter-subject effects F(1,89) = 20.590, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.410.

Multiple comparisons obtained from Bonferroni’s post-hoc tests indicate that students in the high well-being category of eudaimonics have higher scores for general social support during the two measurements (M = 18.73, SD = 1.94 and M = 18.5, SD = 2.08, respectively). Unlike the rest of the categories that presented changes in the two measurements, for example, positive evolution of well-being (M = 12.500, SD = 3.77 and M = 17.29, SD = 1.998), a group of students who presented an increase in the perception of social support as their well-being also increased. In the case of the group of students in the category “negative evolution of well-being” (M = 14,100, SD = 1.48 and M = 13,500, SD = 1.944), their perception of support decreased. In the case of the category of stable low well-being, students in this category did not show substantial changes over time (M = 14,363, SD = 3.16 and M = 15,045, SD = 2,431). Differences between the groups were found, as students from the high stable well-being group presented a higher perception of social support than the rest of the students belonging to the other categories (p < 0.01); the description of the changes found is reflected in Figure 1.

Changes in the Perception of Social Support From Friends According to the Evolution of Emotional and Eudaimonic Well-Being

The results found to analyze the differences in the perception of social support from friends according to the categories of affective well-being indicate statistically significant differences in the scores of perceived social support in the analyzed affectivity groups F(3,188) = 3.459 (p < 0.01), ηp2 = 0.052 (see Figure 2). Statistically significant inter-subject effects are presented F(3,188) = 6.436, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.093. According to the results of Bonferroni’s post-hoc test, statistically significant differences (p < 0.01) were found only between the groups of students in the high affective stable well-being category (M = 5.84, SD = 1.03 and M = 5.72, SD = 1.14), with the low affective stable well-being group (M = 4.92, SD = 1.39 and M = 5.04, SD = 1.35), respectively. In this case, the last group of students shows an increase in their scores over time (see Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Differences in the perception of social support from friends by categories of evolution of affective and eudaimonic well-being.

In the case of the perception of social support from friends by categories of evolution of eudaimonic well-being, statistically significant differences are presented F(1,90) = 11.006, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.268 (see Figure 2). Additionally, there are statistically significant inter-subject effects F(1,90) = 214.267, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.322.

Multiple comparisons obtained from Bonferroni’s post-hoc tests indicate that students in the high well-being category of eudaimonics have higher scores for general social support during the two measurements (M = 6.159, SD = 0.874 and M = 6.138, SD = 0.708, respectively). Unlike the rest of the categories that presented changes in the two measurements, e.g., positive evolution of well-being (M = 12,500, SD = 3.77 and M = 17.29, SD = 1.998), as in the case of the stable low well-being category, students did not present substantial changes over time (M = 5,000, SD = 1.17 and M = 4.954, SD = 1.223). In the case of the group of students in the “negative well-being development” category (M = 5,700, SD = 480 and M = 4,150, SD = 1,024), their perception of support decreased. Differences between the groups were found, as students from the high well-being group with stable debt presented a higher perception of social support than the rest of the students belonging to the other categories (p < 0.01); the description of the changes found is reflected in Figure 2.

Changes in the Perception of Social Support From the Family According to the Evolution of Emotional Well-Being

In the case of differences in the perception of social support from family members according to the categories of affective well-being, statistically significant differences were identified in the scores of perceived social support of this dimension in the affectivity groups analyzed F(3,190) = 3,242, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.049 (see Figure 3). Additionally, statistically significant inter-subject effects are presented F(3,190) = 9.788, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.134. Statistically significant differences (p < 0.01) were only found between the groups of students in the category of high stable affective well-being (M = 5.95, SD = 1.18 and M = 5.83, SD = 1.11) and those of low stable affective well-being (M = 4.80, SD = 1.18 and M = 4.83, SD = 5.95), respectively. In this case, this last group of students shows an increase in their scores over time (see Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Differences in the perception of social support from the family by categories of evolution of affective and eudaimonic well-being.

In the case of the perception of social support from the family by categories of evolution of eudaimonic well-being, statistically significant differences are presented F(1,91) = 11,372, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.273 (see Figure 3). Additionally, there are statistically significant inter-subject effects F(1,91) = 7.557, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.199.

Multiple comparisons obtained from Bonferroni’s post-hoc tests indicate that students from the high stable eudaimonic well-being category present higher scores of general social support during the two measurements (M = 6.270, SD = 0.783 and M = 6.1024, SD = 0.966, respectively). In the low stable well-being category, students did not present substantial changes over time (M = 4.977, SD = 1.221 and M = 5.136, SD = 1.062). The rest of the categories that presented changes in the two measurements, e.g., positive evolution of well-being (M = 4,708, SD = 1,111 and M = 6,125, SD = 0.786) and in the group of students in the category “negative evolution of well-being” (M = 6,100, SD = 0.894 and M = 4,600, SD = 1,126), whose perception of support decreased. Differences between the groups were found, as students from the high well-being group with stable debt presented a higher perception of social support than the rest of the students belonging to the other categories (p < 0.01); the description of the changes found is reflected in Figure 3.

With regard to the identification of changes in the perception of social support from other significant people according to the evolution of affective and eudaimonic well-being, no statistically significant differences were found in the groups evaluated.

Discussion

The aim of this work was to estimate the effect of perceived social support on changes in the emotional and eudaimonic well-being of university students. The results found allowed to confirm that the perception of social support positively predicts the types of well-being of Chilean university students. Next, the findings obtained in relation to the established hypotheses are analyzed, and the limitations presented are commented on and future lines of research that could be projected from the results of this study.

Changes Over Time in the Perception of Social Support and Subjective Well-Being

In this study, only temporary changes were identified in the emotional well-being scores, partially confirming the H1 hypothesis (the perception of social support and emotional well-being in the participating university students varied over time). This result confirms findings of Joshanloo (2018a,b), who refers to which elements of emotional well-being can change due to situations presented in the context, generating changes in people’s moods. This result is congruent with what is established in the theory about affective well-being, where the result of the evaluation that a person makes by means of cognitive and affective processes is associated to his life experiences (Diener et al., 1999).

Positive emotions are considered to be part of the affective component of well-being, which can vary over the course of a day (Pavot and Diener, 2011). When students present these types of emotions, they are better able to handle the new academic demands present at the university during the first years (Ouweneel et al., 2011; Oriol-Granado et al., 2017). The results found in the present study indicate higher affective balance scores at the beginning of the university career (during the beginning of the first academic year), allowing people to expand their repertoire of thoughts and behavior, building their lasting social and personal resources (Fredrickson, 2013). The presence of high affective balance scores at the beginning of the career could be related to the search for immediate well-being, rather than a long-term search (Joshanloo, 2018b).

The decline in measurement scores in Q2 is similar to that reported in other studies. Herke et al. (2019) found a decline in affective well-being in students. Importantly, changes in students’ affect balance may depend on their perception of the context (Lent et al., 2005). The decrease in students’ affective well-being can be explained by the adaptation processes they undergo at the beginning of their university careers. Nightingale et al. (2013) report that when students present low levels of positive emotions, they manifest difficulties in the process of adaptation to university. In that sense, during the first years of the university experience, students tend to adjust their beliefs, knowledge, and perceptions with respect to the real academic context, and it is possible that students experience stressful experiences that can impact on their affectivity (Nicolson, 1990; Coffey et al., 2016; Kyndt et al., 2017).

Relationship Between the Perception of Social Support and Emotional and Eudaimonic Well-Being

As for the existence of a positive relationship between the perception of social support with the affective and eudaimonic well-being (H2), the results allowed to confirm the hypothesis. Positive relations are presented between the dimensions of the perception of social support with the types of well-being. In general, the perception of social support is considered as a protective factor for the well-being of university students (Rosa-Rodríguez et al., 2015). Perceived social support allows to re-evaluate stressful situations contributing to adaptation processes in different contexts (Pillado and Almagiá, 2019).

When assessing relationships specifically, the results show that during Q1 and Q2, the perception of social support from friends, family, and significant others is more related to eudaimonic well-being than to affective well-being. Therefore, family support is more linked to elements associated with personal growth and the optimal development of students. This result coincides with what is reported in the literature, and studies show a close relationship between perceived social support and eudaimonic well-being (Almagia, 2012; Pillado and Almagiá, 2019). Family support and peer support influence the psychological well-being of university students, which, in turn, has a strong positive correlation with improved academic performance (Torales et al., 2018).

The presence of positive relationships between perceived social support and emotional well-being is also consistent with other research establishing that: when students perceive greater social support from family and friends, they report greater satisfaction with life (Marrero and Carballeira, 2010; Kong et al., 2012; Kong and You, 2013; King et al., 2020). There is a consensus that close, deep and meaningful social relationships have a positive effect on subjective well-being and other mental health indices (Dávila Figueras et al., 2011; Feeney and Collins, 2015; Poots and Cassidy, 2020). Additionally, the distinction found in the levels of relationship of perceived social support with types of well-being coincides with another study that establishes that the prediction of the perception of social support can vary depending on the well-being indicator taken into account. Although this variable has an important weight on life satisfaction and psychological adjustment, it explains to a lesser extent the presence of positive or negative affectivity and has little impact on satisfaction (Marrero and Carballeira, 2010).

Predicting the Perception of Social Support in Affective and Eudaimonic Well-Being

The results found confirm the H3 hypothesis that the perception of social support predicted changes over time in affective well-being and changes in eudaimonic well-being in university students. It has been proposed that the perception of social support is vital for well-being (Diener and Oishi, 2005). In other studies, perceived support was a predictor of both increased life satisfaction and decreased negative affect (Siedlecki et al., 2014). Similarly, emotional well-being was affected by the amount of social support, and low levels of social support received were more associated with depressive symptoms (Ye et al., 2019).

As reported in the literature, student groups with high stable and positive emotional well-being had a higher perception of overall social support than groups with lower levels of emotional well-being. This coincides with the results presented by Siedlecki et al. These authors establish that people who have satisfactory relationships report feeling happy more often and report being more satisfied with their lives than those who do not have satisfactory relationships. This is possible because people in satisfying relationships can get support when they need it, whereas those in unsatisfying relationships cannot easily get it when they need it (Siedlecki et al., 2014). It is clear that perceived social support affects the way people perceive themselves and the world around them; having a network of meaningful relationships in life has positive effects on people’s physical and mental health (Holt-Lunstad and Smith, 2012).

A statistically significant effect was found on emotional well-being and changes in the perception of social support from friends and family. There were differences between the groups with high emotional well-being and the group with low emotional well-being. Our results are similar to those found in other studies that establish that perceived social support from friends is positively related to well-being. Such findings reinforce the idea of the adaptive value of peer support in the Higher Education setting (Figueira et al., 2017).

In the case of the perception of social support coming from the family, the findings are similar to those described in other studies that explain that the family represents integral support in different aspects of the life of university students, who even after entering university and feeling adult, continue to consider the family as a valuable source to support them in their needs (Monroy and Ramírez, 2016). Students live a process where the development of a progressive autonomy is encouraged (Barrera-Herrera and Vinet, 2017); however, when they are overtaken by different demands typical of their stage, they may turn to their parents for support (Barrera-Herrera et al., 2019). Studies report that the support received from the family benefits the self-perceived academic performance of university students (López-Angulo et al., 2020). In this sense, the perceived social support exercised by family and friends is an essential element for the success of students (Renk and Smith, 2007; Torales et al., 2018).

Concerning perceptions of social support as predictors of eudaimonic well-being in university students, the results of this research allowed us to conclude that well-being increases from a greater perception of the social support that students have. The relationship between the perception of social support and eudaimonic well-being is explained by the importance for students of developing social networks to help them cope with the demands of the university stage (Rosa-Rodríguez et al., 2015). It is possible to consider perceived social support as a valuable protection mechanism, which can improve eudaimonic well-being (Chu et al., 2010; Thoits, 2011; Liu et al., 2014), benefiting personal growth, autonomy, and cognitive flexibility, which improves the response to the demands of university life (Waterman, 2008; Ryff, 2014, 2016; Malkoç and Mutlu, 2019).

A limitation of this research is the non-inclusion of sociodemographic, mental health (e.g., stress, depression, coping strategies), and contextual variables (e.g., semester assessment period), which could have influenced students’ well-being scores. The analysis could also be enriched by incorporating other educational indicators, such as objective academic performance or levels of adaptation to university (Bailey and Phillips, 2016; Biasi et al., 2018). Despite these limitations, the main strength of this study is the use of a longitudinal research design for the evaluation of these variables.

The results obtained have practical implications since social support among peers works as a positive element for the development of students’ emotional well-being. It is suggested to reinforce in academic institutions the development of social interventions and programs (e.g., initiation ceremonies and mentoring projects with the active participation of older students, activities involving the family), in order to raise the well-being of young university students (Figueira et al., 2017). Future student development programs should focus on improving or enhancing the adaptive capacities of regulating emotions and promoting reciprocal social exchanges (Ye et al., 2019).

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the participating university. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

RC-R contributed to the literature, abstract, and full-text review, as well as the data extraction, the data analysis, and the writing of the manuscript. YL-A contributed to the design of the study, abstract, and full-text review, as well as the design of the statistical analysis and the writing of the manuscript. MP-V contributed to the design of the study, the interpretation of the results, and the writing of the manuscript. AD-M contributed to the interpretation of the results and the writing of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was funded by: Proyect UCO 1808, Laboratorio de Innovación Educativa basada en Investigación para el fortalecimiento de los aprendizajes de ciencias básicas en la Universidad de Concepción (IDECLAB) assigned to Cobo-Rendon R., The scholarships program: CONICYT PFCHA/DOCTORADO NACIONAL (21170795) assigned to López-Angulo and the Proyect ANID-COVID-1012, “Desarrollo e implementación de procedimientos docentes para facilitar la disposición al aprendizaje en condiciones de distanciamiento físico por pandemia de COVID-19, en asignaturas de primer año universitario con mediano o alto riesgo de fracaso” assigned to MP-V and AD-M.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Almagia, E. B. (2012). Influencia de la autoestima y del apoyo social percibido sobre el bienestar psicológico de estudiantes universitarios chilenos. Divers. Perspect. Psicol. 8, 29–38. doi: 10.15332/s1794-9998.2012.0001.02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ato, M., López, J., and Benavente, A. (2013). Un sistema de clasificación de los diseños de investigación en psicología. Anales de Psicología 29, 1038–1059. doi: 10.6018/analesps.29.3.178511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bahar, H. H. (2010). The effects of gender, perceived social support and sociometric status on academic success. Procedia. Soc. Behav. Sci. 2, 3801–3805. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.03.593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, T. H., and Phillips, L. J. (2016). The influence of motivation and adaptation on students’ subjective well-being, meaning in life and academic performance. High. Educ. Res. Dev. 35, 201–216. doi: 10.1080/07294360.2015.1087474

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrantes-Brais, K., and Ureña-Bonilla, P. (2015). Bienestar psicológico y bienestar subjetivo en estudiantes universitarios costarricenses. Revista Intercontinental de Psicología y Educación 17, 101–123.

Google Scholar

Barrera-Herrera, A., Neira-Cofré, M., Raipán-Gómez, P., Riquelme-Lobos, P., and Escobar, B. (2019). Apoyo social percibido y factores sociodemográficos en relación con los síntomas de ansiedad, depresión y estrés en universitarios chilenos. Revista de Psicopatología y Psicología Clínica 24, 105–115. doi: 10.5944/rppc.23676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrera-Herrera, A., and Vinet, E. V. (2017). Adultez Emergente y Características culturales de la etapa en Universitarios chilenos. Terapia Psicológica 35, 47–56. doi: 10.4067/S0718-48082017000100005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biasi, V., De Vincenzo, C., and Patrizi, N. (2018). Cognitive strategies, motivation to learning, levels of wellbeing and risk of drop-out: an empirical longitudinal study for qualifying ongoing university guidance services. J. Educ. Soc. Res. 8, 79–91. doi: 10.2478/jesr-2018-0019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandy, J. M., Penckofer, S., Solari-Twadell, P., and Velsor-Friedrich, B. (2015). Factors predictive of depression in first-year college students. J. Psychosoc. Nurs. Ment. Health Serv. 53, 38–44. doi: 10.3928/02793695-20150126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, J., and Kern, M. L. (2016). The PERMA-profiler: a brief multidimensional measure of flourishing. Int. J. Wellbeing 6, 1–48. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v6i3.526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chu, P. S., Saucier, D. A., and Hafner, E. (2010). Meta-analysis of the relationships between social support and well-being in children and adolescents. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 29, 624–645. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2010.29.6.624

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coffey, J. K., Wray-Lake, L., Mashek, D., and Branand, B. (2016). A multi-study examination of well-being theory in college and community samples. J. Happiness Stud. 17, 187–211. doi: 10.1007/s10902-014-9590-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., and Janicki-Deverts, D. (2009). Can we improve our physical health by altering our social networks? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4, 375–378. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01141.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Contreras, F., Barbosa, D., and Espinosa, J. C. (2010). Personalidad, inteligencia emocional y afectividad en estudiantes universitarios de áreas empresariales Implicaciones para la formación de líderes. Diversitas: Perspectivas en Psicología 6, 65–79.

Google Scholar

Dávila Figueras, A., Ruiz Celis, R., Moncada Arroyo, L., and Gallardo Rayo, I. (2011). Niveles de ansiedad, depresión y percepción de apoyo social en estudiantes de Odontología de la Universidad de Chile. Revista de Psicología 20, 147–172.

Google Scholar

Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: the science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. Am. Psychol. 55, 34–43. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.34

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Biswas-Diener, R., Lyubchik, L., Halpern, D., Vitterso, J., Reis, H., et al. (2018). “Social well-being: research and policy recommendations” in Global happiness policy report: 2018. eds. J. F. Helliwell, R. Layard, and J. Sachs (New York: Global Happiness Council), 129–159.

Google Scholar

Diener, E., and Chan, M. Y. (2011). Happy people live longer: subjective well-being contributes to health and longevity. Appl. Psychol. Health and Well-Being 3, 1–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2010.01045.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Heintzelman, S. J., Kushlev, K., Tay, L., Wirtz, D., Lutes, L. D., et al. (2017). Findings all psychologists should know from the new science on subjective well-being. Canad. Psychol. 58, 87–104. doi: 10.1037/cap0000063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., and Oishi, S. (2005). The nonobvious social psychology of happiness. Psychol. Inq. 16, 162–167. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1604_04

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Oishi, S., and Lucas, R. E. (2015). National accounts of subjective well-being. Am. Psychol. 70, 234–242. doi: 10.1037/a0038899

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., and Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Very happy people. Psychol. Sci. 13, 81–84. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00415

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., and Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: three decades of progress. Psychol. Bull. 125, 276–302. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D. -W., Oishi, S., et al. (2010). New well-being measures: short scales to assess flourishing and positive and negative feelings. Soc. Indic. Res. 97, 143–156. doi: 10.1007/s11205-009-9493-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feeney, B. C., and Collins, N. L. (2015). A new look at social support: a theoretical perspective on thriving through relationships. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 19, 113–147. doi: 10.1177/1088868314544222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Figueira, C. P., Marques-Pinto, A., Pereira, C. R., and Roberto, M. S. (2017). How can academic context variables contribute to the personal well-being of higher education students? Span. J. Psychol. 20, 1–12. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2017.46

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Françoise, C., David, B., and Juan Carlos, E. (2010). Personalidad, inteligencia emocional y afectividad en estudiantes universitarios de áreas empresariales Implicaciones para la formación de líderes. Diversitas: Perspectivas en Psicología 6, 65–79.

Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. (2013). “Chapter one-positive emotions broaden and build” in Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. 47. eds. P. Devine and A. Plant (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 1–53.

Google Scholar

Friedlander, L. J., Reid, G. J., Shupak, N., and Cribbie, R. (2007). Social support, self-esteem, and stress as predictors of adjustment to university among first-year undergraduates. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 48, 259–274. doi: 10.1353/csd.2007.0024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herke, M., Rathmann, K., and Richter, M. (2019). Trajectories of students’ well-being in secondary education in Germany and differences by social background. Eur. J. Pub. Health 29, 960–965. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckz049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt-Lunstad, J., and Smith, T. B. (2012). Social relationships and mortality. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 6, 41–53. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joshanloo, M. (2018a). Investigating the relationships between subjective well-being and psychological well-being over two decades. Emot. 19, 183–187. doi: 10.1037/emo0000414

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joshanloo, M. (2018b). Longitudinal associations between subjective and psychological well-being in Japan: a four-year cross-lagged panel study. J. Pers. Individ. Differ. 134, 289–292. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.06.033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keyes, C., Shmotkin, D., and Ryff, C. (2002). Optimizing well-being: the empirical encounter of two traditions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 1007–1022. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.1007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, C., Heo, J., Lee, J. -W., Hji-Avgoustis, S., and Lee, S. (2020). Subjective well-being, activity types, and social context in undergraduate students’ daily experiences: an experience sampling study. Coll. Stud. J. 54, 106–116.

Google Scholar

Kleiman, E. M., and Riskind, J. H. (2013). Utilized social support and self-esteem mediate the relationship between perceived social support and suicide ideation: a test of a multiple mediator model. Crisis 34, 42–49. doi: 10.1027/0227-5910/a000159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kong, F., and You, X. (2013). Loneliness and self-esteem as mediators between social support and life satisfaction in late adolescence. Soc. Indic. Res. 110, 271–279. doi: 10.1007/s11205-011-9930-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kong, F., Zhao, J., and You, X. (2012). Social support mediates the impact of emotional intelligence on mental distress and life satisfaction in Chinese young adults. Personal. Individ. Differ. 53, 513–517. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kostak, M. A., Dindar, İ., and Dinçkol, R. Z. (2019). Loneliness, depression, social support levels, and other factors involving the internet use of high school students in Turkey. Int. J. Ment. Heal. Addict. 17, 1521–1534. doi: 10.1007/s11469-018-9927-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kyndt, E., Donche, V., Trigwell, K., and Lindblom-Ylänne, S. (Eds.) (2017). Higher education transitions: Theory and research. Londres: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Lakey, B., Orehek, E., Hain, K. L., and Vanvleet, M. (2010). Enacted support’s links to negative affect and perceived support are more consistent with theory when social influences are isolated from trait influences. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 36, 132–142. doi: 10.1177/0146167209349375

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langford, R., Bonell, C. P., Jones, H. E., Pouliou, T., Murphy, S. M., Waters, E., et al. (2014). The WHO health promoting school framework for improving the health and well-being of students and their academic achievement. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 4:CD008958. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD008958.pub2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lent, R. W., Singley, D., Sheu, H. -B., Gainor, K. A., Brenner, B. R., Treistman, D., et al. (2005). Social cognitive predictors of domain and life satisfaction: exploring the theoretical precursors of subjective well-being. J. Couns. Psychol. 52, 429–442. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.52.3.429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lepore, S. J., Glaser, D. B., and Roberts, K. J. (2008). On the positive relation between received social support and negative affect: a test of the triage and self-esteem threat models in women with breast cancer. Psychooncology 17, 1210–1215. doi: 10.1002/pon.1347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, C. H. (2017). University support, adjustment, and mental health in tertiary education students in Hong Kong. Asia Pacific Educ. Rev. 18, 115–122. doi: 10.1007/s12564-016-9466-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lima-Castro, S., Peña-Contreras, E., Cedillo-Quizphe, C., and Cabrera-Vélez, M. (2017). Adaptación del perfil perma en una muestra ecuatoriana. Eureka (Asunción, En línea), 14.

Google Scholar

Liu, H., Li, S., Xiao, Q., and Feldman, M. W. (2014). Social support and psychological well-being under social change in urban and rural China. Soc. Indic. Res. 119, 979–996. doi: 10.1007/s11205-013-0534-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Angulo, Y., Pérez-Villalobos, M. V., Cobo-Rendón, R. C., and Díaz-Mujica, A. E. (2020). Apoyo social, sexo y área del conocimiento en el rendimiento académico autopercibido de estudiantes universitarios chilenos. Formación Universitaria 13, 11–18. doi: 10.4067/S0718-50062020000300011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malkoç, A., and Mutlu, A. K. (2019). Mediating the effect of cognitive flexibility in the relationship between psychological well-being and self-confidence: a study on Turkish university students. Int. J. High. Educ. 8, 278–287. doi: 10.5430/ijhe.v8n6p278

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marrero, R. J., and Carballeira, M. (2010). El papel del optimismo y del apoyo social en el bienestar subjetivo. Salud Mental 33, 39–46.

Google Scholar

Monroy, L. A. V., and Ramírez, C. M. F. (2016). La familia como red de apoyo social en estudiantes universitarios que ingresan a primer semestre. Espiral Revista de Docencia e Investigación 6, 79–90. doi: 10.15332/erdi.v6i2.1651

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers, D. G. (2015). “Balancing individuality and community in public policy” in Positive psychology in practice: Promoting human flourishing in work, health, education, and everyday life. ed. S. Joseph (Hoboken, New Jork: John Wiley and Sons, Inc), 711–730.

Google Scholar

Nicolson, N. (1990). “Transition cycle: cause, outcomes, processes and forms” in On the move: The psychology of change and transition. eds. S. Fisher and C. L. Cooper (New York: Wiley).

Google Scholar

Nightingale, S. M., Roberts, S., Tariq, V., Appleby, Y., Barnes, L., Harris, R. A., et al. (2013). Trajectories of university adjustment in the United Kingdom: emotion management and emotional self-efficacy protect against initial poor adjustment. Learn. Individ. Differ. 27, 174–181. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oriol-Granado, X., Mendoza-Lira, M., Covarrubias-Apablaza, C. -G., and Molina-López, V. -M. (2017). Emociones positivas, apoyo a la autonomía y rendimiento de estudiantes universitarios: el papel mediador del compromiso académico y la autoeficacia. Revista de Psicodidáctica 22, 45–53. doi: 10.1016/S1136-1034(17)30043-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oriol, X., Mendoza Lira, M., Covarrubias, C. G., and Molina, V. c. (2016). Emociones positivas, apoyo a la autonomía y rendimiento de estudiantes universitarios. Revista de Psicodidactica 22, 45–53. doi: 10.1387/RevPsicodidact.14280

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortíz, M., and Baeza, M. J. (2011). Propiedades psicométricas de una escala para medir apoyo social percibido en pacientes chilenos con diabetes tipo 2. Universitas Psychologica 10, 189–196.

Google Scholar

Ouweneel, E., Le Blanc, P. M., and Schaufeli, W. B. (2011). Flourishing students: a longitudinal study on positive emotions, personal resources, and study engagement. J. Posit. Psychol. 6, 142–153. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2011.558847

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pastrana, M. P., and Salazar-Piñeros, F. (2016). Perfil PERMA en una muestra de jóvenes voluntarios colombianos. Katharsis 22, 13–34. doi: 10.25057/25005731.813

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavot, W., and Diener, E. (2011). “Personality and happiness: predicting the experience of subjective well-being” in The wiley-blackwell handbook of individual differences. eds. T. Chamorro-Premuzic, S. v. Stumm, and A. Furnham (John Wiley and Sons, Ltd), 699–717.

Google Scholar

Pillado, C. E., and Almagiá, E. B. (2019). Autoeficacia, apoyo social y bienestar psicológico en estudiantes universitarios asmáticos. Revista de Psicología 14, 141–147.

Google Scholar

Poots, A., and Cassidy, T. (2020). Academic expectation, self-compassion, psychological capital, social support and student wellbeing. Int. J. Educ. Res. 99, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2019.101506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahat, E., and İlhan, T. (2016). Coping styles, social support, relational self-construal, and resilience in predicting students’ adjustment to university life. Educ. Sci. Theory and Practice 16, 187–208. doi: 10.12738/estp.2016.1.0058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Renk, K., and Smith, T. (2007). Predictors of academic-related stress in college students: an examination of coping, social support, parenting, and anxiety. NASPA J. 44, 405–431. doi: 10.2202/1949-6605.1829

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosa-Rodríguez, Y., Cartagena, N. N., Peña, Y. M., Berrios, A. Q., and Osorio, N. T. (2015). Dimensiones de bienestar psicológico y apoyo social percibido con relación al sexo y nivel de estudio en universitarios. Avances en psicología latinoamericana 33, 31–43. doi: 10.12804/apl33.01.2015.03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R., and Deci, E. L. (2000). La Teoría de la Autodeterminación y la Facilitación de la Motivación Intrínseca, el Desarrollo Social, y el Bienestar. Am. Psychol. 55, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D. (1989). Beyond Ponce de Leon and Life satisfaction: new directions in quest of successful aging. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 12, 35–55. doi: 10.1177/016502548901200102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D. (2014). Psychological well-being revisited: advances in the science and practice of eudaimonia. Psychother. Psychosom. 83, 10–28. doi: 10.1159/000353263

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D. (2016). Eudaimonic well-being and education: Probing the connections. Well-being and higher education: a strategy for change and the realization of education’s greater purposes. In Bringing Theory to Practice, Washington, DC (pp. 37–48).

Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D., Radler, B. T., and Friedman, E. M. (2015). Persistent psychological well-being predicts improved self-rated health over 9–10 years: longitudinal evidence from MIDUS. Health Psychol. Open 2:2055102915601582. doi: 10.1177/2055102915601582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D., and Singer, B. (1996). Psychological well-being: meaning, measurement, and implications for psychotherapy research. Psychother. Psychosom. 65, 14–23. doi: 10.1159/000289026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santini, Z. I., Koyanagi, A., Tyrovolas, S., Mason, C., and Haro, J. M. (2015). The association between social relationships and depression: a systematic review. J. Affect. Disord. 175, 53–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarason, B. R., Sarason, I. G., and Pierce, G. R. (1990). “Traditional views of social support and their impact on assessment” in Wiley series on personality processes. Social support: An interactional view. eds. B. R. Sarason, I. G. Sarason, and G. R. Pierce (New York: John Wiley & Sons), 9–25.

Google Scholar

Shensa, A., Sidani, J. E., Escobar-Viera, C. G., Switzer, G. E., Primack, B. A., and Choukas-Bradley, S. (2020). Emotional support from social media and face-to-face relationships: associations with depression risk among young adults. J. Affect. Disord. 260, 38–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.08.092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siedlecki, K. L., Salthouse, T. A., Oishi, S., and Jeswani, S. (2014). The relationship between social support and subjective well-being across age. Soc. Indic. Res. 117, 561–576. doi: 10.1007/s11205-013-0361-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. E. (2011). “Social support: a review, Oxford library of psychology” in The Oxford handbook of health psychology. ed. H. S. Friedman (New York: Oxford University Press), 189–214.

Google Scholar

Thoits, P. A. (2011). Mechanisms linking social ties and support to physical and mental health. J. Health Soc. Behav. 52, 145–161. doi: 10.1177/0022146510395592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomás, J. -M., and Gutiérrez, M. (2019). Aportaciones de la teoría de la autodeterminación a la predicción de la satisfacción escolar en estudiantes universitarios. Revista de Investigación Educativa 37, 471–485. doi: 10.6018/rie.37.2.328191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torales, J., Barrios, I., Samudio, A., and Samudio, M. (2018). Apoyo social autopercibido en estudiantes de medicina de la Universidad Nacional de Asunción (Paraguay). Educación Médica 19, 313–317. doi: 10.1016/j.edumed.2017.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Umberson, D., and Karas Montez, J. (2010). Social relationships and health: a flashpoint for health policy. J. Health Soc. Behav. 51, S54–S66. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383501

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Der Zanden, P. J. A. C., Denessen, E., Cillessen, A. H. N., and Meijer, P. C. (2018). Domains and predictors of first-year student success: a systematic review. Educ. Res. Rev. 23, 57–77. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2018.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waterman, A. S. (2008). Reconsidering happiness: a eudaimonist’s perspective. J. Posit. Psychol. 3, 234–252. doi: 10.1080/17439760802303002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weismayer, C. (2020). Investigating the affective part of subjective well-being (SWB) by means of sentiment analysis. Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 1–16. doi: 10.1080/13645579.2020.1816251

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wörfel, F., Gusy, B., Lohmann, K., Töpritz, K., and Kleiber, D. (2016). Mental health problems among university students and the impact of structural conditions. J. Public Health 24, 125–133. doi: 10.1007/s10389-015-0703-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ye, J., Yeung, D. Y., Liu, E. S. C., and Rochelle, T. L. (2019). Sequential mediating effects of provided and received social support on trait emotional intelligence and subjective happiness: a longitudinal examination in Hong Kong Chinese university students. Int. J. Psychol. 54, 478–486. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12484

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., and Chen, M. (2018). Character strengths, strengths use, future self-continuity and subjective well-being among Chinese university students. Front. Psychol. 9:1040. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., and Farley, G. K. (1988). The multidimensional scale of perceived social support. J. Pers. Assess. 52, 30–41. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5201_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zubieta, E., Muratori, M., and Fernandez, O. (2012). Bienestar subjetivo y psicosocial: explorando diferencias de género. Salud Sociedad 3, 66–76.

Google Scholar

Keywords: perceived social support, eudaimonic well-being, university students, longitudinal study, affective well-being

Citation: Cobo-Rendón R, López-Angulo Y, Pérez-Villalobos MV and Díaz-Mujica A (2020) Perceived Social Support and Its Effects on Changes in the Affective and Eudaimonic Well-Being of Chilean University Students. Front. Psychol. 11:590513. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.590513

Received: 01 August 2020; Accepted: 10 November 2020;
Published: 11 December 2020.

Edited by:

Juan Carlos Oyanedel, Andres Bello University, Chile

Reviewed by:

Emilio Edmundo Moyano-Díaz, University of Talca, Chile
Javier Torres-Vallejos, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Valparaíso, Chile
Andres Mendiburo-Seguel, Andres Bello University, Chile

Copyright © 2020 Cobo-Rendón, López-Angulo, Pérez-Villalobos and Díaz-Mujica. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rubia Cobo-Rendón, cnViaWFjb2JvQHVkZWMuY2w=cnViaWFjb2JvQGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.