Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 08 October 2020
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Measuring Gratitude in Germany: Validation Study of the German Version of the Gratitude Questionnaire-Six Item Form (GQ-6-G) and the Multi-Component Gratitude Measure (MCGM-G)

  • 1University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
  • 2FOM University of Applied Sciences for Economics and Management, Munich, Germany
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Worcester, Worcester, United Kingdom
  • 4Center for Leadership and People Management, LMU Munich, Munich, Germany

The Gratitude Questionnaire-Six Item Form (GQ-6; McCullough et al., 2002) is a well-established instrument for measuring gratitude. Recently, the Multi-Component Gratitude Measure (MCGM) was developed as a more holistic approach (Morgan et al., 2017). While the GQ-6 mainly focuses on the emotional component of gratitude, the MCGM encompasses conceptual, attitudinal, and behavioral aspects. As of today, there is no validated German measure for gratitude. In order to close that research gap, the present study focused on validating the German versions of the GQ-6 (GQ-6-G) and of the MCGM (MCGM-G). In addition, multi-group comparisons were conducted to test for cultural measurement invariance. Construct validity was tested similar to original validation studies of the two scales focusing on affect, well-being, empathy, anxiety and depression. The online survey was completed in random order by 508 participants. The one-factor model of the GQ-6-G and the hierarchical structure of the MCGM-G could be replicated. However, the model fit of the Gratitude Questionnaire was significantly better after eliminating one item (GQ-5-G). Multi-group comparisons revealed cultural measurement invariance was established for the GQ-5-G and partial measurement invariance for five of six factors of the MCGM-G, respectively. Reliability analyses revealed good internal consistency for both instruments, and measures for criterion-related and discriminant validity have shown hypothesized relationships. Thus, the GQ-5-G and the MCGM-G are two instruments with good reliability and validity for measuring gratitude in Germany.

Introduction

Gratitude is a moral virtue that has universal and timeless presence in human life and has been discussed upon an intellectual level for centuries by philosophers and scholars (McCullough et al., 2002). In everyday lived experiences of gratitude, one can easily bring to mind an occasion that has warranted the simple expression of “thank you.” Indeed, knee-jerk expressions of gratitude have arguably become an unconscious part of interacting with others: you thank the person holding the door open for you, you say “thank you” to your colleague for sending an important email, and you express your gratitude after sneezing and receiving a “bless you” from your partner. However, a now diverse field of psychological literature on gratitude has shown it to be much more complex than simple expressions of “thank you.” The complexity of this construct is mirrored in the fact that there is no one agreed definition of the construct. Thus, gratitude can be understood and experienced in multiple ways (Morgan et al., 2017). According to Wood et al. (2010), gratitude can be seen as a trait-like disposition as well as a temporary state, such as a positive mood. Gratitude as an emotion is directed toward appreciating valuable help or actions from others (McCullough et al., 2001). However, there is also some evidence that gratitude can be associated with negative features, e.g., feelings of obligation, guilt, or embarrassment (Morgan et al., 2014). At the dispositional level, gratitude can be understood as “part of a wider life orientation toward noticing and appreciating the positive in the world” (Wood et al., 2010, p. 891) and has been shown to be distinct from facets of personality and other positive psychology constructs such as optimism and hope (Wood et al., 2008). This suggests that a latent grateful disposition exists.

At present there exist four well-established different measures to assess gratitude (Morgan et al., 2017; Lermer, 2019). The firstly developed measure of gratitude, the unifactorial Gratitude Questionnaire-Six Item Form (GQ-6), mainly focuses on the emotional component of gratitude (McCullough et al., 2002). In that context, gratitude was defined as “a generalized tendency to recognize and respond with grateful emotion to the roles of other people’s benevolence in the positive experiences and outcomes that one obtains” (McCullough et al., 2002, p. 112). The GQ-6 consists of six items (e.g., “I have so much in life to be thankful for”) that measure span, frequency, intensity, and density of gratitude (McCullough et al., 2002). In contrast, the Gratitude, Resentment and Appreciation Test (GRAT; Watkins et al., 2003) provides a broader view of gratitude. It consists of three subscales: sense of abundance (e.g., “There never seems to be enough to go around and I’m always coming up short”), simple appreciation (e.g., “I think that it’s important to ‘Stop and smell the roses”’), and appreciation of others (e.g., “I feel deeply appreciative for the things others have done for me in my life”). The third measure, the Appreciation scale, developed by Adler and Fagley (2005), conceptualizes and assesses gratitude as a subordinate facet of the broader construct of appreciation. Originally, appreciation was defined to measure something distinct from gratitude (Adler and Fagley, 2005), but Wood et al. (2008) have reported that gratitude and appreciation are a single-factor trait rather than distinct constructs. This broader appreciation scale comprises eight subscales: a focus on what one has (“have focus”), awe, ritual, present moment, self/social comparison, gratitude, loss/adversity, and interpersonal. After more than 10 years of not having any significant developments measuring gratitude, Morgan et al. (2017) introduced the Multi-Component Gratitude Measure (MCGM). The MCGM offers a holistic approach to gratitude measurement, as the authors consolidated the existing definitions. In total, the MCGM consists of four components. In addition to the original emotional aspect of gratitude, already proposed by McCullough et al. (2002), they added an attitudinal and behavioral aspect in their understanding of gratitude. The attitude component focuses on when to show gratitude and how gratitude is perceived in the context of values (e.g., “I believe it is important to thank people sincerely for the help they give me”). The behavioral component consists of the expression as well as lack of expression of gratitude toward one’s benefactors and of a more spiritual connection toward being thankful (e.g., “I stop to recognize all the good things I have in my life”). In addition, Morgan et al. (2017) proposed that people can understand and experience gratitude in various ways. Thus, the conceptual aspect was added to capture cognitions about gratitude. A fifth measure of gratitude was developed by Bernabé-Valero et al. (2014). The Gratitude Questionnaire-20 Items (G-20) captures four dimensions of gratitude (interpersonal gratitude, gratitude before suffering, the recognition of personal gifts in personal experience, and the expression of gratitude) using 20 items and was established using a sample of 330 Spanish undergraduates. As of today, the instrument is only available in Spanish and was recently validated with an Argentinian sample that supports the four-factor structure (Klos et al., 2020).

Early research found that being grateful is associated with a happier and more optimistic outlook on life (Watkins et al., 2003) and the tendency to overcome adversity more easily (Emmons and McCullough, 2003). Consequently, gratitude was shown to be related with higher subjective well-being (Emmons and McCullough, 2003) and life satisfaction (Peterson et al., 2007). Another well-established finding is the association of gratitude with lower risks of depression and reduced anxiety (Kendler et al., 2003; Froh et al., 2007, 2011). Further health benefits were reported concerning reduced stress (Wood et al., 2008) and improved sleep patterns (Emmons and McCullough, 2003; Wood et al., 2009). In addition, gratitude relates to interpersonal benefits, such as the ability to form and maintain stronger interpersonal bonds (Algoe, 2012; Bartlett et al., 2012) and promoting prosocial behaviors (Bartlett and DeSteno, 2006).

In order to conduct comparable and replicable studies on gratitude in Germany, it is vital to provide reliable and valid measuring tools. As of today, there is no validated German instrument measuring gratitude. Thus, the present study aimed to validate the most frequently used unidimensional measure, the GQ-6 (McCullough et al., 2002), and the newest holistic measure of gratitude, the Multi-Component Gratitude Measure (MCGM; Morgan et al., 2017). The GQ-6 has already been validated in several other languages including Hungarian (Tamás et al., 2014), Dutch (Jans-Beken et al., 2015), Chinese (Chen et al., 2008), Portuguese (Gouveia et al., 2019), and Spanish (Bernabé-Valero et al., 2013; Langer et al., 2016). It was mostly possible to replicate the unidimensional structure of the GQ-6 and to provide similar reliabilities, e.g., the Hungarian version had a good reliability of Cronbach’s alphas between 0.75 and 0.79 (Tamás et al., 2014). However, some studies encountered difficulties to validate the factor structure of the GQ-6, since item 6 (“Long amounts of time can go by before I feel grateful to something or someone”) did not load satisfactory on the proposed factor. Thus, two studies found a better model fit for the Spanish version when removing item six (Bernabé-Valero et al., 2013; Langer et al., 2016). Similar results were found for the Chinese version (Chen et al., 2008). The Dutch version also reported poor fit of item 6, although it was retained, since a possible exclusion did not improve the overall model fit (Jans-Beken et al., 2015). A recent study found that a three item version (GQ-3) of the GQ-5 provides the best model fit for a sample of Filipino high school students (Valdez and Chu, 2018). Due to these mixed results concerning the factor structure of the GQ-6, we decided to include the 6 items form in our study and test whether the six or five item version fits better.

It was hypothesized that the one-factor model of the GQ-6 and the hierarchical structure of the MCGM could be replicated through confirmatory factor analyses for the German versions. In addition, construct validity was examined. We used a number of constructs that have been applied in previous studies such as subjective well-being and positive and negative affect (Morgan et al., 2017), empathy (Ding and Song, 2017), as well as life satisfaction and psychological symptoms (McCullough et al., 2002). In line with findings in existing literature, we expected positive correlations between gratitude and positive affect, well-being, empathy and negative relationships with negative affect, anxiety, and depression.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedure

A total of 508 participants (79% female, 20% male, 1% other) completed the questionnaire using an online survey tool. In return they received their gratitude score in comparison to the United Kingdom sample (Morgan et al., 2017). The link to the survey was distributed through various mailing lists of German universities and through Facebook groups that are associated with psychological studies. Participants’ age ranged from 18 to 67 years (M = 24.80, SD = 8.00). Their highest educational level was 7% primary education, 65% A-levels and 28% university degree, with about 30% being psychology students. The order of presentation of the scales was randomized and average time to complete was 17.30 min (SD = 6.79). Due to using an online survey tool with all questions had to be answered mandatorily, there occurred no missing data.

In addition, a sample with 1,599 participants (52% female) from across the United Kingdom was used to test cultural measurement invariance. It was carefully selected to reflect United Kingdom population estimates. Participants’ age ranged from 18 to 83 years (M = 51.43, SD = 12.96). This sample was used for the initial validation of the MCGM in the United Kingdom (Morgan et al., 2017).

Measures

The German versions of the GQ-6 and the MCGM were derived using back-translation procedure. First, a translator was employed to translate the English versions into German. Thus, another professional translator retranslated these items into English. Eventually, both versions were consolidated by a psychologist and the final versions (GQ-6-G, MCGM-G) were later revised together with the authors to provide the best possible translation. The German items of both instruments are provided in Appendices A and B.

The GQ-6-G consists of six items that measure span, frequency, intensity and density of gratitude and primarily focuses on the unidimensional emotional component of gratitude. Participants have to rate six items (e.g., “I have so much in life to be thankful for”) on a seven-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree; GQ-6: α = 0.82; McCullough et al., 2002).

The MCGM-G consists of 43 items with four corresponding components of gratitude: (a) conceptions (or understandings) of gratitude; (b) grateful emotions; (c) attitudes toward gratitude; and (d) gratitude-related behaviors. For the conceptual component, participants are presented with seven scenarios involving different conceptions of gratitude: baseline, ulterior motive, cost to benefactor, non-realized benefit, malicious intent, value of benefit, and mixed emotions (e.g., baseline: “A colleague nominates you for an award at work. If you win, you will receive recognition of your hard work and a voucher”). After each scenario respondents have to indicate if they are grateful to this person on a five-point Likert scale (ARE ratings; 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree; original α = 0.54) and state what degree of gratitude they would feel in each situation (DEGREE ratings; 0 = not at all grateful to 100 = most grateful you could feel; original α = 0.79). The emotional component consists of the subscale Feelings of gratitude and combines six items (e.g., “I feel grateful for the people in my life”) that are rated on a seven-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree; original α = 0.87). The attitude component is measured analogously and consists of the subscales Attitudes to appropriateness (six items, e.g., “I only show gratitude toward people who clearly intended to benefit me”; original α = 0.85) and Attitude of gratitude (six items, e.g., “I believe gratitude is an important value to have”; original α = 0.74). The behavioral component is measured on a seven-point Likert scale (1 = never to 7 = more than once a day) and consists of the subscales Behavioral shortcomings (four items, e.g., “I overlook how much I have to be grateful for”; original α = 0.82), Rituals/noticing benefits (five items, e.g., “I stop and think about all the things I am grateful for”; original α = 0.92), and Expressions (of gratitude) (four items, e.g., “I express thanks to those who help me”; original α = 0.79; Morgan et al., 2017).

In addition to these two gratitude measures, we used five instruments to measure construct validity:

Positive and negative affect was measured using the German version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988; Krohne et al., 1996). The PANAS is based on the theory that positive and negative emotional states are not simply bipolar opposites but rather independent measures. It consists of 20 items that measure the general tendencies to experience positive (e.g., “excited”) and negative affect (e.g., “distressed”). Participants had to indicate on a five-point Likert scale (1 = not at all to 5 = extremely) how well each of the adjectives described “how they generally feel.” In previous studies (Krohne et al., 1996) the German scale has shown good values of internal consistency (α = 0.84 to α = 0.86).

Subjective well-being was assessed with the Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS; Lyubomirsky and Lepper, 1999; Bieda et al., 2017). The SHS consists of four items that measure global subjective happiness. Participants answered on a seven-point Likert scale whose wording of anchor points depended on the question (e.g., “Compared to most of my peers, I consider myself,” 1 = less happy to 7 = more happy). Internal consistency of the German scale was reported to be good with α = 0.87 (Bieda et al., 2017).

In addition, we measured life satisfaction using the German version of the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al., 1985; Glaesmer et al., 2011). The SWLS consist of five items and measures the cognitive component of subjective well-being. Participants indicated agreement with items (e.g., “In most ways my life is close to ideal”) on a seven-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree). The internal consistency of the German scale lies within an excellent level of α = 0.92 (Glaesmer et al., 2011).

The German short version of the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; Derogatis and Spencer, 1982; Franke et al., 2011) was used to examine psychological symptoms among the participants. Respondents had to rate their general suffering from anxiety and depression symptoms on a five-point Likert scale (1 = not at all to 5 = most intensive). The internal consistency of the German scale has proven to be good with α = 0.84 (Franke et al., 2011).

To measure the disposition toward empathy, the Empathic Concern and Perspective-Taking subscales of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis and Oathout, 1987; Paulus, 2009) were used. Participants had to rate Empathic Concern items (e.g., “When I see someone being taken advantage of, I feel kind of protective toward them”) and Perspective-Taking items (e.g., “Before criticizing somebody, I try to imagine how I would feel if I were in their place”) on a five-point Likert scale (1 = never to 5 = always). Internal consistency of the German scales is acceptable (Empathic Concern: α = 0.77; Perspective-Taking: α = 0.77; Lauterbach and Hosser, 2007).

Statistical Analysis

First, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used in order to test the factor structure of the GQ-6-G and the MCGM-G for the German sample. As data were not normally distributed, robust maximum likelihood estimation (MLM) with Satorra and Bentler (2001) scaled χ2 was used. In order to obtain robust estimates of the fit indices, we also adjusted those for the robust χ2 test statistic (Walker and Smith, 2016). Evaluation of the model fit followed typical conventions: Ideally the χ2-test should not be significant (Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003). However, the χ2-test tends to produce high and statistic significant values when sample size is high (N > 200: Walker and Smith, 2016; N > 250: Bühner, 2011). Thus, the ratio χ2 divided by degrees of freedom (χ2/df) represents a better fit index (Bentler and Bonett, 1980) and should be smaller than 3 (Kline, 1998). Values of the SRMR below 0.05 are good (Byrne, 1998) and should not exceed 0.08 (Hu and Bentler, 1999), though it must be noted that the SRMR decreases with larger sample size (Hooper et al., 2008). Values of the RMSEA below 0.05 indicate good model fit and also should not exceed 0.08 (Browne and Cudeck, 1993), whereas CFI and TLI should be above 0.95 (Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003).

Second, multi-group comparisons (Vandenberg and Lance, 2000; Fischer and Karl, 2019) were conducted to assess measurement invariance between the German and United Kingdom sample. The analysis was based on the three typical phases described by Fischer and Karl (2019). In the first step, the baseline model is compared with the configural model to examine whether the overall factor structure holds up similarly for both groups (configural invariance). The next step is to test whether factor loadings are equivalent across the groups (metric invariance). The last step is to check whether the item intercepts are equivalent across groups (scalar invariance). In case full measurement invariance could not be established, partial invariance was examined (Byrne et al., 1989; Putnick and Bornstein, 2016). Thus, modification indices were used to check for non-invariant items. In order to obtain partial measurement invariance, at least half of the items of a factor should be equal across groups (Steenkamp and Baumgartner, 1998; Vandenberg and Lance, 2000). Evaluation of the model fit followed typical conventions: Since χ2 is sensitive to sample size, differences in the CFI (Little, 1997) and RMSEA (Little et al., 2007) are more informative and should be below 0.01 for each level of invariance (Cheung and Rensvold, 2002; Putnick and Bornstein, 2016; Fischer and Karl, 2019). For reporting results of the multi-group comparisons, we followed the suggestions of Putnick and Bornstein (2016).

Third, analysis of the factor structure and cultural invariance was followed by assessment of Cronbach’s Alpha (α) of the gratitude measures as well a check for normal distribution using the Shapiro-Wilk test. To ensure a good reliability, item-total-correlation should be above 0.30 (Bühner, 2011) and Cronbach’s α should be above 0.70 (Schermelleh-Engel and Werner, 2012).

Finally, the intercorrelations as well as criterion-related and discriminant validity were calculated.

Data were analyzed using R Studio (version 1.2.1335 on macOS, R version 4.0.0). The lavaan package (Rosseel, 2012) was used to calculate the CFAs. The cyc package (Karl, 2020) and semTools (Jorgensen, 2020) were used to conduct the cultural invariance analyses. The level of significance for all analyses was α = 5%.

Results

Factor Structure of GQ-6-G and MCGM-G

We first tested the one-factor structure of the GQ-6-G. Results revealed a mediocre model fit (see Table 1). Factor loading of the reverse-coded item 6 (“Long amounts of time can go by before I feel grateful to something or someone”) appeared to be low (0.29). Thus, we run a second analysis after excluding item 6 (GQ-5-G). All fit indices improved and were acceptable to good apart from the significant χ2 test (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Goodness-of-fit indices of the CFA models (N = 508).

Second, we tested the factor structure of the MCGM-G. Here, three competing models were calculated. Fit indices of the original six-factor model (Morgan et al., 2017) indicated good fit apart from the significant χ2 test. However, items 2 (“Gratitude should be reserved for when someone intends to benefit you”) and 6 (“I only feel grateful when the benefit is of genuine value to me”) of the Attitude of appropriateness scale turned out to have negative factor loadings. We checked whether there occurred any mistake with reverse-coding of these two items during the previously performed analysis. Since no error could be found, we decided to exclude the items. After eliminating both items, model fit increased regarding all fit indices (CFI = 0.93, TFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.058, SRMR = 0.067). Interestingly, item 1 of the same scale appeared to have some issues as well with a low corrected item-total correlation of rit = 0.18. After the exclusion of item 1, the model fit further increased (CFI = 0.94, TFI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.050, SRMR = 0.059). Lastly, the second-order factor model was analyzed. Here, results were slightly worse compared to the modified six-factor model but still in an acceptable to good range (see Table 1).

Measurement Invariance Between Cultures of GQ-5-G and MCGM-G

The results of multi-group tests of measurement invariance of the GQ-5-G and the MCGM-G are presented in Tables 2 and 3.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of cultural invariance analyses between groups for the GQ-6-G and GQ-5-G.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary of cultural invariance analyses between groups for the MCGM-G and MCGM-G without items 1, 2, and 6 in the scale Attitudes to appropriateness.

Fit indices of the baseline model of the GQ-5-G indicate configural measurement invariance (χ2 = 72.43, df = 10, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.984; RMSEA = 0.077; SRMR = 0.020). A comparison with the model testing metric measurement invariance showed that the fit is worse (Δχ2 = 22.31, df = 4, p < 0.001). Since the χ2 difference test is sensitive to sample size and the other fit indices were good (CFI = 0.980; RMSEA = 0.074; SRMR = 0.031), metric measurement invariance can be assumed. Similar results were found for scalar measurement invariance. The χ2 difference indicated a worse fit (Δχ2 = 37.93, df = 4, p < 0.001). However, the other common fit indices were good (CFI = 0.971; RMSEA = 0.078; SRMR = 0.035), so scalar measurement invariance can be assumed.

Fit indices of the baseline model of the MCGM-G indicate configural measurement invariance (χ2 = 1845.90, df = 568, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.955; RMSEA = 0.046; SRMR = 0.039). A comparison with the model testing metric measurement invariance showed that the fit is worse (Δχ2 = 152.44, df = 20, p < 0.001). Since the χ2 difference test is sensitive to sample size and the other fit indices were good (CFI = 0.951; RMSEA = 0.048; SRMR = 0.044), metric measurement invariance can be assumed. However, results indicated a worse overall model fit on the level of scalar measurement invariance. In addition to the significant χ2 difference (Δχ2 = 1027.56, df = 20, p < 0.001), the differences of the other common fit indices were above the acceptable threshold of 0.01 (ΔCFI = 0.035; ΔRMSEA = 0.013). Thus, scalar measurement invariance cannot be assumed. Subsequent analyses using modification indices revealed that several item intercepts on all factors were not invariant across groups. Partial scalar measurement invariance could be established by allowing the intercepts of items 2, 4, 5, and 6 of Feelings of gratitude, items 4 and 5 of Attitudes to appropriateness, items 2 and 3 of Behavioral shortcomings, items 4 and 5 of Rituals/Noticing benefits, item 3 of Expression of gratitude and items 1, 2, 3, and 4 of Attitude of gratitude to vary between groups (χ2 = 2243.13, df = 592, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.942; RMSEA = 0.052; SRMR = 0.046; ΔCFI = 0.009; ΔRMSEA = 0.009; ΔSRMR = 0.002).

Intercorrelations and Reliability

Shapiro-Wilk test indicated that the GQ-5-G and all dimensions (including second-order factors) of the MCGM-G are not normally distributed (Skewness = −1.09 to 0.20, Kurtosis = 2.80–4.05). The average corrected item–total correlation for the GQ-5-G was rit = 0.49 and rit = 0.33–0.62 for the MCGM-G, respectively. All first- and second-order factors of the MCGM-G and the GQ-5-G had acceptable to excellent internal consistencies (Cronbach’s α = 0.73–0.90; Table 4). A detailed overview can be found in Appendices C and D.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Correlations between GQ-5-G and MCGM-G second-order factors and scales.

The GQ-5-G significantly correlated with all first- and second-order factors of the MCGM-G. The strongest relationship refers to the GQ-5-G and the emotional factor of the MCGM-G (r = 0.81, p < 0.01).

Criterion-Related and Discriminant Validity

The correlations between the gratitude scales (GQ-5-G, MCGM-G) and the criterion-related variables are presented in Tables 5, 6. As expected, we can report significant positive correlations between the gratitude measures and positive affect, well-being as well as empathy scales. The strongest relationship was found between the GQ-5-G and life satisfaction (r = 0.51, p < 0.01). In addition, significant negative correlations can be reported for depression and anxiety measures as well as negative affect.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Correlations between GQ-5-G and criterion-related scales.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Correlations between MCGM-G second-order factors and criterion-related scales.

Discussion

The main aim of the current study was to validate the psychometric properties of two gratitude measures in German language (GQ-6-G and the MCGM-G). We first sought to establish well-fitting baseline models for the two instruments conducting CFAs. The next step was to check for cultural measurement invariance using multi-group comparisons.

We replicated the one-factor structure of the GQ-6 and found a good fit after excluding item 6 of the scale. Hence, the German version of the GQ-6 enquires the same problem with item 6 as already reported for the Dutch (Jans-Beken et al., 2015) or Spanish version (Langer et al., 2016). The authors of the Spanish version argue that the five-item version is especially appropriate for younger populations (university students and adolescents) in comparison to older participants (Langer et al., 2016). Similar results were found in a non-Western sample examining gratitude among Filipino high school students (Valdez et al., 2017). Considering the mean age of just about 25 years (SD = 8.00) in the present study, this could also be a reasonable explanation for our findings. Multi-group comparisons were conducted to test measurement invariance across two samples from United Kingdom and Germany. Thus, measurement invariance on all three levels (configural, metric, and scalar) can be assumed for the GQ-5-G but not for the GQ-6-G, since differences in the CFI and RMSEA for the latter were above the threshold of 0.01. These results indicate that participants from both countries conceptualize the one-factor structure of the GQ-5-G in the same way (configural invariance). As metric invariance was also supported, associations between the GQ-5-G and other variables can be compared across samples from United Kingdom and Germany. Eventually, scalar invariance suggests that mean differences between groups are due to differences in the latent construct which allows comparisons of mean differences. We therefore recommend using the five-item version (GQ-5-G) for future research.

We found a good model fit for the MCGM-G after excluding three items of the Attitudes to appropriateness scale (items 1, 2, and 6 were excluded). Interestingly, the deviations compared to the original version of the MCGM only referred to this dimension. As Attitudes to appropriateness captures the degree when gratitude is and is not warranted, the findings might suggest different cultural understandings. Further studies should therefore explore this dimension of gratitude in a more in-depth examination. However, results of the CFAs did not reveal superior model fit of the hierarchical structure of the MCGM-G with second-order factors, as fit indices were almost identical compared to the six-factor solution. In contrast to the original study, both models had slightly worse fit. Thus, future studies should analyze the underlying factor structure more closely comprising larger samples. Nevertheless, on the basis of current knowledge, it seems appropriate to use the second-order factors of MCGM-G, if an economic consideration or merely a comparison of attitude and behavior components of gratitude is required. Multi-group comparisons of the MCGM-G testing for cultural invariance revealed that configural invariance was established. Thus, similar latent factors were present in both countries suggesting that participants from both samples conceptualize the six components of the MCGM-G similarly. Metric invariance was also supported. Accordingly, the factor structure of the six dimensions was equivalent across both groups indicating that individuals attributed the same meaning to the latent constructs. Since metric invariance was established, associations between the MCGM-G and other variables can be compared across samples from United Kingdom and Germany. However, scalar measurement invariance could not be supported. In total, 15-item intercepts on all factors turned out not to be equal across the two samples. When these item intercepts were allowed to vary between the two groups, partial measurement invariance could be obtained. An exception is the dimension Attitude of gratitude since all item intercepts of this factor were non-invariant. Thus, latent mean comparisons for five MCGM-G factors can be conducted, if the corresponding items are allowed to have their own intercept.

The strong correlation between the emotion component of the MCGM-G and the GQ-5-G (r = 0.81, p < 0.001) is even higher than in previous studies (e.g., r = 0.71, p < 0.001; Morgan et al., 2017). This supports the assumption that the GQ-5-G only taps feelings of gratitude, as it was already suggested by Morgan et al. (2017). Thus, all correlations between the other factors of the MCGM-G and the GQ-5-G were lower (r = 0.11 to r = 0.51).

Construct validity with criterion-related scales of both the GQ-5-G and the MCGM-G showed the expected correlations as already reported in previous studies (e.g., McCullough et al., 2002; Breen et al., 2010; Morgan et al., 2017). Overall, gratitude is associated with increased positive and lower negative outcomes. Wellbeing and affect scales revealed medium to strong effects, while effects of empathy, anxiety and depression scales can be characterized as low to medium effects. The associations between the criterion-related scales and the GQ-5-G and the MCGM-G show a similar pattern. However, the MCGM-G yields a more diverse perspective. Thus, our results indicate that the emotional component of gratitude is more strongly related to life satisfaction and subjective well-being, whereas the behavioral component is most strongly associated with positive affect and empathy. The correlations between the attitude component and the criterion-related scales are rather low.

Limitations

As in many psychological studies, the German sample is biased toward female participants and has a low mean age of 24.80 years. This means that the conceptualization of gratitude may only be representative of young females. Although this seems to be a typical bias that also affects other validation studies of gratitude measures (e.g., Gouveia et al., 2019) as well as the original study of the GQ-6 by McCullough et al. (2002), we checked for measurement invariance between gender. The results revealed full measurement invariance across the gender of the participants for the GQ-5-G and the MCGM-G (see Appendix E). Thus, mean comparisons between genders on the latent factors can be analyzed. Nevertheless, future studies should comprise more representative samples regarding age and gender and analyze whether the factor structure reported here is stable across gender-balanced groups. It should also be mentioned that using maximum-likelihood estimations on data that violate multivariate normality can bias results. However, we tried to account for this limitation by using robust maximum likelihood estimation (MLM) with Satorra and Bentler (2001) scaled χ2 as well as robust estimates of the fit indices following the adjustments of Walker and Smith (2016). Overall, our results are based on only two countries. Therefore, future studies are needed to evaluate measurement invariance of the GQ5-G and the MCGM-G in other countries.

Conclusion

The GQ-5-G and the MCGM-G are both reliable and valid instruments for measuring gratitude in Germany. Measurement invariance was established for the GQ-5-G and partial measurement invariance for five of six factors of the MCGM-G, respectively. Psychological research can rely on these tools for future studies on gratitude. In this context, the GQ-5-G can be considered a very good and economic choice if a reliable and valid instrument is needed to measure the emotional component of gratitude. In contrast, the multi-component approach of the MCGM-G offers a more diverse perspective on gratitude.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in an online repository in the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/8v5ej/.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

NB and EL designed the studies and supervised the execution. NB collected the data. MH did the analysis. NB, EL, BM, and MH wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.590108/full#supplementary-material

References

Adler, M. G., and Fagley, N. S. (2005). Appreciation: individual differences in finding value and meaning as a unique predictor of subjective well-being. J. Pers. 73, 79–114. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00305.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Algoe, S. B. (2012). Find, remind, and bind: the functions of gratitude in everyday relationships. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 6, 455–469. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2012.00439.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartlett, M. Y., Condon, P., Cruz, J., Baumann, J., and Desteno, D. (2012). Gratitude: prompting behaviours that build relationships. Cogn. Emot. 26, 2–13. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.561297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartlett, M. Y., and DeSteno, D. (2006). Gratitude and prosocial behavior: helping when it costs you. Psychol. Sci. 17, 319–325. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01705.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentler, P. M., and Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychol. Bull. 88, 588–606. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.588

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernabé-Valero, G., García-Alandete, J., and Gallego-Pérez, J. F. (2013). Análisis comparativo de dos modelos del gratitude questionnaire - six items form [Comparative analysis of two models of the gratitude questionnaire - six items form]. Rev. Latinoam. Psicol. 45, 279–288. doi: 10.14349/rlp.v45i2.811

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernabé-Valero, G., García-Alandete, J., and Gallego-Pérez, J. F. (2014). Construcción de un cuestionario para la evaluación de la gratitud: El Cuestionario de Gratitud-20 ítems (G-20) [Construction of a gratitude questionnaire: the gratitude questionnaire-20 Items (G-20)]. Anal. Psicol. 30, 279–287. doi: 10.6018/analesps.30.1.135511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bieda, A., Hirschfeld, G., Schönfeld, P., Brailovskaia, J., Zhang, X. C., and Margraf, J. (2017). Universal happiness? Cross-cultural measurement invariance of scales assessing positive mental health. Psychol. Assess. 29, 408–421. doi: 10.1037/pas0000353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breen, W. E., Kashdan, T. B., Lenser, M. L., and Fincham, F. D. (2010). Gratitude and forgiveness: convergence and divergence on self-report and informant ratings. Pers. Individ. Differ. 49, 932–937. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browne, M. W., and Cudeck, R. (1993). “Alternative ways of assessing model fit,” in Testing Structural Equation Models, eds K. A. Bollen and J. S. Long (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 136–162.

Google Scholar

Bühner, M. (2011). Einführung in die Test- und Fragebogenkonstruktion. London: Pearson Studium.

Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M. (1998). Structural Equation Modeling with LISREL PRELIS and SIMPLIS: Basic Concepts Applications and Programming. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M., Shavelson, R. J., and Muthén, B. (1989). Testing for the equivalence of factor covariance and mean structures: the issue of partial measurement invariance. Psychol. Bull. 105, 456–466. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.105.3.456

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, L. H., Chen, M.-Y., Kee, Y. H., and Tsai, Y.-M. (2008). Validation of the gratitude questionnaire (GQ) in taiwanese undergraduate students. J. Happiness Stud. 10:655. doi: 10.1007/s10902-008-9112-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, G. W., and Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. 9, 233–255. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cumming, G. (2014). The new statistics: why and how. Psychol. Sci. 25, 7–29. doi: 10.1177/0956797613504966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. H., and Oathout, H. A. (1987). Maintenance of satisfaction in romantic relationships: empathy and relational competence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 53, 397–410. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.2.397

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derogatis, L. R., and Spencer, P. M. (1982). Administration and Procedures: BSI Manual I. Baltimore: Clinical Psychometric Re-search.

Google Scholar

Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, F., and Song, Y. (2017). Gratitude and college students’ Helping behaviors: mediating effect of empathy and its gender difference. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 33, 289–296.

Google Scholar

Emmons, R. A., and McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: an experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 377–389. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.84.2.377

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, R., and Karl, J. A. (2019). A primer to (Cross-Cultural) multi-group invariance testing possibilities in R. Front. Psychol. 10:1507. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franke, G. H., Ankerhold, A., Haase, M., Jäger, S., Tögel, C., Ulrich, C., et al. (2011). Der einsatz des brief symptom inventory 18 (BSI-18) bei psychotherapiepatienten [The usefulness of the brief symptom inventory 18 (BSI-18) in psychotherapeutic patients]. Psychother. Psychos. Medizinische Psychol. 61, 82–86. doi: 10.1055/s-0030-1270518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Froh, J. J., Emmons, R. A., Card, N. A., Bono, G., and Wilson, J. A. (2011). Gratitude and the reduced costs of materialism in adolescents. J. Happ. Stud. 12, 289–302. doi: 10.1007/s10902-010-9195-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Froh, J. J., Miller, D. N., and Snyder, S. F. (2007). Gratitude in children and adolescents: de-velopment, assessment and school-based intervention. Sch. Psychol. Forum 2, 1–13.

Google Scholar

Glaesmer, H., Grande, G., Braehler, E., and Roth, M. (2011). The german version of the satisfaction with life scale (SWLS). Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 27, 127–132. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gouveia, V. V., Ribeiro, M. G. C., de Aquino, T. A. A., Loureto, G. D. L., Nascimento, B. S., and Rezende, A. T. (2019). Gratitude questionnarie (GQ-6): evidence of construct validity in Brazil. Curr. Psychol. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00197-x [Epub ahead of print]

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., and Mullen, M. (2008). Structural equation modelling: guidelines for determining model fit. Electron. J. Bus. Res. Methods 6, 53–60.

Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jans-Beken, L., Lataster, J., Leontjevas, R., and Jacobs, N. (2015). Measuring gratitude: a comparative validation of the dutch gratitude questionnaire (gq6) and short gratitude, resentment, and appreciation test (SGRAT). Psychol. Belgica 55, 19–31. doi: 10.5334/pb.bd

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jorgensen, T. D. (2020). Package ‘semTools’. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/semTools/semTools.pdf (accessed July 20, 2020).

Google Scholar

Karl, J. A. (2020). Ccpsyc. Available online at: https://github.com/Jo-Karl/ccpsyc (accessed July 20, 2020).

Google Scholar

Kendler, K. S., Liu, X. Q., Gardner, C. O., McCullough, M. E., Larson, D., and Prescott, C. A. (2003). Dimensions of religiosity and their relationship to lifetime psychiatric and substance use disorders. Am. J. Psychiatr. 160, 496–503. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.3.496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Klos, M. C., Balabanian, C., Leom, V. N., Bernabé-Valero, G., and García-Alandete, J. (2020). Propiedades psicométricas del cuestionario de gratitud (G-20) en una muestra de adolescentes argentinos [Psychometric properties of the Gratitude Questionnaire (G-20) in a sample of argentine adolescents]. Rev. Argent. Ciencias Comportam. 12, 50–59.

Google Scholar

Krohne, H. W., Egloff, B., Kohlmann, C.-W., and Tausch, A. (1996). Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen version der “positive and negative affect schedule” (PANAS). Diagnostica 42, 139–156. doi: 10.1037/t49650-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langer, ÁI., Ulloa, V. G., Aguilar-Parra, J. M., Araya-Véliz, C., and Brito, G. (2016). Validation of a Spanish translation of the Gratitude Questionnaire (GQ-6) with a Chilean sample of adults and high schoolers. Health Q. Life Outcomes 14, 53–61. doi: 10.1186/s12955-016-0450-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauterbach, O., and Hosser, D. (2007). Assessing empathy in prisoners - a shortened version of the interpersonal reactivity index. Swiss J. Psychol. 66, 91–101. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185.66.2.91

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lermer, E. (2019). Positive Psychologie [Positive Psychology]. Stuttgart: Utb.

Google Scholar

Little, T. D. (1997). Mean and covariance structures (MACS) analyses of cross-cultural data: practical and theoretical issues. Multiv. Behav. Res. 32, 53–76. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3201_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, T. D., Card, N. A., Slegers, D. W., and Ledford, E. C. (2007). “Representing contextual effects in multiple-group MACS models,” in Modeling Contextual Effects in Longitudinal Studies, eds T. D. Little, J. A. Bovaird, and N. A. Card (New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 121–147.

Google Scholar

Lyubomirsky, S., and Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: preliminary reliability and construct validation. Soc. Indic. Res. 46, 137–155. doi: 10.1023/A:1006824100041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCullough, M. E., Emmons, R. A., and Tsang, J. A. (2002). The grateful disposition: a conceptual and empirical topography. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 112–127. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.82.1.112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCullough, M. E., Kilpatrick, S. D., Emmons, R. A., and Larson, D. B. (2001). Is gratitude a moral affect? Psychol. Bull. 127, 249–266. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, B., Gulliford, L., and Kristjánsson, K. (2014). Gratitude in the UK: a new prototype analysis and a cross-cultural comparison. J. Posit. Psychol. 9, 281–294. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2014.898321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, B., Gulliford, L., and Kristjánsson, K. (2017). A new approach to measuring moral virtues: the multi-component gratitude measure. Pers. Individ. Differ. 107, 179–189. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.11.044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, C., Ruch, W., Beermann, U., Park, N., and Seligman, M. E. P. (2007). Strengths of character, orientations to happiness, and life satisfaction. J. Posit. Psychol. 2, 149–156. doi: 10.1080/17439760701228938

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putnick, D. L., and Bornstein, M. H. (2016). Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: the state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Dev. Rev. 41, 71–90. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J. Statist. Softw. 48, 1–36. doi: 10.1002/9781119579038.ch1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satorra, A., and Bentler, P. M. (2001). A scaled difference chi-square test statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika 66, 507–514. doi: 10.1007/BF02296192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schermelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H., and Müller, H. (2003). Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods Psychol. Res. 8, 23–74.

Google Scholar

Schermelleh-Engel, K., and Werner, C. S. (2012). “Methoden der reliabilitätsbestimmung,” in Testtheorie und Fragebogenkonstruktion, eds H. Moosbrugger and A. Kelava (Berlin: Springer), 119–141. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-20072-4_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steenkamp, J.-B. E. M., and Baumgartner, H. (1998). Assessing measurement invariance in cross-national consumer research. J. Consum. Res. 25, 78–107. doi: 10.1086/209528

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamás, M., Magdolna, G., and Judit, D. (2014). Hála Kérdõív magyar változatának (GQ-6-H) bemutatása és pszichometriai elemzés [Introduction and psychometric properties of the Hungarian version of the Gratitude Questionnaire (GQ-6-H)]. Mentálhigiéné Pszichoszom. 15, 203–214. doi: 10.1556/Mental.15.2014.3.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdez, J. P. M., and Chu, S. K. W. (2018). Examining the psychometric validity of the five-item gratitude questionnaire: an item response theory approach. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 38:073428291881654. doi: 10.1177/0734282918816542

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdez, J. P. M., Yang, W., and Datu, J. A. D. (2017). Validation of the gratitude questionnaire in filipino secondary school students. Span. J. Psychol. 20:E45. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2017.51

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandenberg, R. J., and Lance, C. E. (2000). A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organ. Res. Methods 3, 4–70. doi: 10.1177/109442810031002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, D. A., and Smith, T. J. (2016). Computing robust, bootstrap-adjusted fit indices for use with nonnormal data. Measur. Eval. Counsel. Dev. doi: 10.1177/0748175616671365 [Epub ahead of print]

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, P. C., Woodward, K., Stone, T., and Kolts, R. L. (2003). Gratitude and happiness: development of a measure of gratitude and relationships with subjective well-being. Soc. Behav. Pers. Intern. J. 31, 431–452. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2003.31.5.431

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, A. M., Froh, J. J., and Geraghty, A. W. (2010). Gratitude and well-being: a review and theoretical integration. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 890–905. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, A. M., Joseph, S., Lloyd, J., and Atkins, S. (2009). Gratitude influences sleep through the mechanism of pre-sleep cognitions. J. Psychosom. Res. 66, 43–48. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, A. M., Maltby, J., Stewart, N., Linley, P. A., and Joseph, S. (2008). A social-cognitive model of trait and state levels of gratitude. Emotion 8, 281–290.

Google Scholar

Keywords: gratitude, Gratitude Questionnaire-Six Item Form, Multi-Component Gratitude Measure, validation study, confirmatory factor analysis, cultural measurement invariance

Citation: Hudecek MFC, Blabst N, Morgan B and Lermer E (2020) Measuring Gratitude in Germany: Validation Study of the German Version of the Gratitude Questionnaire-Six Item Form (GQ-6-G) and the Multi-Component Gratitude Measure (MCGM-G). Front. Psychol. 11:590108. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.590108

Received: 31 July 2020; Accepted: 08 September 2020;
Published: 08 October 2020.

Edited by:

Bojana M. Dinic, University of Novi Sad, Serbia

Reviewed by:

Marcos Gomez, Adolfo Ibáñez University, Chile
Gloria Bernabé-Valero, Catholic University of Valencia San Vicente Mártir, Spain

Copyright © 2020 Hudecek, Blabst, Morgan and Lermer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Matthias F. C. Hudecek, matthias.hudecek@ur.de

These authors share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.