Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 28 October 2020
Sec. Comparative Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Comparative Psychology of Intelligence: Macphail Revisited View all 20 articles

Simplicity From Complexity in Vertebrate Behavior: Macphail (1987) Revisited

  • 1Department of Psychological Sciences and Brain Health Research Institute, Kent State University, Kent, OH, United States
  • 2Department of Psychological Sciences, Kent State University, Kent, OH, United States

Introduction

Causality is a constraint common to all ecological niches.”

Macphail, 1987

Macphail (1987) claimed that all vertebrate nervous systems rely on detecting, encoding, and acting upon causality, and that there are no differences in intelligence between vertebrate species. The latter claim constitutes what is widely described as the “null hypothesis.” We examine the null hypothesis from the perspective of how vertebrates learn based on the order of events—that is, we will examine the ubiquity and foundations of sequential learning and memory in vertebrate behavior. The claim will be that several neurobehavioral systems subserve vertebrate sequence learning and that these and perhaps other systems together simplify encoding environmental complexity during learning and provide the foundation for performing complex but highly organized behavior.

What constitutes “causality” as coded by vertebrate brains? From an animal's perspective, behavior is inherently sequential and relevant events in the environment occur in probabilistic relationships with behavior. In the laboratory, these relationships may be highly constrained, as in Pavlovian conditioning, instrumental conditioning, and even more complex cognitive or neuroscience-oriented paradigms employing animals as complex as humans. This fact does not imply that the foundations of vertebrate behavior depend on a single underlying mechanism, though it should be no surprise that associative theory is a powerful approach to understanding and describing such behavior. On the contrary, much evidence suggests that vertebrate behavior is the result of multiple complementary systems that converge, interact, and often compete. These systems produce the remarkably adaptive and complex behavior befitting the remarkably diverse and complex environments in which vertebrates live. Yet, despite the diversity of scenarios in which behavior is played out, causality is universally available for organisms to exploit to survive and perpetuate the species. We propose that non-human vertebrates, like humans, abstract representations of simple causal relationships between events from complex environments, that is, they encode “simplicity from complexity.” Furthermore, vertebrates may share separate interacting systems for different types of sequential information.

Note that the critical and most challenging test of MacPhail's null hypothesis claim is not that the simplest processes are conserved “upward” to the most complex vertebrates, but that the most complex processes can be observed when we look “downward” toward the simplest vertebrates. To be explicit, we ask whether vertebrates in general extract “simplicity from complexity” through common learning mechanisms and neural substrates. As a start, this is the question we examine directly comparing humans, rats, mice, and pigeons. We conclude that additional evidence is needed to confirm our speculations regarding the generality of learning consisting of extracting “simplicity from complexity” in all vertebrates, but that Macphail (1987) was not far wrong in proposing that common processes may underlie vertebrate cognitive abilities though not necessarily resulting in equivalent capacities.

What Cognitive Mechanisms are Common To Vertebrate Species?

Lashley (1951) rejected the notion that sequential behavior was accounted for by simple reflex chaining and argued instead for cognitive encoding of hierarchical plans. This notion contributed to the development of cognitive research in both human and non-human animals which continues to this day. Recently, Rosenbaum et al. (2007) shifted the theoretical concern from Lashley's focus on the nature of encoded sequence structures to identifying and describing the processes that contribute to the emergence of sequence structures in behavior. According to their view, even individual component movements are controlled by hierarchically organized plans (Rosenbaum et al., 2007).

Our work on non-human animal sequential rule abstraction, learning, and memory (retention) has also been motivated by Lashley's insights regarding the human capacity to use mental representations of pattern structures to plan ahead. As Reber (1993) and others have shown, plans need not be conscious or verbal. Early on, non-verbal pattern learning methods were developed to study sequence learning in rats (Hunter, 1920; Capaldi et al., 1966; Hulse, 1978), in monkeys (Straub et al., 1979; Straub and Terrace, 1981; Terrace, 1987), and in humans (Restle, 1970, 1973; Restle and Brown, 1970a,b). We developed a more general Serial Multiple Choice (SMC) task to analyze how rats, mice, pigeons, and humans learn complex sequential patterns of responses. A typical method allows animals to respond to circular arrays of 8 items, for example, a circular array of 8 manipulanda, one each on the walls of an octagonal chamber, or a circular array of 8 nosepoke locations on a touchscreen. In both cases, rats learn long but highly-organized patterns of responses on the manipulanda (e.g., Fountain et al., 2012; Garlick et al., 2017). Evidence from a variety of studies using this task indicate that all these species employ multiple concurrent cognitive processes to encode and produce complex sequential behavior (Fountain, 2008; Fountain et al., 2012). In the SMC task, the animal learns to make responses in a circular array (8-walled chamber or a circular touchscreen array). The required sequence of responses is typically a highly organized serial pattern of responses that may be characterized by multi-level hierarchical organization and “exceptions-to-the-rule.” Such patterns recruit multiple concurrent cognitive processes, namely, processes for encoding stimulus-response associations, timing/counting of events, and pattern structure including simple and higher-order rules (e.g., Muller and Fountain, 2010, 2016).

Furthermore, the learning and memory systems involved depend on different behavioral and neural systems as shown by dissociations observed in adulthood long after chronic adolescent drug exposure (Pickens et al., 2013; Renaud et al., 2015; Rowan et al., 2015; Renaud and Fountain, 2016; Sharp et al., 2019). Similar dissociations of cognitive behavioral systems can be seen in normal rats administered acute muscarinic cholinergic blockade (Pickens et al., 2013; Renaud et al., 2015; Rowan et al., 2015; Renaud and Fountain, 2016; Sharp et al., 2019). The SMC method in mice and rats produces data comparable to data in humans in an analogous task (Fountain and Rowan, 1995) and rats use multiple cognitive processes concurrently: rule-learning, stimulus-response (S-R) learning, discrimination learning, and multiple-item memory (Muller and Fountain, 2010, 2016). Fountain and Benson (2006) demonstrated chunking, rule-learning, and multiple-item memory when rats learned to anticipate the elements of two interleaved serial patterns. Mice show more limited abilities, but do show evidence of multiple concurrent learning processes (Fountain et al., 1999). Finally, pigeons in a comparable touchscreen task were able to abstract sequence structure (Garlick et al., 2017). Rats and, to a lesser degree, mice concurrently encode stimulus-response associations, time and count events, and abstract rules describing pattern structure (Muller and Fountain, 2010, 2016).

Taken together, these results strongly support the view that pigeons, mice, rats, and humans likely share multiple dissociable serial pattern learning and memory systems that encode multiple types of sequential information (Fountain et al., 2012). With reference to the underlying processes we have discussed in humans, rats, mice, and pigeons—namely, processes for encoding stimulus-response associations, timing/counting events, and rule-learning—are these common to all vertebrates? A broader survey of more species and new species-specific methods would be required to answer this question.

Can A Single Associative Process Account For All Non-Human “Intelligence?”

In our attempts to characterize how rats learn to anticipate items in a sequence, in one approach we sought to use mathematical models to determine whether a single mechanism might account for all sequence learning in rats. One early success in this line of research that bears on this question was a mathematical model we used to determine whether a simple mathematical model based on simple associative principles could account for rat serial pattern learning for sequences of food quantities presented in a runway (Wallace and Fountain, 2002, 2003). We used a modified version of Metcalfe's Composite Holographic Associative Recall Model (CHARM; Metcalfe, 1990). In CHARM, items to be remembered are represented by vectors of random numbers, where each vector represents an item to be remembered. Our model based on CHARM is named the Sequential Pairwise Associative Memory (SPAM) model. SPAM used the same system of creating vectors of random numbers to represent food quantities, but vectors for different quantities varied in similarity to represent a range of food quantities from small to large. When the appropriate vectors for food quantities were stored for different sequential patterns, SPAM was able to simulate a full range of effects previously reported in the rat sequential learning literature of the time (Wallace and Fountain, 2002, 2003). On the other hand, that model and variations of it have so far failed to account for the variety of differences in learning phenomena revealed in how rats learn highly-structured sequences (Muller and Fountain, 2010, 2016). Nevertheless, the failure of one model, no matter how successful within a single domain yet failing in another, does not preclude the possibility that it might be possible to develop a successful single-process model that would be consistent with Macphail's claim.

Some aspects of cross-species behavioral comparisons of rats, mice, and pigeons (Fountain et al., 1999; Kelley and Rowan, 2004; Garlick et al., 2017) do not easily fit within the SPAM framework. SPAM does not account for several very robust aspects of serial pattern learning in rats in the SMC task. For example, a large body of our work indicates that independent processes mediate different types of learning via dissociable systems that operate concurrently for encoding simple associations, serial position, and lower- and higher-order rule structure (Muller and Fountain, 2010, 2016). These observations suggest that much research is needed to determine whether the same patterns of results would be observed in species other than the species we have already examined, namely, humans, rats, mice, and pigeons.

The Role and Power of “Simple Associations,” “Hierarchical Plans,” and “Implicit Knowledge” in Vertebrate Cognitive Capacities

One conclusion we draw from the foregoing is that Lashley (1951) was correct to reject simple associative chaining accounts of sequential behavior. Lashley argued instead for a more cognitive account proposing that humans encoded and used hierarchical plans based on sequence structure and grouping which now seem fundamental to an analysis of animal sequence learning (Fountain et al., 2000; Muller and Fountain, 2010, 2016). Hierarchical plans, whether implicit or explicit, may be more fundamental than one might suppose given that even 8- to 10-month-old infants “exhibited sensitivity to the difference between hierarchical and non-hierarchical structure” and that the ability “to perceive, learn, and generalize recursive, hierarchical, pattern rules emerges in infancy” (Lewkowicz et al., 2018). The foregoing suggests that rule-learning in infancy must be implicit, and perhaps non-human vertebrates in general likewise can learn highly-organized implicit structures like those we have observed in pigeons, mice, and rats. This notion of hierarchical organization in behavior also unites our conceptions of behaviors as diverse as foraging (Feeney et al., 2011), bird song production and perception (e.g., Cazala et al., 2019), and sequential behavior (Swartz et al., 1991, 2000; Terrace and Chen, 1991a,b; Swartz and Himmanen, 2002; Suge and Okanoya, 2010; Spierings et al., 2015; Ramkumar et al., 2016). We go further to claim that vertebrate behavioral systems in their diversity encode different responses or different types of information, including complex associations, number (Brannon and Terrace, 2002), and time via internal clock processes (Tucci et al., 2014).

Macphail (1987) argued that all vertebrate nervous systems rely on detecting, encoding, and acting upon causality, and that there are no differences in intelligence between vertebrate species. We have described how a range of studies across paradigms and a variety of species support the view that complex learning processes may very well be broadly or even universally available to vertebrates. A challenge for the field is to develop experimental paradigms for assessing potentially common mechanisms in diverse species. At a foundational level, Macphail's claim continues to challenge all empiricists and theorists to consider the power of even simple neural systems to account for animals' ability to encode simplicity in terms of neural representation from the complexity of the surrounding environmental milieu.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed significantly to background reading, organization of the paper, and writing.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Brannon, E. M., and Terrace, H. S. (2002). “The evolution and ontogeny of ordinal numerical ability,” in The Cognitive Animal: Empirical and Theoretical Perspectives on Animal Cognition, eds M. Bekoff, C. Allen, and G. M. Burghardt (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press) 197–204.

Google Scholar

Capaldi, E. J., Veatch, R. L., and Stefaniak, D. E. (1966). Stimulus control of patterning behavior. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 61, 161–164. doi: 10.1037/h0022871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cazala, A., Giret, N., Edeline, J. M., and Del Negro, C. (2019). Neuronal encoding in a high-level auditory area: from sequential order of elements to grammatical structure. J. Neurosci. 39, 6150–6161. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2767-18.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feeney, M. C., Roberts, W. A., and Sherry, D. F. (2011). Black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) anticipate future outcomes of foraging choices. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 37, 30–40. doi: 10.1037/a0019908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fountain, S. B. (2008). Pattern structure and rule induction in sequential learning. Comp. Cogn. Behav. Rev. 3, 66–85. doi: 10.3819/ccbr.2008.30004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fountain, S. B., Benson, A. M., and Wallace, D. G. (2000). Number, but not rhythmicity, of temporal cues determines phrasing effects in rat serial-pattern learning. Learn. Motiv. 31, 301–322. doi: 10.1006/lmot.2000.1057

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fountain, S. B., and Benson, D. M. Jr. (2006). Chunking, rule learning, and multiple item memory in rat interleaved serial pattern learning. Learn. Motiv. 37, 95–112. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2005.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fountain, S. B., Krauchunas, S. M., and Rowan, J. D. (1999). Serial-pattern learning in mice: pattern structure and phrasing. Psychol. Rec. 49, 173–192. doi: 10.1007/BF03395315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fountain, S. B., and Rowan, J. D. (1995). Coding of hierarchical versus linear pattern structure in rats and humans. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 21, 187–202. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.21.3.187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fountain, S. B., Rowan, J. D., Muller, M. D., Kundey, S. M. A., Pickens, L. R. G., and Doyle, K. E. (2012). “The organization of sequential behavior: conditioning, memory, and abstraction,” in Handbook of Comparative Cognition, eds E. A. Wasserman and T. R. Zentall (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 594–614.

Google Scholar

Garlick, D., Fountain, S. B., and Blaisdell, A. P. (2017). Serial pattern learning in pigeons: rule-based or associative? J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Learn. Cogn. 43, 30–47. doi: 10.1037/xan0000109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hulse, S. H. (1978). “Cognitive structure and serial pattern learning by animals,” in Cognitive Processes in Animal Behavior, eds S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, and W. K. Honig (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 311–340.

Google Scholar

Hunter, W. S. (1920). The temporal maze and kinaesthetic sensory processes in the white rat. Psychobiology 2, 1–17. doi: 10.1037/h0073855

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelley, B. M., and Rowan, J. D. (2004). Long-term, low-level adolescent nicotine exposure produces dose-dependent changes in cocaine sensitivity and reward in adult mice. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 22, 339–348. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lashley, K. S. (1951). “The problem of serial order in behavior,” in Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior, ed L. A. Jeffress (New York, NY: Wiley), 112–146.

Google Scholar

Lewkowicz, D. J., Schmuckler, M. A., and Mangalindan, D. M. J. (2018). Learning of hierarchical serial patterns emerges in infancy. Dev. Psychobiol. 60, 243–255. doi: 10.1002/dev.21614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macphail, E. M. (1987). The comparative psychology of intelligence. Behav. Brain Sci. 10, 645–695. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00054984

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metcalfe, J. (1990). Composite holographic associative recall model (CHARM) and blended memories in eyewitness testimony. J. Exp. Psychol. Genrl. 119, 145–160. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.119.2.145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller, M. D., and Fountain, S. B. (2010). Concurrent cognitive processes in rat serial pattern learning: item memory, serial position, and pattern structure. Learn. Motiv. 41, 252–272. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2010.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller, M. D., and Fountain, S. B. (2016). Concurrent cognitive processes in rat serial pattern learning: II. Discrimination learning, rule learning, chunk length, and multiple-item memories. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 105, 155–175. doi: 10.1002/jeab.186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pickens, L. R. G., Rowan, J. D., Bevins, R. A., and Fountain, S. B. (2013). Sex differences in adult cognitive deficits after adolescent nicotine exposure in rats. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 38, 72–78. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2013.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramkumar, P., Acuna, D. E., Berniker, M., Grafton, S. T., Turner, R. S., and Kording, K. P. (2016). Chunking as the result of an efficiency computation trade-off. Nat. Commun. 7:12176. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit Learning and Tacit Knowledge. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Renaud, S. M., and Fountain, S. B. (2016). Transgenerational effects of adolescent nicotine exposure in rats: evidence for cognitive deficits in adult female offspring. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 56, 47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2016.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Renaud, S. M., Pickens, L. R., and Fountain, S. B. (2015). Paradoxical effects of injection stress and nicotine exposure experienced during adolescence on learning in a serial multiple choice (SMC) task in adult female rats. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 48, 40–48. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2014.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Restle, F. (1970). Theory of serial pattern learning: structural trees. Psychol. Rev. 77, 481–495. doi: 10.1037/h0029964

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Restle, F. (1973). Serial pattern learning: higher order transitions. J. Exp. Psychol. 99, 61–69. doi: 10.1037/h0034751

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Restle, F., and Brown, E. R. (1970a). “Organization of serial pattern learning,” in Psychology of Learning and Motivation, ed G. H. Bower (New York, NY: Academic Press), 249–331.

Google Scholar

Restle, F., and Brown, E. R. (1970b). Serial pattern learning. J. Exp. Psychol. 83, 120–125. doi: 10.1037/h0028530

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenbaum, D. A., Cohen, R. G., Jax, S. A., Weiss, D. J., and van der Wel, R. (2007). The problem of serial order in behavior: Lashley's legacy. Hum. Mov. Sci. 26, 525–554. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowan, J. D., Mccarty, M. K., Kundey, S. M. A., Osburn, C. D., Renaud, S. M., Kelley, B. M., et al. (2015). Adolescent exposure to methylphenidate impairs serial pattern learning in the serial multiple choice (SMC) task in adult rats. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 51, 21–26. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2015.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharp, J. L., Miller-cahill, M. E., Renaud, S. M., Kundey, S. M. A., Kelley, B. M., Matoushek, A. W., et al. (2019). Adolescent exposure to fluoxetine impairs serial pattern learning in the serial multiple choice (SMC) task in adult rats. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 164:107048. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2019.107048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spierings, M., de Weger, A., and Ten Cate, C. (2015). Pauses enhance chunk recognition in song element strings by zebra finches. Anim. Cogn. 18, 867–874. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0855-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Straub, R., Seidenberg, M., Bever, T. G., and Terrace, H. S. (1979). Serial learning in the pigeon. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 32, 137–148. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1979.32-137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Straub, R., and Terrace, H. S. (1981). Generalization of serial learning in the pigeon. Anim. Learn. Behav. 9, 454–468. doi: 10.3758/BF03209775

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suge, R., and Okanoya, K. (2010). Perceptual chunking in the self-produced songs of Bengalese finches (Lonchura striata var. domestica). Anim. Cogn. 13, 515–523. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0302-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swartz, K. B., Chen, S., and Terrace, H. S. (1991). Serial learning by rhesus monkeys: I. acquisition and retention of multiple four-item lists. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 17, 396–410. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.17.4.396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swartz, K. B., Chen, S., and Terrace, H. S. (2000). Serial learning by rhesus monkeys: II. learning four-item lists by trial and error. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 26, 274–285. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.26.3.274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swartz, K. B., and Himmanen, S. A. (2002). “Serial learning by rhesus monkeys: Strategies for list learning and execution,” in Animal Cognition and Sequential Behavior: Behavioral, Biological, and Computational Perspectives, eds S. B. Fountain, M. Bunsey, J. H. Danks, and M. K. McBeath (Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic), 58–67.

Google Scholar

Terrace, H. S. (1987). Chunking by a pigeon in a serial learning task. Nature 325, 149–151. doi: 10.1038/325149a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terrace, H. S., and Chen, S. (1991a). Chunking during serial learning by a pigeon: II. integrity of a chunk on a new list. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 17, 94–106. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.17.1.94

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terrace, H. S., and Chen, S. (1991b). Chunking during serial learning by a pigeon: III. What are the necessary conditions for establishing a chunk? J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 17, 107–118. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.17.1.107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucci, V., Buhusi, C. V., Gallistel, R., and Meck, W. H. (2014). Towards an integrated understanding of the biology of timing. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 369:20120470. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0470

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallace, D. G., and Fountain, S. B. (2002). What is learned in sequential learning? an associative model of reward magnitude serial-pattern learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 28, 43–63. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.28.1.43

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallace, D. G., and Fountain, S. B. (2003). An associative model of rat serial pattern learning in 3-element sequences. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 56B, 301–320. doi: 10.1080/02724990244000304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: associations, hierarchical plan, rule learning, multiple cognitive processes, multiple-item memory, stimulus-response learning, timing, internal clock

Citation: Fountain SB, Dyer KH and Jackman CC (2020) Simplicity From Complexity in Vertebrate Behavior: Macphail (1987) Revisited. Front. Psychol. 11:581899. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.581899

Received: 10 July 2020; Accepted: 18 September 2020;
Published: 28 October 2020.

Edited by:

Damian Scarf, University of Otago, New Zealand

Reviewed by:

Gema Martin-Ordas, University of Stirling, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2020 Fountain, Dyer and Jackman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stephen B. Fountain, c2ZvdW50YWkmI3gwMDA0MDtrZW50LmVkdQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.