Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 09 February 2021
Sec. Perception Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Differences in Pain Biology, Perception, and Coping Strategies: Towards Sex and Gender Specific Treatments View all 7 articles

Interaction Between Sex and Cardiac Interoceptive Accuracy in Measures of Induced Pain

  • 1Institute of Health Promotion and Sport Sciences, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
  • 2Department of Individual Differences and Psychological Assessment, Institute of Psychology and Education, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany
  • 3Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, Institute of Psychology and Education, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany
  • 4Department of Molecular Psychology, Institute of Psychology and Education, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany

Pain perception is influenced by several factors, and among them, affect, sex, and perception of bodily signals are assumed to play a prominent role. The aim of the present study is to explore how sex, cardiac interoceptive accuracy, and the interaction of the latter two influence the perception of experimentally induced pain. We investigated a large sample of young adults (n = 159, 50.9% female, age: 23.45, SD = 3.767), assessing current positive and negative affective state with the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (both involved as control variables), cardiac interoceptive accuracy with the mental heartbeat tracking task, and pain sensitivity with electrical stimulation on the back of the dominant hand, applying a repeated-measures staircase protocol. Males showed a significantly higher pain threshold and tolerance level than females, whereas cardiac interoceptive accuracy was not associated with pain sensitivity. The impact of sex × cardiac interoceptive accuracy interaction was significant for pain threshold only, while pain tolerance was predicted only by sex. According to these findings, the associations between pain sensitivity, cardiac IAc, and sex might be more complicated than it was supposed in previous studies. Interactions between factors impacting pain perception appear worthy of further investigation.

Introduction

Pain is defined as an unpleasant experience with sensory and emotional components, associated with potential tissue damage (Merskey and Bogduk, 1994), and strongly influenced by somatic, psychological, and social factors (Moseley, 2007). Neuroanatomical evidence demonstrates that pain processing shows a considerable overlap, with processing of visceral signals representing the actual homeostatic condition of the body; pain was also called the homeostatic emotion (Craig, 2003). Based on these findings, a broadperspective on interoception was proposed, which includes pain, itch, sensual touch, the sense of the metabolic state of the muscles, and other modalities beyond the classic visceroceptive channelsCraig, 2010). Concerning the conscious aspects of interoception, Garfinkel and colleagues (Garfinkel et al., 2015) proposed a multifaceted model, which differentiates objective (called interoceptive accuracy, IAc), subjective (interoceptive sensibility), and metacognitive (interoceptive awareness) dimensions of interoception. Although cardiac IAc cannot be generalized to other interoceptive modalities (Ferentzi et al., 2018), it is often used as an indicator of the acuity of perception of interoceptive signals. As the experience of pain is accompanied by a marked cardiovascular response (Gracely, 1999), the use of cardiac IAc is well justified in this area of research.

Both interoception and pain are interpreted as sensitivity towards bodily or body-related signals, associated with cognitive and emotional regulatory processes (Craig, 2003, 2015); thus, a connection between the two constructs seems plausible. However, the relationship of cardiac IAc and pain is not yet well understood, as empirical evidence provided so far is contradictory.

A systematic review drew the conclusion that patients with chronic pain syndromes are characterized by a less accurate perception of heartbeats than healthy control subjects (Di Lernia et al., 2016). Chronicity of the condition, however, might play an important role in theseresults. Studies that investigated non-clinical samples either found that more accurate heartbeat perception (as assessed by the mental heartbeat tracking task; Schandry, 1981) was associated with decreased pain threshold and tolerance (Pollatos et al., 2012; Weiss et al., 2014) or revealed no association (Werner et al., 2009; Ferentzi et al., 2018). It is an open question what might explain these contradictory findings. Pollatos et al. (2012) investigated 60 participants and found a correlation of medium effect size (cardiac IAc and pain threshold: r = −0.42; p < 0.01; cardiac IAc and pain tolerance: r = −0.33, p < 0.05) similarly to Weiss et al. (2014; n = 30, cardiac IAc and pain threshold: r = −0.53; p < 0.01; cardiac IAc and pain tolerance: r = −0.44, p < 0.05); both studies used a pressure algometry to induce pain. Both the studies of Weiss et al. (2014) and Werner et al. (2009) used preselected samples. The former investigated a clinical sample as well, while the latter matched samples of people with high and low heartbeat perception (n = 62). The two studies that found no association applied heat (Werner et al., 2009) or induced ischemic pain (Ferentzi et al., 2018). The diversity of the applied methodologies could partly influence the incoherence of the findings.

A candidate to help understanding the pain and cardiac IAc relationship is sex because it seems to relate to both pain perception and cardiac IAc. Males usually show higher pain threshold and tolerance level (Rhudy and Williams, 2005; Fillingim et al., 2009) as well as higher cardiac IAc (Whitehead et al., 1977; Jones and Hollandsworth, 1981; Katkin et al., 1981; Katkin, 1985; Harver et al., 1993; Montoya et al., 1993; Grabauskaitė et al., 2017). Interestingly, there is no agreement concerning the explanation of the latter finding. Body composition might be an influencing factor (Montgomery et al., 1984; Jones et al., 1987) and could be in the background of sex differences in IAc (Rouse et al., 1988). There is a study investigating children, but it still found differences between males and females even after controlling for body mass index (Koch and Pollatos, 2014). Cardiodynamic characteristics (e.g., stroke volume and the momentum of ejected blood mass; Schandry et al., 1993; Khalsa et al., 2009) may also lie behind sex differences in heartbeat perception. Moreover, the existence of sex difference in cardiac IAc is not uniformly supported by empirical findings; there are studies that reported no divergence between sexes (e.g., Mussgay et al., 1999; Pollatos and Schandry, 2004).

Unlike cardiac IAc, the difference between males and females in pain sensitivity is widely supported by empirical studies (Fillingim et al., 2009). According to recent findings, females show higher pain sensitivity than males independently from the applied experimental pain evoking modality (Ostrom et al., 2017). Sex differences in pain perception have been explained by various modulating psychological factors (Ostrom et al., 2017); however, differences in cardiac IAc or other interoceptive modalities have not been mentioned among these. In summary, males are characterized by higher cardioceptive IAc and lower pain sensitivity than females. This might indicate that sex plays a moderating role on the association between interoception and pain sensitivity.

Furthermore, according to theoretical accounts and empirical investigations, both pain perception and cardiac IAc are tightly linked to affect. In the case of pain, it is not clear which aspect of affect is relevant. On the one hand, most studies investigating the effect of experimentally induced emotions found that pleasant affective states are associated with higher pain threshold and tolerance, and negative states with increased sensitivity toward pain (Meagher et al., 2001; Carter et al., 2002; Villemure et al., 2003; Rainville et al., 2005). According to other findings, on the other hand, the emotion stimulation itself (along with further modifying factors, such as attention) is associated with reduced pain sensitivity, while the emotional valence of the stimulation is less relevant (Arntz et al., 1991; Arntz and de Jong, 1993; Villemure, 2002; de Tommaso et al., 2009). Interoception, in general, is believed to be tightly linked to emotional experience (James, 1884; Cannon, 1927; Damasio, 2010). Recent empirical studies applying emotional stimuli seem to confirm this view (e.g., Herbert et al., 2007, 2010; Pollatos et al., 2007a). There is evidence that the spontaneous affective state is also related to interoception. For example, state anxiety is associated with higher cardiac IAc as assessed by mental heartbeat tracking task (Schandry, 1981; Ludwick-Rosenthal and Neufeld, 1985; Pollatos et al., 2007b). Thus, controlling for the affective state might shed more light on the relationships between sensitivity to pain, cardiac IAc, and sex.

The aim of the present study was to better understand how current affective state, heartbeat perception ability, and sex influence pain perception in healthy individuals. Our hypotheses are that higher cardiac IAc is associated with lower (1) pain threshold and (2) pain tolerance. Furthermore, sex differences with respect to pain sensitivity were also assumed; we expected (3) higher pain threshold and (4) tolerance for males than females and (5) an interaction between sex and cardiac IAc on measures of pain.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 159 university students (50.9% female, mean age: 23.45, SD = 3.767) participated in the study. They were compensated with 20€. The exclusion criteria included psychiatric or neurological illnesses with current, not treated symptoms. The participants were fluent in German and were recruited from a local database called Ulm Gene Brain Behavior Project. Participation was voluntary and anonymous. All participants signed a written informed consent before the start of the measurements. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ethics committee of Ulm University, Ulm, Germany.

As data collection was part of a larger study (see the details below), a priori sample size calculation was not possible. However, the minimum required sample size for linear multiple regression with five tested predictors and medium effect size (f2 = 0.15) is n = 138 (α = 0.05, power = 0.95; calculated with GPower v3.1.9.4. software, Faul et al., 2007). As the sample size of the present study exceeds this threshold, it can be considered appropriate for testing the hypotheses introduced above. We assumed medium effect size based on the study of Pollatos et al. (2012); this study investigated a similar sample and reported a correlation of r = −0.42 (p < 0.01) between IAc and pain threshold and r = −0.33 (p < 0.05) between cardiac IAc and pain tolerance.

Study Protocol

The data collection started with the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) questionnaire (Watson et al., 1988). After that, all the electrodes were placed (measuring electrocardiogram, electromyogram, and skin conductance—the data of the latter two are not used here) using a portable BIOPAC system, Biopac MP150 system with the modules UIM100C, STP100C, ECG100C, GSR100C, and two EMG100C modules. Data were acquired with AcqKnowledge 4.1.1 (BUILD: 04.23.2010; 07.14.2010c; DISK46). This was followed by the heartbeat tracking task and the pain perception task. The data collection occurred as part of a larger study. The results of the measurements that followed are not reported here (i.e., pain regulation task, cognitive tasks, and emotion expression task).

Measures

Positive and Negative Affect

The PANAS aims to assess positive (PA) and negative (NA) emotional states (Watson et al., 1988). We used the 20-item-long version of the PANAS that measures the current affective state of the participants, with 10 items per each affect (Watson et al., 1988). High scores refer to high level of affect. The internal consistencies assessed by Cronbach’s alpha were α = 0.84 (PA) and 0.83 (NA).

Cardiac Interoceptive Accuracy

Cardiac IAc was measured with the mental tracking method (Schandry, 1981) over four different periods of time with various length (25, 35, 45, and 60 s) presented in random order. The participants were seated comfortably, with palms facing up on a table in front of them. They were asked to count their heartbeats silently while not using any extra tricks to feel their heartbeats better. The participants were encouraged to report any number of heartbeats they could feel by emphasizing that there are no right and wrong answers and by adding that there are not many people who can feel their heartbeats correctly. If they were uncertain or could not feel their heartbeats, they were encouraged to still try to count or estimate the number of heartbeats. The accuracy score was calculated for each trial using the following formula: 1 – | (recorded heartbeats–counted heartbeats)/recorded heartbeats|; the measure of cardiac accuracy score was then calculated as an aggregate mean score over all items, ranging between 0 and 1. Thus, higher values indicate better IAc. Cronbach’s α for the four trials is 0.95 in the current study.

Pain Perception

Pain-related perception was measured with a psychophysical upward staircase threshold estimation task characterized by two thresholds: the pain threshold (when the slightest pain, i.e., more than anunpleasant sensation was felt for the first time) and the pain tolerance (when pain was not tolerable anymore). Pain stimuli were applied via electrical stimulation on the back of the dominant hand; the cathode was applied to the middle phalanx of the index and the anode to the proximal phalanx of the second finger (400 V; with a starting point of 0.1 mA, increasing by 0.1 mA with each trial to set the sensation threshold first and by 0.5-mA steps to measure pain threshold and tolerance). The length of each trial was 5 s of stimulation, with a 6–10-s-long break in between.

The participants were instructed that the aim of the study is to assess their sensitivity to electrical impulses. They were also informed that they will feel pain at a certain point of the assessment. In the case of pain threshold, they were asked to report when they feel the smallest signs or sensations of pain; in case of pain tolerance, they were asked to report when they cannot bear the pain anymore and want to interrupt the stimulation.

The participants verbally indicated when a stimulus crossed the currently assessed threshold. The stimulus intensity (in mA) rated as crossing the threshold (first painful or maximally tolerable stimulus) was taken as a threshold value. Stimulation was immediately interrupted if the participants reported that it crossed the tolerance threshold.

Pain threshold was assessed three times (average correlation between the three values is rho = 0.955, p < 0.001). The assessment re-started at the lowest threshold, minus three steps (0.3 or 1.5 mA). Pain tolerance was assessed two times (rho = 0.975, p < 0.001). The final score for each threshold was the average of the respective trials.

Data Analyses

Statistical analysis was performed with JASP software 0.9.1 (JASP Team, 2019). According to the Shapiro–Wilk test of normality, only PA was normally distributed (p = 0.354; for all the other variables, p < 0.001). Therefore, Spearman correlations were computed to assess the associations between the variables. As sex has a strong influence on subjective pain ratings, sex differences were investigated separately with Mann–Whitney U-test. Effect sizes (ES) were estimated as a rank–biserial correlation. As the frequentist approach to statistical hypothesis testing has recently received criticism (Dienes, 2011; Jarosz and Wiley, 2014), hypotheses were tested using both frequentist and Bayesian methods.

Based on visual inspection, five outlier values were identified for both pain perception values at the trial level (from two participants); these values were winsorized, i.e., the original values were replaced by the nearest value from the same variable set.

To analyze the effect of positive and negative affective state, heartbeat perception score, and sex on pain perception, both frequentist and Bayesian regression analyses were conducted. In the frequentist approach, two different linear regression analyses were conducted for pain threshold and pain tolerance. The variables were entered using the ENTER method in four steps: (1) positive and negative affect, (2) sex (males = 1, females = 2), (3) heartbeat perception score, and (4) sex × heartbeat perception score interaction term. The interaction term was calculated as the product of the centered values of sex and heartbeat perception scores.

In the Bayesian approach, the null model always included positive and negative affect (see the details below). The results are presented as Bayes factors (BF10), showing the likelihood of the alternative hypothesis as compared to the null hypothesis. BF10 between 1 and 3 represent anecdotal, between 3 and 10 represent substantial, between 10 and 30 represent strong, between 30 and 100 represent very strong, and >100 represent decisive evidence in favor of the alternative hypothesis (Jeffreys, 1998; Jarosz and Wiley, 2014).

Results

Descriptive Statistics and Correlation Analyses

Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics and the correlations of the measured variables, while Table 2 presents the findings of the Bayesian correlation analysis. In summary, the results of both frequentist and Bayesian analyses indicate a weak positive correlation between positive affect and pain tolerance. The frequentist approach also shows a weak association between positive affect and pain threshold, whereas associations between pain sensitivity and negative affect were not supported. In fact, BF10 values indicated the superiority of the null hypothesis (i.e., the lack of association with positive and negative affect) for both tolerance and threshold. There is also a weak positive correlation between pain tolerance and cardiac IAc according to the frequentist analysis.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics (mean, standard deviation and minimum-maximum values in the diagonal list) of the assessed variables and Spearman correlation coefficients (with rho and p values); the last two columns present the data divided by sex.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of the Bayesian correlation analysis (Kendall’s tau-b coefficients).

Sex Differences in Pain Measures

Significant sex differences with respect to both assessedpain-related variables were found. The males showed significantly higher pain threshold (W = 4,342, p < 0.001, d = 0.374) and pain tolerance level (W = 5,401, p < 0.001, d = 0.710) than the females. The Bayesian analysis indicated decisive evidence both for pain threshold and tolerance (BF10 = 123 and BF10 = 69511, respectively) in favor of the alternative hypotheses.

Interaction Between Sex and Cardiac IAc on Pain Threshold and Pain Tolerance

The final equation of the linear regression analysis with pain threshold as criterion variable was significant [F(5,153) = 6.214, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.169], with sex (β = −0.324, p < 0.001; males showing higher values) and sex × cardiac IAc interaction (β = −0.192, p = 0.014) as significant predictors (see details in Table 3). Concerning the interaction, higher levels of heartbeat tracking ability were associated with disproportionately lower levels of pain threshold in females compared to males. Figure 1 shows how cardiac IAc relates to pain threshold by gender. The ghost line on the right shows the fitted regression line made for males. Note that the fitted lines of the figure do not consider the impact of positive and negative affect.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of multiple linear regression analysis with pain threshold as criterion variable.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Negative interaction between sex and cardiac IAc in the prediction of pain threshold (fitted regression lines with confidence intervals).

The final equation predicting pain tolerance was also significant[F(5,153) = 18.354, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.375]. The males were again characterized by higher values (β = −0.519, p < 0.001). The interaction term was just above the accepted level of statistical significance (β = −0.126, p = 0.061), just like the cardiac IAc scores (β = 0.120, p = 0.067; Table 4). Figure 2 shows how cardiac IAc relates to pain threshold by gender. (Similar to Figure 1, the ghost line on the right shows the fitted regression line made for males. Note also that the fitted lines of the figure do not consider the impact of positive and negative affect. The cardiac IAc and indicators of the current affective state were not associated with indicators of pain sensitivity.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of multiple linear regression analysis with pain tolerance as criterion variable.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The relation of sex and cardiac IAc in the prediction of pain tolerance (fitted regression lines with confidence intervals).

In the Bayesian approach, when the null model included positive andnegative affect and the alternative model included sex, the Bayes factor was BF10 = 539.991 for pain threshold and BF10 1.075e + 10 for pain tolerance (which means that there is decisive evidence in favor of the alternative model). When sex was also added to the null model and the alternative model contained cardiac IAc, BF10 = 0.567 for pain threshold and BF10 = 0.912 for pain tolerance. In other words, the analyses were inconclusive with respect to the superiority of the null and the alternative model. The regression model predicting pain threshold that included sex × heartbeat perception score interaction term was almost five times more likely (BF10 = 4.695, indicating substantial evidence in favor of the alternative model) as the null model (including positive and negative affect, sex, and heartbeat perception score). In the case of pain tolerance, however, the alternative model was not more likely to occur (BF10 = 1.092). Thus, the Bayesian regression analyses supported the results of the frequentist analyses regarding the interaction term.

Discussion

This study investigated the associations between sex and cardiac interoceptive accuracy with measures related to pain perception, i.e., pain threshold and pain tolerance, in 159 young individuals. After controlling for negative and positive affective state, the interaction between sex and cardiac IAc was a significant predictor of pain threshold, but not for pain tolerance.

The current affective state shows no consistent associations with pain sensitivity, indicating that the natural levels of positive and negative affect do not influence the sensitivity to experimentally induced pain in young individuals. The correlation between positive affect and pain tolerance, however, partly supports the view mentioned in the introduction, namely, that pleasant affective states are associated with lower pain sensitivity (Meagher et al., 2001; Carter et al., 2002; Villemure et al., 2003; Rainville et al., 2005). On the other hand, negative affect did not relate to any of the pain-related measures. In our setting, we focused only on the natural level of affect in a sample of healthy young adults, where negative affect was not particularly high and with a relatively low standard deviation as compared to positive affect.

Furthermore, it must be noted that the assessment of the natural level of affect with the PANAS might be considered as a measurement of personality traits; namely, that negative affect reflects the personality trait neuroticism and that positive affect reflects extraversion (Watson et al., 1992; for a review on state, personality traits, and personality change, see, e.g., Roberts, 2009). Thus, the relation of positive affect and pain sensitivity might reflect the well-documented extraversion pain sensitivity link (e.g., Lynn and Eysenck, 1961; Haslam, 1967).

The investigation of the relationship between pain, stress, and negative affect has a long tradition in pain research, with a strong focus on chronic pain conditions and negative affective states, such as depression and anxiety (Zautra et al., 2005). Studies applying various types of emotion evoking stimuli in healthy subjects (Meagher et al., 2001; Logan et al., 2003) were also not informative regarding the connection between pain perception and the natural changes of affect in the normal population. Our results show that, in a normal sample of young adults, the natural level of affect does not predict pain sensitivity.

Although the level of pain tolerance and threshold showed no association with cardiac IAc (hypotheses 1 and 2), we found interactions between sensitivity to pain and sex, i.e., males with higher level of interoceptive accuracy tend to have a higher pain threshold (hypothesis 5). These finding may be interpreted in the way that higher cardiac IAc (directly or indirectly) helps male participants to down-regulate their response to pain stimuli and thereby achieve higher pain threshold levels, whereas men with low cardiac IAc might rely more on response caution and report lower thresholds. The possible causes of this interaction will be discussed in more detail below.

Cardioceptive accuracy was not significantly associated with pain threshold. In fact, Bayesian correlation analysis indicated the superiority of the null hypothesis (BF < 0.1). Concerning cardiac IAc and tolerance (hypotheses 2), a weak association was revealed in the frequentist correlation analysis, which was not supported by the Bayesian analysis. The association also became non-significant after controlling for sex and affective state in the regression analysis. This lack of association between the two modalities (i.e., pain sensitivity and heartbeat perception scores) that are both considered interoceptive by some authors based on their common neurological pathways (Craig, 2015) is in accordance with the results of previous studies reporting the independence of pain perception and heartbeat perception (Werner et al., 2009; Ferentzi et al., 2017, 2018) and suggesting that interoceptive ability, as it is assessed by one indicator only, is not necessarily generalizable to other internal sensory channels (Ferentzi et al., 2017, 2018). There are some additional factors, however, to take into account. Recent findings confirmed that pain sensitivity might have a modality-specific characteristic, describing distinct pain pathways of visceral and somatic pain (Hockley et al., 2017). Additionally, experimentally induced pain might have different characteristics depending on the method applied to evoke it (e.g. Goodin et al., 2012). Accordingly, the possibility that some types of internal sensory channels are linked not only at the neurophysiological level (Craig, 2015) but also at the level of the individual performance on the sensory measures cannot be excluded. For future studies, more careful consideration is needed to explore which types of sensory tests might be more likely to be linked together.

Both pain threshold and tolerance were predicted by sex. Male participants showed considerably higher pain threshold and tolerance level than females (hypotheses 3 and 4). The theoretically expected interaction between sex and heartbeat perception was also supported by the data for pain threshold (hypothesis 5). In summary, the major determinants of pain sensitivity in healthy individuals appear to be sex and to a lesser extent the interaction between cardiac IAc and sex.

According to our findings, the associations between pain sensitivity, cardiac IAc, and sex might be more complicated than it was supposed in previous studies. Whereas women show no association between indicators of pain sensitivity and cardiac IAc, cardiac IAc and pain threshold are inversely related in men (i.e., those with a higher level of cardiac IAc show a higher pain threshold). These resultscould be explained by the idea of Pennebaker (Pennebaker and Roberts, 1992). He explained the better performance of males at interoceptive tasks in laboratory circumstances by their better abilities to detect internal signals. Women, on the other hand, take into consideration more external (environmental) cues when estimating their current inner state. As both ways are reliable under everyday conditions, the sex difference is not necessarily apparent outside the laboratory where external cues are not controlled for Pennebaker and Roberts (1992). In future studies, it would be worthwhile to investigate the mechanisms of interoceptive abilities in everyday situations.

Males usually show a higher pain threshold and tolerance level (Rhudy and Williams, 2005; Fillingim et al., 2009) as well as higher cardiac IAc (Katkin, 1985; Montoya et al., 1993; Grabauskaitė et al., 2017) (although, in the latter case, the findings are also controversial; Pennebaker, 1982; Mussgay et al., 1999; Pollatos and Schandry, 2004). Our results provide a more complete picture of the connection between the investigated variables. The association between higher pain threshold levels and higher cardiac IAc among men indicates that a sex-dependent extra mechanism might be involved. As pain can be characterized both by affective and by sensory components, it is possible that these can be regulated more effectively with higher inner focus, i.e., higher cardiac IAc among men. According to a brain imaging study, expectancies influence pain sensation (Atlas et al., 2010). A possible explanation of our result is that men with better cardiac IAc have higher expectancies concerning the pain stimulus, which makes them more prepared to get the pain stimulation that consequently becomes more manageable. A similar suggestion has been made by previous studies investigating interoception and emotion regulation (Füstös et al., 2013).

The current study has several limitations. First of all, the validity of the mental heartbeat tracking task by Schandry (1981) has beenquestioned by numerous authors (Ring et al., 2015; Desmedt et al., 2018; Murphy et al., 2018; Ring and Brener, 2018), and there is an ongoing debate about its validity (Zamariola et al., 2018; Ainley et al., 2020; Corneille et al., 2020; Zimprich et al., 2020). According to a recent study, the instructions allowing estimation could lead to different results than a stricter version of the instructions (Desmedt et al., 2020). However, in the original protocol of Schandry (1981), estimation was also allowed. The rationale of this is that heartbeats are weak sensations, and as such (similarly to ambiguous external sensations), estimation can reflect indeed a true but ambiguous sensation. Nevertheless, in the light of the recent findings and the debate mentioned above, more investigation is needed on the exact influence of the type instruction given.

Additionally, as pain has been stimulated electrically, it might not be generalizable to pain sensitivity induced differently, as it has been mentioned above. For example, a comparison of the experimental pain stimulation induced by four types of laser showed differences between the stimulation types (Svensson et al., 1991). The characteristics of the investigated sample (e.g., healthy young individuals) also limit the generalizability of the findings. Finally, the conclusion introduced above is valid only if the assumed medium effect size holds.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of Ulm University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MG, SAM-L, CM, and OP contributed to the conception and design of the study. MG contributed to the assessment of data, while EF and FK processed the data and performed the statistical analyses. EF wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MG, FK, and OP wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors read and commented on the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

EF was supported by a doctorate research grant funded by the German Academic Exchange Service (DAAD).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ainley, V., Tsakiris, M., Pollatos, O., Schulz, A., and Herbert, B. M. (2020). Comment on “Zamariola et al. (2018), Interoceptive accuracy scores are problematic: evidence from simple bivariate correlations”—the empirical data base, the conceptual reasoning and the analysis behind this statement are misconceived and do not support the authors’ conclusions. Biol. Psychol. 152:107870. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arntz, A., and de Jong, P. (1993). Anxiety, attention and pain. J. Psychosom. Res. 37, 423–431.

Google Scholar

Arntz, A., Dreessen, L., and Merckelbach, H. (1991). Attention, not anxiety, influences pain. Behav. Res. Ther. 29, 41–50.

Google Scholar

Atlas, L. Y., Bolger, N., Lindquist, M. A., and Wager, T. D. (2010). Brain mediators of predictive cue effects on perceived pain. J. Neurosci. 30, 12964–12977. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0057-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James-Lange theory of emotions: a critical examination and an alternative theory. Am. J. Psychol. 39, 106–124. doi: 10.2307/1415404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, L. E., McNeil, D. W., Vowles, K. E., Sorrell, J. T., Turk, C. L., Ries, B. J., et al. (2002). Effects of emotion on pain reports, tolerance and physiology. Pain Res. Manag. 7, 21–30.

Google Scholar

Corneille, O., Desmedt, O., Zamariola, G., Luminet, O., and Maurage, P. (2020). A heartfelt response to Zimprich et al. (2020), and Ainley et al. (2020)’s commentaries: acknowledging issues with the HCT would benefit interoception research. Biol. Psychol. 152:107869. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2003). A new view of pain as a homeostatic emotion. Trends Neurosci. 26, 303–307.

Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2010). The sentient self. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 563–577. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0248-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2015). How Do You Feel? An Interoceptive Moment with Your Neurobiological Self. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Damasio, A. (2010). Self Comes to Mind: Constructing the Conscious Brain. New York, NY: Pantheon.

Google Scholar

de Tommaso, M., Calabrese, R., Vecchio, E., De Vito Francesco, V., Lancioni, G., and Livrea, P. (2009). Effects of affective pictures on pain sensitivity and cortical responses induced by laser stimuli in healthy subjects and migraine patients. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 74, 139–148. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desmedt, O., Corneille, O., Luminet, O., Murphy, J., Bird, G., and Maurage, P. (2020). Contribution of time estimation and knowledge to heartbeat counting task performance under original and adapted instructions. Biol. Psychol. 154:107904. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desmedt, O., Luminet, O., and Corneille, O. (2018). The heartbeat counting task largely involves non-interoceptive processes: evidence from both the original and an adapted counting task. Biol. Psychol. 138, 185–188. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Lernia, D., Serino, S., and Riva, G. (2016). Pain in the body. altered interoception in chronic pain conditions: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 71, 328–341. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dienes, Z. (2011). Bayesian versus orthodox statistics: which side are you on? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, 274–290. doi: 10.1177/1745691611406920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). GPower 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191.

Google Scholar

Ferentzi, E., Bogdány, T., Szabolcs, Z., Csala, B., Horváth, Á, and Köteles, F. (2018). Multichannel investigation of interoception: sensitivity is not a generalizable feature. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:223. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferentzi, E., Köteles, F., Csala, B., Drew, R., Tihanyi, B. T., Pulay-Kottlár, G., et al. (2017). What makes sense in our body? Personality and sensory correlates of body awareness and somatosensory amplification. Pers. Individ. Dif. 104, 75–81. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.07.034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fillingim, R. B., King, C. D., Ribeiro-Dasilva, M. C., Rahim-Williams, B., and Riley, J. L. (2009). Sex, gender, and pain: a review of recent clinical and experimental findings. J. Pain 10, 447–485. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Füstös, J., Gramann, K., Herbert, B. M., and Pollatos, O. (2013). On the embodiment of emotion regulation: interoceptive awareness facilitates reappraisal. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 911–917. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garfinkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzuki, K., and Critchley, H. D. (2015). Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 104, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodin, B. R., Quinn, N. B., King, C. D., Page, G. G., Haythornthwaite, J. A., Edwards, R. R., et al. (2012). Salivary cortisol and soluble tumor necrosis factor-α receptor II responses to multiple experimental modalities of acute pain. Psychophysiology 49, 118–127. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01280.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grabauskaitė, A., Baranauskas, M., and Griškova-Bulanova, I. (2017). Interoception and gender: what aspects should we pay attention to? Conscious. Cogn. 48, 129–137. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gracely, R. H. (1999). Pain measurement. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 43, 897–908. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-6576.1999.430907.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harver, A., Katkin, E. S., and Bloch, E. (1993). Signal-detection outcomes on heartbeat and respiratory resistance detection tasks in male and female subjects. Psychophysiology 30, 223–230.

Google Scholar

Haslam, D. R. (1967). Individual differences in pain threshold and level of arousal. Br. J. Psychol. 58, 139–142.

Google Scholar

Herbert, B. M., Pollatos, O., Flor, H., Enck, P., and Schandry, R. (2010). Cardiac awareness and autonomic cardiac reactivity during emotional picture viewing and mental stress. Psychophysiology 47, 342–354. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00931.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbert, B. M., Pollatos, O., and Schandry, R. (2007). Interoceptive sensitivity and emotion processing: an EEG study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 65, 214–227. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hockley, J. R. F., González-Cano, R., McMurray, S., Tejada-Giraldez, M. A., McGuire, C., Torres, A., et al. (2017). Visceral and somatic pain modalities reveal NaV1.7-independent visceral nociceptive pathways. J. Physiol. 595, 2661–2679. doi: 10.1113/JP272837

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind 9, 188–205.

Google Scholar

Jarosz, A., and Wiley, J. (2014). What are the odds? a practical guide to computing and reporting Bayes factors. J. Probl. Solving 7:2. doi: 10.7771/1932-6246.1167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

JASP Team (2019). JASP(Version 0.9.0.1) [Computer Software] (Version 0.9.0.1). Available online at: https://jasp-stats.org/ (accessed March 15, 2019).

Google Scholar

Jeffreys, S. H. (1998). The Theory of Probability, 3rd Edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Jones, G. E., and Hollandsworth, J. G. (1981). Heart rate discrimination before and after exercise-induced augmented cardiac activity. Psychophysiology 18, 252–257.

Google Scholar

Jones, G. E., Jones, K. R., Rouse, C. H., Scott, D. M., and Caldwell, J. A. (1987). The effect of body position on the perception of cardiac sensations: an experiment and theoretical implications. Psychophysiology 24, 300–311. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1987.tb00300.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katkin, E. S. (1985). Blood, sweat, and tears: individual differences in autonomic self-perception. Psychophysiology 22, 125–137. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1985.tb01573.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katkin, E. S., Blascovich, J., and Goldband, S. (1981). Empirical assessment of visceral self-perception: individual and sex differences in the acquisition of heartbeat discrimination. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1095–1101. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.40.6.1095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalsa, S. S., Rudrauf, D., Sandesara, C., Olshansky, B., and Tranel, D. (2009). Bolus isoproterenol infusions provide a reliable method for assessing interoceptive awareness. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 72, 34–45. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koch, A., and Pollatos, O. (2014). Cardiac sensitivity in children: sex differences and its relationship to parameters of emotional processing. Psychophysiology 51, 932–941. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, H. L., Gedney, J. J., Sheffield, D., Xiang, Y., and Starrenburg, E. (2003). Stress influences the level of negative affectivity after forehead cold pressor pain. J. Pain 4, 520–529. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2003.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludwick-Rosenthal, R., and Neufeld, R. W. (1985). Heart beat interoception: a study of individual differences. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 3, 57–65.

Google Scholar

Lynn, R., and Eysenck, H. J. (1961). Tolerance for pain, extraversion, and neuroticism. Percept. Mot. Skills 12, 161–162. doi: 10.2466/pms.1961.12.2.161

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meagher, M. W., Arnau, R. C., and Rhudy, J. L. (2001). Pain and emotion: effects of affective picture modulation. Psychosom. Med. 63, 79–90. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200101000-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merskey, H., and Bogduk, N. (eds) (1994). Classification of Chronic Pain, 2nd Edn. Available online at: http://www.iasp-pain.org/PublicationsNews/Content.aspx?ItemNumber=1673 (accessed March 15, 2019).

Google Scholar

Montgomery, W. A., Jones, G. E., and Hollandsworth, J. G. (1984). The effects of physical fitness and exercise on cardiac awareness. Biol. Psychol. 18, 11–22. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(84)90022-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montoya, P., Schandry, R., and Müller, A. (1993). Heartbeat evoked potentials (HEP): topography and influence of cardiac awareness and focus of attention. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 88, 163–172.

Google Scholar

Moseley, G. L. (2007). Reconceptualising pain according to modern pain science. Phys. Ther. Rev. 12, 169–178. doi: 10.1179/108331907X223010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, J., Brewer, R., Hobson, H., Catmur, C., and Bird, G. (2018). Is alexithymia characterised by impaired interoception? Further evidence, the importance of control variables, and the problems with the heartbeat counting task. Biol. Psychol. 136, 189–197. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mussgay, L., Klinkenberg, N., and Rüddel, H. (1999). Heart beat perception in patients with depressive, somatoform, and personality disorders. J. Psychophysiol. 13, 27–36. doi: 10.1027//0269-8803.13.1.27

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ostrom, C., Bair, E., Maixner, W., Dubner, R., Fillingim, R. B., Ohrbach, R., et al. (2017). Demographic predictors of pain sensitivity: results from the OPPERA study. J. Pain 18, 295–307.

Google Scholar

Pennebaker, J. W. (1982). The Psychology of Physical Symptoms. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Pennebaker, J. W., and Roberts, T.-A. (1992). Toward a his and hers theory of emotion: gender differences in visceral perception. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 11, 199–212. doi: 10.1521/jscp.1992.11.3.199

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Füstös, J., and Critchley, H. D. (2012). On the generalised embodiment of pain: how interoceptive sensitivity modulates cutaneous pain perception. Pain 153, 1680–1686. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.04.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Herbert, B. M., Matthias, E., and Schandry, R. (2007a). Heart rate response after emotional picture presentation is modulated by interoceptive awareness. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 63, 117–124. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., and Schandry, R. (2004). Accuracy of heartbeat perception is reflected in the amplitude of the heartbeat-evoked brain potential. Psychophysiology 41, 476–482. doi: 10.111/1469-8986.2004.00170.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Traut-Mattausch, E., Schroeder, H., and Schandry, R. (2007b). Interoceptive awareness mediates the relationship between anxiety and the intensity of unpleasant feelings. J. Anxiety Disord. 21, 931–943. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rainville, P., Bao, Q. V. H., and Chrétien, P. (2005). Pain-related emotions modulate experimental pain perception and autonomic responses. Pain 118, 306–318. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.08.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhudy, J. L., and Williams, A. E. (2005). Gender differences in pain: do emotions play a role? Gend. Med. 2, 208–226.

Google Scholar

Ring, C., and Brener, J. (2018). Heartbeat counting is unrelated to heartbeat detection: a comparison of methods to quantify interoception. Psychophysiology 55:e13084. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ring, C., Brener, J., Knapp, K., and Mailloux, J. (2015). Effects of heartbeat feedback on beliefs about heart rate and heartbeat counting: a cautionary tale about interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 104, 193–198. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, B. W. (2009). Back to the future: personality and assessment and personality development. J. Res. Pers. 43, 137–145. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2008.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rouse, C. H., Jones, G. E., and Jones, K. R. (1988). The effect of body composition and gender on cardiac awareness. Psychophysiology 25, 400–407.

Google Scholar

Schandry, R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology 18, 483–488. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb02486.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schandry, R., Bestler, M., and Montoya, P. (1993). On the relation between cardiodynamics and heartbeat perception. Psychophysiology 30, 467–474. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb02070.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svensson, P., Bjerring, P., Arendt-Nielsen, L., Nielsen, J. C., and Kaaber, S. (1991). Comparison of four laser types for experimental pain stimulation on oral mucosa and hairy skin. Lasers Surg. Med. 11, 313–324. doi: 10.1002/lsm.1900110402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villemure, C. (2002). Cognitive modulation of pain: how do attention and emotion influence pain processing? Pain 95, 195–199.

Google Scholar

Villemure, C., Slotnick, B. M., and Bushnell, M. C. (2003). Effects of odors on pain perception: deciphering the roles of emotion and attention. Pain 106, 101–108.

Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., McIntyre, C. W., and Hamaker, S. (1992). Affect, personality, and social activity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 1011–1025. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.1011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, S., Sack, M., Henningsen, P., and Pollatos, O. (2014). On the interaction of self-regulation, interoception and pain perception. Psychopathology 47, 377–382. doi: 10.1159/000365107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Werner, N. S., Duschek, S., Mattern, M., and Schandry, R. (2009). The relationship between pain perception and interoception. J. Psychophysiol. 23, 35–42. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803.23.1.35

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitehead, W. E., Drescher, V. M., Heiman, P., and Blackwell, B. (1977). Relation of heart rate control to heartbeat perception. Biofeedback Self Regul. 2, 371–392. doi: 10.1007/BF00998623

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zamariola, G., Maurage, P., Luminet, O., and Corneille, O. (2018). Interoceptive accuracy scores from the heartbeat counting task are problematic: evidence from simple bivariate correlations. Biol. Psychol. 137, 12–17. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zautra, A. J., Johnson, L. M., and Davis, M. C. (2005). Positive affect as a source of resilience for women in chronic pain. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 73, 212–220. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.2.212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimprich, D., Nusser, L., and Pollatos, O. (2020). Are interoceptive accuracy scores from the heartbeat counting task problematic? A comment on Zamariola et al. (2018). Biol. Psychol. 152:107868. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: interoception, cardiac interoceptive accuracy, pain perception, affect, sex differences

Citation: Ferentzi E, Geiger M, Mai-Lippold SA, Köteles F, Montag C and Pollatos O (2021) Interaction Between Sex and Cardiac Interoceptive Accuracy in Measures of Induced Pain. Front. Psychol. 11:577961. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.577961

Received: 30 June 2020; Accepted: 31 December 2020;
Published: 09 February 2021.

Edited by:

Anna Maria Aloisi, University of Siena, Italy

Reviewed by:

Jun Yin, Ningbo University, China
Vivien Ainley, Royal Holloway, University of London, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2021 Ferentzi, Geiger, Mai-Lippold, Köteles, Montag and Pollatos. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Eszter Ferentzi, ZmVyZW50emkuZXN6dGVyQHBway5lbHRlLmh1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.