Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 September 2020
Sec. Psychology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic The Role of Declarative and Procedural Memory in Language View all 7 articles

Explicit but Not Implicit Memory Predicts Ultimate Attainment in the Native Language

  • 1Chair of Language and Cognition, Department of English and American Studies, Friedrich Alexander University Erlangen-Nuremberg, Erlangen, Germany
  • 2Department of English Language and Linguistics, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom

The present paper examines the relationship between explicit and implicit memory and ultimate attainment in the native language. Two groups of native speakers of English with different levels of academic attainment (i.e., high vs. low) took part in three language tasks which assessed grammar, vocabulary and collocational knowledge, as well as phonological short-term memory (assessed using a forward digit-span task), explicit associative memory (assessed using a paired-associates task) and implicit memory (assessed using a deterministic serial reaction time task). Results revealed strong relationships between phonological short-term memory and explicit associative memory on the one hand and the three language tasks on the other hand, and no relation between linguistic performance and implicit memory. Taken together, these results cast doubts on the common assumption that L1 grammar learning depends almost entirely on implicit memory and align with the claims of usage-based models of language acquisition that grammatical and lexical knowledge depend on the same cognitive mechanisms.

Introduction

Learning a language involves storing vast amounts of information. This means that memory plays an essential role in language learning, knowledge and use (Ullman, 2016). Traditionally, memory has been divided into two well-studied systems when considered in relation to language learning: declarative/explicit memory and procedural/implicit memory1. The declarative/explicit memory system, involving the hippocampus and medial temporal lobe structures, is thought to be mainly in charge of the conscious learning, storage and recollection of factual information (Henke, 2010; Squire and Wixted, 2011; Ullman, 2016). Learning through this system is thus effortful and controlled but evidence of learning can be found relatively fast (e.g., Carey and Bartlett, 1978). On the other hand, the procedural/implicit memory system, which is located in the basal ganglia, is assumed to underlie the implicit (non-conscious) learning and automatization of perceptual-motor as well as cognitive skills (Ullman, 2004; Ashby et al., 2010). Learning using the procedural/implicit memory system is usually unconscious as well as effortless and mostly automatic, but time and extensive repetition are required for learning to take place (e.g., Nicolson et al., 2010). In the present study, we assess the relationship between performance in tasks tapping into these two memory systems and ultimate attainment in native speakers’ linguistic knowledge in three different areas of language: grammar, vocabulary and collocations. Additionally, following from previous studies examining individual differences (e.g., Dąbrowska, 2018, 2019; Dąbrowska et al., under review), a second research question that we investigate is whether the strong relationship between lexical and grammatical knowledge which has been observed in numerous language acquisition studies survives into adulthood.

Language and Memory in First Language Acquisition

The link between language and memory has been a recurrent research topic in recent years (Kidd and Kirjavainen, 2011; Kidd, 2012; Lum and Kidd, 2012; Lum et al., 2013; Morgan-Short et al., 2014; Hamrick, 2015; Hamrick et al., 2018). With regard to grammar, it has been almost universally assumed that first language (L1) grammatical development depends on implicit memory, and that the resulting knowledge is also almost entirely implicit (e.g., Ellis, 1996; Ullman, 2001; DeKeyser et al., 2010), in the sense that native speakers are rarely able to verbalize the grammatical rules of their language. Work on L1 acquisition has provided experimental evidence of a relationship between implicit memory and various aspects of grammatical knowledge; however, this relationship has proved to be somewhat elusive. Thus, Conti-Ramsden et al. (2015) reported a significant correlation between children’s scores in a syntactic comprehension test and their performance on a Serial Reaction Time task (SRT), which is the most commonly used measure of implicit memory for sequences (e.g., Nissen and Bullemer, 1987; Cleeremans and McClelland, 1991; Jiménez et al., 1996; Kaufman et al., 2010). Likewise, Kidd (2012) found a significant correlation between SRT and syntactic priming in a picture description task (see also Kidd and Arciuli, 2016 and Lum et al., 2012). However, several other studies have failed to find evidence of such a relationship (Gabriel et al., 2011, 2012, 2013, 2015; Lum and Kidd, 2012). A recent meta-analysis by Hamrick et al. (2018), which pooled findings from 56 correlations and 665 participants, does report a significant relationship; however, the average effect size is very small (r = 0.27).

Because of the aforementioned assumption that grammar learning is mostly implicit, the potential relationship between grammar and explicit memory remains less well understood. For example, Hamrick et al. (2018) list only 3 child language studies examining this relationship, all of which show weak and non-significant correlations between grammar measures and explicit associative memory tested through verbal paired-associates recall (Kidd, 2012; Lum and Kidd, 2012; Conti-Ramsden et al., 2015; but see West et al., 2018). In spite of this, in principle, there are theoretically and empirically motivated reasons to believe that explicit memory may also play a role in L1 grammatical development. In the first place, usage-based models of language acquisition (Tomasello, 2000; Goldberg, 2006; Behrens, 2009; Bybee, 2010) assume that all linguistic knowledge is represented in the same form. As a strong relationship between explicit memory and L1 lexical knowledge has been repeatedly found (Kidd and Kirjavainen, 2011; Kidd, 2012; Lum and Kidd, 2012), if lexical and grammatical knowledge were represented in the same format, it would then follow that the explicit memory system could also contribute to the acquisition of grammatical knowledge. Secondly, this idea is partially supported by a series of studies showing significant correlations between intelligence, executive function and language aptitude, predictors mostly related to explicit learning, on the one hand, and L1 grammatical abilities, on the other (Skehan, 1989; Brooks and Kempe, 2018; West et al., 2018; Dąbrowska, 2019).

With regard to vocabulary, the common assumption is that lexical learning is supported by the explicit memory system and that vocabulary knowledge is mostly explicit (Lum et al., 2013; Hamrick et al., 2018). A very obvious argument for this is that, for example, upon hearing the word cat, most people will think of a miaoing quadruped and will be able to verbalize that knowledge. Older children and adults often learn the meanings of new words from their definitions, a scenario in which there is no doubt that learning and recollection are explicit. Explicit memory is also likely to be at the core of word learning through fast mapping since, in such a setting, learning of form-meaning mappings can be observed after as little as one single exposure to a given word (Carey and Bartlett, 1978; Golinkoff et al., 1992; Wilkinson and Mazzitelli, 2003). In fact, there is ample evidence that lexical knowledge relates to explicit memory, as moderately strong correlations have been reported between receptive vocabulary and verbal paired-associates recall ability (Kidd and Kirjavainen, 2011; Kidd, 2012; Lum and Kidd, 2012; Lum et al., 2012).

Nevertheless, implicit memory may also be relevant for vocabulary learning. Studies on cross-situational learning have repeatedly shown that form-meaning mappings can be learned by unconsciously tracking co-occurrence patterns for objects and their labels (Smith and Yu, 2008; Smith et al., 2011; Yurovsky and Frank, 2015). As a matter of fact, cross-situational learning has been suggested by some as the mechanism through which children acquire their L1 vocabulary (Pereira et al., 2013; Vlach and Johnson, 2013). Furthermore, learning new words may also involve the implicit tallying of collocational patterns for specific words (Dąbrowska, 2009). In spite of this, although several studies have probed the link between lexical knowledge and implicit memory, none has found a significant relationship between the two (Gabriel et al., 2011, 2012, 2013, 2015; Kidd and Kirjavainen, 2011; Kidd, 2012; Lum et al., 2012).

Finally, little is known about the mechanisms underlying the learning and storage of collocations. By collocations, we mean combinations of words that conform to the rules of grammar and have compositional meanings, but occur together more frequently than it would be expected from the frequencies of the words themselves (e.g., boost production or heavy traffic; Dąbrowska, 2014). A reasonable assumption put forward by some L2 researchers (e.g., Forsberg Lundell and Sandgren, 2013), is that the learning of collocations relies on the implicit memory system because it results from the implicit tallying of co-occurrences of words in speech and texts. In line with this idea is the fact that native speakers are generally not aware of collocational restrictions unless these are violated. Partial evidence in support for this hypothesis for L1 learners has been provided by Yi (2018), who found that processing speed for multi-word sequences in the L1 is predicted by one’s implicit memory as measured by an SRT task.

Another memory system which is relevant to language learning is phonological short-term memory (PSTM), or the phonological loop. Speech is fleeting; and the phonological loop is a buffer in which incoming information is maintained before it can be permanently encoded in long-term memory (Baddeley et al., 1998). Phonological short-term memory is an explicit system in the sense that the learner is aware of its content and often intentionally attempts to maintain it in memory (e.g., by subvocal rehearsal); and PSTM measures are known to correlate with IQ.

It has been proposed that the phonological loop is basically a language learning device (cf. Baddeley et al., 1998). This hypothesis is consistent with the fact that impairments of phonological memory in children are associated with language delay (Gathercole and Baddeley, 1990b; Gray, 2003; Gathercole, 2006; Estes et al., 2007) and that adults with acquired phonological memory deficits are unable to learn the phonological forms of new words (Baddeley et al., 1988; Trojano and Grossi, 1995). Furthermore, a number of studies have demonstrated the existence of robust correlations between phonological memory measures such as non-word repetition and vocabulary size and/or speed of vocabulary learning in young children (Gathercole and Baddeley, 1989, 1990a; Adams and Gathercole, 1996, 2000; Gathercole et al., 1997) and L2 learners at lower levels of proficiency (Cheung, 1996; Atkins and Baddeley, 1998). There is some evidence that such a relationship may also be present in older children and adolescents and advanced L2 learners; however, the evidence for this is less consistent, with many studies failing to find such a relationship (see Hummel, 2009 and Cheetham, 2014 for reviews). This is most likely due to the fact that speakers with larger vocabularies can use long-term memory representations to support phonological encoding while processing novel words. Some studies have also found that performance on phonological memory tasks correlates with learner’s knowledge of syntax; however, the evidence for this relationship is less consistent than the evidence linking PSTM to vocabulary learning (cf. Hummel, 2009), and the relationship may be mediated by vocabulary (Service and Kohonen, 1995).

Phonological short-term memory can be measured using digit, letter or word span tasks, which involve participants repeating series of digits, letters or words of increasing length. An alternative method involves using non-word repetition or recognition: participants are asked to either repeat sequences of nonsense syllables or to decide whether two sequences of nonsense syllables are the same or different (Gathercole et al., 1999). Gathercole et al. (1997) argue that non-word repetition is a purer measure of PSTM than digit span because it requires the participant to attend to subtle phonological distinctions and develop finer-grained representations of the stimuli, while performance on digit span is more affected by prior linguistic knowledge. In their data, non-word repetition was more strongly associated with vocabulary size than digit span, and the relationship was more specific. Non-word repetition tasks also discriminate better between typically developing children and children with language impairment (Gray, 2003; Gathercole, 2006). On the other hand, digit span tasks require the participant to encode longer stretches of text, and this characteristic may make it a better predictor of the ability to learn multi-word units such as collocations and grammatical patterns. Furthermore, to the extent that later vocabulary learning relies more on extracting information from the linguistic as opposed to the extra-linguistic context, the digit span may be a more relevant predictor of later vocabulary learning. For this reason, in this study we use a digit span task as a measure of phonological memory.

The Link Between Grammar, Vocabulary and Collocations

The second goal of the present paper is to provide a clear characterization of the relationship between the ultimate attainment of grammar, vocabulary and collocational knowledge. This is a theoretically important issue because modular and usage-based models of language acquisition make diverging predictions about the relationship between grammar and vocabulary. Modular models (Chomsky, 1965; Pinker, 1994, 1999; Ullman et al., 1997; DeKeyser et al., 2010) assume that grammar and vocabulary are learned through fundamentally different mechanisms and rely on the two main memory systems differently. Thus, according to these models, there is no a priori reason to predict a strong relationship between grammatical and lexical knowledge except perhaps in early stages of acquisition, where a critical mass of lexical items may be required to trigger grammatical learning and, conversely, knowledge about the syntactic contexts in which words occur may bootstrap lexical acquisition. By contrast, usage-based models (Tomasello, 2000; Langacker, 2009; Bybee, 2010) and related frameworks such as emergentism (e.g., MacWhinney, 2015) and connectionism (e.g., Rumelhart and McClelland, 1986; Elman et al., 1996), assume that all linguistic knowledge is acquired using the same mental mechanisms and represented in the same format. Therefore, all of these approaches predict a relatively strong relationship between grammatical and lexical knowledge.

There is a large amount of evidence supporting the existence of a robust relationship between grammatical and lexical development in first language acquisition (Bates et al., 1988; Caselli et al., 1999; Marchman et al., 2004). For example, Bates et al. (1988) found that children’s use of grammatical morphemes at 20 months of age was strongly correlated with their total vocabulary (r = 0.87). In addition, these authors found that vocabulary size at 1;8 years of age predicted mean length of utterance (MLU) at 2;4 years even better than MLU at 1;8 years. Nonetheless, these results in early L1 acquisition have more than one possible explanation. While it can indeed be the case that vocabulary and grammar development are strongly related across the board, an alternative explanation is that the relationship is especially strong in early L1 acquisition because vocabulary paces grammatical development: that is, grammatical development is constrained by vocabulary learning because grammatical patterns can only be extracted from the input if the words are known in the first place (Marchman and Bates, 1994; Hoff et al., 2018). Note, however, that recent evidence suggests that the relationship between grammatical and lexical knowledge also holds for adult speakers (Dąbrowska, 2018; Dąbrowska et al., under review), although it may be weaker than for children (r = 0.40 in Dąbrowska, 2018).

Finally, there is little research examining whether (and if so, how) knowledge of collocations relates to other aspects of language. There is some evidence, however, that collocational knowledge is indeed related to both grammatical knowledge and vocabulary. Riches et al. (under review) report significant correlations between tasks probing knowledge of collocations and grammar (r = 0.51) and collocations and vocabulary (r = 0.50) in children learning their native language. Similarly, Dąbrowska (2018) observed a significant correlation between collocations and grammar (r = 0.36) and an even stronger one between collocations and vocabulary (r = 0.57) for adult speakers. A close relationship between collocations and grammar fits well with the idea put forward by Forsberg Lundell and Sandgren (2013) that collocational learning relies on the implicit tallying of co-occurrence statistics, which is also the mechanism said to underlie native grammar acquisition (e.g., Ellis, 1996; Ullman et al., 1997). The strong relationship between collocations and vocabulary outlined above, on the other hand, goes hand in hand with the proposal that vocabulary learning from mid-childhood onward depends on tracking co-occurrence patterns between specific words and their collocates (e.g., Dąbrowska, 2009).

The Present Study

In the present study, we investigate the relationship between ultimate attainment in grammar, vocabulary and collocations on the one hand, and implicit and explicit memory on the other. Native speakers of English were tested in three language tasks, each concerned with one specific language area, and three memory tasks assessing implicit memory for sequences, explicit memory for cross-modal associations, and phonological short-term memory span. Measures obtained from the memory tasks were subsequently used as predictors of accuracy in the language tasks.

The vast majority of psychological and psycholinguistic research is conducted with university students and graduates, which raises the concern that the results may not be generalizable to other populations (Henrich et al., 2010; Jones, 2010; Rad et al., 2018). To avoid this possible limitation, we recruited participants from two different educational backgrounds and explicitly compared their performance. This is especially relevant for the questions examined in the present study because academic attainment has been shown to modulate performance on a variety of linguistic tasks (Street and Dąbrowska, 2010; Street, 2017; Dąbrowska, 2018). In spite of this, it still remains unclear whether the effects of academic attainment on linguistic knowledge are attributable to differences in education itself or other factors that correlate with education, such as print exposure. In order to shed some light on this issue, we also collected information about participants’ reading habits.

Based on previous results, and in agreement with usage-based models of language acquisition (Tomasello, 2000; Langacker, 2009; Bybee, 2010), we predict that all three memory measures will be associated with knowledge of grammar, vocabulary and collocations, although not necessarily to the same degree. This is because we assume that all linguistic knowledge is represented in the same format (i.e., form-meaning pairings, or constructions) and acquired by means of the same cognitive mechanisms. For the same reason, we additionally expect performances on the three language tasks to be strongly correlated.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Sixty native speakers of English took part in this study in exchange for a small payment. All participants gave their informed consent to participate and the study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Forty participants (29 females; mean age = 21.63, SD = 0.81) were university students recruited at several campuses around the United Kingdom. This group will be henceforth referred to as High Academic Attainment (HAA). The remaining 20 participants (2 females; mean age = 19.65, SD = 2.39) were vocational students recruited at United Kingdom colleges offering vocational training. This group will be referred to as Low Academic Attainment (LAA). All participants filled in a background questionnaire in which they provided basic demographic information as well as information about their reading habits.

Materials

This study consisted of three cognitive tasks and three language tasks. Tasks are described one by one below in the order in which they were presented to participants.

Forward Digit Span Task

Digit span is the standard test of phonological short-term memory used in psychological studies and assessment batteries (e.g., Richardson, 2007; Wechsler, 2008). In our version of the task, sequences of numbers were presented on the screen, one number at a time, and participants were subsequently asked to recall the numbers in their order of appearance. Each number stayed on the screen for 1000 ms and the interstimulus interval was 300 ms. The initial sequences were three digits long (e.g., 397) and length increased gradually until reaching nine digits (e.g., 214569821). All numbers from 0 to 9 were possible and so were repetitions of the same numbers within each sequence. There were three sequences of each length (i.e., from three to nine) for a total of 21 trials. Participants responded using the numeric keys on the computer keyboard.

Serial Reaction Time Task

The serial reaction time (SRT) task is commonly used to examine implicit memory for sequences (e.g., Nissen and Bullemer, 1987; Cleeremans and McClelland, 1991; Jiménez et al., 1996; Kaufman et al., 2010). Our task was adapted from the High-Level Language Aptitude Battery (Hi-LAB; Linck et al., 2013). Participants were seated in front of a computer screen which showed four underscores (_). On each trial, an asterisk () appeared right above one of the underscores, and the participants’ task was to press the corresponding button of the response box (labeled from 1 to 4) as quickly and accurately as possible. 500 ms after the button press, the asterisk appeared in a different position. The task was divided into 6 blocks of 96 trials each. In the first and last blocks, the positions in which the asterisk appeared were determined in a pseudorandom order, while in blocks 2, 3, 4, and 5, the asterisk appeared in a repeating sequence of length 12 (i.e., 1–2–1–4–3–2–4–1–3–4–2–3).

Some researchers (e.g., Kaufman et al., 2010; Granena, 2013; Suzuki and DeKeyser, 2015) have argued that probabilistic SRT tasks, in which the sequence of stimuli is generated by a probabilistic rule, provide a better measure of implicit memory than the traditional deterministic task used here because the deterministic variant may be influenced by explicit learning during the task. However, we decided to use the traditional version of the task because it has been found to be more reliable than the probabilistic version and reliability was an important concern due to our individual differences approach. For example, Granena (2018) and Granena and Yilmaz (2018) both report a split-halves reliability of 0.79 for deterministic SRT tasks, while split-halves reliabilities for the probabilistic SRT tasks in Kaufman et al. (2010); Granena (2013) and Suzuki and DeKeyser (2015) are 0.33, 0.44 and 0.42, respectively. The potential influence of differences between SRT variants on the results reported here will be further taken up in the discussion.

Paired-Associates Task

As a measure of explicit memory for cross-modal associations, we used LLAMA-B, a computerized paired-associates test from the LLAMA Language Aptitude Tests (LLAMA-B; Meara, 2005). LLAMA-B is a vocabulary learning task that is loosely based on the paired-associates test in the Modern Language Aptitude Test (Carroll and Sapon, 1959). Participants were presented with 20 target images, each of which corresponded to a word from a Central American language, and were given two minutes to study the word-picture associations. In the subsequent test phase, the twenty words were displayed one by one and, for each of them, participants had to identify the correct picture on the screen.

Grammar

Grammatical proficiency was examined by means of a computerized, written grammaticality judgment task (GJT). The items used in this task were a subset of those used in DeKeyser (2000) and Johnson and Newport (1989). They were 60 English sentences, of which 30 were grammatical and the other 30 contained a grammatical error. DeKeyser examined the performance of L2 learners on English sentences targeting 10 different aspects of grammar that are often problematic for non-native speakers (e.g., past tense, plural marking, third-person singular, etc.). When selecting the stimuli, we made sure that our task included a balanced number of grammatical and ungrammatical items for each of the different features. A list of the items used in our study is provided in the Supplementary Materials.

Participants were told that they would be visually presented with English sentences and that their task would be to assess whether the sentences were possible/accurate/proper English sentences or not. In each trial, a sentence was presented in the middle of the screen for 5000 ms and then disappeared. This was done in order to prompt relatively fast responses from participants2. Together with the sentence, two small boxes appeared at either side of the screen: a green one with “correct” written on it on the left and a red one with “incorrect” on the right. Participants were instructed to press the leftmost key of the response box (with a green key top) if they considered the sentence to be correct, and the rightmost key (with a red key top) if they considered it not to be correct. Note that the keys to be pressed matched the boxes visually presented in color and spatial location.

Collocations

Knowledge of English collocations was measured using a computerized version of the Words that Go Together Test (Dąbrowska, 2014). This test contains 40 multiple-choice items, each consisting of five short phrases. The participants’ task was to choose, in each case, the phrase that they considered to look “the most natural or familiar.” Target phrases were adjective-noun and verb-noun collocations (e.g., blatant lie, raise prices; see Dąbrowska, 2014 for further details), while the four distractor phrases for each trial were semantically similar adjective-noun or verb-noun combinations with lower mutual information scores (e.g., clear lie, grow prices). Participants were presented with the five phrases preceded by the numbers 1 to 5 on the computer screen and were asked to press the button on the response box corresponding to their response.

Vocabulary

A computerized implementation of the revised and re-standardized vocabulary component of the Shipley Institute of Living Scale (Shipley-2; Shipley et al., 2009) was used as English vocabulary test. This is a multiple-choice receptive-vocabulary test consisting of 40 English words of differing lexical frequencies. In each trial, one English word was presented on the screen in uppercase letters (e.g., MASSIVE) and four possible responses were provided in lowercase preceded by the numbers 1 to 4 (e.g., 1. bright, 2. large, 3. speedy, 4. low, “large” being the correct answer). Participants had to press the button of the response box (labeled from 1 to 4) corresponding to the word whose meaning was most similar to that of the uppercase word.

Procedure

Participants were tested individually or in groups of two in a quiet room at the university or college where recruitment took place. The experimental session took approximately 45 min. The testing station consisted of a Lenovo Thinkpad L580 laptop with a 15.6-inch screen, a seven-button Cedrus RB-740 response box and a set of noise-reducing headphones. The paired-associates task was administered using the software that comes with the LLAMA Test. All remaining tasks were created and run using Psychopy3 (v. 3.0.2; Peirce et al., 2019).

Data Analysis

Individual values for each task were extracted before conducting any statistical analyses. For the digit span task (henceforth Digit Span), paired-associates task (henceforth LLAMA-B) and the three language tasks, individual scores were calculated as the proportion of correct responses provided by each participant. For the serial reaction time task (henceforth SRT), two scores were calculated. The first was computed using the traditional method, i.e., by subtracting the mean RT for correct trials in the final sequential block (5) from the final random block (6). Therefore, higher scores were indicative of stronger effects of implicit sequence recollection. This measure will be henceforth referred to as SRT (traditional). The second score was based on the slope of the log-transformed RT function over the 4 sequential blocks (i.e., blocks 2, 3, 4, and 5) for each participant. This measures how much RT decreased as a function of exposure to the repeating sequence. SRT slopes were calculated following the procedure outlined in Divjak and Milin (2020). On this measure, lower (i.e., more negative) scores correspond to a steeper decrease in RTs and hence evidence a stronger impact of exposure to the sequence. Finally, we calculated a measure of print exposure, the Reading Index, on the basis of participants’ responses to two questions in the background questionnaire. The Reading Index was the mean of responses to the questions “How much do you read in a typical week?” and “How much did you read last week?”. Responses were given on a scale going from 1 to 6, where 1 corresponded to “less than one hour,” 2 to “1–5 h,” 3 to “6–10 h,” 4 to “11–15 h” and 6 to “more than 20 h”.

Results

Data from two participants, one from each group, for the Digit Span task were missing due to computer malfunction, as well as data from one LAA participant for the Vocabulary task and one for SRT. In addition, results for one LAA participant in the Grammar task and another LAA participant in the Collocations task were excluded because their responses were abnormally quick. They showed more than 5 consecutive trials with RTs shorter than 1000 ms and average RTs over the whole task shorter than 1500 ms and therefore much shorter than the mean RT for these tasks (mean RT for Grammar = 2717 ms; Collocations = 5314 ms). This strongly suggested that they had not fully engaged with the task. Table 1 shows the means and standard deviations for all tasks for the two groups as well as for all participants pooled together. Comparisons via Welch two sample t-tests, in order to account for the samples’ unequal variances (Welch, 1947), indicated that participants in the HAA group outperformed participants in the LAA group in all experimental tasks except for the SRT task, where there is only a marginally significant difference for slopes and a non-significant difference for the traditional SRT measure. In addition, Reading Indices were also higher for the HAA than the LAA group.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Means and standard deviations (in parentheses) for all measures by group and with the two groups pooled together.

So as to get a first impression of the relationships between language and memory tasks, as well as relationships between tasks within each set, we computed simple correlations between all the measures. These are presented in Table 2. Three facts are immediately observable on inspecting the table. First of all, there are very strong relationships (rs ranging from 0.70 to 0.82) between the three language tasks. Secondly, performance on all three language tasks is robustly correlated with phonological short-term memory as measured by the Digit Span task (rs ranging from 0.56 to 0.64) and associative memory as measured by LLAMA-B (rs from 0.61 to 0.65). Weaker but nevertheless significant correlations are also observed between one of the measures of implicit memory, namely the slope of the decrease in RTs, and accuracy in the vocabulary and collocations tasks (rs of −0.34 and −0.28, respectively). Finally, all three language tasks also show small- to medium-sized correlations with the Reading Index (rs from 0.32 to 0.48).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlation matrix for the three language tasks (Grammar, Vocabulary, and Collocations), the three memory tasks [Digit Span, LLAMA-B, SRT (traditional + slope)] and Reading Index for all participants together.

After this first correlational analysis, data were submitted to further analyses in order to gain more detailed insights on the aforementioned relationships. Missing values in the dataset consisting of individual scores per measure were imputed by means of productive mean matching using the mice package (van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn, 2011) in R (version 3. 5. 2; R Core Team, 2017). Subsequently, a linear mixed-effects regression model was fit (lme4 package 1.1-20; Bates et al., 2015) with Accuracy (i.e., proportion correct responses for the three language tasks3) as dependent variable. Categorical independent variables included Language Task (Grammar/Vocabulary/Collocations) and Group (HAA/LAA).

Language Task was factor coded with Grammar as the reference level. Group was contrast coded such that LAA was −0.5 and HAA corresponded to 0.5 and was therefore treated as numeric in the analysis. Other numeric independent variables included Digit Span (phonological short-term memory), LLAMA-B (explicit associative memory), SRT Slope (implicit memory)4 and Reading Index. These four predictors were centered and scaled. Two-way interactions between Group and Task, as well as between Task and each of the four numeric independent variables, and Group and each of the four numeric predictors were also included. These were necessary in order to assess whether the effects of Digit Span, LLAMA-B, SRT slope and Reading Index differed as a function of the language task on the one hand and as a function of the academic attainment of participants on the other. Finally, we also included three-way interactions between Task, Group and each of the four numeric independent variables because these allowed us to elucidate more complex relationships between predictors (e.g., whether differences in the influence of LLAMA-B between the three language tasks were additionally modulated by group). Random intercepts by participant were included to account for the fact that there were 3 values per participant in the dataset (i.e., Grammar, Vocabulary, and Collocations accuracies). The significance of main effects and interactions was assessed using hierarchical partitioning of the variance via nested model comparisons. This means that the full model was fitted first, and then predictors were removed one by one, beginning with the highest-level interaction terms. If the absence of a particular predictor resulted in the model exhibiting a worse fit to the data, as determined through log-likelihood ratio tests, the predictor was considered to have a significant effect and was kept in the model. If it did not, the predictor was removed and the procedure started again for the next predictor.

Model comparisons revealed significant main effects of Language Task [χ2 (2) = 148.01, p < 0.001], Group [χ2 (1) = 25.06, p < 0.001], Digit Span [χ2 (1) = 12.43, p < 0.001] and LLAMA-B [χ2 (1) = 11.98, p < 0.001]. The coefficients and significance levels of all predictors in the best-fitting model are reported in Table 3. These results indicate, in the first place, that participants’ accuracy differed between the three language tasks and that participants in the LAA group were overall less accurate than participants in the HAA group. This is observable in Figure 1, which shows both group and individual accuracy values per task per group. Secondly, our results show that accuracy in the language tasks was modulated by scores in the Digit Span and LLAMA-B tasks: the higher the Digit Span and LLAMA-B scores, the higher the accuracy in the language tasks. In addition to this, the model also revealed significant two-way interactions between Language Task and Group [χ2 (2) = 9.64, p < 0.01] and Group and LLAMA-B [χ2 (1) = 6.99, p < 0.01]. The interaction between Language Task and Group indicates that differences in accuracy between the groups varied depending on the task examined, with the collocations task showing the largest group differences (see Figure 1). The interaction between Group and LLAMA-B further shows that the influence of LLAMA-B on linguistic proficiency differed for the two groups, being stronger for the LAA group than for the HAA group. This difference is apparent in Figure 2, which showcases the relationship between individual LLAMA-B scores and accuracy for each of the three language tasks for the two groups of participants. Crucially, the fact that there were no interactions between Language Task and Digit Span [χ2 (2) = 2.15, p = 0.34], Language Task and LLAMA-B [χ2 (2) = 0.37, p = 0.83] or three-way interactions involving Language Task, Group and one of the two explicit memory predictors (both p > 0.5) indicates that the effects of Digit Span and LLAMA-B do not differ significantly across linguistic domains. Additional least-squares regression analyses with data split by task and all significant variables in the main analysis as predictors are provided in the Supplementary Materials to further illustrate the consistency of results across tasks.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Coefficients and significance values for the final, best fitting linear mixed-effects regression model assessing the effects of Task (Grammar, Vocabulary, and Collocations), Group (HAA and LAA), Digit Span, LLAMA-B, SRT (slope), and Reading Index on accuracy in the language tasks.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Proportion of correct responses in the Grammar (left), Vocabulary (center), and Collocations (right) tasks by Group (LAA – in dark gray; HAA – in light gray). Dots showcase the distributions of individual values within each group and the black diamonds signal group means.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Scatterplots of individual values for LLAMA-B scores and individual scores for Grammar (left), Vocabulary (center), and Collocations (right) by Group (LAA – in dark gray; HAA – in light gray). Regression lines for each group are provided for illustration purposes.

Discussion

The present study investigated the relationship between explicit and implicit memory on the one hand and ultimate L1 language attainment in the areas of grammar, vocabulary and collocations on the other. We also examined the potential modulating role of academic attainment and print exposure in these relationships and the relationships between grammar, vocabulary and collocations scores. The following discussion is divided into sections addressing (i) the effects of academic attainment and reading, (ii) the effects of explicit memory, (iii) the lack of robust effects of implicit memory, and (iv) the relationship between the three language tasks.

Academic Attainment and Reading

Our results showed a strong effect of academic attainment on participants’ performance on both language and cognitive tasks. Participants in the HAA group outperformed those in the LAA group in all three language tasks. These group differences replicate the findings of several earlier studies which demonstrated a strong link between language and academic attainment (Dąbrowska, 1997, 2018; Chipere, 2003; Street and Dąbrowska, 2010; Street, 2017). Furthermore, effects of academic attainment were also observed for the digit span task assessing explicit, phonological short-term memory and LLAMA-B, which examined participants’ ability to explicitly establish cross-modal associations5. Differences between groups for the digit span task are not surprising, as this measure is often included in IQ test batteries, and academic achievement is known to correlate strongly with IQ (e.g., Wechsler, 1958; Kaufman and Lichtenberger, 2006; Deary et al., 2007; Hogan et al., 2010). Likewise, the differences observed for LLAMA-B fit well with previous research showing that explicit memory for cross-modal associations correlates with reading achievement6 and other factors related to educational success (Vellutino et al., 1975; Poulsen and Elbro, 2018; Ehm et al., 2019). Thus, the differences between high and low academic attainment participants are not necessarily attributable to education as such, but could be due to other factors that are known to correlate with academic attainment7.

The results for the SRT task are less conclusive. When implicit memory was assessed using the traditional measure (i.e., by subtracting the RTs for the last sequential block from those for the final random block), the HAA group showed a modest benefit of exposure to the sequence (22.4 ms) that was similar to those observed in some previous studies (20–40 ms; Granena, 2018; Granena and Yilmaz, 2018; Yilmaz and Granena, 2019). The LAA participants, however, showed virtually no changes when their performance was assessed as a group (3 ms). However, as discussed earlier, the group difference is not statistically significant. A similar pattern can be seen when performance is assessed using the slope measure, which, according to Divjak and Milin (2020), is more sensitive: participants in the HAA groups showed, as expected, negative slopes (i.e., their RTs got shorter as the task progressed), whereas the LAA group had an average slope that was very close to zero (and in fact positive). In this case, the between-group comparison was marginally significant.

Clearly, further research is necessary to determine whether this apparent group difference is real. The finding is surprising, since implicit memory for sequences has repeatedly been shown not to relate to factors which are known to be strongly tied to academic attainment, such as explicit learning abilities and intelligence (Reber et al., 1991; Maybery et al., 1995, Gebauer and Mackintosh, 2007). However, most studies which used SRT to measure implicit memory for sequences in adults tested HAA participants (e.g., Stefaniak et al., 2008; Romano et al., 2010; Hamrick, 2015; Faretta-Stutenberg and Morgan-Short, 2018). If individual differences in implicit memory are indeed related to academic attainment, these findings may then not generalize to other populations.

Regarding the potential influence of print exposure and reading practices on L1 ultimate attainment, the individual self-reported reading measure used in the present study showed significant correlations with all three language tasks but its effect failed to reach significance in the subsequent regression analyses. The lack of an effect in the latter is at odds with some earlier studies. For instance, Dąbrowska (2018) reports a small but significant contribution of print exposure to predicting scores in a task assessing grammatical knowledge, and much stronger effects for vocabulary and collocation scores. For all three tasks, the effects were observed over and above the effects of IQ and educational attainment. Likewise, Stanovich (1986); Cunningham and Stanovich (1998), and many others have found robust correlations between print exposure and vocabulary. The most likely explanation for this difference is that the self-reported measure used here is not as reliable as the objective measures used in these earlier studies, and hence more power would be required to detect the effect.

Explicit Memory

Our results show strong and significant correlations between the two explicit memory measures, namely phonological short-term memory (measured by the forward Digit Span task) and memory for cross-modal associations (measured by LLAMA-B), and scores on all three language tasks. Subsequent mixed-effects regression analyses provided additional insights into these results by showing that performance on the Digit Span task predicted performance on the language tasks for both groups of participants, while LLAMA-B scores robustly predicted accuracy for the LAA group only.

The effect of phonological short-term memory adds to previous research emphasizing the relevance of this type of memory for vocabulary acquisition in L1-learning children (Gathercole and Baddeley, 1990a; Adams and Gathercole, 1996, 2000; Verhagen and Leseman, 2016) and L2 learners at lower levels of proficiency (Cheung, 1996; Atkins and Baddeley, 1998). Similarly, this is also in line with the findings of previous research linking PSTM to grammar acquisition in early L1 acquisition (Blake et al., 1994; Adams and Gathercole, 1995). As mentioned in the introduction, earlier research has found that effects of PSTM were much more consistent for vocabulary than for grammar, and in younger learners than in older learners. To our knowledge, our study is the first to find robust effects of PSTM in adult native speakers. It is noteworthy that we found effects of a similar magnitude for all three areas of language we studied, including collocations, which has received little attention from this perspective. This is most likely due to the fact that the digit span task requires participants to maintain longer stretches of text, and thus the phonological encoding thereof, in short-term memory and is therefore more relevant for aspects of language which involve syntagmatic relations, namely phraseology and syntax. The relationship between digit span and vocabulary knowledge may be explained by the fact that later vocabulary development depends to a large extent on learning from the linguistic contexts in (primarily written) texts (Dąbrowska, 2009).

Regarding explicit memory for cross-modal associations, the fact that performance on LLAMA-B predicted vocabulary attainment, even if only for LAA participants, fits well with studies which found a relationship between explicit associative memory and L1 lexical development (Kidd and Kirjavainen, 2011; Kidd, 2012; Lum and Kidd, 2012; Lum et al., 2012). The significant relationship between associative memory and grammar found in the LAA group, however, contrasts with the findings of several studies which failed to find a relationship between the two (Kidd, 2012; Lum and Kidd, 2012; Conti-Ramsden et al., 2015; see, however, Kidd and Kirjavainen, 2011). Importantly, as it will be further discussed below, the fact that grammar and vocabulary show similar relationships with explicit memory can be seen as support for the usage-based view that lexical and grammatical knowledge are represented in the same format (Tomasello, 2000; Goldberg, 2006; Behrens, 2009; Bybee, 2010).

As we have seen, LLAMA-B predicted performance on the language tasks for participants with lower academic attainment but not for the HAA group. It should be stressed that the lack of an effect in the HAA group cannot be attributed to ceiling effects. As shown in Figure 2, HAA participants’ scores on LLAMA-B range from 0.2 to 1 and there is a visible spread of values between the two ends. It is true that there is less variation in performance on the language tasks, particularly on grammar. However, the fact that Digit Span shows comparable effects for both groups of participants suggests that there is sufficient variation in scores in the HAA group to show an effect, if there is one. An alternative explanation for the interaction between LLAMA-B and group is that the relationship between explicit associative memory and language is not strictly linear, but instead, associative memory only affects language attainment up to a certain level. Note that scores for more than half of LAA participants were below the lowest score for HAA. Therefore, it could be the case that better memory for cross-modal associations aids language learning only in individuals who achieve relatively low scores on this memory measure.

Another finding that is worth noting is that both of our explicit memory tasks correlated more strongly with the language tasks (Digit Span: r from 0.56 to 0.64; LLAMA-B: r from 0.61 to 0.65) than with each other (r = 0.44). In fact, comparisons of correlations from dependent samples via Fischer’s r-to-z transformations followed by an asymptotic z-test (Steiger, 1980; Lee and Preacher, 2013) show that, apart from the correlation between Digit Span and Vocabulary (z = 1.26, p = 0.10), all other correlations between the two explicit memory measures on the one hand and the language tasks on the other are significantly higher (z > 1.6, p < 0.05) than the correlation between Digit Span and LLAMA-B. This somewhat puzzling pattern of correlations may be due to the fact that phonological short-term memory and longer-term memory for cross-modal associations support language learning in rather different ways. Explicit memory for phonological sequences makes it possible for the learner to acquire syntagmatic associations. These are obviously relevant for grammar and collocations, and, to the extent that learning word meanings depends on tracking co-occurrence patterns in texts (cf. Dąbrowska, 2009), they could also support vocabulary learning. The ability to form cross-modal associations, on the other hand, helps with learning sound-meaning correspondences. These are obviously relevant for vocabulary learning, but, in a usage-based model, also for grammar and collocations, which according to this model are also represented as form-meaning pairings.

Implicit Memory

Our results did not show a robust relationship between implicit memory (assessed using an SRT task) and ultimate attainment in the native language. While weak but significant correlations between SRT slopes and vocabulary and collocations (but not grammar) were observed, the effect of this predictor failed to reach significance in a linear mixed-effects regression analysis. Our failure to find a significant relationship does not, of course, mean that implicit memory is not relevant to language acquisition. As pointed out in the introduction, the results of previous research are mixed: while a number of studies have found such a relationship, there are also many that did not. As briefly discussed in West et al. (2018), it is possible that this is due to the fact that SRT tasks are simply not very reliable.

However, there are also three additional possible explanations for the lack on an effect in the present study that do not necessarily stem from task unreliability. A first possibility is that our sequential or deterministic SRT task could in principle have been influenced by explicit memory to a certain extent, which could have distorted the results. However, we consider this rather unlikely in the light of the findings that (i) participants in the LAA group show very little learning when examined as a group, and (ii) explicit memory was found to significantly predict performance in the language tasks. Particularly, point (ii) suggests that, if explicit memory had indeed played a role in the SRT task, then it may have boosted the predictive power of SRT rather than reduced it, and yet SRT was not found to relate to performance in the language tasks. The second possibility is that the role of implicit memory changes over the course of development, being more relevant in the earlier developmental stages than in adulthood. Critically, this hypothesis is in line with the fact that most research addressing the link between implicit memory for sequences and language and showcasing a relationship between the two were run on children (Kidd, 2012; Lum et al., 2012; Conti-Ramsden et al., 2015; Kidd and Arciuli, 2016), while the existence of such a relationship for adult L1 speakers is less clear (but see Misyak et al., 2010). Finally, a third non-exclusive alternative is that the effect is simply hard to find because true individual differences in implicit memory are small, and hence would require more statistical power to be detected.

The Relationship Between Grammar, Vocabulary, and Collocations

Lastly, we found very strong correlations between participants’ scores in the grammar, vocabulary and collocations tasks. The tight connection between grammar and vocabulary in adult L1 speakers reported here goes hand in hand with the results of recent studies (Dąbrowska, 2018; Dąbrowska et al., under review) indicating that this relationship, well documented for L1-learning children (Bates et al.,1988; Caselli et al., 1999; Marchman et al., 2004), persists into adulthood. In addition, also in agreement with Dąbrowska (2018, 2019), we provide further evidence that collocational knowledge in adult L1 speakers relates to both grammatical knowledge and lexical knowledge. These results, taken together, are fully compatible with the essential premise of usage-based models of language acquisition that all linguistic knowledge is represented in the same format, as pairings between forms and their meaning, and therefore depend on the same learning mechanisms and rely on the same memory systems (Tomasello, 2000; Langacker, 2009; Bybee, 2010). In contrast, our results are more difficult to accommodate in modular models (e.g., Chomsky, 1965; Pinker, 1994, 1999; Ullman et al., 1997; DeKeyser et al., 2010), which claim that grammar and vocabulary constitute different cognitive modules and are acquired using fundamentally different learning mechanisms.

Conclusion

In sum, the present study shows that (explicit) phonological short-term memory and explicit memory for cross-modal associations, but not implicit memory for sequences, predict L1 linguistic knowledge in adults. This is the case for vocabulary, collocations, and most importantly, grammar. These results challenge the widely held assumption that L1 grammar learning depends almost entirely on the implicit memory system (Ellis, 1996; Ullman, 2001; DeKeyser et al., 2010). In addition, as argued above, our results are potentially problematic for modular theories of language acquisition because of the similarity of the relationships found between explicit memory predictors and language scores for grammatical, lexical and collocational knowledge, and the very strong relationships between scores in the three language tasks. Both of these findings align more closely with the view of language acquisition put forward by usage-based models.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets presented in this study are included in the article/Supplementary Material.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethik-Kommission der Medizinischen Fakultät der FAU. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

ML and ED conceptualized the study, ML implemented the experimental tasks and processed and analyzed the data. ML and ED drafted and revised the manuscript. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This project was funded by an Alexander von Humboldt Professorship (ID-1195918) awarded to ED.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.569586/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ The terms declarative/explicit and procedural/implicit are not, strictly speaking, synonymous: the distinction between declarative and procedural relates to the brain systems, while the distinction between explicit and implicit is concerned with awareness. In the present paper, we only use the terms declarative and procedural in the introductory section, where we discuss the neurological underpinnings of memory. In the rest of the paper, we use the terms explicit and implicit.
  2. ^ Scores for the grammar task were calculated for all trials as well as only for trials with response times shorter than 5000 ms. This was done in order to ensure that individual scores were not simply a reflection of reading speed (i.e., lower scores caused by not being able to read the sentence in the 5000 ms window). Individual scores were almost identical for the two datasets, mainly because RT were in their vast majority shorter than 5000 ms (mean RT = 2717 ms). Because of this, we decided to use the dataset containing all trials for statistical analyses.
  3. ^ This analysis was also conducted on accuracy scores corrected for guessing following the formula outlined in Dąbrowska (2018). As the main results of interest did not differ from those of the analysis with raw proportions, only the latter are reported for the sake of easier interpretation of effects and regression coefficients.
  4. ^ The traditional measure of learning in the SRT task (Linck et al., 2013) was not included in the regression analysis in order to minimize multicollinearity. This decision was motivated by the fact that SRT slope appeared to be a more sensitive predictor of linguistic ability in the correlational analyses than the traditional subtraction measure.
  5. ^ As women have been reported to outperform men in tasks involving declarative memory as well as in language tasks (see Ullman et al., 2007 for a review) we examined whether sex differences in performance on the language and explicit memory tasks were apparent in the present study for the HAA group, where there were enough participants of each sex to make comparisons. However, no substantial differences were found in the means for women and men for any of the three language tasks nor for the Digit Span and LLAMA-B task.
  6. ^ The language tasks in this study presented stimuli in written form. Hence, participants’ reading abilities may have had an influence on their performance. This is especially the case for the grammar task, where participants were prompted to make grammaticality judgments quickly and reading difficulties could have possibly led to errors for participants in the LAA group. However, recent research in our lab suggests that grammaticality judgments by LAA speakers are not strongly affected by the modality of presentation (written vs. oral). Becker and Dąbrowska (2020) administered a grammaticality judgment task to a group of LAA speakers very similar to our participants and found no effects of modality for this group: in other words, the participants were equally good at judging sentences that were presented in writing and auditorily presented sentences. For this reason, we consider that the role that differences in reading abilities may have played in our results is a limited one.
  7. ^ A reviewer pointed out that the group differences observed in the grammar task could be partly due to the fact that the LAA group was likely to include more participants who spoke non-standard varieties of English. Although we cannot completely rule out this possibility, dialectal differences are unlikely to have had a significant effect on our results. As indicated in the Methods section, our stimuli for the grammaticality judgment task were taken from those used by Johnson and Newport (1989) and DeKeyser (2000). These studies were concerned with comparing the performance of native and non-native speakers, and hence the stimuli were designed to be as dialect-neutral as possible. However, after consulting with a dialectologist, we were made aware that four of the sentences which we had classified as ungrammatical are acceptable in some dialects of English. These items, however, did not discriminate between the HAA and LAA participants better than other items in the test.

References

Adams, A.-M., and Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Phonological working memory and speech production in preschool children. J. Speech Hear. Res. 38, 403–414. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3802.403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adams, A.-M., and Gathercole, S. E. (1996). Phonological working memory and spoken language development in young children. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 49, 216–233. doi: 10.1080/027249896392874

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adams, A.-M., and Gathercole, S. E. (2000). Limitations in working memory: implications for language development. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 35, 95–116. doi: 10.1080/136828200247278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashby, F. G., Turner, B. O., and Horvitz, J. C. (2010). Cortical and basal ganglia contributions to habit learning and automaticity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 208–215. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkins, P. W. B., and Baddeley, A. (1998). Working memory and distributed vocabulary learning. Appl. Psycholinguist. 19, 537–552. doi: 10.1017/s0142716400010353

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., and Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychol. Rev. 105, 158–173. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.105.1.158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A., Papagno, C., and Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. J. Mem. Lang. 27, 586–595. doi: 10.1016/0749-596x(88)90028-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, E., Bretherton, I., and Snyder, L. (1988). From First Words to Grammar: Individual Differences and Dissociable Mechanisms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Becker, L., and Dąbrowska, E. (2020). Can experience with written language influence grammaticality intuitions? Paper Presentation at the UK Cognitive Linguistics Conference, Birmingham.

Google Scholar

Behrens, H. (2009). Usage-based and emergentist approaches to language acquisition. Linguistics 47, 383–411. doi: 10.1515/ling.2009.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blake, J., Austin, W., Cannon, M., Lisus, A., and Vaughan, A. (1994). The relationship between memory span and measures of imitative and spontaneous language complexity in preschool children. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 17, 91–107. doi: 10.1177/016502549401700106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, P. J., and Kempe, V. (2018). More is more in language learning: reconsidering the less-is-more hypothesis. Lang. Learn. 69, 13–41. doi: 10.1111/lang.12320

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bybee, J. (2010). Language, Usage and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Carey, S., and Bartlett, E. (1978). “Acquiring a single new word,” in Proceedings of the Stanford Child Language Conference Vol. 15, Stanford, CA, 17–29.

Google Scholar

Carroll, J. B., and Sapon, S. (1959). Modern Languages Aptitude Test- Form A. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Caselli, C., Casadio, P., and Bates, E. (1999). A comparison of the transition from first words to grammar in English and Italian. J. Child Lang. 26, 69–111. doi: 10.1017/s0305000998003687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheetham, D. (2014). The phonological loop and language acquisition. Engl. Lit. Lang. 50, 47–64. doi: 10.1075/lald.23.05fis

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, H. (1996). Nonword span as a unique predictor of second-language vocabulary language. Dev. Psychol. 32, 867–873. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.32.5.867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chipere, N. (2003). Understanding Complex Sentences: Native Speaker Variations in Syntactic Competence. Basingstoke: Palgrave.

Google Scholar

Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

Google Scholar

Cleeremans, A., and McClelland, J. L. (1991). Learning the structure of event sequences. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 120, 235–253. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.120.3.235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conti-Ramsden, G., Ullman, M. T., and Lum, J. A. G. (2015). The relation between receptive grammar and procedural, declarative, and working memory in specific language impairment. Front. Psychol. 6:1090. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunningham, A. E., and Stanovich, K. E. (1998). What reading does for the mind. Am. Educ. 22, 8–17.

Google Scholar

Dąbrowska, E. (1997). The LAD goes to school: a cautionary tale for nativists. Linguistics 35, 735–766. doi: 10.1515/ling.1997.35.4.735

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dąbrowska, E. (2009). “Words as constructions,” in New Directions in Cognitive Linguistics, eds V. Evans and S. Pourcel (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), 201–223.

Google Scholar

Dąbrowska, E. (2014). Words that go together: measuring individual differences in native speakers’ knowledge of collocations. Ment. Lex. 9, 401–418. doi: 10.1075/ml.9.3.02dab

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dąbrowska, E. (2018). Experience, aptitude and individual differences in native language ultimate attainment. Cognition 178, 222–235. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dąbrowska, E. (2019). Experience, aptitude, and individual differences in linguistic attainment: a comparison of native and nonnative speakers. Lang. Learn. 69, 72–100. doi: 10.1111/lang.12323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deary, I. J., Strand, S., Smith, P., and Fernandes, C. (2007). Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence 35, 13–21. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeKeyser, R. (2000). The robustness of critical period effects in second language acquisition. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 22, 499–533. doi: 10.1017/s0272263100004022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeKeyser, R., Alfi-Shabtay, I., and Ravid, D. (2010). Cross-linguistic evidence for the nature of age effects in second language acquisition. Appl. Psycholinguist. 31, 413–438. doi: 10.1017/s0142716410000056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Divjak, D., and Milin, P. (2020). Exploring and exploiting uncertainty: statistical learning ability affects how we learn to process language along multiple dimensions of experience. Cogn. Sci. 44:e12835. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehm, J.-H., Lonnemann, J., Brandenburg, J., Huschka, S. S., Hasselhorn, M., and Lervåg, A. (2019). Exploring factors underlying children’s acquisition and retrieval of sound–symbol association skills. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 177, 86–99. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellis, N. C. (1996). Sequencing in SLA. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 18, 91–126. doi: 10.1017/s0272263100014698

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elman, J. L., Bates, E., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., and Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Estes, K. G., Evans, J. L., and Else-Quest, N. M. (2007). Differences in the nonword repetition performance of children with and without specific language impairment: a meta-analysis. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 50, 177–195. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/015)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faretta-Stutenberg, M., and Morgan-Short, K. (2018). The interplay of individual differences and context of learning in behavioral and neurocognitive second language development. Second Lang. Res. 34, 67–101. doi: 10.1177/0267658316684903

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forsberg Lundell, F., and Sandgren, M. (2013). “High-level proficiency in late L2 acquisition: relationships between collocational production, language aptitude and personality,” in Sensitive Periods, Language Aptitude, and Ultimate L2 Attainment, eds G. Granena and M. H. Long (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), 231–255. doi: 10.1075/lllt.35.09for

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabriel, A., Maillart, C., Guillaume, M., Stefaniak, N., and Meulemans, T. (2011). Exploration of serial structure procedural learning in children with language impairment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 336–343. doi: 10.1017/s1355617710001724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabriel, A., Maillart, C., Stefaniak, N., Lejeune, C., Desmottes, L., and Meulemans, T. (2013). Procedural learning in specific language impairment: effects of sequence complexity. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 19, 264–271. doi: 10.1017/s1355617712001270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabriel, A., Meulemans, T., Parisse, C., and Maillart, C. (2015). Procedural learning across modalities in French-speaking children with specific language impairment. Appl. Psycholinguist. 36, 747–769. doi: 10.1017/s0142716413000490

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabriel, A., Stefaniak, N., Maillart, C., Schmitz, X., and Meulemans, T. (2012). Procedural visual learning in children with specific language impairment. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 21, 329–341. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0044)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: the nature of the relationship. Appl. Psycholinguist. 27, 513–543. doi: 10.1017/s0142716406060383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gathercole, S. E., and Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: a longitudinal study. J. Mem. Lang. 28, 200–213. doi: 10.1016/0749-596x(89)90044-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gathercole, S. E., and Baddeley, A. D. (1990a). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: a study of young children learning new names. Br. J. Psychol. 81, 439–454. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1990.tb02371.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gathercole, S. E., and Baddeley, A. D. (1990b). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? J. Mem. Lang. 29, 336–360. doi: 10.1016/0749-596x(90)90004-j

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gathercole, S. E., Hitch, G. J., and Martin, A. J. (1997). Phonological short-term memory and new word learning in children. Dev. Psychol. 33, 966–979. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.6.966

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gathercole, S. E., Service, E., Hitch, G. J., Adams, A. M., and Martin, A. J. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and vocabulary development: further evidence on the nature of the relationship. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 13, 65–77. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0720(199902)13:1<65::aid-acp548>3.0.co;2-o

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gebauer, G. F., and Mackintosh, N. J. (2007). Psychometric intelligence dissociates implicit and explicit learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 33, 34–54. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.1.34

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Bailey, L. M., and Wenger, N. R. (1992). Young children and adults use lexical principles to learn new nouns. Dev. Psychol. 28, 99–108. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.99

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Granena, G. (2013). Individual differences in sequence learning ability and second language acquisition in early childhood and adulthood. Lang. Learn. 63, 665–703. doi: 10.1111/lang.12018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Granena, G. (2018). Cognitive aptitudes and L2 speaking proficiency. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 41, 313–336. doi: 10.1017/s0272263118000256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Granena, G., and Yilmaz, Y. (2018). Corrective feedback and the role of implicit sequence-learning ability in L2 online performance. Lang. Learn. 69, 127–156. doi: 10.1111/lang.12319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, S. (2003). Diagnostic accuracy and test–retest reliability of nonword repetition and digit span tasks administered to preschool children with specific language impairment. J. Commun. Disord. 36, 129–151. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9924(03)00003-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamrick, P. (2015). Declarative and procedural memory abilities as individual differences in incidental language learning. Learn. Individ. Differ. 44, 9–15. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamrick, P., Lum, J. A. G., and Ullman, M. T. (2018). Child first language and adult second language are both tied to general-purpose learning systems. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, 1487–1492. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1713975115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henke, K. (2010). A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 523–532. doi: 10.1038/nrn2850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature 466:29. doi: 10.1038/466029a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoff, E., Quinn, J. M., and Giguere, D. (2018). What explains the correlation between growth in vocabulary and grammar? New evidence from latent change score analyses of simultaneous bilingual development. Dev. Sci. 21:e12536. doi: 10.1111/desc.12536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogan, M., Parker, J., Wiener, J., Watters, C., Wood, L., and Oke, A. (2010). Academic success in adolescence: relationships among verbal IQ, social support and emotional intelligence. Aust. J. Psychol. 62, 30–41. doi: 10.1080/00049530903312881

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hummel, K. M. (2009). Aptitude, phonological memory, and second language proficiency in nonnovice adult learners. Appl. Psycholinguist. 30, 225–249. doi: 10.1017/s0142716409090109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiménez, L., Méndez, C., and Cleeremans, A. (1996). Comparing direct and indirect measures of sequence learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 22, 948–969. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.22.4.948

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, J. S., and Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: the influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cogn. Psychol. 21, 60–99. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, D. (2010). A WEIRD view of human nature skews psychologists’ studies. Science 328:1627. doi: 10.1126/science.328.5986.1627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, A. S., and Lichtenberger, E. O. (2006). Assessing Adolescent and Adult Intelligence, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Jiménez, L., Brown, J., and Mackintosh, N. (2010). Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition 116, 321–340. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kidd, E. (2012). Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax. Dev. Psychol. 48, 171–184. doi: 10.1037/a0025405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kidd, E., and Arciuli, J. (2016). Individual differences in statistical learning predict children’s comprehension of syntax. Child Dev. 87, 184–193. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kidd, E., and Kirjavainen, M. (2011). Investigating the contribution of procedural and declarative memory to the acquisition of past tense morphology: evidence from Finnish. Lang. Cogn. Process. 26, 794–829. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2010.493735

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., and Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 82, 1–26. doi: 10.18637/jss.v082.i13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langacker, R. W. (2009). Cognitive (construction) grammar. Cogn. Linguist. 20, 167–176. doi: 10.1515/cogl.2009.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, I. A., and Preacher, K. J. (2013). Calculation for the Test of the Difference BETWEEN Two Dependent Correlations with One Variable in Common. Available online at: http://quantpsy.org (accessed March 19, 2020).

Google Scholar

Linck, J. A., Hughes, M. M., Campbell, S. G., Silbert, N. H., Tare, M., Jackson, S. R., et al. (2013). Hi-LAB: a new measure of aptitude for high-level language proficiency. Lang. Learn. 63, 530–566. doi: 10.1111/lang.12011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lum, J. A. G., Conti-Ramsden, G., Page, D., and Ullman, M. T. (2012). Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Cortex 48, 1138–1154. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lum, J. A. G., and Kidd, E. (2012). An examination of the associations among multiple memory systems, past tense, and vocabulary in typically developing 5-year-old children. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 55, 989–1006. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0137)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lum, J. A. G., Ullman, M. T., and Conti-Ramsden, G. (2013). Procedural learning is impaired in dyslexia: evidence from a meta-analysis of serial reaction time studies. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 3460–3476. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.07.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacWhinney, B. (2015). “Emergentism,” in Handbook of Cognitive Linguisitcs, eds E. Dąbrowska and D. Divjak (Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton), 689–706.

Google Scholar

Marchman, V. A., and Bates, E. (1994). Continuity in lexical and morphological development: a test of the critical mass hypothesis. J. Child Lang. 21, 339–366. doi: 10.1017/s0305000900009302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marchman, V. A., Martinez-Sussmann, C., and Dale, P. S. (2004). The language-specific nature of grammatical development: evidence from bilingual language learners. Dev. Sci. 7, 212–224. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00340.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maybery, M., Taylor, M., and O’Brien-Malone, A. (1995). Implicit learning: sensitive to age but not IQ. Aust. J. Psychol. 47, 8–17. doi: 10.1080/00049539508258763

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meara, P. (2005). LLAMA Language Aptitude Tests: The Manual. Swansea: Lognostics.

Google Scholar

Misyak, J. B., Christiansen, M. H., and Tomblin, J. B. (2010). On-line individual differences in statistical learning predict language processing. Front. Psychol. 1:31. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan-Short, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., Brill-Schuetz, K. A., Carpenter, H., and Wong, P. C. M. (2014). Declarative and procedural memory as individual differences in second language acquisition. Bilingualism 17, 56–72. doi: 10.1017/s1366728912000715

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., Brookes, R. L., and Needle, J. (2010). Procedural learning and dyslexia. Dyslexia 16, 194–212. doi: 10.1002/dys.408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nissen, M. J., and Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures. Cogn. Psychol. 19, 1–32. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peirce, J. W., Gray, J. R., Simpson, S., MacAskill, M. R., Höchenberger, R., Sogo, H., et al. (2019). Psychopy2: experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Method 51, 195–203. doi: 10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pereira, A. F., Smith, L. B., and Yu, C. (2013). A bottom-up view of toddler word learning. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 21, 178–185. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0466-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York, NY: Morrow.

Google Scholar

Pinker, S. (1999). Words and Rules: The Ingredients of Language. New York, NY: HarperCollins.

Google Scholar

Poulsen, M., and Elbro, C. (2018). The short- and long-term predictions of reading accuracy and speed from paired-associate learning. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 174, 77–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2017). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Rad, M. S., Martingano, A. J., and Ginges, J. (2018). Toward a psychology of Homo sapiens: making psychological science more representative of the human population. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, 11401–11405. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1721165115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reber, A. S., Walkenfeld, F. F., and Hernstadt, R. (1991). Implicit and explicit learning: individual differences and IQ. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 17, 888–896. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.17.5.888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, J. T. E. (2007). Measures of short-term memory: a historical review. Cortex 43, 635–650. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70493-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romano, J. C., Howard, J. H., and Howard, D. V. (2010). One-year retention of general and sequence-specific skills in a probabilistic, serial reaction time task. Memory 18, 427–441. doi: 10.1080/09658211003742680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rumelhart, D. E., and McClelland, J. L. (eds) (1986). Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition. Volume 2: Psychological and Biological Models. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Service, E., and Kohonen, V. (1995). Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition? Appl. Psycholinguist. 16, 155–172. doi: 10.1017/s0142716400007062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shipley, W. C., Gruber, C. P., Martin, T. A., and Klein, A. M. (2009). Shipley-2 Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Skehan, P. (1989). Individual Differences in Second Language Learning. London: Edward Arnold.

Google Scholar

Smith, K., Smith, A. D. M., and Blythe, R. A. (2011). Cross-situational learning: an experimental study of word-learning mechanisms. Cogn. Sci. 35, 480–498. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01158.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, L., and Yu, C. (2008). Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition 106, 1558–1568. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Squire, L. R., and Wixted, J. T. (2011). The cognitive neuroscience of human memory since H.M. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 259–288. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Read. Res. Q. 21, 360–407. doi: 10.1598/rrq.21.4.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stefaniak, N., Willems, S., Adam, S., and Meulemans, T. (2008). What is the impact of the explicit knowledge of sequence regularities on both deterministic and probabilistic serial reaction time task performance? Mem. Cogn. 36, 1283–1298. doi: 10.3758/mc.36.7.1283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiger, J. H. (1980). Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychol. Bull. 87, 245–251. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.87.2.245

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Street, J. A. (2017). This is the native speaker that the non-native speaker outperformed: individual, education-related differences in the processing and interpretation of object relative clauses by native and non-native speakers of English. Lang. Sci. 59, 192–203. doi: 10.1016/j.langsci.2016.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Street, J. A., and Dąbrowska, E. (2010). More individual differences in language attainment: How much do adult native speakers of English know about passives and quantifiers? Lingua 120, 2080–2094. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2010.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suzuki, Y., and DeKeyser, R. (2015). Comparing elicited imitation and word monitoring as measures of implicit knowledge. Lang. Learn. 65, 860–895. doi: 10.1111/lang.12138

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. (2000). Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition 74, 209–253. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00069-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trojano, L., and Grossi, D. (1995). Phonological and lexical coding in verbal short-term-memory and learning. Brain Lang. 51, 336–354. doi: 10.1006/brln.1995.1064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ullman, M. T. (2001). The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language: the declarative/procedural model. Bilingualism 4, 105–122. doi: 10.1017/s1366728901000220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model. Cognition 92, 231–270. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ullman, M. T. (2016). The declarative/procedural model. Neurobiol. Lang. 953–968. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-407794-2.00076-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ullman, M. T., Corkin, S., Coppola, M., Hickok, G., Growdon, J. H., Koroshetz, W. J., et al. (1997). A neural dissociation within language: evidence that the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory, and that grammatical rules are processed by the procedural system. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 266–276. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.2.266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ullman, M. T., Miranda, R. A., and Travers, M. L. (2007). Sex differences in the neurocognition of language. Sex Differ. Brain 291–310. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195311587.003.0015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Buuren, S., and Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. J. Stat. Softw. 45, 1–67. doi: 10.18637/jss.v045.i03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vellutino, F. R., Steger, J. A., Harding, C. J., and Phillips, F. (1975). Verbal vs non-verbal paired-associates learning in poor and normal readers. Neuropsychologia 13, 75–82. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(75)90050-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhagen, J., and Leseman, P. (2016). How do verbal short-term memory and working memory relate to the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar? A comparison between first and second language learners. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 141, 65–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vlach, H. A., and Johnson, S. P. (2013). Memory constraints on infants’ cross-situational statistical learning. Cognition 127, 375–382. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1958). Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence, 4th Edn. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkens.

Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV). San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson.

Google Scholar

Welch, B. L. (1947). The generalization of ‘student’s’ problem when several different population variances are involved. Biometrika 34, 28–35. doi: 10.2307/2332510

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, G., Vadillo, M. A., Shanks, D. R., and Hulme, C. (2018). The procedural learning deficit hypothesis of language learning disorders: we see some problems. Dev. Sci. 21:e12552. doi: 10.1111/desc.12552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilkinson, K. M., and Mazzitelli, K. (2003). The effect of ‘missing’ information on children’s retention of fast-mapped labels. J. Child Lang. 30, 47–73.

Google Scholar

Yi, W. (2018). Statistical sensitivity, cognitive aptitudes, and processing of collocations. Stud. Second Lang. Acquisit. 40, 831–856. doi: 10.1017/s0272263118000141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yilmaz, Y., and Granena, G. (2019). Cognitive individual differences as predictors of improvement and awareness under implicit and explicit feedback conditions. Mod. Lang. J. 103, 686–702. doi: 10.1111/modl.12587

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yurovsky, D., and Frank, M. C. (2015). An integrative account of constraints on cross-situational learning. Cognition 145, 53–62. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: implicit memory, explicit memory, ultimate attainment, individual differences, declarative/procedural model, usage-based models

Citation: Llompart M and Dąbrowska E (2020) Explicit but Not Implicit Memory Predicts Ultimate Attainment in the Native Language. Front. Psychol. 11:569586. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.569586

Received: 04 June 2020; Accepted: 04 September 2020;
Published: 25 September 2020.

Edited by:

Nicolas Stefaniak, Université de Reims Champagne-Ardenne, France

Reviewed by:

Alexandre Obert, Jean-François Champollion University Center for Teaching and Research, France
Gerard H. Poll, Miami University, United States

Copyright © 2020 Llompart and Dąbrowska. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Miquel Llompart, bGxvbXBhcnQuZ2FyY2lhQGZhdS5kZQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.