- 1Center for Evolutionary Cognitive Sciences, Graduate School of Art and Sciences, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
- 2Center for Advanced School Education and Evidence-based Research (CASEER), Graduate School of Education, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
- 3Medical Institute of Developmental Disabilities Research, Showa University, Tokyo, Japan
- 4Department of Language Sciences, Tokyo Metropolitan University, Tokyo, Japan
- 5Research Center for Language, Brain and Genetics, Tokyo Metropolitan University, Tokyo, Japan
- 6Graduate School of Letters, Arts and Sciences, Waseda University, Tokyo, Japan
- 7Department of Neuropsychiatry, Graduate School of Medicine, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
- 8Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Medical Science, Tokyo, Japan
- 9Department of Information Media Technology, School of Information and Telecommunication Engineering, Tokai University, Tokyo, Japan
- 10UTokyo Institute for Diversity & Adaptation of Human Mind (UTIDAHM), The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
- 11The International Research Center for Neurointelligence (IRCN), The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
- 12UTokyo Center for Integrative Science of Human Behaviour (CiSHuB), The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
Introduction: Previous studies on metacognitive ability were explored using self-report questionnaires that are difficult to adequately measure and evaluate when the capacity for self-reference is undeveloped. This study aimed to validate the Congruency-based Metacognition Scale (CMS) to measure metacognition and the feeling of confidence abilities and to investigate the development of metacognition during adolescence.
Methods: The CMS was administered to 633 child–parent pairs in Japan (child, mean age = 16.0 years, 46.0% female; parent, mean age = 48.3 years, 94.9% mother). The CMS metacognition score was assessed based on congruency scores between the self-report of the child from a third-person perspective (3PP) and the parent’s report from the first-person perspective (1PP). The CMS self-judgment accuracy score was assessed by the congruency scores between the children’s self-report from the 1PP and 3PP. For both measures, the more distant the 3PP on the self-report was from the 1PP on the parent’s report and child self-report means low ability. An exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was conducted to examine construct validity and then a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used. Criterion validity was examined by calculating Pearson’s correlation coefficients with scores on the Beck Cognitive Insight Scale (BCIS) and Autism Quotient (AQ). We used intraclass correlation and Cronbach’s alpha to examine the test–retest and internal consistency reliability.
Results: Based on the results of the EFA and CFA, we adopted one factor structure with five items. The CMS metacognition and CMS self-judgment accuracy showed evidence criterion validity, exhibiting significant correlations with the BCIS self-reflectiveness (r = 0.16) and self-certainty scores (r = 0.17), respectively. Regarding to the AQ, only the CMS metacognition score had significant correlations with the social skills (r = 0.22) and total scale score (r = 0.20). The test–retest reliability showed adequate (intraclass correlation coefficient 0.70–0.81 and the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient 0.63–0.59). Adolescents were found to have significantly lower metacognitive ability compared to young adults.
Conclusion: CMS could be a valid and reliable measure to examine metacognitive abilities for adolescents.
Introduction
Metacognition is commonly referred to as thinking about thinking (i.e., reflecting on one’s own thinking) (Flavell, 1979). The usage of metacognition has developed over 40 years since the first term was introduced (Flavell, 1979), for example, level of confidence in a cognitive action (monitoring), behavioral regulation (control), judgment of learning, executive skills, higher-order skills, and feeling of knowing and psychological status (Veenman et al., 2006; Fleming et al., 2010). Therefore, some reports defined metacognition as cognitive and mental processes to know and regulate their behaviors and psychological status, whereas others as the awareness and understandings of their status itself. In this study, we used metacognition as the awareness and understandings of their status (Flavell, 1979), which was defined as individual’s self-conscious supervision of the cognitive processes. To see the metacognition, we intend to use the combination of children’s third-person perspective (3PP) and parent’s first-person perspective (1PP) reports regarding children’s personal trait.
The assessment of metacognitive ability has been generally used with self-report questionnaires (Wolters et al., 2012; Lachat Shakeshaft et al., 2020). Self-report questionnaires are easy to administer and frequently used to assess metacognitive ability; however, these self-reports present several limitations, for example, it is difficult to evaluate for people with difficulties in self reference (e.g., autism spectrum disorders) and who are still developing (e.g., adolescence) (Sebastian et al., 2008). In psychology, parental reports are commonly used to assess temperament (De Los Reyes and Kazdin, 2005; Fox et al., 2005; Durbin et al., 2007; Hash et al., 2019), emotional difficulties (Theunissen et al., 2019), behavioral problems (Fujikawa et al., 2016, 2018), and depression (Park et al., 2019) in children, as they are often incapable of accurately reporting how they feel. Yet, important questions remain concerning whether the constructs assessed by the parental reports can be used to verify metacognitive ability in children.
Some researchers (Ruby and Decety, 2001; Ruby et al., 2009; Hashimoto et al., 2017; Brown et al., 2019) have combined the concept of self and the other thoughts with the 1PP and 3PP taking. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies on perspective taking have observed significant activity associated with brain regions related to social cognition, including the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC), and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC)/precuneus. Activities in the dmPFC and PCC/precuneus have been reported during 3PP judgment, particularly when contrasted with 1PP judgment (Ruby and Decety, 2001; Ruby et al., 2009). When comparing 3P self-perspective (3PP_Self) to 1P self-perspective (1PP_Self), significant activity was observed in the PCC/precuneus (Hashimoto et al., 2017). According to a previous study, the PCC/precuneus is specifically involved in distinguishing self-produced actions from those generated by others (Ruby and Decety, 2001). In another study (Brown et al., 2019), dmPFC and vmPFC deactivation was observed while participants/actors pretended to be Romeo (male participants) or Juliet (female participants) from Shakespeare’s famous drama. These findings suggest that assessing perspective taking would more appropriately reflect metacognitive ability compared to self/child and/or other/parental questionnaires. However, to date, no study has explored whether a questionnaire on perspective taking could assess metacognitive ability in developing individuals.
This study served to develop the Congruency-based Metacognition Scale (CMS) to measure metacognitive and self-judgment accuracy abilities. The CMS metacognition score was assessed based on congruency between the child’s self-report from a third-person perspective (3PP_Self; e.g., “Does your mother/father think you are kind?”) and the parental report from the first-person perspective (1PP_Other; e.g., “Do you think your son/daughter is kind?”). The CMS self-judgment accuracy score was assessed by the congruency between the children’s self-report from a first-person perspective (1PP_Self; e.g., “Do you think you are kind?”) and third-person perspective (3PP_Self). For both measures, the more distant the 3PP on the self-report was from the 1PP on the parent’s report and child self-report means low ability. The study aimed to examine the construct, criterion-related validity, and test–retest reliability of the CMS as well as investigate metacognitive development during adolescence. Regarding the development, this study hypothesized that, consistent with normative development, children/adolescents would present with lower metacognitive ability relative to late adolescents/young adults.
Materials and Methods
Participants and Procedures
A total of 633 child–parent pairs participated in this study using three surveys from the Tokyo prefecture between September 2013 and January 2020 (child, mean age = 16.0 years, SD = 3.1, age range = 10.8–29.3 years, 46.0% female; parent, mean age = 48.3 years, SD = 5.0, age range = 34.0–66.0 years, 94.9% mother; see Table 1). In the first survey, 152 pairs were recruited via an authorized job recruitment board or internet site at 20 colleges or universities between November 2013 and July 2014 (child, mean age = 19.8 years, SD = 1.2, age range = 18.0–25.9 years, 42.1% female; parent, mean age = 51.5 years, SD = 3.6, age range = 44.0–62.0 years, 94.7% mother). In the second survey, 74 pairs were recruited from recruitment boards placed in several high schools and universities, and a commercial internet advertisement between May 2017 and October 2018 (child, mean age = 19.6 years, SD = 2.5, age range = 15.5–29.3 years, 51.4% female; parent, mean age = 52.7 years, SD = 4.8, age range = 41.0–66.0 years, 91.9% mother). In the third survey, 407 pairs were assessed in the TEEN Cohort project between September 2013 and January 2020 (child, mean age = 13.9 years, SD = 1.2, age range = 10.8–17.3 years, 46.4% female; parent, mean age = 46.4 years, SD = 4.4, age range = 34.0–58.0 years, 95.6% mother) (Okada et al., 2018; Ando et al., 2019). Following the third survey, 36 child–parent pairs were assessed on two occasions for test–retest reliability by an interval mean of 58.7 days (mean age = 16.0 years, SD = 0.7, age range = 15.0–16.9 years, 34.1% female; parent, mean age = 48.7 years, SD = 4.3, age range = 40.1–56.3 years, 91.7% mother) between July 2019 and January 2020. All participants received the questionnaires by post and responded alone at home.
Ethics Statement
This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee at The University of Tokyo (No. 18-50) and the Department of Medicine at The University of Tokyo [No. 10069-(21)]. All participants, or rather their caregivers (if the participant’s age was 18 years or less), provided informed consent prior to their participation in this study in accord with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Development of the Congruency-Based Metacognition Scale
The CMS was developed based on previous studies that have examined metacognitive ability (Ruby and Decety, 2001, 2004; D’Argembeau et al., 2007; Ruby et al., 2009; Hashimoto et al., 2017). In an fMRI study (Hashimoto et al., 2017), participants were instructed to read metacognitive sentences and decide whether they agreed with the sentence by pressing a corresponding button. The task consisted a 2 × 2 factorial design: “self” or “other” and “first-person perspective (1PP)” or “third-person perspective (3PP).” The participants were shown a sentence (in Japanese) in the form of “A think(s) that B is/are C,” where A and B represent either “YOU” (self) or “YOUR MOTHER” (Other, or “YOUR FATHER” if the parent that responded was one’s father) and C represents an adjective describing a personality trait (e.g., “YOUR MOTHER thinks that YOU are kind”). Our fMRI task used 88 Japanese adjectives translated by a native Japanese speaker (R.H.) from 110 English adjectives describing personality traits (Bochner and Van Zyl, 1985); these were also selected based on 20 Japanese volunteers, who rated how much they liked every adjective using a seven-level Likert scale. A previous study confirmed the equal characteristics in the number of letters, moras, word frequency, and imaginability among the 88 adjectives (Hashimoto et al., 2017). Of these, 20 highly familiar and frequently employed words were used in this study, with a counterbalance of 10 positive (e.g., kind, affable, brave, polite, graceful, persistent, serious, artistic, organized, and cheerful) and 10 negative words (e.g., quick-tempered, snobby, scruffy, unkind, noisy, hardheaded, talkative, boring, snaky, and irritable); see Supplementary Appendix A–F. The same set of 20 trait adjectives was used in the factor analysis of our current study.
The CMS was adapted for two domains in children (first person self: 1PP_Self and third person self: 3PP_Self) and one domain for their parents (first person other: 1PP_Other; see Table 2). The 1PP_Self condition requires a child to evaluate adjectives describing their own subjective feelings (e.g., “Do you think you are kind?”), whereas the 1PP_Other condition requires his/her parents to evaluate adjectives describing the subjective feelings of their children (e.g., “Do you think your son/daughter is kind?”). In the 3PP_Self condition, the child is asked to estimate how his/her parent would assess his/herself with respect to the adjectives (e.g., “Does your mother/father think you are kind?”). For each statement, the participants responded using a four-point Likert-type scale (1 = disagree, 2 = somewhat disagree, 3 = somewhat agree, and 4 = agree).
Table 2. Scheme of the Congruency-based Metacognition Scale (CMS) metacognition and self-judgment accuracy.
We subsequently conducted a preliminary test to confirm whether 10-year-old children (n = 26) and their mothers (n = 26) were able to respond to the CMS questions without difficulty. We interviewed the participants after they completed the CMS and confirmed that all participants understood the questionnaire and responded appropriately.
The CMS metacognition variables was defined using the absolute differences between each adjective of the 3PP_Self and 1PP_Other scores. The CMS self-judgment accuracy score was also measured by CMS and defined as the absolute differences between each adjective of the 1PP_Self and 3PP_Self scores. After the construct validity discussed in the Statistical Analysis section, the sum of absolute differences between each adjective of the 3PP_Self and 1PP_Other scores and 1PP_Self and 3PP_Self scores was considered as CMS metacognition and CMS self-judgment accuracy, respectively (lower scores corresponding to greater capacity in both measurements).
Measurement for Criterion Validity
From the first survey, we acquired children’s scores on the Beck Cognitive Insight Scale (BCIS) (Beck et al., 2004; Uchida et al., 2009) and the 50-item Autism Quotient (AQ-50) (Wakabayashi et al., 2004) for criterion validity.
The Beck Cognitive Insight Scale
The BCIS consists of 15 items that assess self-reflectiveness and self-certainty. Originally, the BCIS was developed to evaluate reflectiveness and overconfidence in the interpretations of the experiences of patients with mental health problems (Beck et al., 2004; Uchida et al., 2009). Items that comprise the self-reflectiveness subscale measure objectivity, reflectiveness, and openness to feedback. The self-certainty subscale assesses decision-making and dogmatic certainty regarding beliefs and conclusions. Therefore, in general, the BCIS is used as a self-report measure of the ability to reflect on personal cognitive perceptions and beliefs and to re-evaluate subjective interpretations. Thus, the BCIS would be a suitable scale for testing the validity of the CMS metacognitive and self-judgment accuracy abilities. Using the Japanese version of the BCIS (BCIS-J) (Uchida et al., 2009), respondents rated their agreement on a four-point Likert-type scale from 0 (do not agree at all) to 3 (agree completely). The self-reflectiveness subscale was calculated as the sum of nine items (possible range 0–27, with higher scores corresponding to increased self-reflectiveness). The self-certainty subscale was calculated as the sum of the remaining six items (possible range 0–18, with higher scores corresponding to more overconfidence). It was hypothesized that higher levels of self-certainty would diminish an individual’s capacity for self-reflection, so a composite score was calculated by subtracting the self-certainty total from the self-reflectiveness total, and this was used as the principal indicator of cognitive insight (Beck et al., 2004). The range of the composite score of cognitive insight is −18 through 27, with higher scores indicating better cognitive insight.
The Autism-Spectrum Quotient
The original AQ (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) consists of a 50-item self-report questionnaire, designed to assess autistic spectrum traits in the general population (Skuse et al., 2005; Ronald and Hoekstra, 2011). In a general population, autism traits measured by the AQ provide evidence of a reliable association with metacognition (Carpenter et al., 2019). Thus, the AQ would be an appropriate scale for testing the validity of the CMS metacognition and self-judgment accuracy. The AQ is divided into five different areas of functioning related to autistic traits: social skills, attention switching, attention to detail, communication, and imagination. Using the Japanese version of the AQ (AQ-J-50) (Wakabayashi et al., 2004), participants rated their agreement on a four-point Likert-type scale from 1 (definitely disagree) to 4 (definitely agree). Higher scores indicated more autism-related cognitive traits. Please see the original version of the AQ (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) for scoring.
The Child–Parent Communication
We inquired into the levels of child–parent communication using three questions on both the children and their parents in surveys 1 and 2. The questions were related to frequency of direct contact (“How often do you usually meet with your parents/child?”), telephone (“How often do you usually call your parents/child?”), and email (“How often do you usually send emails to your parents/child?”) communication. Participants were asked to rate their own frequency of communication using a four-point Likert scale (1: almost every day, 2: at least once a week, 3: at least once a month, and 4: less than once a month). The total score from a child response was used as the level of communication between child and parent.
Statistical Analysis
We performed an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) to explore factor structure of the CMS metacognition and CMS self-judgment accuracy. Two EFAs (metacognition and self-judgment accuracy) were evaluated using maximum likelihood estimation with Promax rotation. Eigenvalues ≥ 1.00 were considered to indicate factors. Our approach to item adaptation was both statistical and structural. We included the items that loaded greater than 0.35 in the first and second EFAs and were found in both measures (CMS metacognition and self-judgment accuracy) in the second EFA.
After selecting the final items from the 20 items through the EFAs, a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted for the selected items to test the fitness of the data to the one factor. We applied the following fit parameters: root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA ≤ 0.05 good fit and ≤ 0.08 acceptable fit); standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR ≤ 0.05 good fit and ≤ 0.10 acceptable fit); goodness-of-fit index (GFI), adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI); and comparative fit index (CFI), with values ≥ 0.90 considered acceptable (McDonald and Ho, 2002).
The criterion validity was assessed using a Pearson’s correlation coefficient between the CMS metacognition and self-judgment accuracy scores and the BCIS and AQ-J-50 scores. A Pearson’s correlation coefficient was also performed to test the potential influence of child–parent communication on CMS scores. The strength of correlation coefficient was set as weak (r ≤ 0.4), moderate (0.4 < r ≤ 0.7), and strong (r > 0.7) (Dancey and Reidy, 2007).
Reliability of the scale was assessed by the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) and Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the internal consistency. The ICC was conducted through the two-way mixed-effect model and absolute agreement type. An ICC of over 0.6 was considered acceptable (Weir, 2005). An alpha value above 0.7 was considered to indicate good internal consistency, and values between 0.6 and 0.7 can be accepted providing other indicators are good (Mayers, 2013).
To test the development of metacognition during adolescence measured using the CMS metacognition and self-judgment accuracy scores, we compared the linear and non-linear regression models with age as an independent variable on all participant samples. All the analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 25 and AMOS version 25 (IBM SPSS Inc. Chicago, IL, United States) for Windows. The level of statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.
Results
Sample Characteristics
Descriptive characteristics are shown in Table 1. A one-way ANOVA revealed a significant difference between surveys in the CMS metacognition [F(2, 630) = 15.4, p < 0.01] but not in CMS self-judgment accuracy [F(2, 630) = 1.4, p = 0.24]. For the CMS metacognition, a Bonferroni post-hoc test showed that survey 3 (mean = 4.7, SD = 2.6) presented with a significantly higher score (i.e., lower metacognitive ability) compared to surveys 1 (mean = 3.5, SD = 2.1) and 2 (mean = 3.5, SD = 2.1).
In terms of the concordance between parent and child communicative reports, the three communicative measures were observed as moderate to strong, ranging from 0.53 to 0.88 (direct contact r = 0.88, via telephone r = 0.58, and via email r = 0.53; p < 0.01). The correlations between the level of communication and the CMS scores were not significant (metacognition: r = 0.07, p = 0.33; self-judgment accuracy: r = 0.01, p = 0.86).
Construct Validity
In the first EFA, six items for the CMS metacognition (polite, serious, kind, affable, graceful, and boring) and 10 items for the self-judgment accuracy (affable, persistent, polite, graceful, kind, cheerful, brave, serious, boring, and unkind) had greater than 0.35 factor loadings. The second EFA for the CMS metacognition and self-judgment accuracy scales revealed one factor solution (eigenvalues = 2.13 and 2.67) explaining 35.4% and 26.7% of the variance, respectively (Table 3). Any other eigenvalues explained only small amounts of additional variance. The items loading on factor 1 represented for both scales were five positive adjectives, “polite,” “serious,” “kind,” “affable,” and “graceful;” see Supplementary Appendix A–F.
The CFA were found acceptable for the one-factor models. For the CMS metacognition, SRMR = 0.05, GFI = 0.97, and AGFI = 0.92 were considered as good fit, while CFI = 0.89 and RMSEA = 0.11 were slightly below and above the reference, respectively. For the CMS self-judgment accuracy, SRMR = 0.04, GFI = 0.99, AGFI = 0.96, and CFI = 0.94 were considered as good fit, while RMSEA = 0.07 was acceptable fit.
Criterion Validity and Reliability
There was a significant positive correlation between CMS metacognition score and the BCIS self-reflectiveness (weak, r = 0.16, p = 0.047; see Table 4). There was a significant positive correlation between CMS self-judgment accuracy score and the self-certainty scores (weak, r = 0.17, p = 0.032). No correlations were observed with the composite scores. The CMS metacognition score was also associated with the AQ-J-50 social skills subscale (weak, r = 0.22, p < 0.01; see Table 5) and total score (weak, r = 0.20, p < 0.05). Yet, no correlations were observed in relation to the CMS self-judgment accuracy. No differences in the CMS scores between genders (p > 0.05) were observed, and the correlations between child–parent communication and CMS scores were not significant (p > 0.05).
Table 4. Pearson correlations of CMS metacognition and self-judgment accuracy abilities with Beck Cognitive Insight Scale scores (n = 152).
Table 5. Pearson correlations of CMS metacognition and self-judgment accuracy with AQ-J-50 scales (n = 148).
The ICC was considered acceptable for the CMS metacognition (r = 0.70) and for the self-judgment accuracy (r = 0.81), respectively. Cronbach’s alpha values were also considered acceptable for the whole 10 items (α = 0.64) and for the five items of the CMS metacognition (α = 0.63) and CMS self-judgment accuracy (α = 0.59).
Age Effect in the CMS Metacognitive and Self-Judgment Accuracy Abilities
A negative correlation was observed between child age and the CMS metacognition score (r = −0.27, p < 0.01), but not on self-judgment accuracy score (r = −0.03, p = 0.40). To verify the age curve-fitting of effectiveness, the regression analysis showed that both the linear model [F(1, 631) = 49.6, p < 0.01; B = −0.22, B SE = 0.03, β = −0.27, p < 0.01] and non-linear model (logarithmic) [F(1, 631) = 55.7, p < 0.01; B = −3.76, B SE = 0.50, β = −0.29, p < 0.01] were significant for CMS metacognition. On the other hand, the standard error of the regression for the non-linear model (logarithmic) was lower than the linear model (SE = 2.396 vs. 2.406), suggesting that the non-linear model (logarithmic) fit better relative to the linear model (see Figure 1). In addition, for confirmatory analysis to verify the impact that the young group and old group in the age range of 10.8–12.4 years old (n = 29) and 22.0–29.3 (n = 14) had on the current result, we excluded these age ranges and reanalyzed the correlation between child age and the CMS metacognition score. The result confirms our previous results using all participants of the negative correlation between child age and the CMS metacognition score (r = −0.19, p < 0.01).
Figure 1. The correlation between the Congruency-based Metacognition Scale (CMS) metacognitive score and child age. The lower the CMS metacognition score, the higher the level of metacognitive ability.
Discussion
This is the first study that examined the development of metacognitive ability based on child–parent response congruency in a large sample dataset from childhood to young adulthood. The present study demonstrated evidence of validity through the correlation of the CMS with the BCIS and test–retest reliability. Furthermore, CMS metacognition was positively correlated with the child AQ-J-50 total and social skills subscales, suggesting the CMS reflects the underdevelopment of metacognitive ability. Finally, a non-linear model showed that the metacognitive ability measured using CMS decreased during early adolescence.
Criterion Validity and Reliability
CMS metacognition and self-judgment accuracy scores showed a weak correlation with BCIS self-reflectiveness and self-certainty, respectively. The weak correlation is arguably explained by the fact that CMS metacognition and self-judgment accuracy are based on using a congruency measurement and the parental report was used as a reference to measure “performance,” while the BCIS is a self-report measure. The current result is consistent with the previous findings (Kanie et al., 2014) of a study that investigated the relationship between BCIS scores and the Social Cognition Screening Questionnaire (SCSQ) metacognition subscale, a measure based on confidence levels. Moreover, in the same study (Kanie et al., 2014), it was reported that impaired metacognition involves a decreased ability to evaluate the accuracy of one’s own judgments, often due to overconfidence.
Positive correlations were found between CMS metacognition score and the AQ-J-50 total and social skills scores of the same construct, but not with CMS self-judgment accuracy. A negative correlation trend has been previously observed between the AQ score and the left PCC/precuneus as a function of adopting a 3PP_Self (Hashimoto et al., 2017). We speculate that the combination of different perspectives in the self and in the other are necessary to negatively correlate the AQ with brain structures in the CMS, such as the PCC/precuneus.
The reliability of the CMS was determined using the ICC and Cronbach’s alpha coefficient that showed acceptable test–retest reliability. It is worth noting this because numerous metacognition studies have not examined test–retest reliability (Bacow et al., 2009; Larøi et al., 2009; Hsu, 2010; Cook et al., 2014; Martin et al., 2014; Bailey and Wells, 2015; Fernie et al., 2015; Kollmann et al., 2016; Kolubinski et al., 2017; Alma et al., 2018; Caselli et al., 2018; Lloyd et al., 2018). In addition, the present findings are consistent with some previous studies (Cartwright-Hatton et al., 2004; Wilson et al., 2011; Lachat Shakeshaft et al., 2020) with respect to the stability of metacognition over the period of time, although the correlation was weak (r = 0.24–0.34) (Cartwright-Hatton et al., 2004) or unstable for some domains (r = 0.24–0.90) (Wilson et al., 2011) of the metacognitive questionnaire when comparing with our test–retest (r = 0.70–0.81). Regarding to the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient, although the results revealed acceptable internal reliability values for both the whole 10 items (α = 0.64) and for the five items of the CMS metacognition (α = 0.63), the exception was the CMS self-judgment accuracy for which Cronbach’s α was 0.59. This might be related to the fact that during EFA, the items of the CMS self-judgment accuracy had to be adapted to the items of the CMS metacognition. Although, if we used another way of interpreting the alpha statistic, this value could be considered as satisfactory (Taber, 2018).
Age Effect in the CMS Metacognitive Ability
In our study, the developmental trajectory of metacognition appeared as follows: low metacognitive ability (high scores) in childhood, followed by a gradual increase metacognitive ability (low score) during adolescence to late adolescence/young adulthood. Previous studies have demonstrated that metacognitive ability tends to be higher in adolescents compared to children (Ormond et al., 1991; Flavell et al., 1998) but not late adolescents/young adults (Vukman, 2005; Weil et al., 2013). This is because during adolescence, increases in white matter volume and decreases in gray matter volume in the frontal cortex accompany aging (Dumontheil, 2014), ultimately impacting their cognitive capacity for abstraction and self-reflection (Sebastian et al., 2008).
Some previous studies on adolescents using self-report questionnaires have not found age-related increases in metacognitive beliefs (Cartwright-Hatton et al., 2004; Ellis and Hudson, 2011; Wilson et al., 2011). This discrepancy between results can be arguably explained by the methodology used to measure metacognition, as it was based on self-report measures and a limited age range (11–17 years old). Our study based our operationalization of metacognition on both the child’s (third person) and parent’s (first-person) perspectives. According to previous studies (Desoete, 2008; Saraç and Karakelle, 2017), children’s assessments (self) of their personal metacognition abilities were consistent with their teacher’s (other) assessments at the age of approximately 8–10 years old, for example. These findings suggest that metacognitive processes are complex and require assessment using a variety of perspectives. In terms of the limited age range, it is worth noting that a study has shown that 13-year-old adolescents had the highest scores on the metacognitive ability relative to older adolescents (age range: 13–17 years old), suggesting that metacognitive ability is nearly fully formed by the age of 13 years old (Cartwright-Hatton et al., 2004). Other studies found non-correlation with age, in the similar age ranges of 11–16 years old (Wilson et al., 2011) and 12–17 years old (Ellis and Hudson, 2011). On the other hand, using the same metacognition questionnaire, a significant negative correlation (age range of 13–16 years old) (Matthews et al., 2007) and a positive correlation (age range of 12–18 years old; age range of 13–17 years old) (Wolters et al., 2012; Lachat Shakeshaft et al., 2020) were found. As such, these findings further demonstrate the importance of examining metacognitive ability across a broader age range. Therefore, it is possible to conclude that, with age, the metacognitive ability improves through, in our case, the difference of the children–parental responses that become more focused and accurate—and this trajectory tend to improve in young adulthood.
The items for the scale consist of five positive adjectives. The results replicated a previous study showing that the association between parental and adolescents’ metacognitive beliefs of rumination was significant in the case of positive metacognitive belief, but not negative (Chow and Lo, 2017). The child’s response for negative personality trait could be biased especially in adolescence, and therefore, the gap with parental response may not be appropriate for the measurement of metacognition.
Three limitations were present in the current study. First, the majority of “other/parents” were represented by maternal rather than paternal figures. A future study could incorporate “other/parental” people, involved in more distant relationships with the child to represent 1PP_other in CMS metacognition. Second, survey 3 consisted of majority of the sample. Therefore, though the whole sample consisted of 633 child–parent pairs, the criterion validity with BCIS was indeed based on the sample of survey 1, which was 152 child–parent pairs. This, however, does not diminish the importance of the current validity results from the CMS metacognition values only used on the basis of survey 1. In addition, due to the fact that collecting data from children and parents simultaneously can be challenging and based on the sample size from previous studies (sample size = 42–214) (Cartwright-Hatton et al., 2004; Matthews et al., 2007; Ellis and Hudson, 2011; Wilson et al., 2011; Wolters et al., 2012; Lachat Shakeshaft et al., 2020) that investigate the validation of metacognition in adolescents, our sample size is representative enough to assess the validity of CMS metacognition. Thus, in future studies, it will be important to replicate the present study. Third, because the sample size of the children over age 22 was small, we are unable fully to generalize the method of metacognitive measurement after adolescence. One reason for smaller sample size in this study was the difficulties in recruitment for the pairs in which the children were over age 22. Therefore, the questionnaire may be more reliable for measuring for adolescents.
In conclusion, the present study demonstrates that the CMS could be a useful tool for assessing metacognition developmentally—from childhood to young adulthood. This study demonstrated that metacognitive ability appears to develop particularly in the early phases of adolescence. Although our results indicate that CMS metacognition reflects underdevelopment in metacognitive ability, in future studies, we aim to verify the utility of CMS in the assessment of autism spectrum disorders in children.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author. The datasets for this article are not publicly available due to state restrictions, such as privacy or ethical restrictions. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to Shinsuke Koike, c-koike@g.ecc.u-tokyo.ac.jp.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Research Ethics Committee at The University of Tokyo (No. 18-50) and Department of Medicine of The University of Tokyo (No. 10069-21). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.
Author Contributions
KHdSK and SK conceived the experiment and drafted the manuscript. KHdSK, YU, SY, and KO collected and analyzed data. RH, AN, SA, HN, KK, and SK critically reviewed and approved the final version of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This study was supported by the JSPS KAKENHI Grant Nos. 17H05921, JP16H06395, 16H06396, and 16H06399 to SK and also a part supported by the Tokyo Teen Cohort Study.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This work is part of the Tokyo Teen Cohort Study, and we would like to thank all researchers for substantial support in sample management. We also thank the families who gave so generously of their time throughout this project.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.565231/full#supplementary-material
References
Alma, L., Spada, M. M., Fernie, B. A., Yilmaz-Samanci, A. E., Caselli, G., and Nikcevic, A. V. (2018). Metacognitions in smoking: evidence from a cross-cultural validation of the metacognitions about smoking questionnaire in a Turkish sample. Psychiatry Res. 259, 160–168. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.10.015
Ando, S., Nishida, A., Yamasaki, S., Koike, S., Morimoto, Y., Hoshino, A., et al. (2019). Cohort profile: the tokyo teen cohort study (TTC). Int. J. Epidemiol. 48, 1414–1414. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyz033
Bacow, T. L., Pincus, D. B., Ehrenreich, J. T., and Brody, L. R. (2009). The metacognitions questionnaire for children: development and validation in a clinical sample of children and adolescents with anxiety disorders. J. Anxiety Disord. 23, 727–736. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.02.013
Bailey, R., and Wells, A. (2015). Development and initial validation of a measure of metacognitive beliefs in health anxiety: the MCQ-HA. Psychiatry Res. 230, 871–877. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.11.035
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., and Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, malesand females, scientists and mathematicians. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 31, 5–17.
Beck, A. T., Baruch, E., Balter, J. M., Steer, R. A., and Warman, D. M. (2004). A new instrument for measuring insight: the beck cognitive insight scale. Schizophrenia Res. 68, 319–329. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(03)00189-0
Bochner, S., and Van Zyl, T. (1985). Desirability ratings of 110 personality-trait words. J. Soc. Psychol. 125, 459–465. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1985.9713524
Brown, S., Cockett, P., and Yuan, Y. (2019). The neuroscience of romeo and juliet: an fMRI study of acting. R. Soc. Open Sci. 6:181908. doi: 10.1098/rsos.181908
Carpenter, K. L., Williams, D. M., and Nicholson, T. (2019). Putting your money where your mouth is: examining metacognition in ASD using post-decision wagering. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 49, 4268–4279. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04118-6
Cartwright-Hatton, S., Mather, A., Illingworth, V., Brocki, J., Harrington, R., and Wells, A. (2004). Development and preliminary validation of the meta-cognitions questionnaire-adolescent version. J. Anxiety Disord. 18, 411–422. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(02)00294-3
Caselli, G., Fernie, B., Canfora, F., Mascolo, C., Ferrari, A., Antonioni, M., et al. (2018). The metacognitions about gambling questionnaire: development and psychometric properties. Psychiatry Res. 261, 367–374. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.01.018
Chow, K.-W., and Lo, B. C. (2017). Parental factors associated with rumination related metacognitive beliefs in adolescence. Front. Psychol. 8:536. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00536
Cook, S. A., Salmon, P., Dunn, G., and Fisher, P. (2014). Measuring metacognition in cancer: validation of the metacognitions questionnaire 30 (MCQ-30). PLoS One 9:e107302. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0107302
Dancey, C. P., and Reidy, J. (2007). Statistics Without Maths for Psychology. London: Pearson education.
D’Argembeau, A. R., Perrine, R., Collette, F., Degueldre, C., Balteau, E., and Luxen, A. (2007). Distinct regions of the medial prefrontal cortex are associated with self-referential processing and perspective taking. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 935–944. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.935
De Los Reyes, A., and Kazdin, A. E. (2005). Informant discrepancies in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: a critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for further study. Psychol. Bull. 131:483. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.483
Desoete, A. (2008). Multi-method assessment of metacognitive skills in elementary school children: how you test is what you get. Metacogn. Learn. 3:189. doi: 10.1007/s11409-008-9026-0
Dumontheil, I. (2014). Development of abstract thinking during childhood and adolescence: the role of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 57–76. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.07.009
Durbin, C. E., Hayden, E. P., Klein, D. N., and Olino, T. M. (2007). Stability of laboratory-assessed temperamental emotionality traits from ages 3 to 7. Emotion 7:388. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.388
Ellis, D. M., and Hudson, J. L. (2011). Test of the metacognitive model of generalized anxiety disorder in anxiety-disordered adolescents. J. Exp. Psychopathol. 2, 28–43. doi: 10.5127/jep.011910
Fernie, B. A., Maher-Edwards, L., Murphy, G., Nikcevic, A. V., and Spada, M. M. (2015). The metacognitions about symptoms control scale: development and concurrent validity. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 22, 443–449. doi: 10.1002/cpp.1906
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new area of cognitive–developmental inquiry. Am. Psychol. 34:906. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.34.10.906
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., and Flavell, E. R. (1998). The mind has a mind of its own: developing knowledge about mental uncontrollability. Cogn. Dev. 13, 127–138. doi: 10.1016/s0885-2014(98)90024-7
Fleming, S. M., Weil, R. S., Nagy, Z., Dolan, R. J., and Rees, G. (2010). Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure. Science 329, 1541–1543. doi: 10.1126/science.1191883
Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Marshall, P. J., Nichols, K. E., and Ghera, M. M. (2005). Behavioral inhibition: linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 235–262. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141532
Fujikawa, S., Ando, S., Nishida, A., Usami, S., Koike, S., Yamasaki, S., et al. (2018). Disciplinary slapping is associated with bullying involvement regardless of warm parenting in early adolescence. J. Adolesc. 68, 207–216. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.07.018
Fujikawa, S., Ando, S., Shimodera, S., Koike, S., Usami, S., Toriyama, R., et al. (2016). The association of current violence from adult family members with adolescent bullying involvement and suicidal feelings. PLoS One 11:e0163707. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163707
Hash, J. B., Oxford, M. L., Fleming, C. B., Ward, T. M., Spieker, S. J., and Lohr, M. J. (2019). Impact of a home visiting program on sleep problems among young children experiencing adversity. Child Abuse Neglect. 89, 143–154. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.12.016
Hashimoto, R., Itahashi, T., Ohta, H., Yamada, T., Kanai, C., Nakamura, M., et al. (2017). Altered effects of perspective-taking on functional connectivity during self- and other-referential processing in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Soc. Neurosci. 12, 661–672.
Hsu, L. L. (2010). Metacognitive Inventory for nursing students in Taiwan: instrument development and testing. J. Adv. Nurs. 66, 2573–2581. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2010.05427.x
Kanie, A., Hagiya, K., Ashida, S., Pu, S. H., Kaneko, K., Mogami, T., et al. (2014). New instrument for measuring multiple domains of social cognition: construct validity of the social cognition screening questionnaire (Japanese version). Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 68, 701–711. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12181
Kollmann, J., Gollwitzer, M., Spada, M. M., and Fernie, B. A. (2016). The association between metacognitions and the impact of Fibromyalgia in a German sample. J. Psychosom. Res. 83, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2016.02.002
Kolubinski, D. C., Nikcevic, A. V., Lawrence, J. A., and Spada, M. M. (2017). The metacognitions about self-critical rumination questionnaire. J. Affect. Disord. 220, 129–138. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.06.002
Lachat Shakeshaft, Y., Lecerf, T., Morosan, L., Badoud, D. M., and Debbané, M. (2020). Validation of the French version of the «Meta-Cognition Questionnaire» for adolescents (MCQ-Af): evolution of metacognitive beliefs with age and their links with anxiety during adolescence. PLoS One 15:e0230171. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0230171
Larøi, F., Van der Linden, M., and d’Acremont, M. (2009). Validity and reliability of a French version of the metacognitions questionnaire in a nonclinical population. Swiss. J. Psychol. 68, 125–132. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185.68.3.125
Lloyd, O., Ownsworth, T., Fleming, J., and Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. (2018). Development and preliminary validation of the Paediatric Awareness Questionnaire for children and adolescents with traumatic brain injury. Child Neuropsychol. 24, 702–722. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2017.1332173
Martin, J., Padierna, A., Unzurrunzaga, A., Gonzalez, N., Berjano, B., and Quintana, J. M. (2014). Adaptation and validation of the metacognition questionnaire (MCQ-30) in Spanish clinical and nonclinical samples. J. Affect. Disord. 167, 228–234. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.06.009
Matthews, L., Reynolds, S., and Derisley, J. (2007). Examining cognitive models of obsessive compulsive disorder in adolescents. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 35:149. doi: 10.1017/s1352465806003213
McDonald, R. P., and Ho, M.-H. R. (2002). Principles and practice in reporting structural equation analyses. Psychol. Methods 7:64. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.64
Okada, N., Ando, S., Sanada, M., Hirata-Mogi, S., Iijima, Y., Sugiyama, H., et al. (2018). Population-neuroscience study of the Tokyo TEEN Cohort (pn-TTC): cohort longitudinal study to explore the neurobiological substrates of adolescent psychological and behavioral development. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 73, 231–242. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12814
Ormond, C., Luszcz, M. A., Mann, L., and Beswick, G. (1991). A metacognitive analysis of decision making in adolescence. J. Adolesc. 14, 275–291. doi: 10.1016/0140-1971(91)90021-i
Park, B.-E., Lee, J.-S., Kim, H.-Y., Bae, J.-N., Kim, W.-H., Kim, H.-Y., et al. (2019). The influence of depression and school life on the quality of life of korean child and adolescent patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a comparison of the perspectives of the patients and their caregivers. J. Korean Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 30, 2–8. doi: 10.5765/jkacap.180027
Ronald, A., and Hoekstra, R. A. (2011). Autism spectrum disorders and autistic traits: a decade of new twin studies. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 156, 255–274. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31159
Ruby, P., Collette, F., D’Argembeau, A., Peters, F., Degueldre, C., Balteau, E., et al. (2009). Perspective taking to assess self-personality: what’s modified in Alzheimer’s disease? Neurobiol. Aging 30, 1637–1651. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.12.014
Ruby, P., and Decety, J. (2001). Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 546–550. doi: 10.1038/87510
Ruby, P., and Decety, J. (2004). How would you feel versus how do you think she would feel? A neuroimaging study of perspective-taking with social emotions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 988–999. doi: 10.1162/0898929041502661
Saraç, S., and Karakelle, S. (2017). On-line and off-line assessment of metacognition. Int. Electron. J. Element. Educ. 4, 301–315.
Sebastian, C., Burnett, S., and Blakemore, S.-J. (2008). Development of the self-concept during adolescence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 441–446.
Skuse, D. H., Mandy, W. P., and Scourfield, J. (2005). Measuring autistic traits: heritability, reliability and validity of the social and communication disorders checklist. Br. J. Psychiatry 187, 568–572. doi: 10.1192/bjp.187.6.568
Taber, K. S. (2018). The use of Cronbach’s alpha when developing and reporting research instruments in science education. Res. Sci. Educ. 48, 1273–1296. doi: 10.1007/s11165-016-9602-2
Theunissen, M. H., de Wolff, M. S., and Reijneveld, S. A. (2019). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire self-report: a valid instrument for the identification of emotional and behavioral problems. Acad. Pediatr. 19, 471–476. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2018.12.008
Uchida, T., Matsumoto, K., Kikuchi, A., Miyakoshi, T., Ito, F., Ueno, T., et al. (2009). Psychometric properties of the Japanese version of the beck cognitive insight scale: relation of cognitive insight to clinical insight. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 63, 291–297. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.01946.x
Veenman, M. V., Van Hout-Wolters, B. H., and Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning: conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacogn. Learn. 1, 3–14. doi: 10.1007/s11409-006-6893-0
Vukman, K. B. (2005). Developmental differences in metacognition and their connections with cognitive development in adulthood. J. Adult Dev. 12, 211–221. doi: 10.1007/s10804-005-7089-6
Wakabayashi, A., Tojo, Y., Baron-Cohen, S., and Wheelwright, S. (2004). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ) Japanese version: evidence from high-functioning clinical group and normal adults. Shinrigaku Kenkyu Japanese J. Psychol. 75, 78–84. doi: 10.4992/jjpsy.75.78
Weil, L. G., Fleming, S. M., Dumontheil, I., Kilford, E. J., Weil, R. S., Rees, G., et al. (2013). The development of metacognitive ability in adolescence. Conscious. Cogn. 22, 264–271. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.01.004
Weir, J. P. (2005). Quantifying test-retest reliability using the intraclass correlation coefficient and the SEM. J. Strength Condition. Res. 19, 231–240. doi: 10.1519/00124278-200502000-00038
Wilson, C., Budd, B., Chernin, R., King, H., Leddy, A., Maclennan, F., et al. (2011). The role of meta-cognition and parenting in adolescent worry. J. Anxiety Disord. 25, 71–79. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.08.005
Wolters, L. H., Hogendoorn, S. M., Oudega, M., Vervoort, L., De Haan, E., Prins, P. J., et al. (2012). Psychometric properties of the dutch version of the meta-cognitions questionnaire-adolescent version (MCQ-A) in non-clinical adolescents and adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Anxiety Disord. 26, 343–351. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.11.013
Keywords: child, adolescent, metacognition, scale development, validity
Citation: dos Santos Kawata KH, Ueno Y, Hashimoto R, Yoshino S, Ohta K, Nishida A, Ando S, Nakatani H, Kasai K and Koike S (2021) Development of Metacognition in Adolescence: The Congruency-Based Metacognition Scale. Front. Psychol. 11:565231. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.565231
Received: 24 May 2020; Accepted: 18 November 2020;
Published: 06 January 2021.
Edited by:
Ann Dowker, University of Oxford, United KingdomReviewed by:
Lucia Ponti, University of Florence, ItalyDan Kolubinski, London South Bank University, United Kingdom
Copyright © 2021 dos Santos Kawata, Ueno, Hashimoto, Yoshino, Ohta, Nishida, Ando, Nakatani, Kasai and Koike. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Shinsuke Koike, c-koike@g.ecc.u-tokyo.ac.jp