Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 16 September 2020
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Psychological Factors as Determinants of Medical Conditions, Volume II View all 18 articles

Type C Personality: Conceptual Refinement and Preliminary Operationalization

  • 1Institute of Psychology, Cardinal Stefan Wyszyński University in Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland
  • 2URPP Social Networks University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

In this paper, we have presented our proposal for reconceptualization and operationalization of Type C (cancer-prone) personality. Based on theoretical analyses, taking into account both the literature on Type C and models of personality structure, we have proposed a two-facet structure of Type C, comprising Submissiveness (the interpersonal aspect) and Restricted Affectivity (the intrapersonal aspect). The study devoted to the validation of the measure of Type C involved 232 participants aged 18–70 (M = 29.35, SD = 8.93; 54% male). We used (a) our proposed measure of Type C personality and (b) the Circumplex of Personality Metatraits Questionnaire (CPM-Q-SF; Strus and Cieciuch, 2017), assessing personality metatraits. The measure of Type C proved to have acceptable internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha was 0.85 for Submissiveness and 0.78 for Restricted Affectivity). The measurement model in confirmatory factor analysis with two latent variables proved to be well-fitted to the data. We have also confirmed the hypothesis concerning the location of the two facets of Type C personality close to each other in the theoretically predicted area between the Delta-Plus/Self-Restraint and Beta-Minus/Passiveness metatraits (in the Circumplex of Personality Metatraits). The clinical value of the theoretically refined Type C can be tested in the next step in research on patients with cancer.

Introduction

Type C: The Search for Psychological Determinants of Cancer

The belief that somatic diseases depend also on psychological factors has been the underlying assumption of many studies that sought to identify those personality characteristics that increased the risk of specific somatic diseases or were responsible for general susceptibility to diseases (Friedman and Rosenman, 1959; Greer and Morris, 1975; Denollet et al., 1995; Dolińska-Zygmunt, 2001b; Ogińska-Bulik and Juczyński, 2008; Horwood et al., 2015; Šmigelskas et al., 2015). One of the personality constructs claimed to be associated with the occurrence of cancer is Type C personality (Eysenck, 1994; Bozo et al., 2014; Habibi et al., 2015), also referred to as Type C behavior (Greer and Watson, 1985), Pattern C behavior (Dolińska-Zygmunt, 2001b), or cancer-prone personality (Eysenck, 1994; Watson et al., 1999).

The introduction of Type C into the literature is usually attributed to Greer and Morris (1975), who conducted research on a sample of women with breast cancer and found the co-occurrence of cancer with a certain pattern of behavior associated with abnormal expression of emotions, which they later named Type C behavior (Greer and Watson, 1985). The characteristics of Type C and its relations to other personality types and traits were also the subject of the work of Eysenck (1991). What is particularly often provided in the literature is the graphic presentation of Types C (cancer-prone personality), Type A (coronary heart disease-prone personality), and Type B (normal, not disease-prone personality) in relation to the traits of extraversion and neuroticism, proposed by Eysenck (1991), as shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Hypothetical relations of Types A, B, and C to extraversion and neuroticism (Eysenck, 1991).

According to this perspective, cancer-prone personality (Type C) is associated with neuroticism and introversion, while coronary heart disease-prone personality is associated with neuroticism and extraversion (Eysenck, 1991). Empirical results, however, did not always confirm this pattern of theoretical relations, and sometimes, they even directly contradicted it. Already in the first study of Kissen and Eysenck (1962) conducted on a sample of men with lung cancer, it turned out that the occurrence of cancer was related to a low rather than high level of neuroticism. Also in his other publications, Eysenck (1985) discussed studies in which it was low neuroticism (e.g., Morris et al., 1981) or high extraversion that was significantly related to cancer incidence (e.g., Coppen and Metcalfe, 1963; Hagnell, 1966). There were attempts to explain the empirically found negative relationship between neuroticism and cancer as stemming from the emotional repression that may accompany low neuroticism (Eysenck, 1991) and the negative link between cancer and introversion as due to the better condition of the immune system in introverts (Eysenck, 1985).

A number of further studies on cancer patients revealed additional links between Type C and psychosocial factors but they were not always consistent either. For example, a study by Garssen and Goodkin (1999) confirmed that a low level of social support, a tendency toward helplessness, and repression of negative emotions were significant in the development of cancer. Lehto et al. (2006) found that emotional defensiveness, avoidant coping, and a high level of social support were risk factors for cancer. Reynolds et al. (2000) highlight the significance of low emotion expression and low perceived social support for the worse prognosis of patients with cancers. Furthermore, in Chinese studies on a group of patients with breast cancer (Wei et al., 2019), sense of coherence was negatively associated with Type C personality and depression, Type C personality was significantly positively associated with depression, and sense of coherence played a partial mediating role between Type C personality and depression, reducing the influence of Type C personality on depression.

In view of the diverse and not always consistent findings, an important research effort was the meta-analysis performed by McKenna et al. (1999), covering studies published from 1975 until 1996. It revealed that the factors significant in the development of cancer were: (a) denial/repression coping, (b) separation/loss experiences, (c) stressful life events, and (d) conflict-avoidant personality style (McKenna et al., 1999). These, however, are only four of the eight factors considered in the meta-analysis. Those that proved not to be significant were: increased anxiety/depression, childhood family environment (lack of support in childhood), difficulties with the expression of anger and resentment, and extraversion/introversion level. Another meta-analysis of 76 studies on depression and cancer mortality indicated that depression is associated with an increased risk of mortality in cancer patients and those who develop the disease (Pinquart and Duberstein, 2010). A meta-analysis conducted by Jokela et al. (2014) including six prospective cohort studies focused on the relationship between personality traits (extraversion, neuroticism, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness to experience) with cancer incidence and mortality as a result of cancer. The results indicated no association between any of the personality traits and the incidence of all cancers and any site-specific cancers (lung, colon, breast, prostate, skin, and leukemia/lymphoma) included in the analysis. None of the personality traits were also associated with cancer mortality.

It is worth noting that the role of personality and especially personality traits for occurrence and cancer courses are still heavily studied. In recent studies, on the one hand, de Mol et al. (2020) argue that personality traits are not associated with health-related quality of life and general quality of life, except for association between conscientiousness and physical health, in patients with advanced-stage lung cancer at the start of chemotherapy. However, on the other hand, several new studies suggest such relations; in particular, (a) neuroticism was strongly positively associated with emotional stress and mental health problems in oncological patients (Macía et al., 2020; Perry et al., 2020) and negatively associated with better health behaviors and health (Rochefort et al., 2018); (b) extraversion was positively associated with physical health, regardless of cancer diagnosis (Rochefort et al., 2018) and negatively associated with emotional stress and mental health problems in patients with cancer (Macía et al., 2020; Perry et al., 2020); when combined with two personality traits, a low level of neuroticism and a high level of extraversion were associated with better mental health (Macía et al., 2020); and (c) conscientiousness was weakly negatively associated with emotional stress in patients with cancer (Perry et al., 2020) and positively with better health behaviors and health (Rochefort et al., 2018).

Other characteristics of Type C personality can also be found in the literature, similar to the above to some extent, but also going beyond those already mentioned. Type C was supposed to characterize passive individuals, incapable of helping themselves, strongly focused on other people, unable to express their emotions, anger-repressing, helpless (Temoshok, 1987; Dolińska-Zygmunt, 2001a; Kurrass, 2004; Lehto et al., 2006; Ogden, 2007; Bozo et al., 2014; Lysaker et al., 2014), self-sacrificing (Kurrass, 2004), unable to manage their psychological behaviors, submissive, pathologically kind and agreeable, cooperative, excessively patient (Temoshok, 1987; Habibi et al., 2015), and showing excessive control of emotional features (Lysaker et al., 2014; Habibi et al., 2015); it has been attributed to individuals who have strong defense mechanisms resulting in the inability to verbalize and recognize negative emotions, who show secondary negative responses such as the sense of helplessness and uselessness, who lack self-control in stressful situations, and who are submissive to authorities (Temoshok, 1987).

The above characteristics of Type C personality show its high diversity. The problem of the consistency of these characteristics has not, essentially, been addressed at the theoretical level yet. The theoretical aim of the present article is to fill this gap.

Problems Associated With Type C Personality

The literature devoted to Type C personality and its links with the occurrence of cancer contains many doubts and ambiguities. The major ones will be discussed below.

First, many research results that can be found in the literature do not confirm the significance of various components of Type C personality for the occurrence of cancer (cf. Schwarz and Geyer, 1984; Phillips et al., 2008; Archer et al., 2015). For example, Archer et al. (2015) did not confirm the link of chronic depressive symptoms with cancer occurrence. Phillips et al. (2008) found anxiety, depression, coping, and social support not to be significantly related to patients’ prognosis. In the study by Schwarz and Geyer (1984), demographic characteristics, serious stressful life events, and activity control were not significantly related to the occurrence of cancer.

Second, the description of Type C personality includes various contents with unclear structure, which may stem from the method used to construct the psychological variable referred to as Type C. This method consisted in Type C personality being distinguished based on the observation of the behaviors of patients suffering from cancer (Greer and Morris, 1975; Greer and Watson, 1985). Consequently, these behaviors constituted atheoretical indicators, observed in the behavior of patients diagnosed with this disease. Type C personality was analyzed in terms of its relations to other elements of personality structure to a small extent only, and its description relied on knowledge in the field of personality psychology to a very small degree. In the literature, it is possible to find different, sometimes, divergent or even contradictory characteristics attributed to Type C as the personality basis for cancer. For instance, Garssen and Goodkin (1999) underscored the significance of low social support for the development of cancer, whereas Lehto et al. (2006) highlighted the significance of high social support. Also, unclear and full of contradictions is the role of neuroticism and extraversion, which has been discussed above (Kissen and Eysenck, 1962; Eysenck, 1985, 1991; Blanchard and Abell, 2019).

Third, there are visible deficiencies in the acceptable operationalization of Type C. Even though the construct is interesting and attracts the attention of many researchers, it has not been properly operationalized and no sound measure has been developed to assess it. One of the best-known attempts at developing such measure is the questionnaire by Watson and Greer (1983); in view of the fact that the key element in Type C personality is usually considered to be emotional repression, these authors prepared a measure of emotional control (the Courtauld Emotional Control Scale, CECS), in which they distinguished three scales: Anger Control, Anxiety Control, and Depression Control. This questionnaire, however, does not measure Type C personality directly and can be seen as assessing only one element included in this construct.

Given the breadth of the construct, its special character, and the divergences concerning both the components distinguished in it and the results of research, one can conclude that the current measurement of Type C personality is inadequate and that a comprehensive operationalization of this construct is needed. Still, what is needed before the construction of the measure is a theoretical conceptualization and a precise definition of Type C, taking into account both the existing literature about it and the current knowledge about personality structure. The theoretical conceptualization of Type C is, therefore, a precondition of solving the third problem (i.e., the lack of comprehensive operationalization). It should be performed in such a way as to solve the second problem (i.e., conceptualization taking into account the current knowledge about personality structure), which, consequently, may give hope for a solution for the first problem (i.e., relation to cancer-proneness) in future research.

Attempt to Solve the Problems Associated With Type C Personality: Conceptualization

We performed the postulated reconceptualization of Type C personality in four steps. In the first step, we compiled a list of all Type C contents and characteristics reported in the literature. In the second step, we combined those elements that were close to one another in terms of content, thus reducing the list of characteristics from Step 1. In the third step, we examined the structure of the groups distinguished and the possibilities of grouping the obtained elements into a smaller number of broader categories. In the fourth step, we linked the characteristics of Type C obtained in the previous steps with knowledge about personality structure. In particular, we looked at the characteristics of Type C from the point of view offered by the CPM (Strus et al., 2014; Strus and Cieciuch, 2017), which, on the one hand, integrates many models of temperament, personality, emotion, motivation, health, and well-being (Strus and Cieciuch, 2017), and which, on the other hand, can be seen as a tool for testing and refining constructs in other domains (see Cieciuch and Topolewska, 2017; Rogoza et al., 2019b).

The Reconstruction of the Contents of Type C (Steps 1–3)

In the first step, we identified 20 content units in the definitions of Type C present in the literature. These were: self-sacrifice, passiveness, calmness, peacefulness, strong focus on other people, submissiveness to others, submissiveness to authorities, pathological kindness, pathological agreeableness, excessive patience, cooperativeness, inability to help oneself, repression of negative emotions, strong defense mechanisms, inability to verbalize and identify the emotions experienced, sense of helplessness and uselessness, suppression of anger, excessive control of emotional symptoms, lack of self-control in stressful situations, and low awareness of the emotions experienced (Greer and Watson, 1985; Temoshok, 1987; Dolińska-Zygmunt, 2001a; Kurrass, 2004; Ogden, 2007; Bozo et al., 2014; Lysaker et al., 2014; Habibi et al., 2015).

In the second step, we grouped the above characteristics by combining semantically close contents. This resulted in six groups: (1) excessively high (pathological) kindness; (2) self-sacrifice, strong focus on other people, inability to help oneself; (3) peacefulness and excessive patience, pathological agreeableness and submissiveness; (4) repression of negative emotions, strong defense mechanisms, suppression of anger, excessive control of emotional symptoms, calmness; (5) low awareness of the emotions experienced, inability to verbalize and identify the emotions experienced; and (6) sense of helplessness and uselessness, lack of self-control in stressful situations, passiveness.

In the third step, we divided the contents from Step 2 into two domains: interpersonal and intrapersonal. The above presentation of contents in Step 2 was organized with a view to Step 3, which is why interpersonal contents are in Groups 1–3 and interpersonal contents are in Groups 4–6. The distinction between these two domains in Type C personality was inspired by the popular model of Type D personality structure (also referred to as distress personality) proposed by Denollet (2005).

The fourth step consisted in introducing an external frame of reference into the analysis of contents attributed to Type C from the perspective of personality psychology. The description of Type C had been developed as a description of behaviors or characteristics of individuals suffering from or prone to cancer. This process of construct development should be confronted with the current knowledge about personality structure. In personality psychology, there are many different models, which – as has been proposed for some time in the literature – can be integrated in the two-factor model of personality (DeYoung, 2015; Cieciuch and Strus, 2017), whose extension is the CPM (Strus et al., 2014; Strus and Cieciuch, 2017). This model offers a theoretical matrix that enables the synthesis and organization of various kinds of constructs, frequently also making it possible to conceptualize them more precisely. What can serve as an example is the integration of different perspectives on narcissism in CPM performed by Rogoza et al. (2019b), the integration of personality disorder categories performed by Zawadzki (2016, 2017), or the synthesis of various models in the area of identity formation performed by Cieciuch and Topolewska (2017), who applied the method itself rather than the CPM itself. Below, we will present the basic assumptions of CPM and then proceed to use the model to refine the theoretical definition of Type C and to further clarify its content aspects.

The Location of Type C Personality in the Circumplex of Personality Metatraits (Step 4)

The CPM model is an extension of the two-factor model of personality (Digman, 1997; DeYoung et al., 2002; Cieciuch and Strus, 2017), depicting the structure of personality at the level of two higher-order factors (metatraits): Alpha (Stability) and Beta (Plasticity). Dimensions of Alpha and Beta constitute a system of orthogonal coordinates, in which Strus et al. (2014) additionally distinguished the dimensions of Gamma (being a reinterpretation of the general factor of personality; Musek, 2007) and Delta (being a discovery that the logic of this model led to). In each metatrait, we distinguished two poles, whose psychological contents are not reducible to a pair of opposites. This resulted in an octantal structure. The current version of the model, modified after a series of studies (Strus and Cieciuch, 2017, 2019; Rogoza et al., 2019a), is presented in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Graphic presentation of the CPM (modified by Strus et al., 2014). N, neuroticism; E, extraversion; O, openness to experience; A, agreeableness; C, conscientiousness; +, means the positive pole of the trait; −, means the negative pole of the trait.

CPM emerged from the tradition of research on the five-factor model of personality (McCrae and Costa, 2003), which is why Figure 2 presents the constellation of the Big Five traits for each metatrait. In turn, thanks to its circumplex structure, CPM creates a space in which the meaning of other constructs can be specified (see Rogoza et al., 2019a). If CPM is treated as an external system of coordinates synthesizing various models developed in the field of personality psychology, the question arises about the location of Type C within the circumplex. The scope of its contents presented above locates Type C between two metatraits: Delta-Plus/Self-Restraint (peacefulness, excessive patience, submissiveness, and adjustment in relations with people) and Beta-Minus/Passiveness (passiveness, helplessness and hopelessness, and low self-awareness). It seems that those personality characteristics that are situated in CPM between Delta-Plus and Beta-Minus – namely, high behavioral control, tendency to adjust oneself, conventionality, conformism, and submissiveness in interpersonal relations, repressed affectivity (both positive and negative) inhibition, apathy, and passiveness – correspond particularly strongly to the characteristics attributed to Type C personality (see Strus and Cieciuch, 2017). We considered this location of Type C personality in CPM as a broader integrating model of personality in order to theoretically clarify and refine Type C itself.

Definition of Type C

Considering the existing descriptions of Type C and its location in CPM, we propose the following definition and structure of Type C: Type C personality is composed of two facets: Submissiveness, corresponding to the interpersonal domain, and Restricted Affectivity, corresponding to the intrapersonal domain. Submissiveness and Restricted Affectivity have elements in common with both personality metratraits mentioned above, but Submissiveness is slightly closer to Delta-Plus/Self-Restraint, while Restricted Affectivity is closer to Beta-Minus/Passiveness. Submissiveness manifests itself in pathological agreeableness, compliance, kindness toward others, uncritical adjustment to them, dependence, excessive patience, peacefulness, inability to refuse, and even in excessive focus on other people and sacrificing oneself for them at the cost of one’s own needs. Restricted Affectivity manifests itself in the repression and suppression of negative emotions (particularly anger), low awareness of the emotions experienced, inability to identify, name, and express them, anhedonia, passiveness, and helplessness in the face of adversities.

The Problem of the Present Study: Operationalization of Type C Personality

We subjected the conceptualization of Type C proposed above to a procedure of operationalization. In the first stage, we generated a pool of items measuring the components of Type C distinguished in the above definition (27 items for the Submissiveness scale and 17 items for the Restricted Affectivity scale). The aim of the study was to select items from the pool to be included in the final version of the questionnaire and to test its psychometric properties. We formulated the following hypotheses concerning the final version of the measure: internal consistency (measured with Cronbach’s alpha) is acceptable for both scales (Hypothesis 1). The measurement model in confirmatory factor analysis with two latent variables is well-fitted to the data set (Hypothesis 2). The measurement of Submissiveness and Restricted Affectivity is invariant at configural, metric, and scalar level across gender (Hypothesis 3). Both facets of Type C are located in the CPM between Delta-Plus/Self-Restraint and Beta-Minus/Passiveness; the predicted location of Submissiveness is closer to Delta-Plus, while the predicted location of Restricted Affectivity is closer to Beta-Minus, as shown in Figure 2 (Hypothesis 4).

Materials and Methods

Measures

Type C Personality

In the course of work on operationalizing the definition of Type C formulated above, our team generated 44 items: 27 items for the Submissiveness scale and 17 items for the Restricted Affectivity scale. We used a five-point Likert scale (from 1 – completely untrue about me, to 5 – completely true about me). This was the initial pool, from which, in the presented study, we selected the items for the final version of the measure.

We used four selection criteria. The first criterion consisted in removing those items that significantly positively correlated with well-being, as we decided that indicators of the potentially pathological Type C personality should not be positively related to mental health symptoms. The second criterion consisted in removing the items that did not differentiate respondents’ answers (low mean score and standard deviation). The items that remained after the selection performed according to the first two criteria were entered into the exploratory factor analysis for each of the two facets of Type C separately (which was the third criterion of selection). We removed the items with the lowest factor loadings. The fourth criterion was expert assessment of the contents of those items whose removal or retention was suggested by psychometric indicators. Decision concerning each item was based on theoretical reflection rather than made automatically.

Well-Being

To measure well-being, which was one of the item selection criteria for the measure of Type C, we used the following items: (1) “I often feel simply happy”; (2) “Little everyday things often give me joy”; (3) “If I could live my life again, I would change almost nothing”; and (4) “My natural mood can be called cheerfulness.” Answers were given on a five-point Likert scale, from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree); Cronbach’s α was 0.70.

Personality Metatraits

To measure personality metatraits, we used the Circumplex of Personality Metatraits Questionnaire (CPM-Q-SF; Strus and Cieciuch, 2017). The questionnaire consists of 72 items describing a variety of human behaviors, feelings, thoughts, and attitudes. It measures the eight metatraits distinguished in CPM: Alpha-Plus/Stability, Alpha-Minus/Disinhibition, Beta-Plus/Plasticity, Beta-Minus/Passiveness, Gamma-Plus/Integration, Gamma-Minus/Disharmony, Delta-Plus/Self-Restraint, and Delta-Minus/Sensation Seeking. Answers were given on a five-point Likert scale from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). Cronbach’s α of the scales ranged from 0.72 (Alpha-Plus/Stability) to 0.85 (Gamma-Minus/Disharmony).

Participants

The participants in the study were 232 individuals aged 18–70 (M = 29.35, SD = 8.93). Men (n = 126) constituted 54.3% of the sample. The participants completed the paper-and-pencil version of the set of measures; their anonymity was fully ensured. When collecting the data, we were aided by psychology students, who recruited participants among their friends, acquaintances, and distant relatives. The inclusion criteria was age (18 years or more) and approximate equal number of men and women.

Results

We removed seven items after applying Criterion 1 of item selection (see description in the Measures section), and we eliminated two further items after applying Criterion 2. By applying Criteria 3 and 4, we selected 10 items for each scale, which are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Factor loadings in the confirmatory factor analysis of the questionnaire measuring Type C.

Internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) was 0.85 for Submissiveness and 0.79 for Restricted Affectivity, which can be regarded as acceptable values, confirming Hypothesis 1.

In categorical confirmatory factor analysis (after the inclusion of seven error correlations between items similar in meaning), the measurement model had the following measurement fit: chi2 = 353.6, df = 162, RMSEA = 0.071 (0.061–0.082), CFI = 0.927, WRMR = 1.06. The correlation between the latent variables was 0.66. With the obtained fit indices, Hypothesis 2 can be considered as confirmed.

In order to test for measurement invariance (Hypothesis 3) across gender, we ran a multigroup confirmatory factor analysis. For technical reasons, in six cases, one answer was randomly changed to the neighboring value (e.g., answer 4 was changed to 5) in order to have all answers for all items in all groups, which is a requirement for categorical measurement invariance. We obtained the following model fit indices (a) at the configural level: chi2 = 524.9, df = 324, CFI = 0.927, RMSEA = 0.073 (0.061–0.084); (b) at the metric level: chi2 = 552.7, df = 342, CFI = 0.923; RMSEA = 0.073 (0.061–0.084); and (c) at the scalar level: chi2 = 622.0, df = 400, CFI = 0.919, RMSEA = 0.069 (0.058–0.080). According to commonly used criteria (Cieciuch and Davidov, 2015), one can conclude that configural, metric, and scalar measurement invariance across gender is established. This means that both facets of Type C have the same meaning (metric invariance) and are measured in the same way (scalar invariance) across gender.

We tested Hypothesis 4 using the procedure of orthogonal Procrustes rotation (Schönemann, 1966), which Strus and Cieciuch (2017) had applied when testing hypotheses concerning the location of variables in CPM and which is recommended by Rogoza et al. (2019a) for analyzing circumplex models. The aim of the analyses was to compare the theoretical location of the facets of Type C (Submissiveness at 150 degree and Restricted Affectivity at 165 degree) with their actual empirical location. The theoretical angular location is converted in accordance with trigonometric functions into coordinates (target matrix), and the empirical location is the factor loadings of the analyzed variables in the two-factor solution (obtained matrix). Procrustes rotation rotates the empirically obtained results to the theoretically expected ones without changing the results. There are two types of Procrustes rotations: with or without row normalization. Both of them are presented in Table 2, while Figure 3 presents the results with row normalization. In the rotation with row normalization, empirical loadings are transformed to the same unit-metric space as the target matrix (coordinates of 1 and +1). Factor loadings can be converted into angles, in accordance with trigonometric functions, and the angles can be compared with the theoretically expected ones. The degree of similarity for the entire matrix and for each variable separately is expressed as the congruence coefficient. It is assumed that a coefficient value >0.90 indicates high congruence and a value >0.95 indicates very high congruence (Barrett, 1986; Lorenzo-Seva and ten Berge, 2006). We also report R2, which is a measure of explained variance in a given variable explained by other variables included in the model.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Target and obtained matrices for Circumplex of Personality Metatraits (CPM) and the two facets of Type C.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Factor loading plots for CPM metatraits and facets of Type C from the rotated matrix with row normalization.

Table 2 presents target and obtained matrices for CPM and the two facets of Type C, followed by congruence coefficient, R2, and empirically obtained angle.

As expected, the two facets of Type C were located close to each other, in the space between Delta-Plus and Beta-Minus, in places theoretically predicted, as evidenced by the very high congruence coefficients. The obtained results make it reasonable to consider Hypothesis 3 confirmed, but two issues should be pointed out. First, despite the high congruence coefficients, the two facets of Type C were located very close to each other – somewhat closer than expected. What we expected was that they would be located at 15-degree intervals between Beta-Minus and Delta-Plus, whereas the empirical results actually placed both facets halfway between Beta-Minus and Delta-Plus. Second, R2 is relatively low, which means that the metatraits distinguished in the CPM do not explain a particularly high proportion of the variance in Type C. This result means that Type C is not reducible to personality metatraits, even though it is quite precisely located in the space defined by them.

Discussion

In research aimed at finding the personality determinants of somatic diseases, Type C personality has been proposed and treated as a significant predictor of the incidence of cancer. The analysis of the available literature on Type C has revealed a number of characteristics postulated as elements of Type C, which were not always internally consistent and did not form a precisely defined whole. This might have been one of the causes behind the divergence, reported in the literature, in research results concerning the significance of Type C personality in predicting cancers. The relatively blurred meaning of this construct and the wide scope of characteristics included in it certainly did not facilitate its operationalization, which might have been an argument in favor of measuring only selected components rather than attempting to capture Type C as a whole.

We see our research as the first step toward clarify the theorizing and results on Type C personality. The key element of our approach was the literature-based attempt to organize the components of Type C, which led to its reconceptualization. It was on the theoretical plane, thus built that we proposed a comprehensive operationalization of Type C. The measure we developed was preliminarily found to have acceptable psychometric properties (internal consistency, factorial validity, and external validity).

Of course, what remains an unsolved problem is the significance of Type C for cancer (its appearance, development, and treatment prognosis). This problem should be addressed by future studies, or even by entire research programs. However, we propose a modification in the overall approach to this kind of research. The approach, so far, has been to find atheoretical personality indicators of cancer by looking for the typical characteristics of people suffering from it. As meta-analyses have shown, this approach did not lead to conclusive results (McKenna et al., 1999). Moreover, in some studies, no association between any of the personality traits and cancer and cancer mortality was found (Jokela et al., 2014). One of the causes might have been the primacy of direct observation over the strength of a good theoretical model. This primacy resulted in little reflection being undertaken on the theoretical meaning of Type C construct, on its internal consistency and structure, and on its location in broader models of personality structure. The approach we have proposed overcomes these weaknesses and uses the CPM (Strus et al., 2014) as the theoretical personality context and a point of reference. It should be stressed that CPM does not absorb other constructs in such a way as to make them redundant. On the contrary, it turned out that Type C was irreducible to metatraits but it was worth applying the logic of CPM in its description and structure. This resulted in an internally consistent construct that has a well-defined structure and is clearly related to the broader model of personality.

Perhaps, it will turn out that our conceptualization and operationalization of Type C will not solve problems associated with the personality determinants of cancer but it seems that this kind of personality structure attempt is necessary. It can be said that this means giving Type C a chance as a reformed and reconceptualized theoretical construct in its role of a predictor of psychosomatic problems. Time will tell if this proves to be a chance of success or failure. It seems, however, that without this chance Type C personality would be slowly receding into the history of psychology.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Commission of Ethics and Bioethics at the Cardinal Stefan Wyszyński University in Warsaw. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

KR and AT prepared the first set of items to measure Type C. WS, KR, AT, and JC prepared the reconceptualization of Type C. KR, WS, and JC prepared the final set of items, designed the study, and run the analysis. KR in collaboration with JC and WS prepared the first version of the paper. JC revised the paper. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The work of KR was supported by grant 2014/14/M/HS6/00919 from the National Science Centre, Poland. The work of JC was supported by the University Research Priority Program Social Networks of the University of Zurich.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Archer, G., Pikhart, H., and Head, J. (2015). Do depressive symptoms predict cancer incidence? 17-year follow-up of the Whitehall II study. J. Psychosom. Res. 79, 595–603. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, P. (1986). Factor comparison: an examination of three methods. Pers. Individ. Differ. 7, 327–340. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(86)90008-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, M. P., and Abell, S. (2019). “Personality and cancer” in Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences. eds. V. Zeigler-Hill and T. K. Shackelford (Cham: Springer), 1–4.

Google Scholar

Bozo, Ö., Tathan, E., and Yılmaz, T. (2014). Does perceived social support buffer the negative effects of type C personality on quality of life of breast cancer patients? Soc. Indic. Res. 119, 791–801. doi: 10.1007/s11205-013-0503-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cieciuch, J., and Davidov, E. (2015). Establishing measurement invariance across online and offline samples. A tutorial with the software packages Amos and Mplus. Studia Psychologica 15, 83–99. doi: 10.21697/sp.2015.14.2.06

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cieciuch, J., and Strus, W. (2017). “Two-factor model of personality” in Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences. eds. V. Zeigler-Hill and T. K. Shackelford (Cham: Springer), 1–17.

Google Scholar

Cieciuch, J., and Topolewska, E. (2017). Circumplex of identity formation modes: a proposal for the integration of identity constructs developed in the Erikson–Marcia tradition. Self Identity 16, 37–61. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2016.1216008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coppen, A., and Metcalfe, M. (1963). Cancer and extraversion. Br. Med. J. 2, 18–19.

Google Scholar

de Mol, M., Visser, S., Aerts, J., Lodder, P., van Walree, N., Belderbos, H., et al. (2020). The association of depressive symptoms, personality traits, and sociodemographic factors with health-related quality of life and quality of life in patients with advanced-stage lung cancer: an observational multi-center cohort study. BMC Cancer 20:431. doi: 10.1186/s12885-020-06823-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denollet, J. (2005). DS14: standard assessment of negative affectivity, social inhibition, and type D personality. Psychosom. Med. 67, 89–97. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000149256.81953.49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denollet, J., Sys, S. U., and Brutsaert, D. L. (1995). Personality and mortality after myocardial infarction. Psychosom. Med. 57, 582–591. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199511000-00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeYoung, C. G. (2015). Cybernetic Big Five theory. J. Res. Pers. 56, 33–58. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2014.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., and Higgins, D. M. (2002). Higher-order factors of the Big Five predict conformity: are there neuroses of health? Pers. Individ. Differ. 33, 533–552. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00171-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 73, 1246–1256. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.6.1246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolińska-Zygmunt, G. (ed.) (2001a). “Psychologiczne aspekty chorób nowotworowych (Psychological aspects of cancer)” in Podstawy psychologii zdrowia (Basics of health psychology). Wrocław: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Wrocławskiego, 209–225.

Google Scholar

Dolińska-Zygmunt, G. (ed.) (2001b). “Psychologiczne uwarunkowania podatności na choroby (Psychological determinants of disease susceptibility)” in Podstawy psychologii zdrowia (Basics of health psychology). Wrocław: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Wrocławskiego, 169–189.

Google Scholar

Eysenck, H. J. (1985). Personality, cancer and cardiovascular disease: a causal analysis. Pers. Individ. Differ. 6, 535–556. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(85)90003-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, H. J. (1991). “Type A behaviour and coronary heart disease: the third stage” in Type A behavior. ed. M. J. Strube (Newbury Park-London-New Delhi: SAGE Publications), 25–44.

Google Scholar

Eysenck, H. J. (1994). Cancer, personality and stress: prediction and prevention. Adv. Behav. Res. Ther. 16, 167–215. doi: 10.1016/0146-6402(94)00001-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, M., and Rosenman, R. H. (1959). Association of specific overt behavior pattern with blood and cardiovascular findings. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 169, 1286–1296. doi: 10.1001/jama.1959.03000290012005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garssen, B., and Goodkin, K. (1999). On the role of immunological factors as mediators between psychosocial factors and cancer progression. Psychiatry Res. 85, 51–61. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(99)00008-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greer, S., and Morris, T. (1975). Psychological attributes of women who develop breast cancer: a controlled study. J. Psychosom. Res. 19, 147–153. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(75)90062-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greer, S., and Watson, M. (1985). Towards a psychobiological model of cancer: psychological considerations. Soc. Sci. Med. 20, 773–777. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(85)90330-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Habibi, M., Kazemi, M., Ghanbari, N., and Monajemi, M. B. (2015). The relationship alexithymia and types C and D personalities in people with depression disorders. Int. J. Indian Psychol. 3, 94–103. doi: 10.25215/0301.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagnell, O. (1966). The premorbid personality of persons who develop cancer in a total population investigated in 1947 and 1957. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 125, 846–855. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1966.tb45435.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horwood, S., Anglim, J., and Tooley, G. (2015). Type D personality and the five-factor model: a facet-level analysis. Pers. Individ. Differ. 83, 50–54. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.03.041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jokela, M., Batty, G. D., Hintsa, T., Elovainio, M., Hakulinen, C., and Kivimäki, M. (2014). Is personality associated with cancer incidence and mortality? An individual-participant meta-analysis of 2156 incident cancer cases among 42 843 men and women. Br. J. Cancer 110, 1820–1824. doi: 10.1038/bjc.2014.58

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kissen, D. M., and Eysenck, H. J. (1962). Personality in male lung cancer patients. J. Psychosom. Res. 6, 123–127. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(62)90062-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurrass, J. A. (2004). The development of a behavioral measure of Type C. Available at: http://www.geocities.ws/jkurrass/measurementpaper.pdf (Accessed August 29, 2020).

Google Scholar

Lehto, U. S., Ojanen, M., Dyba, T., Aromaa, A., and Kellokumpu-Lehtinen, P. (2006). Baseline psychosocial predictors of survival in localized melanoma. J. Psychosom. Res. 94, 1245–1252. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzo-Seva, U., and ten Berge, J. M. F. (2006). Tucker’s congruence coefficient as a meaningful index of factor similarity. Methodology 2, 57–64. doi: 10.1027/1614-2241.2.2.57

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lysaker, P. H., Olesek, K., Buck, K., Leonhardt, B. L., Vohs, J., Ringer, J., et al. (2014). Metacognitive mastery moderates the relationship of alexithymia with cluster C personality disorder traits in adults with substance use disorders. Addict. Behav. 39, 558–561. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macía, P., Gorbena, S., Gomez, A., Barranco, M., and Iraurgi, I. (2020). Role of neuroticism and extraversion in the emotional health of people with cancer. Heliyon 6:e04281. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e04281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R., and Costa, P. T. Jr. (2003). Personality in adulthood: A five-factor theory perspective. 2nd Edn. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

McKenna, M. C., Zevon, M. A., Corn, B., and Rounds, J. (1999). Psychosocial factors and the development of breast cancer: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 18, 520–531. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.18.5.520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, T., Greer, S., Pettingale, K. W., and Watson, M. (1981). Patterns of expression of anger and their psychological correlates in women with breast cancer. J. Psychosoc. Res. 25, 111–117.

Google Scholar

Musek, J. (2007). A general factor of personality: evidence of the Big One in the five-factor model. J. Res. Pers. 41, 1213–1233. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogden, J. (2007). Health psychology: A textbook. New York: Open University Press.

Google Scholar

Ogińska-Bulik, N., and Juczyński, Z. (2008). Właściwości osobowości sprzyjające chorobom somatycznym ‐ rola typu D (Personality traits contributing to somatic diseases development—role of type D personality). Psychoonkologia 12, 7–13.

Google Scholar

Perry, L. M., Hoerger, M., Molix, L. A., and Duberstein, P. R. (2020). A validation study of the mini-IPIP five-factor personality scale in adults with cancer. J. Pers. Assess. 102, 153–163. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2019.1644341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, K. -A., Osborne, R. H., Giles, G. G., Dite, G. S., Apicella, C., Hopper, J. L., et al. (2008). Psychosocial factors and survival of young women with breast cancer: a population-based prospective cohort study. J. Clin. Oncol. 26, 4666–4671. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2007.14.8718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinquart, M., and Duberstein, P. R. (2010). Depression and cancer mortality: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 40, 1797–1810. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709992285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, P., Hurley, S., Torres, M., Jackson, J., Boyd, P., and Chen, V. W. (2000). Use of coping strategies and breast cancer survival: results from the black/white cancer survival study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 152, 940–949. doi: 10.1093/aje/152.10.940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rochefort, C., Hoerger, M., Turiano, N. A., and Duberstein, P. (2018). Big Five personality and health in adults with and without cancer. J. Health Psychol. 24, 1494–1504. doi: 10.1177/1359105317753714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogoza, R., Cieciuch, J., and Strus, W. (2019a). A three-step procedure for analysis of circumplex models: an example of narcissism located within the Circumplex of Personality Metatraits. Pers. Individ. Differ. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.109775 (in press).

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogoza, R., Cieciuch, J., Strus, W., and Baran, T. (2019b). Seeking a common framework for research on narcissism: an attempt to integrate the different faces of narcissism within the Circumplex of Personality Metatraits. Eur. J. Personal. 33, 437–455. doi: 10.1002/per.2206

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schönemann, P. H. (1966). A generalized solution of the orthogonal procrustes problem. Psychometrika 31, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/BF02289451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, R., and Geyer, S. (1984). Social and psychological differences between cancer and noncancer patients: cause or consequence of the disease? Psychother. Psychosom. 41, 195–199. doi: 10.1159/000287809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Šmigelskas, K., Žemaitienė, N., Julkunen, J., and Kauhanen, J. (2015). Type A behavior pattern is not a predictor of premature mortality. Int. J. Behav. Med. 22, 161–169. doi: 10.1007/s12529-014-9435-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strus, W., and Cieciuch, J. (2017). Towards a synthesis of personality, temperament, motivation, emotion and mental health models within the Circumplex of Personality Metatraits. J. Res. Pers. 66, 70–95. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2016.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strus, W., and Cieciuch, J. (2019). Are the questionnaire and the psycho-lexical Big Twos the same? Towards an integration of personality structure within the Circumplex of Personality Metatraits. Int. J. Personal. Psychol. 5, 18–35. doi: 10.21827/ijpp.5.35594

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strus, W., Cieciuch, J., and Rowiński, T. (2014). The Circumplex of Personality Metatraits: a synthesizing model of personality based on the Big Five. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 18, 273–286. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Temoshok, L. R. (1987). Personality, coping style, emotion and cancer: towards an integrative model. Cancer Surv. 6, 545–567.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Watson, M., and Greer, S. (1983). Development of a questionnaire measure of emotional control. J. Psychosom. Res. 27, 299–305. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(83)90052-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, M., Haviland, J. S., Greer, S., Davidson, J., and Bliss, J. M. (1999). Influence of psychological response on survival in breast cancer: a population-based cohort study. Lancet 354, 1331–1336. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(98)11392-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, M., Guo, L., Zhu, Y., Guo, Y., Yv, S., Namassevayam, G., et al. (2019). Type C personality and depression among newly diagnosed breast cancer patients: the mediating role of sense of coherence. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 15, 3519–3529. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S230278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zawadzki, B. (2016). “Gamma i Delta w ujęciu Kołowego Modelu Metacech Osobowości a przekonania w zaburzeniach osobowości (Gamma and Delta in the perspective of the Circumplex of Personality Metratraits and beliefs in personality disorders)” in Wybrane metody i paradygmaty badawcze w psychologii (Selected research methods and paradigms in psychology). eds. A. Rynkiewicz, K. S. Jankowski, and W. Oniszczenko (Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe SCHOLAR), 203–220.

Google Scholar

Zawadzki, B. (2017). The location of personality disorders in the Circumplex of Personality Metatraits. Ann. Psychol. 20, 493–512. doi: 10.18290/rpsych.2017.20.2-7en

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Type C personality, Circumplex of Personality Metatraits, submissiveness, restricted affectivity, cancer-prone personality

Citation: Rymarczyk K, Turbacz A, Strus W and Cieciuch J (2020) Type C Personality: Conceptual Refinement and Preliminary Operationalization. Front. Psychol. 11:552740. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.552740

Received: 16 April 2020; Accepted: 20 August 2020;
Published: 16 September 2020.

Edited by:

Valentina Cazzato, Liverpool John Moores University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Izydorczyk Bernadetta, Jagiellonian University, Poland
Małgorzata Anna Basińska, Kazimierz Wielki University of Bydgoszcz, Poland

Copyright © 2020 Rymarczyk, Turbacz, Strus and Cieciuch. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Karolina Rymarczyk, ay5yeW1hcmN6eWtAdWtzdy5lZHUucGw=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.