Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 21 December 2020
Sec. Cultural Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Iowa Gambling Task, Somatic Marker Hypothesis, and Neuroeconomics: Rationality and Emotion in Decision Under Uncertainty View all 19 articles

Recollecting Cross-Cultural Evidences: Are Decision Makers Really Foresighted in Iowa Gambling Task?

\r\nWe-Kang Lee,We-Kang Lee1,2Ching-Jen Lin,Ching-Jen Lin3,4Li-Hua LiuLi-Hua Liu1Ching-Hung Lin,*Ching-Hung Lin3,4*Yao-Chu Chiu*Yao-Chu Chiu1*
  • 1Department of Psychology, Soochow University, Taipei, Taiwan
  • 2Shin Kong Wu Ho-Su Memorial Hospital Sleep Center, Taipei, Taiwan
  • 3Department of Psychology, Kaohsiung Medical University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan
  • 4Research Center for Nonlinear Analysis and Optimization, Kaohsiung Medical University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan

The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) has become a remarkable experimental paradigm of dynamic emotion decision making. In recent years, research has emphasized the “prominent deck B (PDB) phenomenon” among normal (control group) participants, in which they favor “bad” deck B with its high-frequency gain structure—a finding that is incongruent with the original IGT hypothesis concerning foresightedness. Some studies have attributed such performance inconsistencies to cultural differences. In the present review, 86 studies featuring data on individual deck selections were drawn from an initial sample of 958 IGT-related studies published from 1994 to 2017 for further investigation. The PDB phenomenon was found in 67.44% of the studies (58 of 86), and most participants were recorded as having adopted the “gain-stay loss-randomize” strategy to cope with uncertainty. Notably, participants in our sample of studies originated from 16 areas across North America, South America, Europe, Oceania, and Asia, and the findings suggest that the PDB phenomenon may be cross-cultural.

Introduction

In recent decades, the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT; Bechara et al., 1994) has gradually become a classic experimental paradigm of dynamic decision making (Dunn et al., 2006) and has even been used to clinically assess patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) dysfunction related to brain lesions (Bechara, 2007, 2016). The IGT is a dynamic task that simulates the uncertain conditions of a real-life situation. In the task, four decks are displayed with a pseudorandomized and symmetrical gain-loss schedule that is not disclosed to the participants. Based on the schedule developed by Bechara et al. (1994), decks A and B are defined as “bad decks” due to their long-term disadvantageous outcome despite a large gain (e.g., $100) in each selection, while decks C and D are scheduled with a small gain (e.g., $50) in each selection and defined as “good decks” due to their long-term advantageous outcome. Furthermore, decks A and C contain five times as many losses, while decks B and D contain an average of only one loss for every 10 trials. Compared to patients with vmPFC lesions, Bechara et al. (1994) theorized that control participants would form a “somatic marker” (Damasio, 1994) when making deck selections and that the gut feeling related to the somatic marker would lead to foresighted and rational decision making—that is, choosing “good decks” (C and D) in the IGT. Moreover, a series of studies by Bechara et al. (1994; 1997; 1998; 1999; 2000) replicated these results.

However, Dunn et al. (2006) undertook a review of IGT-related studies and noted several possible issues, including the possibility that the inconsistencies identified between prior studies’ findings were due to variability of the normal (control) participants. Recently, though, others have shown that the IGT participants in control groups typically favor bad deck B not only more than deck A, but also more than good decks C or D (Wilder et al., 1998; Toplak et al., 2005; Fernie and Tunney, 2006; Lin et al., 2007, 2013; Steingroever et al., 2013), which is inconsistent with the basic assumption proposed by Bechara et al. (1994). This finding has been defined as the “prominent deck B (PDB) phenomenon” (Lin et al., 2007), and researchers have inferred that the selection preference is due to a “gain-loss frequency effect”—that is, like good deck D, bad deck B features nine gains and one loss across 10 trials, in terms of net value (Lin et al., 2007; Chiu et al., 2008). The PDB phenomenon has been acknowledged as a critical issue in IGT-related research (Chiu et al., 2018), yet, few studies (Chiu et al., 2012; Steingroever et al., 2013) have fully examined whether it exists in relation to prior IGT-related findings.

Some researchers have attributed a preference for a particular IGT deck with high-frequency gain to cultural differences (Ekhtiari et al., 2009; Bakos et al., 2010). For example, Bakos et al. (2010) found that culture or birth country could partially influence participants’ behavior in the IGT. However, a similar finding regarding high-frequency gain preference in the IGT was also observed in a sample of Iranian participants. Ekhtiari et al. (2009) attributed the demonstration of the phenomenon in this example to the restriction on gambling within Islamic culture and the country’s relatively late development of a bourgeois class.

Chiu et al. (2012) undertook a review of the PDB phenomenon and found that out of 16 studies, 13 (81.25%) obtained results for individual deck selections (i.e., the mean selection number with respect to each deck was presented in the study) that demonstrated the PDB phenomenon. Steingroever et al. (2013) published the results of two reviews related to the IGT: the first examined 17 studies that utilized data regarding selections from four decks (479 normal participants in total); the second review examined 39 groups and the corresponding mean selections from good and bad decks (1,427 normal participants in total). The research team then sent emails requesting the raw data. After receiving responses from seven authors, the researchers collected data from 162 normal participants and analyzed the 8 data sets. Ultimately, both reviews concluded that the normal participants had a preference for low-frequency loss deck B, and the selections persisted until the end of the IGT (Steingroever et al., 2013). The issue of cultural difference, however, was not clearly specified in these review studies.

Following the findings of Chiu et al. (2012) and Steingroever et al. (2013), but in contrast to the observations made by Ekhtiari et al. (2009) and Bakos et al. (2010), we hypothesized that the PDB phenomenon (i.e., a preference of normal participants for the high-frequency gain bad deck B in the IGT) exists cross-culturally. That is, cultural difference may not be a critical factor for interpreting decision-making behavior in the IGT. To test this hypothesis, we reviewed past studies that were identified through a PubMed search of the MEDLINE biomedical database and further integrated the findings of review studies (Chiu et al., 2012; Steingroever et al., 2013) to examine the geographical distribution of IGT-related studies that found individual deck selections in the IGT and plot a global map of the PDB phenomenon.

Methods

Procedure

A search for IGT-related studies dating from 1994 to March 31, 2017 was performed on the MEDLINE biomedical database using the PubMed search engine and the keywords “Iowa gambling” and “Bechara card task.” We found 945 articles that featured “Iowa gambling” and 18 articles using “Bechara card task” as keywords. Once we had excluded 12 overlapping IGT-related studies, 951 IGT-related studies were individually reviewed.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

We ultimately identified 140 articles that presented deck decisions in the main text, tables, or figures. Regarding the version of the IGT, testing procedure consistencies, and the ages of participants, we excluded 22 studies that used revised versions of the IGT (e.g., the Hungry Donkey task, the inverted IGT, the simple IGT, the net-value IGT, and the Soochow Gambling Task), 9 studies that manipulated testing procedures, 9 studies that did not present the control group data, 9 studies that included participants younger than 17 years of age, 7 studies that presented the results of fewer than 100 trials, 3 studies that only presented representative data, and 2 studies in which the data for deck selection were unclear even though each selection of every participant was presented. Consequently, 79 studies that used the original IGT’s gain-loss structure and presented data for individual deck choices were further analyzed.

Table 1 presents the deck selection data of control participants in 100 IGT trials (namely, over 100 IGT trials were not depicted here) that we extracted from these 79 studies. For studies that presented figures without precise means and standard deviations, we measured and estimated the values based on the scale of the figures.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Data of normal participants in Iowa Gambling Task (IGT)-related studies from PubMed search, which showed individual deck selections.

To increase the integrity of reviewing IGT-related studies, we investigated the studies originally reviewed by Chiu et al. (2012) and Steingroever et al. (2013) that had focused on the issue of high-frequency gain deck preference in the IGT (see Table 2). Six studies were selected after we excluded repeated articles from the database mentioned above. The original selection data of 100 trials in a concurrent IGT condition published in Chiu et al. (2012) were also obtained and included in the present research. In total, there were seven studies sourced from Chiu et al. (2012) and Steingroever et al. (2013).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Data of normal participants in IGT-related studies included in Chiu et al. (2012) and Steingroever et al. (2013).

Study Selection and Data Extraction

Two authors (W-KL and C-JL) independently retrieved the studies that presented individual deck selections (i.e., in the main text, tables, or figures). They independently reviewed each study to extract the data and measured the average selection numbers (i.e., based on the scale of the figures). Any disagreement with respect to the process of study selection or data extraction was resolved through consensus via repeated measurements and discussion. All average numbers of choice obtained through measurement by two researchers were controlled under the difference ≤1 selection approach.

Data Analysis

Data analysis was performed on a total of 86 studies, 79 of which were retrieved from the MEDLINE biomedical database and 7 from the 2 review studies noted above (Chiu et al., 2012; Steingroever et al., 2013). Notably, each experimental condition performed by normal participants in the 86 studies was considered as a single data set, even though there may in fact have been 2 (Fernie and Tunney, 2006; Zamarian et al., 2008; van den Bos et al., 2009, 2013; Carvalho et al., 2012; Tchanturia et al., 2012; Beitz et al., 2014; Evans and Hampson, 2015; Huang et al., 2015; Okdie et al., 2016; Piper et al., 2016), 3 (Zhang et al., 2015c), or 4 experimental conditions (Overman, 2004) in the original study (experimental conditions are marked in the note of Table 1). In total, 102 data sets obtained from 86 studies were subsequently analyzed.

To verify whether a “gain-stay loss-randomize” decision strategy was demonstrated in the different data sources, we conducted a decks-by-groups repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) using IBM SPSS Statistics (version 22) and analyzed individual deck selection data. To visualize whether the PDB phenomenon is cross-cultural, all studies that presented each of four deck selections obtained from the database search and review studies were marked on an IGT global map according to the source and origin of the study’s participants (Figure 1). We defined selections of bad deck B equal to 25 or more (i.e., higher than the randomized choices of 100 trials, or chance level), as being a “PDB phenomenon.”

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) global map. The figure illustrates the geographical distribution of IGT-related studies that showed individual deck selections. Red circles indicate studies demonstrating the PDB phenomenon, green circles indicate studies that support the original IGT assumptions, and gray circles indicate studies that were unclassifiable. AR, Argentina; AT, Austria; AU, Australia; BR, Brazil; CA, Canada; CL, Chile; CN, China; CZ, Czechia; DE, Germany; ES, Spain; FR, France; GB, United Kingdom; IN, India; IT, Italy; JP, Japan; KR, South Korea; NL, Netherlands; NZ, New Zealand; TW, Taiwan; and US, United States of America. Adapted from “Robinson projection, national borders, areas grouped” (https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Wikipedia:Blank_maps#/media/File:BlankMap- World.svg) in the public domain.

This standard was strictly applied while we were identifying whether the PDB phenomenon existed in the 86 studies—specifically, every experimental condition performed by normal participants had to consistently exhibit the PDB phenomenon, even in studies that featured more than one experimental condition (as discussed above). As a result, there were four studies (Overman, 2004; Zamarian et al., 2008; van den Bos et al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2015c) that we were unable to classify due to the phenomenon existing inconsistently across different experimental conditions: the PDB phenomenon existed in only three of the four experimental conditions in Overman (2004), one of the three experimental conditions in Zhang et al. (2015c), and one of the two experimental conditions in Zamarian et al. (2008) and van den Bos et al.(2009; see Table 1 and Figure 1). Although unclassifiable in the global map (see gray circles in Figure 1), the data sets from the four studies were still included in the following analysis.

Results

The repeated measures ANOVA showed that the interaction effect of groups (studies retrieved from the database search and review studies) and decks was nonsignificant, F (2.377, 237.73) = 0.445, p = 0.676 (Greenhouse–Geisser correction). The main effect of the decks was significant, F (2.377, 237.73) = 39.141, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.281, but that of the groups was not, F (1, 100) = 0.123, p = 0.726. In short, the results indicated no difference between the data obtained from the MEDLINE database and the review studies (Chiu et al., 2012; Steingroever et al., 2013).

As there was no difference between the two data sources, we combined the data obtained from the two sources (86 studies in total) and further conducted a repeated measures ANOVA to test for differences between decks. The results showed a significant difference with respect to the selections of individual decks, F (2.379, 240.293) = 171.702, p < 0.001, and η2 = 0.63 (Greenhouse–Geisser correction). The selection of deck B was significantly higher than that of decks A, p < 0.001, and C, p < 0.001. Moreover, the selection of deck C was higher than that of deck A, and the selection of deck D was higher than those of all other decks, ps < 0.001. These results suggest that the PDB phenomenon was common in the reviewed studies (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean number of card selections in 86 IGT-related studies. The figure was produced by averaging the numbers of the four decks chosen across the 86 IGT-related studies. Selections of deck B were relatively higher than those of decks A and C, demonstrating that the PDB phenomenon was present. This finding is consistent with those obtained in a growing number of other IGT-related researches.

Combining the results of the studies from the MEDLINE database and the review studies, we found that 67.44% (58 of 86) featured a selection of the disadvantageous deck B ≥ 25 times, and this preference corresponded to our definition of the PDB phenomenon (detailed above). As shown in Figure 1, the normal participants in these 58 studies originated from 16 regions of North America, South America, Europe, Oceania, and Asia: specifically, Argentina, Australia, Brazil, Canada, Chile, China, Germany, Israel, Italy, Japan, Netherlands, South Korea, Spain, Taiwan, United Kingdom, and the United States.

Discussion

Most IGT-related studies have used the calculation (C + D) − (A + B) to define decision-making performance. Correspondingly, the basic assumption of the IGT (Bechara et al., 1994, 1997) posited that normal (control group) participants could perform advantageously and make rational decisions guided by implicit emotion, in contrast to participants who were unable to access an emotional system due to a vmPFC lesion. However, the present article found that in 67.44% (58 out of 86) of the IGT-related studies that showed individual deck selection data, a preference for the disadvantageous deck B was observed. The participants in these 58 studies originated from 16 different areas across North America, South America, Europe, Asia, and Oceania. Therefore, we infer that the PDB phenomenon in the IGT is cross-cultural.

Individual and Cultural Issues in the IGT

A prior critical review article (Dunn et al., 2006) reiterated Bechara and Damasio’s (2002) finding that about 20% of normal participants performed poorly in the IGT. In fact, Bechara and Damasio (2002) showed that 37% of normal (control group) participants performed within the range of vmPFC patients, referring to the criterion of the net score (C + D) − (A + B) < 10. The present research showed that in more than 60% of sample studies, normal participants consistently favored the disadvantageous deck B. This PDB phenomenon is evidently different from the results obtained by Bechara and Damasio (2002).

Previous IGT-related studies have attributed participants’ preference for decks with a high-frequency gain (decks B and D) to cultural issues. For example, Bakos et al. (2010) postulated that a preference for deck B only existed in certain cultures and further investigated decision-making differences between Brazilians and Americans. In their study, 17% of the Brazilian participants in an IGT were categorized as “normal decision makers,” compared to the 60% of American participants who were categorized as normal, according to the measure (C + D) − (A + B) > 18, a criterion proposed by Denburg et al. (2005). These results suggested that Americans perform better than Brazilians in the IGT; Bakos et al. (2010) posited that the difference might relate to capitalism in the United States making the daily lives of Americans much more reliant on their ability to manage financial issues compared to Brazilians. However, the study did not clarify whether a preference for decks with a high-frequency gain existed in both Americans and Brazilians.

Similar to our study, Ekhtiari et al. (2009) performed an analysis of individual decks and found that Iranian participants favored the high-frequency gain decks B and D. The researchers attributed the phenomenon to two possible causes: (1) the limitations on gambling under Islamic law meant that Iranian participants were unclear about or lacking gambling concepts, which further affected their decision-making performance in the IGT according to frequency-based valuations; and (2) the late development of a bourgeois class in Iran, and therefore of concepts such as land ownership and work ownership, meant that the country’s workers lacked long-term decision-making experience (Ekhtiari et al., 2009). In contrast to the cultural difference perspective, we suggested a cross-cultural preference for high-frequency gain decks B and D in the IGT. This is supported by our confirmation that the phenomenon exists in 16 areas across North America, South America, Europe, Asia, and Oceania.

However, although our sample of studies showed that the PDB phenomenon existed cross-culturally, the finding was limited by the lack of analysis regarding cultural factors. Future studies could further analyze the performance of normal participants under different cultural factors (e.g., Western or Eastern cultural contexts) and examine whether the PDB phenomenon exists universally.

Methodological Issue in Iowa Gambling Task-Related Studies

Furthermore, of the 951 IGT-related studies originally sourced through the MEDLINE database, only 140 showed individual deck data, and most of the remaining 811 studies used the calculation (C + D) − (A + B) to differentiate the performances of clinical versus control participants. It is possible that the scoring method may have obscured the existence of the PDB phenomenon in control participants (Chiu and Lin, 2007; Lin et al., 2007, 2013; Steingroever et al., 2013) and further neglect differences between the preferences of clinical versus control participants for individual decks. Consequently, researchers may be missing opportunities to observe differences regarding more specific decision-making patterns.

In the present research, we further analyzed the 86 studies that featured individual deck data according to the criterion (C + D) − (A + B) < 10, as used by Bechara and Damasio (2002). According to this analysis, 45.35% (39 out of 86) of the studies showed that normal participants performed within the range of vmPFC patients (see Supplementary Table 1). Additionally, even more studies (67.44%, 58 out of 86) demonstrated that the normal participants consistently preferred the disadvantageous deck B. These findings significantly challenge the basic assumption of the IGT and suggest that evidence of PDB phenomenon is obscured by the use of the measure (C + D) − (A + B). We therefore recommend that future studies should investigate and compare the individual deck selections of clinical participants based on the consistent performance of the control participants.

A New Raising Perspective: Gain–Loss Frequency

The preference for bad deck B shown by normal participants in the IGT was first demonstrated by Wilder et al. (1998), and the phenomenon has since been documented by other researchers (Toplak et al., 2005; Fernie and Tunney, 2006; Lin et al., 2007, 2013; Takano et al., 2010; Steingroever et al., 2013). Prior studies have defined participants’ preferences for bad deck B and good deck D in the IGT as the “gain-loss frequency effect” (Lin et al., 2007; Chiu et al., 2008), as the preference is associated with the high-frequency gain structure (i.e., nine gains, one loss) of both bad deck B and good deck D. The observed preference also implies that, under uncertainty conditions, control participants will use a “gain-stay loss-randomize” strategy, meaning that the probability of choosing the same deck will increase when participants face continuous gains, whereas the choice will be randomized when they face loss (Chiu et al., 2008; Worthy et al., 2013b; Lin et al., 2016). This strategy has been employed in recent IGT-related model studies (Worthy et al., 2013b; Lin et al., 2016).

Notably, the findings of our research depart from the original IGT study by Bechara et al. (1994) who proposed that normal (control group) participants would form a “somatic marker” (Damasio, 1994) when experiencing the gains and losses in the IGT and gradually develop a sensitivity to the long-term outcome—that is, preferring advantageous decks C and D and avoiding disadvantageous decks A and B. However, other studies (Wilder et al., 1998; Toplak et al., 2005; Fernie and Tunney, 2006; Lin et al., 2007, 2013; Takano et al., 2010; Steingroever et al., 2013) and the current findings have failed to replicate their results obtained in relation to normal participants. The present study also supports the argument that the PDB phenomenon should be evaluated in contemporary IGT-related studies given the apparent inconsistency with respect to the original IGT hypothesis (Chiu et al., 2018). In other words, the hypothesis proposed in the original IGT study should be carefully reconsidered and revised.

Conclusion

The present review found that in over 60% of IGT studies, most normal (control group) participants favored the disadvantageous deck B and consistently applied a gain–loss frequency strategy. These findings are incongruent with the original inference made by Bechara and Damasio (2002), Ekhtiari et al. (2009), and Bakos et al. (2010) that the poor performance of normal participants was due to individual and cultural differences. The PDB phenomenon and the influence of gain–loss frequency in the IGT might be obscured by the analysis and presentation methodology being principally based on the net score measure (C + D) − (A + B). Considering the present integrative review and analysis of 958 studies, we conclude that gain–loss frequency could be a cross-cultural factor during decision making under dynamic-uncertain conditions.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the article/Supplementary Material.

Author Contributions

Y-CC initiated this research topic and C-HL focused on refining the issue. Y-CC and C-HL constructed the research strategy and developed its main structure. Y-CC, C-HL, and W-KL undertook the literature review and defined the research database. Y-CC, C-HL, and W-KL developed the idea of mapping the geographical distribution of IGT-related studies for this manuscript, and W-KL created the corresponding artwork. W-KL completed the first round of the literature data collection, defined the categorization criteria and data administration, and drafted the preliminary manuscript. C-JL completed the second round of literature data collection, redefined the categorization criteria, and finalized the data re-categorization, as well as providing some interpretation. L-HL created the initial preliminary uncompleted draft in Chinese. W-KL, C-JL, C-HL, and Y-CC undertook several rounds of discussion, and all authors finalized and approved the manuscript.

Funding

C-HL’s work was supported in part by NSYSU-KMU JOINT RESEARCH PROJECT (#NSYSUKMU 109-I003).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Special thanks to the Charlesworth Group for providing English-language editing services on this manuscript. We are deeply appreciative to Miss Ya-Hsiu Tsai for her great assistance to the W-KL and C-JL authors in relation to the categorization criteria and triple checking some of the source literature. We sincerely appreciate both reviewers’ kind and careful comments, which helped us better define our position, more precisely pinpoint cultural issues concerning the IGT, and make this article more accessible for the general readership.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.537219/full#supplementary-material

References

Adida, M., Jollant, F., Clark, L., Besnier, N., Guillaume, S., Kaladjian, A., et al. (2011). Trait-related decision-making impairment in the three phases of bipolar disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 357–365. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahn, W. Y., Busemeyer, J. R., Wagenmakers, E. J., and Stout, J. C. (2008). Comparison of decision learning models using the generalization criterion method. Cogn. Sci. 32, 1376–1402. doi: 10.1080/03640210802352992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alameda-Bailen, J. R., Paíno-Quesada, S., and Mogedas Valladares, A. I. (2012). [Decision making in cannabis users]. Adicciones 24, 161–172.

Google Scholar

Alameda-Bailen, J. R., Salguero-Alcañiz, M. P., Merchan-Clavellino, A., and Paíno-Quesada, S. (2014). [Cognitive mechanisms in risky decision-making in cannabis users]. Adicciones 26, 146–158.

Google Scholar

Alarcon, D., Amian, J. G., and Sanchez-Medina, J. A. (2015). Enhancing emotion-based learning in decision-making under uncertainty. Psicothema 27, 368–373. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2015.45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakos, D. S., Denburg, N., Fonseca, R. P., and Parente, M. A. D. M. P. (2010). A cultural study on decision making: performance differences on the Iowa gambling task between selected groups of Brazilians and Americans. Psychol. Neurosci. 3, 101–107. doi: 10.3922/j.psns.2010.1.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bark, R., Dieckmann, S., Bogerts, B., and Northoff, G. (2005). Deficit in decision making in catatonic schizophrenia: an exploratory study. Psychiatry Res. 134, 131–141. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechara, A. (2007). Iowa Gambling Task Professional Manual. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources,Inc.

Google Scholar

Bechara, A. (2016). Iowa Gambling Task, Version 2 Professional Manual. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

Google Scholar

Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., and Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50, 7–15. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechara, A., and Damasio, H. (2002). Decision-making and addiction (part I): impaired activation of somatic states in substance dependent individuals when pondering decisions with negative future consequences. Neuropsychologia 40, 1675–1689. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00015-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Damasio, A. R., and Lee, G. P. (1999). Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. J. Neurosci. 19, 5473–5481. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.19-13-05473.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., and Anderson, S. W. (1998). Dissociation Of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 18, 428–437. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.18-01-00428.1998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., and Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 275, 1293–1295. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechara, A., Tranel, D., and Damasio, H. (2000). Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain 123(Pt 11), 2189–2202. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beitz, K. M., Salthouse, T. A., and Davis, H. P. (2014). Performance on the Iowa gambling task: from 5 to 89 years of age. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 1677–1689. doi: 10.1037/a0035823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besnard, J., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Chauvire, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., and Le Gall, D. (2015). Decision-making of prefrontal patients with the Iowa gambling task: unexpected spared performances and preliminary evidence for the need of alternative measures. Clin. Neuropsychol. 29, 509–521. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2015.1050458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besnard, J., Le Gall, D., Chauvire, V., Aubin, G., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., and Allain, P. (2016). Discrepancy between social and nonsocial decision-making under uncertainty following prefrontal lobe damage: the impact of an interactionist approach. Soc. Neurosci. 12, 430–447. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2016.1182066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, E. C., Hack, S. M., Gold, J. M., Carpenter, W. T. Jr., Fischer, B. A., Prentice, K. P., et al. (2015). Integrating frequency and magnitude information in decision-making in schizophrenia: an account of patient performance on the Iowa gambling task. J. Psychiatr. Res. 66-67, 16–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buelow, M. T., and Suhr, J. A. (2014). Risky decision making in smoking and nonsmoking college students: examination of Iowa Gambling Task performance by deck type selections. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 3, 38–44. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2012.691065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bull, P. N., Tippett, L. J., and Addis, D. R. (2015). Decision making in healthy participants on the Iowa gambling task: new insights from an operant approach. Front. Psychol. 6:391. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardoso, C. D. O., Branco, L. D., Cotrena, C., Kristensen, C. H., Schneider Bakos, D. D., and Fonseca, R. P. (2014). The impact of frontal and cerebellar lesions on decision making: evidence from the Iowa gambling task. Front. Neurosci. 8:61. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caroselli, J. S., Hiscock, M., Scheibel, R. S., and Ingram, F. (2006). The simulated gambling paradigm applied to young adults: an examination of university students’ performance. Appl. Neuropsychol. 13, 203–212. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an1304_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carvalho, J. C., Cardoso Cde, O., Shneider-Bakos, D., Kristensen, C. H., and Fonseca, R. P. (2012). The effect of age on decision making according to the Iowa gambling task. Span. J. Psychol. 15, 480–486. doi: 10.5209/rev_sjop.2012.v15.n2.38858

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiu, Y. C., Huang, J. T., Duann, J. R., and Lin, C. H. (2018). Editorial: twenty years after the iowa gambling task: rationality, emotion, and decision-making. Front. Psychol. 8:2353. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiu, Y. C., and Lin, C. H. (2007). Is deck C an advantageous deck in the Iowa gambling task? Behav. Brain Funct. 3:37. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-3-37

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiu, Y. C., Lin, C. H., and Huang, J. T. (2012). “Prominent deck B phenomenon: are decision-makers sensitive to long-term outcome in the Iowa gambling task?,” in Psychology of Gambling: New Research, ed. A. Cavanna (New York, NY: Nova), 93–118.

Google Scholar

Chiu, Y. C., Lin, C. H., Huang, J. T., Lin, S., Lee, P. L., and Hsieh, J. C. (2008). Immediate gain is long-term loss: are there foresighted decision makers in the Iowa gambling task? Behav. Brain Funct. 4:13. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-4-13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cotrena, C., Branco, L. D., Zimmermann, N., Cardoso, C. O., Grassi-Oliveira, R., and Fonseca, R. P. (2014). Impaired decision-making after traumatic brain injury: the Iowa gambling task. Brain Inj. 28, 1070–1075. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2014.896943

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York, NY: G. P. Putnam’s Sons.

Google Scholar

Denburg, N. L., Tranel, D., and Bechara, A. (2005). The ability to decide advantageously declines prematurely in some normal older persons. Neuropsychologia 43, 1099–1106. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, B. D., Dalgleish, T., and Lawrence, A. D. (2006). The somatic marker hypothesis: a critical evaluation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 239–271. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekhtiari, H., Behzadi, A., Dehghani, M., Jannati, A., and Mokri, A. (2009). Prefer a cash slap in your face over credit for halva. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 4, 534–542.

Google Scholar

Escartin, G., Junque, C., Juncadella, M., Gabarros, A., de Miquel, M. A., and Rubio, F. (2012). Decision-making impairment on the Iowa Gambling Task after endovascular coiling or neurosurgical clipping for ruptured anterior communicating artery aneurysm. Neuropsychology 26, 172–180. doi: 10.1037/a0024336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, K. L., and Hampson, E. (2015). Sex differences on prefrontally-dependent cognitive tasks. Brain Cogn. 93, 42–53. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernie, G., and Tunney, R. J. (2006). Some decks are better than others: the effect of reinforcer type and task instructions on learning in the Iowa gambling task. Brain Cogn. 60, 94–102. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fum, D., Napoli, A., and Stocco, A. (2008). “Somatic markers and frequency effects: does emotion really play a role on decision making in the iowa gambling task?,” in Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Washington, DC.

Google Scholar

Gansler, D. A., Jerram, M. W., Vannorsdall, T. D., and Schretlen, D. J. (2011). Comparing alternative metrics to assess performance on the Iowa gambling task. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 33, 1040–1048. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2011.596820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gansler, D. A., Jerram, M. W., Vannorsdall, T. D., and Schretlen, D. J. (2012). Hierarchical organization of cortical morphology of decision-making when deconstructing Iowa gambling task performance in healthy adults. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 18, 585–594. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gescheidt, T., Czekoova, K., Urbanek, T., Marecek, R., Mikl, M., Kubikova, R., et al. (2012). Iowa Gambling Task in patients with early-onset Parkinson’s disease: strategy analysis. Neurol. Sci. 33, 1329–1335. doi: 10.1007/s10072-012-1086-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawthorne, M. J., and Pierce, B. H. (2015). Disadvantageous deck selection in the iowa gambling task: the effect of cognitive load. Eur. J. Psychol. 11, 335–348. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v11i2.931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, X., Zheng, L., and Li, X. (2015). Impaired decision making is associated with poor inhibition control in nonpathological lottery gamblers. J. Gambl. Stud. 31, 1617–1632. doi: 10.1007/s10899-014-9509-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hori, H., Yoshimura, R., Katsuki, A., Atake, K., and Nakamura, J. (2014). Relationships between brain-derived neurotrophic factor, clinical symptoms, and decision-making in chronic schizophrenia: data from the Iowa gambling task. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:417. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horstmann, A., Villringer, A., and Neumann, J. (2012). Iowa gambling task: there is more to consider than long-term outcome. Using a linear equation model to disentangle the impact of outcome and frequency of gains and losses. Front. Neurosci. 6:61. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y. H., Wood, S., Berger, D. E., and Hanoch, Y. (2015). Age differences in experiential and deliberative processes in unambiguous and ambiguous decision making. Psychol. Aging 30, 675–687. doi: 10.1037/pag0000038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jollans, L., Whelan, R., Venables, L., Turnbull, O. H., Cella, M., and Dymond, S. (2017). Computational EEG modelling of decision making under ambiguity reveals spatio-temporal dynamics of outcome evaluation. Behav. Brain Res. 321, 28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.12.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kester, H. M., Sevy, S., Yechiam, E., Burdick, K. E., Cervellione, K. L., and Kumra, S. (2006). Decision-making impairments in adolescents with early-onset schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 85, 113–123. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.02.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. T., Lee, K. U., and Lee, S. J. (2009). Deficit in decision-making in chronic, stable schizophrenia: from a reward and punishment perspective. Psychiatry Investig. 6, 26–33. doi: 10.4306/pi.2009.6.1.26

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. T., Sohn, H., and Jeong, J. (2011). Delayed transition from ambiguous to risky decision making in alcohol dependence during Iowa Gambling Task. Psychiatry Res. 190, 297–303. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. T., Sohn, H., Kim, S., Oh, J., Peterson, B. S., and Jeong, J. (2012). Disturbances of motivational balance in chronic schizophrenia during decision-making tasks. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 66, 573–581. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2012.02403.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kloeters, S., Bertoux, M., O’Callaghan, C., Hodges, J. R., and Hornberger, M. (2013). Money for nothing - Atrophy correlates of gambling decision making in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage Clin. 2, 263–272. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavin, C., San Martin, R., and Rosales Jubal, E. (2014). Pupil dilation signals uncertainty and surprise in a learning gambling task. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:218. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Berre, A. P., Rauchs, G., La Joie, R., Mezenge, F., Boudehent, C., Vabret, F., et al. (2014). Impaired decision-making and brain shrinkage in alcoholism. Eur. Psychiatry 29, 125–133. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2012.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, W. K., Su, Y. A., Song, T. J., Chiu, Y. C., and Lin, C. H. (2014). Are normal decision-makers sensitive to changes in value contrast under uncertainty? Evidence from the Iowa gambling task. PLoS One 9:e101878. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y., Kim, Y. T., Seo, E., Park, O., Jeong, S. H., Kim, S. H., et al. (2007). Dissociation of emotional decision-making from cognitive decision-making in chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 152, 113–120. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeGris, J., Toplak, M., and Links, P. S. (2014). Affective decision making in women with borderline personality disorder. J. Pers. Disord. 28, 698–719. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2014_28_140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, C. H., Chiu, Y. C., Lee, P. L., and Hsieh, J. C. (2007). Is deck B a disadvantageous deck in the Iowa gambling task? Behav. Brain Funct. 3:16. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-3-16

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, C. H., Lin, Y. K., Song, T. J., Huang, J. T., and Chiu, Y. C. (2016). A simplified model of choice behavior under uncertainty. Front. Psychol. 7:1201. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, C. H., Song, T. J., Chen, Y. Y., Lee, W. K., and Chiu, Y. C. (2013). Reexamining the validity and reliability of the clinical version of the iowa gambling task: evidence from a normal subject group. Front. Psychol. 4:220. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, S., Zang, Y., Cheung, V., and Chan, C. C. (2015). Importance of punishment frequency in the Iowa gambling task: an fMRI study. Brain Imaging Behav. 9, 899–909. doi: 10.1007/s11682-015-9353-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martino, D. J., Bucay, D., Butman, J. T., and Allegri, R. F. (2007). Neuropsychological frontal impairments and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 152, 121–128. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martino, D. J., Strejilevich, S. A., Torralva, T., and Manes, F. (2011). Decision making in euthymic bipolar I and bipolar II disorders. Psychol. Med. 41, 1319–1327. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuzawa, D., Shirayama, Y., Niitsu, T., Hashimoto, K., and Iyo, M. (2015). Deficits in emotion based decision-making in schizophrenia; a new insight based on the Iowa gambling task. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 52–59. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, M., Sheridan, M., Cardoos, S. L., and Hinshaw, S. P. (2013). Impaired decision-making as a young adult outcome of girls diagnosed with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in childhood. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 19, 110–114. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000975

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogedas Valladares, A. I., and Alameda-Bailen, J. R. (2011). [Decision-making in drug-dependent patients]. Adicciones 23, 277–287.

Google Scholar

North, N. T., and O’Carroll, R. E. (2001). Decision making in patients with spinal cord damage: afferent feedback and the somatic marker hypothesis. Neuropsychologia 39, 521–524. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00107-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Northoff, G., Grimm, S., Boeker, H., Schmidt, C., Bermpohl, F., Heinzel, A., et al. (2006). Affective judgment and beneficial decision making: ventromedial prefrontal activity correlates with performance in the Iowa gambling task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 572–587. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Carroll, R. E., and Papps, B. P. (2003). Decision making in humans: the effect of manipulating the central noradrenergic system. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 74, 376–378. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.74.3.376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okdie, B. M., Buelow, M. T., and Bevelhymer-Rangel, K. (2016). It’s all in how you think about it: construal level and the Iowa gambling task. Front. Neurosci. 10:2. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Overman, W. H. (2004). Sex differences in early childhood, adolescence, and adulthood on cognitive tasks that rely on orbital prefrontal cortex. Brain Cogn. 55, 134–147. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00279-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pedersen, A., Goder, R., Tomczyk, S., and Ohrmann, P. (2017). Risky decision-making under risk in schizophrenia: a deliberate choice? J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 56, 57–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penolazzi, B., Leone, L., and Russo, P. M. (2013). Individual differences and decision making: when the lure effect of gain is a matter of size. PLoS One 8:e58946. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petry, N. M. (2001). Substance abuse, pathological gambling, and impulsiveness. Drug Alcohol Depend. 63, 29–38. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(00)00188-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petry, N. M., Bickel, W. K., and Arnett, M. (1998). Shortened time horizons and insensitivity to future consequences in heroin addicts. Addiction 93, 729–738. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1998.9357298.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piper, B., Mueller, S. T., Talebzadeh, S., and Ki, M. J. (2016). Evaluation of the validity of the psychology experiment building language tests of vigilance, auditory memory, and decision making. PeerJ 4:e1772. doi: 10.7717/peerj.1772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reavis, R., and Overman, W. H. (2001). Adult sex differences on a decision-making task previously shown to depend on the orbital prefrontal cortex. Behav. Neurosci. 115, 196–206. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.1.196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ritter, L. M., Meador-Woodruff, J. H., and Dalack, G. W. (2004). Neurocognitive measures of prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 68, 65–73. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00086-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodriguez-Sanchez, J. M., Crespo-Facorro, B., Perez-Iglesias, R., Gonzalez-Blanch, C., Alvarez-Jimenez, M., Llorca, J., et al. (2005). Prefrontal cognitive functions in stabilized first-episode patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a dissociation between dorsolateral and orbitofrontal functioning. Schizophr. Res. 77, 279–288. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, C. J., Beninger, R. J., and Smith, C. T. (2014). Post learning sleep improves cognitive-emotional decision-making: evidence for a ‘deck B sleep effect’ in the Iowa Gambling Task. PLoS One 9:e112056. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, C. J., Smith, C. T., MacDonald, K. J., and Beninger, R. J. (2016). Ventromedial prefrontal theta activity during rapid eye movement sleep is associated with improved decision-making on the Iowa gambling task. Behav. Neurosci. 130, 271–280. doi: 10.1037/bne0000123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sevy, S., Burdick, K. E., Visweswaraiah, H., Abdelmessih, S., Lukin, M., Yechiam, E., et al. (2007). Iowa gambling task in schizophrenia: a review and new data in patients with schizophrenia and co-occurring cannabis use disorders. Schizophr. Res. 92, 74–84. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shurman, B., Horan, W. P., and Nuechterlein, K. H. (2005). Schizophrenia patients demonstrate a distinctive pattern of decision-making impairment on the Iowa gambling task. Schizophr. Res. 72, 215–224. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smart, C. M., and Krawitz, A. (2015). The impact of subjective cognitive decline on Iowa Gambling Task performance. Neuropsychology 29, 971–987. doi: 10.1037/neu0000204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steingroever, H., Wetzels, R., Horstmann, A., Neumann, J., and Wagenmakers, E. J. (2013). Performance of healthy participants on the Iowa gambling task. Psychol. Assess. 25, 180–193. doi: 10.1037/a0029929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takano, Y., Takahashi, N., Tanaka, D., and Hironaka, N. (2010). Big losses lead to irrational decision-making in gambling situations: relationship between deliberation and impulsivity. PLoS One 5:e9368. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tchanturia, K., Liao, P. C., Forcano, L., Fernandez-Aranda, F., Uher, R., Treasure, J., et al. (2012). Poor decision making in male patients with anorexia nervosa. Eur. Eat Disord. Rev. 20, 169–173. doi: 10.1002/erv.1154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomb, I., Hauser, M., Deldin, P., and Caramazza, A. (2002). Do somatic markers mediate decisions on the gambling task? Nat. Neurosci. 5, 1103–1104. doi: 10.1038/nn1102-1103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toplak, M. E., Jain, U., and Tannock, R. (2005). Executive and motivational processes in adolescents with Attention-Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Behav. Brain Funct. 1:8. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-1-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Upton, D. J., Kerestes, R., and Stout, J. C. (2012). Comparing the Iowa and soochow gambling tasks in opiate users. Front. Neurosci. 6:34. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Bos, R., Harteveld, M., and Stoop, H. (2009). Stress and decision-making in humans: performance is related to cortisol reactivity, albeit differently in men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1449–1458. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Bos, R., Homberg, J., and de Visser, L. (2013). A critical review of sex differences in decision-making tasks: focus on the Iowa gambling task. Behav. Brain Res. 238, 95–108. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Toor, D., Roozen, H. G., Evans, B. E., Rombout, L., Van de Wetering, B. J., and Vingerhoets, A. J. (2011). The effects of psychiatric distress, inhibition, and impulsivity on decision making in patients with substance use disorders: a matched control study. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 33, 161–168. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2010.493300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vassileva, J., Ahn, W. Y., Weber, K. M., Busemeyer, J. R., Stout, J. C., Gonzalez, R., et al. (2013). Computational modeling reveals distinct effects of HIV and history of drug use on decision-making processes in women. PLoS One 8:e68962. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visagan, R., Xiang, A., and Lamar, M. (2012). Comparison of deck- and trial-based approaches to advantageous decision making on the Iowa gambling task. Psychol. Assess. 24, 455–463. doi: 10.1037/a0025932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visser-Keizer, A. C., Westerhof-Evers, H. J., Gerritsen, M. J., van der Naalt, J., and Spikman, J. M. (2016). To fear is to gain? The role of fear recognition in risky decision making in TBI patients and healthy controls. PLoS One 11:e0166995. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166995

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viswanath, B., Janardhan Reddy, Y. C., Kumar, K. J., Kandavel, T., and Chandrashekar, C. R. (2009). Cognitive endophenotypes in OCD: a study of unaffected siblings of probands with familial OCD. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 33, 610–615. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilder, K. E., Weinberger, D. R., and Goldberg, T. E. (1998). Operant conditioning and the orbitofrontal cortex in schizophrenic patients: unexpected evidence for intact functioning. Schizophr. Res. 30, 169–174. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(97)00135-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolk, J., Sutterlin, S., Koch, S., Vogele, C., and Schulz, S. M. (2014). Enhanced cardiac perception predicts impaired performance in the Iowa Gambling Task in patients with panic disorder. Brain Behav. 4, 238–246. doi: 10.1002/brb3.206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Worthy, D. A., Hawthorne, M. J., and Otto, A. R. (2013a). Heterogeneity of strategy use in the Iowa gambling task: a comparison of win-stay/lose-shift and reinforcement learning models. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 364–371. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0324-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Worthy, D. A., Pang, B., and Byrne, K. A. (2013b). Decomposing the roles of perseveration and expected value representation in models of the Iowa gambling task. Front. Psychol. 4:640. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, R. J., Rakow, T., and Russo, R. (2017). Go for broke: the role of somatic states when asked to lose in the Iowa gambling task. Biol. Psychol. 123, 286–293. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yechiam, E., Telpaz, A., Krupenia, S., and Rafaeli, A. (2016). Unhappiness intensifies the avoidance of frequent losses while happiness overcomes it. Front. Psychol. 7:1703. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zamarian, L., Sinz, H., Bonatti, E., Gamboz, N., and Delazer, M. (2008). Normal aging affects decisions under ambiguity, but not decisions under risk. Neuropsychology 22, 645–657. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.5.645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Dong, Y., Ji, Y., Tao, R., Chen, X., Ye, J., et al. (2015a). Trait-related decision making impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder: evidence from decision making under ambiguity but not decision making under risk. Sci. Rep. 5:17312. doi: 10.1038/srep17312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Tang, J., Dong, Y., Ji, Y., Tao, R., Liang, Z., et al. (2015b). Similarities and differences in decision-making impairments between autism spectrum disorder and schizophrenia. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:259. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Wang, K., Zhu, C., Yu, F., and Chen, X. (2015c). Trait anxiety has effect on decision making under ambiguity but not decision making under risk. PLoS One 10:e0127189. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0127189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: iowa gambling task, IGT global map, foresight, prominent deck B phenomenon, gain-loss frequency, gain-stay loss-randomize decision strategy, cross-cultural, dynamic decision-making

Citation: Lee W-K, Lin C-J, Liu L-H, Lin C-H and Chiu Y-C (2020) Recollecting Cross-Cultural Evidences: Are Decision Makers Really Foresighted in Iowa Gambling Task? Front. Psychol. 11:537219. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.537219

Received: 22 February 2020; Accepted: 06 October 2020;
Published: 21 December 2020.

Edited by:

Keiko Ishii, Nagoya University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Mitsue Nagamine, Tokyo Institute of Technology, Japan
Marc Oliver Rieger, University of Trier, Germany

Copyright © 2020 Lee, Lin, Liu, Lin and Chiu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yao-Chu Chiu, eWFvY2h1QG1haWwyMDAwLmNvbS50dw==; Ching-Hung Lin, ZWFuZHk5MjRAZ21haWwuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.