Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 14 January 2021
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Students’ Motivation and Affection Profiles and Their Relation to Mathematics Achievement, Persistence, and Behaviors

  • 1Department of Psychology, Henan Normal University, Xinxiang, China
  • 2Department of Mathematics and Statistics, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, United States

Objective: We aimed to explore profiles of subgroups of United States students based on their motivational and affective characteristics and investigate the differences in math-related behaviors, persistence, and math achievement across profiles.

Method: We used 1,464 United States students (male 743 51%, female 721 49%, age 15.82 ± 0.28) from PISA 2012 United States data in our study. First, we employed latent profile analysis and secondary clustering to identify subgroups of students based on motivational (math self-concept, interest in math, perceived control, and instrumental motivation) and affective factors (math anxiety). Next, we used regression to compare differences in math behavior, persistence, and achievement among all identified subgroups.

Results: We found five distinct groups of students with different patterns of motivation and affection. The subgroup of students with the lowest math anxiety and the highest motivation levels showed the highest math achievement and levels of persistence. The groups with high math interest, math self-concept, and instrumental motivation showed the most frequent math-related behaviors.

Conclusions: Our findings reveal the complexity of the students’ motivational and affective profiles. Our findings are significant for teachers and educators to understand the diversity of students and provide theoretical and practical support for individualized and differentiated instruction.

Introduction

Mathematics underperformance is a critical issue among students in the United States (Geary et al., 2012). The United States students’ average math performance in international large-scale assessment ranks behind several developed countries in Asia and Europe. Insufficient math ability limits students’ educational and career development. Students who are struggling with mathematics are more likely to drop out of school or avoid math-related careers (Rozek et al., 2015). Thus, it is important for researchers and educators to understand the factors that affect students’ math performance and maintain their aspirations in learning math, which is associated with math-related activities and persistence. Therefore, proper instruction can be provided for students based on their characteristics. In response, the literature indicates that some non-cognitive factors, like math self-concept, interest, perceived control in math, instrumental motivation to learn math, and math anxiety, play critical roles in understanding students’ math behavior, persistence, and performance in math learning (Hudson, 1999; Ma and Xu, 2004; Marsh et al., 2005; Antunes and Fontaine, 2007; Schiefele, 2009; Fernández-Villaverde et al., 2015). While math self-concept, interest, perceived control in math, instrumental motivation to learn math positively associate with frequencies of math behaviors, persistence when facing difficulties and challenges, and math achievement, math anxiety is inversely correlated with them.

This view is grounded in the expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation (EVM). EVM integrates a variety of constructs into a comprehensive model based on theories on expectancy (e.g., self-efficacy theory and control theories) and theories on reasons for engagement (e.g., intrinsic motivation theories), explaining why individuals perform better on certain tasks but poorer on others (Pintrich and Schrauben, 1992; Eccles and Wigfield, 2002; Marsh and Martin, 2011; Barron and Hulleman, 2015; Ng et al., 2016; Ramirez et al., 2016; Batchelor et al., 2019). According to EVM, one’s expectancies for success and subjective task values on tasks directly determine their persistence, performance, and task choices (Eccles and Wigfield, 2002; Barron and Hulleman, 2015; Jiang et al., 2018). Students with high levels of expectancy for success and/or perceived task values are more likely to engage in learning, even in difficult situations (Murayama et al., 2013).

Expectancies for success are broad beliefs about individuals’ abilities in a specific domain (Eccles, 1983). These beliefs that an individual has explained why he/she is willing to do challenging tasks affect his/her behaviors and choices (Eccles, 1983; Eccles and Wigfield, 2002; Wigfield and Cambria, 2010). Despite some theoretical distinctions from expectancy beliefs, empirically math self-concept and perceived control in math are close to expectancy beliefs in the math domain (Eccles and Wigfield, 2002). Math self-concept refers to the student’s self-perceived competence and self-appraisal in mathematics (Marsh et al., 2005; Antunes and Fontaine, 2007). It is a domain-specific academic self-concept, which is a component of the global self-concept that develops through interaction between one’s environment and self-attribution (Gniewosz et al., 2015; Tomasetto et al., 2015). However, math self-concept is distinguished from global self-concept or non-academic self-concept construct; it is closer to the concept, math self-efficacy, both of which are self-perceived ability beliefs related to math (Ferla et al., 2009). Theoretically, there are subtle differences between math self-concept and math self-efficacy. Self-concept in math emphasizes self-appraising on the whole math domain, whereas math self-efficacy, which refers to students’ beliefs on their ability to succeed in a specific math task, emphasizes self-perception on a specific task. However, Eccles and Wigfield (2002) pointed out that based on empirical studies, children and adolescents do not distinguish between self-efficacy and self-concept. Many studies have emphasized the strong effect of academic self-concepts on important factors that are associated with learning, such as emotion (Chen et al., 2015), interest (Marsh et al., 2005), school engagement (Raufelder et al., 2013; Frawley et al., 2014), learning outcomes (Tornare et al., 2015), and career choice (Sax et al., 2015). Relatively strong linkage has been found between self-concept and math achievement (Antunes and Fontaine, 2007; McWilliams et al., 2013; Parker et al., 2013). In one study, Marsh et al. (2006) employed data from a large German study, which includes 4,475 participants from 149 randomly selected upper secondary schools, to reveal the relationship among various dimensions of self-concept and core personality constructs. The results showed that math self-concept had strong correlations with math school grades (r = 0.71), math standardized achievement test scores (r = 0.59), and the likelihood of taking advanced math courses (r = 0.51). Another study investigated the influence of academic self-concept and other motivational variables on later achievement based on data from PISA 2003 and 2004, which included 6,020 German students. Using structural equation modeling controlling intelligence and prior achievement, they found that academic self-concept significantly predicted later achievement, explaining 4% of the variance (Kriegbaum et al., 2015).

Perceived control in math refers to a belief that one has the controllability of what he/she thinks is important (ability, powerful others, or luck) to succeed in math (d’Ailly, 2003). It is an individual’s perception of the causal relationship between his/her math behavior and math learning outcomes (Rotter, 1966). Perceived control is associated with individual’s learning-related behavior (Patrick et al., 1993), perseverance when facing challenging tasks (Cervone and Peake, 1986), intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (Patrick et al., 1993), and learning outcomes (Stupnisky et al., 2007). Empirical studies on perceived control and academic achievement consistently found a positive correlation between these two variables (Damon et al., 2006). For example, Stupnisky et al. (2007) found that perceived control is a powerful determinant of college students’ first-year learning achievement (β = 0.14). Murayama et al. (2013) examined the impact of motivational variables like perceived control, cognitive learning strategies, and intelligence on the long-term growth of students’ math achievement. They used data drawn from a Germany longitudinal study involving annual assessments of 3,530 students from grade 5 to grade 10. Using latent growth curve model, they found that intelligence (β = 4.72) contributed to the initial level of achievement, whereas perceived control (β = 3.78), intrinsic motivation (β = 4.64), and learning strategies (β = 4.51) predicted the growth of math achievement.

Subjective values in EVM are associated with incentives or reasons for doing activities or tasks, explaining why individuals choose to or not to do certain tasks in achievement-related situations (Eccles and Wigfield, 2002; Wigfield and Cambria, 2010). Eccles posits four types of task values: intrinsic value, utility value, attainment value, and cost. The first three values are positively associated with learning activities and achievement, while the fourth one is negatively associated with the other three. In our study, we are interested in two positive value components, interest, which is the same as intrinsic values and instrumental motivation, which is the same as utility value, and the negative value component, cost. Math interest refers to positive emotion associated with math-related activities, which may contribute to an internal drive to engage in these activities for them per se (Hidi, 1990; Eccles and Wigfield, 2002; Lee et al., 2014; Grigg et al., 2018). Interest can be divided into individual interest (a long-lasting preference for objects and activities) and situational interest (a temporary emotional state aroused by environmental stimuli when performing a task) (Krapp, 2002, 2005; Lohbeck et al., 2016). It includes two highly correlated but distinguished aspects, emotional and cognitive (Schiefele and Csikszentmihalyi, 1995; Lohbeck et al., 2016). The emotional aspect refers to positive feelings associating with objects or activities, typically, feelings of enjoyment and involvement (Krapp, 2005). The cognitive aspect refers to the appraisal of the value of the objects or activities (Schiefele et al., 2012). Empirically, studies have found that interest is positively associated with memory, attention (Tobias, 1994), comprehension (Hidi and Anderson, 1992), sophisticated learning strategies (Hidi, 1990), time and efforts invested in learning (Macher et al., 2012; Trautwein et al., 2015), and academic achievement and performance (Hidi, 2000; Fisher et al., 2012). A longitudinal study on 168 children from a diverse background with a mean age of 4.39 showed that prior interest was significantly predicting later math skills (β = 0.13) with control for initial skills and intervention status (Fisher et al., 2012).

Instrumental motivation refers to the external drive to involve in tasks or activities because of practical or pragmatic reasons, such as improving career prospects and opportunities (Hudson, 1999). Studies concerning instrumental motivation started from second-language learning area. Lukmani (1972) examined the association between English proficiency and different types of motivation for learning English among a group of high school students and found that instrumental motivation more strongly correlated with English proficiency than integrative motivation. The researchers recently found the important role of instrumental motivation in science learning (Yu, 2012; Acosta and Hsu, 2014; Mujtaba and Reiss, 2014; Perera, 2014). Studies have found that students with higher instrumental motivation for science are more likely to continue learning science-related subjects even when they are not compulsory and tend to have higher achievement in science. Since 2003, large-scale international assessment started to investigate instrumental motivation to learn mathematics. Based on all these, instrumental motivation might have the same effects on math learning.

As mentioned above, an abundance of studies has demonstrated linkages between ability beliefs, positive dimensions of task values, and learning. Only until recently, researchers have started to explore the negative dimension of subject task value, the cost, to fully explain the reason why students engage in learning (Jiang et al., 2018). Cost refers to a perception of the negative aspect of engaging in a task (Eccles and Wigfield, 2002; Barron and Hulleman, 2015; Jiang et al., 2018), consisting of negative emotion of anxiety and fear of failure, efforts required to succeed, and the lost opportunities to engage in another task. In our study, we focused on math anxiety, an important negative emotional aspect of cost. Math anxiety refers to unpleasant feelings like “fear, tension, and apprehension that many people experience when engaging with math” (Ashcraft, 2002; Ahmed et al., 2012) and worrying of failure when engaging in math activities (Barron and Hulleman, 2015). Math anxiety has been found negatively associated with learning and performance (Ho et al., 2000; Ma and Xu, 2004; Miller and Bichsel, 2004; Ramirez et al., 2016; Namkung et al., 2019). Ramirez et al. (2016) examined the relationship among math anxiety, math achievement, and strategy use with a sample of 564 children (256 first grader and 308 s grader). They found that math anxiety impedes advanced memory-based strategy use and impairs math achievement by diminishing the use of advanced memory-based strategies. Further, a recent twin study confirmed a negative correlation between math anxiety and performance, which is attributed to both environmental and genetic factors quantitatively (Malanchini et al., 2020).

From existing studies, we found three possible explanations of why math anxiety inversely affects persistence in learning, math behavior, and achievement. First, math anxiety causes avoidance of involving in math-related tasks and activities (Ashcraft and Faust, 1994; Chinn, 2009; Trezise and Reeve, 2014). Second, the state of anxiety interferes with working memory to a certain extent, and dysfunction of working memory causes low math achievement (Ashcraft and Kirk, 2001; Ramirez et al., 2018). Last, math anxiety may be due to a lack of mathematical ability, which may cause poor math achievement (Ramirez et al., 2018).

Theoretically, expectancy beliefs and the positive dimensions of subjective task values are positively correlated with each other (Eccles and Wigfield, 2002). Initially, in the academic domain, students’ ability beliefs and perceived task-values may be independent of each other; however, their ability beliefs may change the perception of task values gradually when they grow older (Wigfield, 1994). An early study indicated that changes in competence beliefs caused the changes in interest (Mac Iver et al., 1991). Some recent studies also showed that most students with high competency beliefs demonstrate a high perceived task value (Trautwein et al., 2012). Grigg et al. (2018) found that prior math interest positively predicted subsequent math ability beliefs (β = 0.186). On the other hand, negative emotion is negatively correlated with expectancy beliefs and task values. A prior study supported a reciprocal relationship between math self-concept and math anxiety (β = −0.07–−0.15) (Ahmed et al., 2012). The theoretical explanation is that one’s appraisal of incompetence may trigger a perceived environmental threat since one’s self-concept involves appraisal of one’s competence to cope with environmental demands (Bandura, 1997). In consequence, perceived environmental threat causes anxiety. In addition, Gottfried (1982) found that math anxiety and intrinsic motivation are negatively correlated with each other among both four graders (β = −0.54) and seventh graders (β = −0.40). The students with a high level of math interest may be more likely to accept any challenges in learning rather than avoid them, while students with high math anxiety may view challenges as threats to them, thus avoid challenging tasks.

More complex interplay of math anxiety and the motivational factors (i.e., math self-concept, perceived control, interest in math, and instrumental motivation in math) on learning has been emerging. On one hand, math anxiety negatively affects motivational factors. Math anxiety may diminish perceived values of activities or tasks, expectancy, and ultimately motivation to engage in math-related learning (Wigfield and Meece, 1988; Eccles and Wigfield, 2002; Trautwein et al., 2012). On the other hand, the motivational factors compensate for the impaired efficiency caused by math anxiety (Justicia-Galiano et al., 2017). Students with certain levels of math anxiety may not necessarily engage less in math activities if they have comparatively higher levels of motivational factors that can counteract the negative impact of math anxiety by making more efforts. With the influence of math motivation, math anxiety and math performance probably demonstrate curvilinear relation. The study of Wang et al. (2015) on 237 undergraduate students and 262 pairs of same-sex twins show the distinct patterns of the relationship between math anxiety and performance on math-related tasks at different levels of math motivation. Students with higher math motivation performed the best on math-related tasks when their math anxiety levels were at a medium level, and the worst when their math anxiety levels were extremely low or high. However, the negative linear relationship between math anxiety and math performance was found among students with lower math motivation.

Most of the past studies on math motivation and affection used variable-centered approaches and have proved the significant association between motivational and affective variables and their relations to outcome variables, like academic achievement, in an entire population or observable groups. One limitation of these studies is that they ignored the potential unobservable student subgroups based on math motivational and affective factors, which will provide nuanced information on the patterns of the levels of students’ motivation and affection related to math. To address this limitation, the person-centered approach, latent profile analysis (LPA), can be employed. LPA that can be used to identify subgroups of students based on math motivation and affection helps us to understand the characteristics of students within the group that they are assigned to in accordance with the highest probability (Spurk et al., 2020). Additionally, LPA allows us to compare differences of outcome variables between latent subgroups, thus we can understand whether the combination of the motivational and affective factors affect outcome variables, which cannot be addressed by using variable-centered approaches. To our best knowledge, there is a scant study focusing on investigating the characteristics of students’ latent profiles based on math motivational and affective variables. We only found one study (Wang et al., 2018) that showed the existence and patterns of students’ profiles based on math motivation and math anxiety. Therefore, in the current study, we aimed to identify the characteristics of students’ subgroups based on the motivational and affective factors. More specifically, we are interested to find out the different combinations of the motivational and affective factors. Although, as we mentioned before that motivational variables and math anxiety are inversely associated with each other, we did not assume that we would only find the pattern of the combination of these variables as high level of math motivation vs. low level of math anxiety or high level of math anxiety vs. low level of math motivation. The reason is that Wang et al. (2018) found eight groups of students and showed complex combinations rather than a simple “high vs. low” pattern. Moreover, we intend to examine whether there would be differences in outcome variables, i.e., math achievement, persistence, and math behaviors, between the identified students’ subgroups. The purpose of doing this is to find out which combination of motivational factors and math anxiety is most favorable for better outcomes.

Materials and Methods

Data

Data used in our study were derived from the Program for International Student Assessment (PISA) 2012 United States data. PISA is a large-scale international assessment that is coordinated by Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD) focusing on students’ mathematics, reading, and science competencies, as well as learning-related factors, such as self-concept, interest, anxiety, and more, approaching the end of compulsory schooling (OECD, 2014). PISA is conducted every 3 years.

Participants

PISA 2012 includes 510,000 students from 65 countries and economies, and the major subject PISA 2012 assesses is mathematics. The participants were between the age of 15 years 3 months and age 16 years 2 months at the time of the test (OECD, 2014). More details about PISA 2012 data collection or test design can be found in the PISA 2012 Technical Report. United States data originally include a sample of 4,978 students (male = 2,525 51%, female = 2,453 49%) from 162 schools. We are using 37 response items, gender, socio-economic status, and math achievement items for this study. Of the 37 response items, 1,719 students (34.5%) miss 27 or more items, another 1,689 students (33.9%) miss 10–26 items. Among them, 1,590 students miss all items for math behavior, instrumental motivation, interest, persistence, and self-control. Another 1,540 students miss all items from anxiety and self-concept. Of all these missing data, 95.8% are due to “questionnaire rotation” by design, which complies with missing at complete random (MCAR). Thus, we used list-wise deletion, which should not bring bias to subsequent analysis under MCAR assumption (please see Supplementary Material for more details in describing missingness in this data). Finally, 1,464 participants (male 743 51%, female 721 49%, age 15.82 ± 0.28) with complete information were included in our study.

Measures

Mathematics Anxiety (Anxiety)

Anxiety was measured by a five-item ANXMAT scale that was used in the main survey of PISA 2012 and PISA 2003. The items are rated on a four-point Likert scale (1 = strongly agree; 4 = strongly disagree), measuring the levels of anxiety students feel when they are involved in math-related activities (e.g., “I often worry that it will be difficult for me in mathematics classes.”) (OECD, 2014). Internal consistency in this sample was reliable (Cronbach’s α = 0.88).

Mathematics Self-Concept (Self-Concept)

Self-concept was measured by a five-item SCMAT scale that was used in the main survey of PISA 2012 and PISA 2003. The items are rated on a four-point Likert scale (1 = strongly agree; 4 = strongly disagree), measuring how students feel about their abilities in math (e.g., “I have always believed that mathematics is one of my best subjects.”) (OECD, 2014). Internal consistency in this sample was reliable (Cronbach’s α = 0.90).

Instrumental Motivation to Learn Mathematics (Instrumental Motivation)

Instrumental motivation was measured by the INSTMOT scale used in both PISA 2003 and PISA 2012, consisting of four items. The items are rated on a four-point Likert scale (1 = strongly agree; 4 = strongly disagree), measuring how much the students feel that they learn math due to the benefits it will bring for them in their future study and career (e.g., “Making an effort in mathematics is worth it because it will help me in the work that I want to do later on.”) (OECD, 2014). Internal consistency in this sample was reliable (Cronbach’s α = 0.91).

Mathematics Interest (Interest)

Interest was measured by four items in the main survey of PISA 2012. The items are rated on a four-point Likert scale (1 = strongly agree; 4 = strongly disagree), measuring how much the students feel they engaged in math (e.g., “I do mathematics because I enjoy it.”) (OECD, 2014). Internal consistency in this sample was reliable (Cronbach’s α = 0.92).

Perceived Control in Math (Perceived Control)

Perceived control was measured by six items in the main survey of PISA 2012. The items are rated on a four-point Likert scale (1 = strongly agree; 4 = strongly disagree), measuring how students feel that their success in math is due to their ability and efforts (i.e., “If I put in enough effort I can succeed in mathematics.” “Whether or not I do well in mathematics is completely up to me.” “Family demands or other problems prevent me from putting a lot of time into my mathematics work.” “If I had different teachers I would try harder in mathematics.” “If I wanted to I could do well in mathematics.” “I do badly in mathematics whether or not I study for my exams.”) (OECD, 2014). Internal consistency in this sample was not reliable (Cronbach’s α = 0.68).

Math-Related Behaviors (Behavior)

Behavior was measured by a newly created scale in PISA 2012. The scale consists of eight items that are rated on a four-point Likert scale (1 = always or almost always; 4 = never or rare)(OECD, 2014). This scale measures how often students were involved in math-related activities (e.g., “I talk about mathematics problems with my friends.”). Internal consistency in this sample was reliable (Cronbach’s α = 0.83).

Persistence in Learning Math (Persistence)

Persistence in learning math was measured by eight items that are rated on a five-point Likert scale (1 = very much like me; 5 = not at all like me). The scale measures how likely the students will persist on a task when they have difficulties (e.g., “When confronted with a problem, I do more than what is expected of me.”) (OECD, 2014). Internal consistency in this sample was not reliable (Cronbach’s α = 0.76).

Math Achievement (Achievement)

Achievement was measured by five plausible values based on students’ responses to math tests. Plausible values were drawn from posterior distribution estimated by item response theory, with a mean of 500 and a standard deviation of 100 (OECD, 2014). The item responses are formatted in three types: open construct-response, closed construct-response, and selected-response. Open constructed-response items are scored by trained experts based on students’ extended written responses. Closed construct-response and selected items are scored based on whether a student provides a correct answer (OECD, 2013). The PISA 2012 mathematics test items were designed within four broad areas: change and relationships, space and shape, quantity, and uncertainty and data, assessing seven fundamental mathematical capabilities: communication, mathematizing, representation, reasoning and argument, devising strategies for solving problems, using symbolic, formal, and technical language and operations, and using mathematical tools (OECD, 2013). The test items are developed based on problems that an individual may encounter in real-world settings.

Socio-Economic Status (SES) and Gender

It is generally accepted and proved by empirical studies that students’ family SES and gender are associated with math persistence, behavior, and achievement (Passolunghi et al., 2014). The development of expectancy and value is shaped by personal and environmental factors, like gender and SES (Eccles and Wigfield, 2002; Barron and Hulleman, 2015). Therefore, we added these two variables as covariates in our regression models (Passolunghi et al., 2014). Gender was self-reported. Students’ family information was used to produce an index to reflect SES, which is scaled to have a standard normal distribution.

Analysis

Our data analyses were run in R (version 3.5.1) with packages reshape2 (2_1.4.3), ggplot2 (2_3.1.0), mclust (version 5.4.5), factoextra (version 1.0.5), multcomp (version 1.4–10), lavaan (version 0.6–3), lmtest (0.9–37), lsmeans (2.30), and agricolae (1.3–1).

First, arithmetic means of all response items for math anxiety, math self-concept, perceived control in math, math interest, instrumental motivation to learn math, persistence in learning math, math-related behaviors, and math achievement were calculated. For anxiety, self-concept, perceived control, interest, and instrumental motivation, the calculated means were further subtracted from 5. For persistence, the calculated mean was subtracted from 6. Thus, a larger number is corresponding to higher anxiety, perceived control, self-concept, interest, instrumental motivation, behavior, and persistence. Pairwise Pearson correlations among all eight measures were calculated. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to confirm the validity of 24 response items measuring the five constructs (anxiety, interest, self-concept, perceived control, and instrumental motivation), which are used in later latent profile analysis (LPA) (Supplementary Table 1).

Then, LPA was employed to cluster students based on these five motivational and affective factors. Anxiety, self-concept, perceived control, interest, and instrumental motivation were further residualized using linear regressions adjusted for SES and gender to remove variations attributed to SES and gender (Hart et al., 2016). The resultant values of anxiety, self-concept, perceived control, interest, and instrumental motivation were converted to Z scores and used as input of LPA, which was done using “mclust” package in R. Model selection was conducted comprehensively considering Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), Integrated Complete-data Likelihood (ICL), Bootstrap Likely Ratio Test (BLRT), and normalized entropy (Enorm calculated by formula 1) from 14 types of models (Scrucca et al., 2016) with 1–12 profiles. According to mclust package, these 14 types of models are defined by three-letter names, such as “EEI,” “VEI,” or “VVV.” They are basically gaussian finite mixture models with different constraints on variance–covariance structures. Scrucca (2016) has clearly explained these concepts. Briefly, the first two letters define the constraints on variances of sub-group distributions. The most restricted “EI” model means that all variables from all different groups share the same variance. The unrestricted “VV” models mean all variances from all variables of different groups are freely estimated. The third letter defines correlations among variables. “∗∗I” models have no correlations at all, “∗∗E” models have the same correlations among groups, while “∗∗V” models have variable correlations among groups. Normalized entropies were calculated using the following formula:

E norm = 1 + i = 1 N k = 1 K P ik log P ik N log K (1)

where N is the number of individuals, K is the number of groups, Pik is the probability of individual i belonging to group k. Higher entropy means less overlapping of latent distributions, or more accurate classifications (Celeux and Soromenho, 1996). To select the model that best fits our data, we first compared BIC and ICL of all 168 models and chose several models with the best BIC and/or ICL values. Then, normalized entropies were calculated for selected models. The ones with the highest entropy were kept. Finally, if there are more than one model with the same variance–covariance structures but different number of latent groups (very likely), BLRT was conducted to test if a more complex model (with more latent groups) is significantly better than a simpler model (with less latent groups).

To increase the interpretability of our results, we conducted secondary clustering to merge groups with significant overlapping from the initial LPA output (Baudry et al., 2010). Briefly, two groups were merged at a time sequentially. Groups to be merged at each step were chosen based on a criteria that the merged clusters show the smallest entropy E, which is calculated using following formula:

E = - i = 1 N k = 1 K P ik log P ik (2)

Thus, this method could generate results with the number of clusters from the same number of clusters from LPA down to one cluster. The number of clusters was chosen using “elbow method,” which finds the number of clusters when the decrease in entropy becomes minimum. This process was assisted by piecewise linear regression (Fraley and Raftery, 1998). The means of instrumental motivation, interest, self-concept, perceived control, and anxiety among subgroups were compared using ANOVA followed by Tukey HSD.

Last, students’ persistence, behavior (log-transformed due to right skewness), and achievements of different subgroups were compared using linear regressions with gender and SES as covariates. The effect size of subgroup membership was calculated according to Cohen (1988) using the following formula, where R2 is the differences of the R2 of the full model and the model without covariate of interest:

f 2 = R 2 1 - R 2 (3)

The significance of variables was checked using the Likelihood Ratio Test (LRT). Significance of pairwise comparisons between all subgroups was done using the Tukey HSD post hoc tests.

Results

After removing individuals with missing values in included response items, 1,464 individuals were included. Cronbach’s alpha was used to check the internal reliability of measured constructs. A value greater than 0.8 is thought as reliable (Peterson, 1994). Six constructs have values greater than 0.8, except for self-concept and perceived control, whose Cronbach’s alphas are 0.68 and 0.76, respectively (see the “Materials and Methods” section for details). Descriptive statistics of arithmetic means of response items for all the variables included in our analysis are shown in Table 1. Most of these variables are roughly symmetrically distributed. However, math behavior is right-skewed, for most students reported “sometimes” or “never or rarely” participated in math-related activities, like “talk about mathematics problems with friends.” Table 2 shows the pairwise correlations of all factors included in this study. Using Cohen’s empirical cutoffs for correlation coefficient (Cohen, 2013), interest, perceived control, self-concept, instrumental motivation, and persistence are positively correlated with each other, showing moderate (0.3 < r < 0.5) to strong (r > 0.5) correlations, except for the correlation between behavior and persistence, which showed a weak positive correlation (r = 0.21). Behavior correlated moderately and positively with interest and self-concept, and with instrumental motivation positively but weakly. Achievement positively and moderately correlated with perceived control, self-concept, and with persistence positively but weakly. The strongest positive correlation was observed between interest and self-concept (r = 0.65). Anxiety negatively correlated with all the other variables (r ≤ −0.1). The strongest negative correlation was observed between self-concept and anxiety (r = −0.75).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlation between variables.

Confirmatory factor analysis was used to interrogate the validity of measurement constructs for anxiety, instrumental motivation, interest, self-concept, and perceived control (Supplementary Figure 1). Fitted model has significant Chi-square test (χ2 = 2,489.822, p < 0.001). The Comparative Fit Index (CFI) is 0.907. The Standardized Root Mean Residual (SRMR) is 0.071. The Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) is 0.077. All the fitting statistics show that the CFA model was just acceptable (Wang and Wang, 2009). This is mainly due to the construct of perceived control, which shows a high residual correlation with other items, especially from anxiety and self-concept (Supplementary Figure 2). Overall, CFA indicates that the data fits a proposed measurement model for these five variables.

Latent profile analysis was used to explore latent subgroups of individuals defined by five constructs: anxiety, instrumental motivation, interest, self-concept, and perceived control. Considering different variance and evident correlations among the five variables, models with 14 different constraints on variance–covariance matrix structure were included for comparison, from one latent subgroup to 12 latent subgroups. Different structures for the variance–covariance matrix are represented by three letters, indicating “scale,” “shape,” and “orientation” of latent multivariable distribution, respectively (Scrucca et al., 2016). Bayesian information criterion (BIC) and Integrated Complete-data Likelihood (ICL, which is BIC penalized by estimated mean entropy) are used first to compare all 168 models (Supplementary Figures 3A,B) (Biernacki et al., 2000). Both criteria showed that the “VVE” model (varying variances and equal correlations) with 10 latent subgroups are the best. The likelihood ratio test (LRT) for assessing the number of latent subgroups was performed. Again, the VVE model with 10 subgroups was significant compared with the VVE model with 9 latent subgroups. However, the VVE model with 11 subgroups was not significant compared to the VVE model with 10 subgroups. The model with 10 subgroups had a normalized entropy (Enorm, equation 1) of 0.822. The VVE models with 9 and 12 subgroups have the second and third highest BIC. The normalized entropies were both 0.79, lower than that of the model with 10 subgroups. Above all, the VVE model with 10 subgroups demonstrated the best model fit and was chosen for further analysis. The smallest subgroup includes 2.5% of all individuals (seen in Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Latent profile analysis (LPA) fitting results.

The 10-group model provided the best BIC and ICL. There were some subgroups with very similar patterns (Supplementary Figure 4). To make our results more interpretable, we conducted secondary clustering (Baudry et al., 2010). This secondary clustering procedure has no assumptions on underlying distributions; it solely focuses on reducing entropies by combining severely overlapped groups. Entropies (E, equation 2) of results with 1 to 10 subgroups were plotted, and piecewise linear regression was used to find the “elbow” point where decreasing of entropy becomes less (Supplementary Figure 5). Two elbow points at seven groups and five groups were found, and the clustering with five resultant subgroups was chosen for interpretability and simplicity (seen in Figure 1 and Table 4).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Latent subgroups of students from latent profile analysis (LPA) and secondary clustering. Means of residualized Z scores of INMM, MI, MSC, MPC, and MA are shown for each latent subgroup. Ellipse indicates a non-significant difference between these subgroups of certain variables. Group 1—high math anxiety and medium-high motivation. Group 2—high math anxiety and low motivation. Group 3—medium math anxiety and medium-low motivation. Group 4—low math anxiety and medium motivation. Group 5—low anxiety and high motivation. INMM, instrumental motivation in math; MA, math anxiety; MI, interest in math; MPC, math perceived control; MSC, math self-concept; Gp, latent subgroups. Significant level at 5%.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Latent subgroup means.

Class 1—high math anxiety and medium-high motivation. The size of this subgroup was the smallest among all five subgroups with approximately 2.3% of all students (n = 33). About 57.6% of the students in this subgroup were males (n = 19) and 42.4% were females (n = 14). The SES mean was the lowest of all the five subgroups. The students in this subgroup were characterized by the highest levels of math anxiety among all the subgroups and medium-high motivation, except for perceived control that is low.

Class 2—high math anxiety and low motivation. The size of this subgroup was the second smallest among all the subgroups with around 4.3% of the students (n = 63). About 54% of the students in this subgroup were males (n = 34) and 46% were females (n = 29). The SES mean of this subgroup was the highest of all subgroups. The students in this subgroup were characterized by very high math anxiety, the lowest MFs. The math anxiety levels of the students in this subgroup showed no significant difference from the math anxiety levels of the students in class 1, but the MFs levels of the students in this subgroup were significantly lower than the MFs levels in subgroup 1.

Class 3—medium math anxiety and medium-low motivation. The size of this subgroup was medium with approximately 9.4% of the students (n = 139). About 47.5% of the students in this subgroup were males (n = 66) and 52.5% were females (n = 73). The mean SES of this subgroup was very close to the mean SES of all 1,464 students (0.192). The students in this subgroup were characterized by medium math anxiety, a medium level of motivation and perceived control, and a low level of interest and self-concept.

Class 4—low math anxiety and medium motivation. The size of this subgroup is the largest with approximately 79.2% of the students (n = 1,159). About 50.5% of the students in this subgroup were males (n = 585) and 49.5% were females (n = 574). Mean SES is very close to the mean SES of all 1,464 students. The students in this subgroup were characterized by low math anxiety and medium MFs.

Class 5—low math anxiety and high motivation. The size of this subgroup is relatively small with approximately 4.8% of the students (n = 70). About 55.7% of the students in this subgroup were males (n = 39) and 44.3% were females (n = 31). The SES mean of this subgroup was higher than that of classes 1, 3, and 4, but lower than that of class 2. The students in this subgroup were characterized by the lowest anxiety among all the subgroups and the highest MFs among all the subgroups.

Math achievement of students from all five subgroups was compared using linear regression with gender and SES as covariates (adjusted R2 = 0.201). In this model, male students performed significantly better than female students (χ2 = 18.06, p < 0.001). SES significantly positively associates with math achievement (χ2 = 261.15, p < 0.001). Math achievement is significantly associated with subgroup membership with a small effect size (f2 = 0.039, χ2 = 66.90, p < 0.001) (Cohen, 1992). We used a pairwise comparison to determine the differences in math achievement between subgroups. As the results shown in Table 5, among all the five subgroups, the classes 4 and 5 have the highest achievements, significantly higher than classes 1 and 2. Both classes 4 and 5 have higher achievements than that of class 3, but only class 4 reached significance. There are no significant differences in math achievement between classes 4 and 5. Class 1 has the lowest math achievement.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Subgroup comparison.

Math-related behaviors of students among all the five subgroups were compared using linear regression with gender and SES as covariates (adjusted R2 = 0.147). Male students showed that they were significantly more frequently involved in math-related activities than female students (χ2 = 33.14, p < 0.001) and SES positively associates with strong persistence (χ2 = 15.70, p < 0.001). Math-related behaviors also were significantly associated with subgroup membership with a small effect size (f2 = 0.137, χ2 = 193.55, p < 0.001). Results of pairwise comparison for behavior among subgroups are shown (seen in Table 5). Classes 1 and 5 showed the highest frequency of math behavior and there was no significant difference of math behavior between them. No significant difference was found between math behavior of classes 4 and 3, which showed less frequency than math behavior of classes 1 and 5. Class 2 demonstrated the least frequent math-related behavior.

Students’ persistence in learning math among all five subgroups were compared using linear regression with gender and SES as covariates (adjusted R2 = 0.093). Male students showed significantly stronger persistence than female students (χ2 = 9.64, p = 0.002), and SES positively associates with strong persistence (χ2 = 31.10, p < 0.001). Persistence is also significantly associated with subgroup membership with a small effect size (f2 = 0.076, χ2 = 110.79, p < 0.001). Through pairwise comparison, we found that class 5 showed the highest level of persistence, significantly higher than all other subgroups. Class 4 has the second-highest level of persistence, significantly higher than classes 1, 2, and 3. There was no significant difference among the persistence of classes 1, 2, and 3 (Table 5).

To sum up, we found five subgroups of students with different patterns of motivational and affective factor (math anxiety). Significant differences in math behavior, persistence, and achievement were found among subgroups.

Discussion

The main goal of our study was to investigate the latent profiles of the students with respect to five non-cognitive factors: math interest, math self-concept, perceived control in math, instrumental motivation to learn math, and math anxiety. We also sought to investigate the relationship between group classification and mathematics achievement, persistence, and behavior.

Latent Profile Analysis and Secondary Clustering

To identify the profiles of students, we employed LPA. For getting practically interpretable results, we adopted the secondary clustering technique to reduce group numbers. This method helps us merge subgroups overlapping significantly in order to avoid ambiguity of classification of students. In practice, the resultant five subgroups are easier to be applied compared with the initial 10 classes suggested by BIC criteria to a small group of students such as a class or a school. Most of the time, BIC criteria would generate appropriate results in the latent profile analysis. However, due to the assumption about underlying distribution (multivariate gaussian in our case), BIC could report overlapping clusters without warning. ICL took into account entropy to punish overlapping clusters (Biernacki et al., 2000). ICL could be viewed as BIC with an extra term of entropy. This means the weight of entropy in ICL could not be adjusted, and it also could potentially return too many clusters when the underlying distribution does not match our assumptions. In other words, the BIC criterion is seeking for underlying distributions other than clusters. ICL punishes less on deviation from distribution assumption but still depends on it. In our study, both BIC and ICL provided us with 10 subgroups as the best results. However, some subgroups showed very similar patterns in means (Supplementary Figure 4). To collapse these closely related subgroups, we further clustered these 10 subgroups into five subgroups, purely based on entropy (Baudry et al., 2010).

Overall, we found five distinct latent profiles that describe US students’ motivation and affection. According to our results, except for class 1, the other four subgroups all show a positive correlation among motivational variables. Indeed, Trautwein et al. (2012) found that students who believed that they had high ability in an academic domain perceived high task values in that domain. In addition, except for class 1, the other four subgroups all showed a seemingly negative correlation between motivational variables and anxiety. This is consistent with previous variable-centered studies that suggested an inverse relationship between math anxiety and motivational variables (Meece et al., 1990; Ahmed et al., 2012). A recent study employing ACE model on 3,410 twins showed negative correlations between math anxiety and two math attitude variables, interest and self-efficacy, both phenotypically (−0.45) and genetically (−0.7) (Malanchini et al., 2020). Interestingly, with a person-centered approach, our study found an exceptional subgroup, class 1, of which the students with medium math interest, instrumental motivational to learn, and math self-concept had high math anxiety and low rather than medium perceived control in math. Compared with other subgroups, these students were probably extremely anxious about learning mathematics. They believed in their mathematics ability to some extent and perceived that mathematics was enjoyable to learn and valuable for their future studies and careers, but they doubted whether they can control their ability to succeed in math.

Our findings provide distinguishing profiles of students based on motivational and affective factors. This will help teachers to understand the diversity of the students’ math motivation and affection. Moreover, identifying students’ subgroups and the understanding of their characteristics are beneficial for teachers who attempt to customize an educational plan with ideal instruction type and teaching strategies for specific groups of students. Oberlin (Bonijoly et al., 1982) posited that an identical teaching method for the entire class would cause anxiety. Thus, it is important for teachers to identify the subgroups of students by their psychological characteristics to facilitate differential teaching based on students’ needs. It is helpful if teachers identify groups of students who are potentially at risk of failure and give those students appropriate instruction.

To better understand the characteristics of the students’ in different subgroups, we further investigated the differences in mathematics achievement, persistence, and math behaviors (math-related activities) using regression. In the regression models, we used gender and SES as covariates, for a number of studies have revealed that gender and SES affect students’ math achievement, persistence, and math behaviors (Eccles and Wigfield, 2002; Passolunghi et al., 2014; Barron and Hulleman, 2015).

Differences in Math Behaviors Across Profiles

Using regression, we found that there are differences among subgroups concerning math-related behaviors. Math behavior was measured by eight items with values bounded between one and four. Moreover, math behavior values are strongly right-skewed, which was partially corrected by logarithm transformation. Diagnostic plots of the regression model showed no digress from the normality assumption (data not shown). The regression effect size (f2 = 0.137) of subgroups on math behavior is small (0.02–0.15) according to Cohen (1992). Interestingly, subgroup covariate boosted R2 from 0.03 to 0.15, indicating that our latent subgroups explained much more variation in math behavior than gender and SES in the linear regression model. This is different from math achievement, where gender and SES explain a significant portion of variation (see below). As the pairwise comparison results show, the students in classes 1 and 5 reported the most frequently involved in math-related activities, followed by classes 4, 3, and 2 (no significant differences were found between classes 3 and 2). Without regard to class 1, the other subgroups of students with higher motivation and lower math anxiety more frequently participated in math-related activities, like “talk about mathematics problems with my friends” and “take part in mathematics competitions.” This finding agrees with previous studies that demonstrated that academic motivation directly or indirectly positively affects math-related activities (Rotgans and Schmidt, 2009; Green et al., 2012). However, given the existence of class 1 of which students were highly anxious with medium motivation but low perceived control, we assume that the controllability of math and negative emotion probably did not affect the involvement of certain students in math-related activities. This reveals that math self-concept, math interest, and instrumental motivation to learn math are more significant than math anxiety and perceived control in math to determine math behaviors.

Differences in Persistence Across Profiles

We also found significant differences in persistence across five subgroups of United States students with a small effect size (f2 = 0.076). Similar to math behavior, persistence is bounded by one and five. However, persistence values are not skewed. In our multiple regression model, SES and gender alone explain less than 3% variation of math persistence (R2 = 0.026); adding subgroups as covariate boosted R2 to 0.097, indicating that math persistence is barely determined by gender or SES, but more determined by subgroups. The students in class 5, which is characterized by the lowest math anxiety and highest motivation, tended to be the most persistent, followed by classes 4, 3, 2, and 1, there was no significant difference found among classes 1–3. As the results show, students who are highly motivated and less anxious are more likely to persist when facing difficulties and challenges without considering the unique subgroup, class 1. The findings are consistent with the prior findings that higher levels of perceived competence and subjective task values predicted higher levels of persistence (Jacobs et al., 1984; Lavigne et al., 2007). However, regarding the students in class 1, they tended to avoid challenging themselves when facing difficulties regardless of relatively high instrumental motivation, interest, and self-concept. It is possible that the negative emotion diminishes students’ willingness to engage, even if they perceive these tasks useful and enjoyable, and themselves competent (Eccles and Wigfield, 2002; Jiang et al., 2018).

Differences in Mathematics Achievement Across Profiles

The results of the study indicate that profile membership was significantly related to the United States students’ mathematics achievement with a small effect size (f2 = 0.039). Contrasting to what we observed on math behavior and persistence, gender and SES explains around 20% of the variation in math achievement (R2 = 0.204). We found that the mathematics achievement of classes 4 and 5 was the highest of all, followed by classes 3, 2, and 1. Based on our findings, the subgroup with higher motivation and lower math anxiety had higher mathematics achievement than the subgroup with lower motivation and higher math anxiety except for class 1. This is supported by the previous study, which also employed latent profile analysis and found that a combination of lower motivation and higher math anxiety suggested lower math achievement (Wang et al., 2018). Our findings are also supported by previous studies, which investigate motivational and affective variables separately. The studies show perceived competence positively affected academic achievement (You et al., 2011; Petersen and Hyde, 2017). Besides, Murayama et al. (2013) demonstrated that perceived control, extrinsic motivation, and intrinsic motivation positively predicted subsequent mathematics achievement.

The unexpected finding of class 1 shows that a subgroup of students with a medium level of math interest, instrumental motivation, and math self-concept, but low perceived control and high math anxiety had low mathematics achievement. This indicates that the students with comparatively high motivation may not reach relatively high math achievement, if they have extremely high math anxiety, suggesting that math anxiety negatively affects mathematics achievement. Indeed, while the students are motivationally engaging in learning, high MA may interfere cognitive engagement by disturbing working memory (Ashcraft, 2002); therefore, they may perform poorly in math assessment (Ramirez et al., 2016).

Overall, the significant differences in mathematics achievement show that five latent profiles not only describe patterns of students’ motivation and affection, but also can be interpreted as performance-based subgroups of students. We found that there were lower achievers with high math anxiety regardless of the motivation level they had. Teachers should pay more attention to these students, for they may demonstrate that they are comparatively confident and both intrinsically and extrinsically motivated to learn math, but still perform poorly. The possible reason lies in that, although these students may invest time and efforts in learning due to their comparatively high motivation, the math anxiety may cause dysfunction of working memory, which impairs learning outcomes (Ramirez et al., 2018). However, it is probably difficult for teachers to identify these students merely based on observation of their behaviors in learning without referring to their test scores.

We observed significant differences among different subgroups of math behavior, math persistence, and mathematics achievement. They share certain patterns such as students with high motivation and low anxiety are more involved in math activities, more persistent when facing math challenges, and achieved higher. However, there are some differences: there are students whose math behaviors are more driven by math self-concept, instrumental motivation, and interest, while are less influenced by math perceived control and anxiety. For other groups of students, math persistence and mathematics achievement are more affected or even dominated by math anxiety and/or math perceived control. This could be due to the fact that math behavior measures more the involvement of activities, which normally does not incur pressure on participants, which will not drive away students who fear doing other challenging math tasks due to high math anxiety or low perceived control, like the students we observed in class 1.

In conclusion, our findings reveal the complexity of the students’ motivational and affective profiles. We found five subgroups of the United States students based on motivational and affective factors. Consistent with a number of previous studies, four out of five subgroups of students demonstrated a negative relationship between motivation and math anxiety. Interestingly, one subgroup showed both medium to high motivation and very high anxiety at the same time. In addition, our findings show that students with frequent involvement in math activities had high self-concept, instrumental motivation, and math interest, while students with a high level of persistence and math achievement are characterized by low math anxiety and high perceived control in math. Our findings are significant for teachers and educators to understand the diversity of students and provide theoretical and practical support for individualized and differentiated instruction.

Limitation

There are several limitations to our study. First, the self-reported math behavior scale does not cover all math-related behaviors in reality. Using interviews or observations to capture more information about students’ math-related behaviors are suggested in future research. Second, we only used data from 1,464 students who have no missingness among all used items in this study. The sample size of 1,464 is still larger than other studies of similar design (Hart et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2018). In addition, due to the fact that students are receiving different subsets of questions of PISA questionnaire by rotated test design (OECD, 2013) means we can assume MCAR. Under this assumption, our approach (LD) will not bring bias to the following analysis (see Supplementary Section “Missingness”). To further validate our results using LD, we imputed missing data from 1,640 students five times. A similar analysis was conducted on these five imputed datasets and similar results were found (for details, please see Supplementary Section “Partial Multiple Imputation Using 3,104 Students Showed Similar Results”). However, the sample size could be potentially increased by using appropriate imputation methods (such as FIML) under some reasonable assumptions about the pattern of missingness. Third, the process of determining the final number of groups of secondary clustering was partially subjective. Even though we used piecewise linear regression to assist in finding elbow points, deciding the number of clusters in this process is still subjective. To our knowledge, there is no formal statistical testing that can be used to determine optimal clusters. Last, our study only focused on the United States sample. Thus, the results should be extended to students from other countries with caution. The future study could attempt to use cross-national data to increase power (detecting more rare subgroups), compare students’ subgroups across countries.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The data included in this study was derived from PISA. Therefore, the study did not require ethics approval.

Author Contributions

FX made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the study, data analysis, and interpretation. LS made substantial contributions to the data analysis and interpretation. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the Social Science Association Fund of Henan Education Department (China) (SKL-2019-507) and the Provincial Philosophy and Social Science Foundation of Henan Province (China) (2019CJY031).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.533593/full#supplementary-material

References

Acosta, S., and Hsu, H.-Y. (2014). Shared academic values: testing a model of the association between Hong Kong parents’ and adolescents’ perception of the general value of science and scientific literacy. Educ. Stud. 40, 174–195. doi: 10.1080/03055698.2013.866889

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahmed, W., Minnaert, A., Kuyper, H., and Werf, M. (2012). Reciprocal relationships between math self-concept and math anxiety. Learning Individ. Differ. 22, 385–389. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.12.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antunes, C., and Fontaine, A. (2007). Gender differences in the causal relation between adolescents’ maths self-concept and scholastic performance. Psychol. Belgica 47, 71–94. doi: 10.5334/pb-47-1-71

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Math Anxiety: Personal, Educational, and Cognitive Consequences. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 11, 181–185. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashcraft, M. H., and Faust, M. W. (1994). Mathematics anxiety and mental arithmetic performance: An exploratory investigation. Cognit. Emot. 8, 97–125. doi: 10.1080/02699939408408931

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashcraft, M., and Kirk, E. (2001). The Relationships Among Working Memory, Math Anxiety, and Performance. Journal of experimental psychology. General 130, 224–237. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.130.2.224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: W H Freeman.

Google Scholar

Barron, K. E., and Hulleman, C. S. (2015). Expectancy-value-cost model of motivation. Psychology 84, 261–271.

Google Scholar

Batchelor, S., Torbeyns, J., and Verschaffel, L. (2019). Affect and mathematics in young children: an introduction. Educ. Stud. Math. 100, 201–209. doi: 10.1007/s10649-018-9864-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baudry, J.-P., Raftery, A. E., Celeux, G., Lo, K., and Gottardo, R. (2010). Combining Mixture Components for Clustering. J. Computat. Graph. Statist. 19, 332–353. doi: 10.1198/jcgs.2010.08111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biernacki, C., Celeux, G., and Govaert, G. (2000). Assessing a mixture model for clustering with the integrated completed likelihood. IEEE Transact. Pattern Anal. Machine Intellig. 22, 719–725. doi: 10.1109/34.865189

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonijoly, M., Oberlin, M., and Oberlin, A. (1982). A possible mechanism for natural graphite formation. Int. J. Coal Geol. Int. J. Coal. Geol. 1, 283–312. doi: 10.1016/0166-5162(82)90018-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Celeux, G., and Soromenho, G. (1996). An entropy criterion for assessing the number of clusters in a mixture model. J. Classificat. 13, 195–212. doi: 10.1007/BF01246098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cervone, D., and Peake, P. K. (1986). Anchoring, efficacy, and action: The influence of judgmental heuristics on self-efficacy judgments and behavior. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 50, 492–501. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.3.492

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, B. H., Chiu, W.-C., and Wang, C.-C. (2015). The Relationship Among Academic Self-concept, Learning Strategies, and Academic Achievement: A Case Study of National Vocational College Students in Taiwan via SEM. Asia Pacific Educ. Res. 24, 419–431. doi: 10.1007/s40299-014-0194-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chinn, S. (2009). Mathematics anxiety in secondary students in England. Dyslexia 15, 61–68. doi: 10.1002/dys.381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). “Statistical Power Analysis for the Social Sciences” (2nd. Edition). Hillsdale, New Jersey, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112, 155–159.

Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Cambridge: Academic press.

Google Scholar

d’Ailly, H. (2003). Children’s autonomy and perceived control in learning: A model of motivation and achievement in Taiwan. J. Educ. Psychol. 95, 84–96. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.84

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damon, W., Lerner, R. M., and Eisenberg, N. (2006). Handbook of Child Psychology, Social, Emotional, and Personality Development. New Jersey, NJ: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Eccles, J. (1983). “Expectancies, values and academic behaviors,” in Achievement and achievement motives: Psychological and sociological approaches, ed. J. T. Spence (San Francisco, CA: Free man), 75–146.

Google Scholar

Eccles, J. S., and Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational Beliefs, Values, and Goals. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 109–132. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferla, J., Valcke, M., and Cai, Y. (2009). Academic self-efficacy and academic self-concept: Reconsidering structural relationships. Learning Individ. Differ. 19, 499–505. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Villaverde, J., Guerrón-Quintana, P., Kuester, K., and Rubio-Ramírez, J. (2015). Fiscal Volatility Shocks and Economic Activity. Am. Econom. Rev. 105, 3352–3384. doi: 10.1257/aer.20121236

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, P. H., Dobbs-Oates, J., Doctoroff, G. L., and Arnold, D. H. (2012). Early math interest and the development of math skills. J. Educ. Psychol. 104, 673–681. doi: 10.1037/a0027756

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraley, C., and Raftery, A. E. (1998). How Many Clusters? Which Clustering Method? Answers Via Model-Based Cluster Analysis. Comp. J. 41, 578–588. doi: 10.1093/comjnl/41.8.578

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frawley, D., McCoy, S., Banks, J., and Thornton, M. (2014). Affective School Engagement and Self-Concept: How are Irish Boys and Girls Faring? Child Indicat. Res. 7, 843–859. doi: 10.1007/s12187-014-9247-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., and Bailey, D. H. (2012). Mathematical Cognition Deficits in Children With Learning Disabilities and Persistent Low Achievement: A Five-Year Prospective Study. J. Educat. Psychol. 104, 206–223. doi: 10.1037/a0025398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gniewosz, B., Eccles, J. S., and Noack, P. (2015). Early Adolescents’ Development of Academic Self-Concept and Intrinsic Task Value: The Role of Contextual Feedback. J. Res. Adolesc. 25, 459–473. doi: 10.1111/jora.12140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottfried, A. E. (1982). Relationships between academic intrinsic motivation and anxiety in children and young adolescents. J. School Psychol. 20, 205–215. doi: 10.1016/0022-4405(82)90050-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, J., Liem, G. A. D., Martin, A. J., Colmar, S., Marsh, H. W., and McInerney, D. (2012). Academic motivation, self-concept, engagement, and performance in high school: Key processes from a longitudinal perspective. J. Adolesc. 35, 1111–1122. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grigg, S., Perera, H. N., McIlveen, P., and Svetleff, Z. (2018). Relations among math self efficacy, interest, intentions, and achievement: A social cognitive perspective. Contemp. Educat. Psychol. 53, 73–86. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hart, S. A., Logan, J. A. R., Thompson, L., Kovas, Y., McLoughlin, G., and Petrill, S. A. (2016). A latent profile analysis of math achievement, numerosity, and math anxiety in twins. J. Educat. Psychol. 108, 181–193. doi: 10.1037/edu0000045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hidi, S. (1990). Interest and Its Contribution as a Mental Resource for Learning. Rev. Educat. Res. 60, 549–571. doi: 10.3102/00346543060004549

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hidi, S. (2000). “An interest researcher’s perspective: The effects of extrinsic and intrinsic factors on motivation,” in Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance, eds C. Sansone and J. M. Harackiewicz (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 309–339. doi: 10.1016/b978-012619070-0/50033-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hidi, S., and Anderson, V. (1992). “Situational interest and its impact on reading and expository writing,” in The role of interest in learning and development, eds A. Krapp, S. Hidi, and K. A. Renninger (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 215–238.

Google Scholar

Ho, H.-Z., Senturk, D., Lam, A. G., Zimmer, J. M., Hong, S., Okamoto, Y., et al. (2000). The Affective and Cognitive Dimensions of Math Anxiety: A Cross-National Study. J. Res. Math. Educat. 31, 362–379. doi: 10.2307/749811

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hudson, G. (1999). Essential Introductory Linguistics. New Jersey, NJ: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Jacobs, B., Prentice-Dunn, S., and Rogers, R. W. (1984). Understanding persistence: An interface of control theory and self-efficacy theory. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 5, 333–347. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp0504_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, Y., Rosenzweig, E. Q., and Gaspard, H. (2018). An expectancy-value-cost approach in predicting adolescent students’ academic motivation and achievement. Contemp. Educat. Psychol. 54, 139–152. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Justicia-Galiano, M. J., Martín-Puga, M. E., Linares, R., and Pelegrina, S. (2017). Math anxiety and math performance in children: The mediating roles of working memory and math self-concept. Br. J. Educat. Psychol. 87, 573–589. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krapp, A. (2002). Structural and dynamic aspects of interest development: theoretical considerations from an ontogenetic perspective. Learning Instruct. 12, 383–409. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00011-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krapp, A. (2005). Basic needs and the development of interest and intrinsic motivational orientations. Learning Instruct. 15, 381–395. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2005.07.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kriegbaum, K., Jansen, M., and Spinath, B. (2015). Motivation: A predictor of PISA’s mathematical competence beyond intelligence and prior test achievement. Learning Individ. Differ. 43, 140–148. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.08.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavigne, G. L., Vallerand, R. J., and Miquelon, P. (2007). A motivational model of persistence in science education: A self-determination theory approach. Eur. J. Psychol. Educat. 22:351. doi: 10.1007/BF03173432

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, W., Lee, M.-J., and Bong, M. (2014). Testing interest and self-efficacy as predictors of academic self-regulation and achievement. Contemp. Educat. Psychol. 39, 86–99. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lohbeck, A., Nitkowski, D., and Petermann, F. (2016). A Control-Value Theory Approach: Relationships Between Academic Self-Concept, Interest, and Test Anxiety in Elementary School Children. Child Youth Care Forum 45, 887–904. doi: 10.1007/s10566-016-9362-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lukmani, Y. M. (1972). Motivation to learn and language proficiency. Language Learning 22, 261–273. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-1770.1972.tb00087.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, X., and Xu, J. (2004). The causal ordering of mathematics anxiety and mathematics achievement: a longitudinal panel analysis. J. Adolesc. 27, 165–179. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mac Iver, D. J., Stipek, D. J., and Daniels, D. H. (1991). Explaining within-semester changes in student effort in junior high school and senior high school courses. J. Educat. Psychol. 83, 201–211. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.83.2.201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macher, D., Paechter, M., Papousek, I., and Ruggeri, K. (2012). Statistics anxiety, trait anxiety, learning behavior, and academic performance. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 27, 483–498. doi: 10.1007/s10212-011-0090-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malanchini, M., Rimfeld, K., Wang, Z., Petrill, S. A., Tucker-Drob, E. M., Plomin, R., et al. (2020). Genetic factors underlie the association between anxiety, attitudes and performance in mathematics. Translat. Psychiatry 10:12. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-0711-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., and Martin, A. J. (2011). Academic self-concept and academic achievement: Relations and causal ordering. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 81, 59–77. doi: 10.1348/000709910X503501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., Köller, O., and Baumert, J. (2005). Academic Self-Concept, Interest, Grades, and Standardized Test Scores: Reciprocal Effects Models of Causal Ordering. Child Dev. 76, 397–416. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00853.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., Köller, O., and Baumert, J. (2006). Integration of Multidimensional Self-Concept and Core Personality Constructs: Construct Validation and Relations to Well-Being and Achievement. J. Personal. 74, 403–456. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00380.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McWilliams, M. A., Nier, J. A., and Singer, J. A. (2013). The implicit self and the specificity-matching principle: Implicit self-concept predicts domain-specific outcomes. Personal. Individ. Differ. 54, 474–478. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.09.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meece, J. L., Wigfield, A., and Eccles, J. S. (1990). Predictors of math anxiety and its influence on young adolescents’ course enrollment intentions and performance in mathematics. J. Educ. Psychol. 82, 60–70. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.60

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, H., and Bichsel, J. (2004). Anxiety, working memory, gender, and math performance. Personal. Individ. Differ. 37, 591–606. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mujtaba, T., and Reiss, M. J. (2014). A survey of psychological, motivational, family and perceptions of physics education factors that explain 15-year-old students’ aspirations to study physics in post-compulsory english schools. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 12, 371–393. doi: 10.1007/s10763-013-9404-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murayama, K., Pekrun, R., Lichtenfeld, S., and vom Hofe, R. (2013). Predicting long−term growth in students’ mathematics achievement: The unique contributions of motivation and cognitive strategies. Child Dev. 84, 1475–1490. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Namkung, J. M., Peng, P., and Lin, X. (2019). The Relation Between Mathematics Anxiety and Mathematics Performance Among School-Aged Students: A Meta-Analysis. Rev. Educ. Res. 89, 459–496. doi: 10.3102/0034654319843494

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, B. L. L., Liu, W. C., and Wang, J. C. K. (2016). Student Motivation and Learning in Mathematics and Science: A Cluster Analysis. Int. J. Scie. Math. Educ. 14, 1359–1376. doi: 10.1007/s10763-015-9654-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

OECD (2013). PISA 2012 Assessment and Analytical Framework: Mathematics, Reading, Science, Problem Solving and Financial Literacy. Paris: OECD Publishing.

Google Scholar

OECD (2014). PISA 2012 technical report. Paris: OECD Publishing.

Google Scholar

Parker, P., Marsh, H., Ciarrochi, J., Marshall, S., and Abduljabbar, A. (2013). Juxtaposing math self-efficacy and self-concept as predictors of long-term achievement outcomes. Educ. Psychol. 34:29. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2013.797339

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Passolunghi, M. C., Rueda Ferreira, T. I., and Tomasetto, C. (2014). Math–gender stereotypes and math-related beliefs in childhood and early adolescence. Learning Individ. Differ. 34, 70–76. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patrick, B. C., Skinner, E. A., and Connell, J. P. (1993). What motivates children’s behavior and emotion? Joint effects of perceived control and autonomy in the academic domain. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 65, 781–791. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.4.781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perera, L. D. H. (2014). Parents’ Attitudes Towards Science and their Children’s Science Achievement. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 36, 3021–3041. doi: 10.1080/09500693.2014.949900

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, J. L., and Hyde, J. S. (2017). Trajectories of self-perceived math ability, utility value and interest across middle school as predictors of high school math performance. Educ. Psychol. 37, 438–456. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2015.1076765

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, R. A. (1994). A Meta-Analysis of Cronbach’s Coefficient Alpha. J. Consumer Res. 21, 381–391. doi: 10.1086/209405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pintrich, P. R., and Schrauben, B. (1992). “Students’ motivational beliefs and their cognitive engagement in classroom academic tasks,” in Student perceptions in the classroom, eds J. Meece and D. H. Schunk (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 149–183.

Google Scholar

Ramirez, G., Chang, H., Maloney, E. A., Levine, S. C., and Beilock, S. L. (2016). On the relationship between math anxiety and math achievement in early elementary school: The role of problem solving strategies. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 141, 83–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramirez, G., Shaw, S. T., and Maloney, E. A. (2018). Math anxiety: Past research, promising interventions, and a new interpretation framework. Educ. Psychol. 53, 145–164. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2018.1447384

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raufelder, D., Drury, K., Jagenow, D., Hoferichter, F., and Bukowski, W. (2013). Development and validation of the Relationship and Motivation (REMO) scale to assess students’ perceptions of peers and teachers as motivators in adolescence. Learning Individ. Differ. 24, 182–189. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotgans, J., and Schmidt, H. (2009). Examination of the context−specific nature of self−regulated learning. Educ. Stud. 35, 239–253. doi: 10.1080/03055690802648051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychol. Monogr. General Appl. 80, 1–28. doi: 10.1037/h0092976

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozek, C. S., Hyde, J. S., Svoboda, R. C., Hulleman, C. S., and Harackiewicz, J. M. (2015). Gender differences in the effects of a utility-value intervention to help parents motivate adolescents in mathematics and science. J. Educat. Psychol. 107, 195–206. doi: 10.1037/a0036981

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sax, L. J., Kanny, M. A., Riggers-Piehl, T. A., Whang, H., and Paulson, L. N. (2015). “But I’m not good at math”: The changing salience of mathematical self-concept in shaping women’s and men’s STEM aspirations. Res. Higher Educ. 56, 813–842. doi: 10.1007/s11162-015-9375-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiefele, U. (2009). “Situational and individual interest,” in Handbook of motivation at school, eds K. Wentzel and A. Wigfield (New York, NY: Routledge), 197–222.

Google Scholar

Schiefele, U., and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1995). Motivation and ability as factors in mathematics experience and achievement. J. Res. Math. Educ. 26, 163–181. doi: 10.2307/749208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiefele, U., Schaffner, E., Möller, J., and Wigfield, A. (2012). Dimensions of Reading Motivation and Their Relation to Reading Behavior and Competence. Reading Res. Quart. 47, 427–463. doi: 10.1002/RRQ.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scrucca, L., Fop, M., Murphy, T. B., and Raftery, A. E. (2016). mclust 5: Clustering, Classification and Density Estimation Using Gaussian Finite Mixture Models. R J. 8, 289–317. doi: 10.32614/rj-2016-021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spurk, D., Hirschi, A., Wang, M., Valero, D., and Kauffeld, S. (2020). Latent profile analysis: A review and “how to” guide of its application within vocational behavior research. J. Vocat. Behav. 120:103445. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2020.103445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stupnisky, R. H., Renaud, R. D., Perry, R. P., Ruthig, J. C., Haynes, T. L., and Clifton, R. A. (2007). Comparing self-esteem and perceived control as predictors of first-year college students’ academic achievement. Soc. Psychol. Educ. Int. J. 10, 303–330. doi: 10.1007/s11218-007-9020-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobias, S. (1994). Interest, Prior Knowledge, and Learning. Rev. Educat. Res. 64, 37–54. doi: 10.2307/1170745

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasetto, C., Mirisola, A., Galdi, S., and Cadinu, M. (2015). Parents’ math–gender stereotypes, children’s self-perception of ability, and children’s appraisal of parents’ evaluations in 6-year-olds. Contemp. Educat. Psychol. 42, 186–198. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tornare, E., Czajkowski, N. O., and Pons, F. (2015). Children’s emotions in math problem solving situations: Contributions of self-concept, metacognitive experiences, and performance. Learning Instruct. 39, 88–96. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.05.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., Nagy, N., Lenski, A., Niggli, A., and Schnyder, I. (2015). Using individual interest and conscientiousness to predict academic effort: Additive, synergistic, or compensatory effects? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 109, 142–162. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trautwein, U., Marsh, H. W., Nagengast, B., Lüdtke, O., Nagy, G., and Jonkmann, K. (2012). Probing for the multiplicative term in modern expectancy–value theory: A latent interaction modeling study. J. Educat. Psychol. 104, 763–777. doi: 10.1037/a0027470

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trezise, K., and Reeve, R. A. (2014). Working memory, worry, and algebraic ability. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 121, 120–136. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C. K. J., Liu, W. C., Nie, Y., Chye, Y. L. S., Lim, B. S. C., Liem, G. A., et al. (2017). Latent profile analysis of students’ motivation and outcomes in mathematics: an organismic integration theory perspective. Heliyon 3:e00308. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2017.e00308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, W.-T., and Wang, C.-C. (2009). An empirical study of instructor adoption of web-based learning systems. Comp. Educ. 53, 761–774. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2009.02.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Lukowski, S. L., Hart, S. A., Lyons, I. M., Thompson, L. A., Kovas, Y., et al. (2015). Is math anxiety always bad for math learning? The role of math motivation. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1863–1876. doi: 10.1177/0956797615602471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Shakeshaft, N., Schofield, K., and Malanchini, M. (2018). Anxiety is not enough to drive me away: A latent profile analysis on math anxiety and math motivation. PLoS One 13:e0192072. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0192072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wigfield, A. (1994). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation: A developmental perspective. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 6, 49–78. doi: 10.1007/BF02209024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wigfield, A., and Cambria, J. (2010). Students’ achievement values, goal orientations, and interest: Definitions, development, and relations to achievement outcomes. Dev. Rev. 30, 1–35. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2009.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wigfield, A., and Meece, J. L. (1988). Math anxiety in elementary and secondary school students. J. Educ. Psychol. 80, 210–216. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.80.2.210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

You, S., Hong, S., and Ho, H.-Z. (2011). Longitudinal Effects of Perceived Control on Academic Achievement. J. Educ. Res. 104, 253–266. doi: 10.1080/00220671003733807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, C. H. (2012). Examining the relationships among academic self-concept, instrumental motivation, and TIMSS 2007 science scores: a cross-cultural comparison of five East Asian countries/regions and the United States. Educ. Res. Eval. 18, 713–731. doi: 10.1080/13803611.2012.718511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: math achievement, math self-concept, perceived control in math, persistence, math anxiety, LPA, math interest

Citation: Xiao F and Sun L (2021) Students’ Motivation and Affection Profiles and Their Relation to Mathematics Achievement, Persistence, and Behaviors. Front. Psychol. 11:533593. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.533593

Received: 09 February 2020; Accepted: 10 December 2020;
Published: 14 January 2021.

Edited by:

Jin Eun Yoo, Korea National University of Education, South Korea

Reviewed by:

Graziella Orrù, University of Pisa, Italy
Eun Hye Ham, Kongju National University, South Korea

Copyright © 2021 Xiao and Sun. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Feiya Xiao, feiyaxiao@hotmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.