Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 02 September 2020
Sec. Psychology of Language

Commentary: A Compositional Neural Architecture for Language

  • 1Vivian L. Smith Department of Neurosurgery, McGovern Medical School, University of Texas Health Science Center, Houston, TX, United States
  • 2Texas Institute for Restorative Neurotechnologies, University of Texas Health Science Center, Houston, TX, United States

A Commentary on
A Compositional Neural Architecture for Language

by Martin, A. E. (2020). J. Cogn. Neurosci. 38, 1–20. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01552

Martin (2020) defends a model of linguistic computation relying on gain modulation. I will argue that any gain modulation model of language faces a number of internal conceptual and empirical difficulties, and that our current understanding of gain modulation does not support its use as a core component of linguistic computation.

The Proposal

Martin (2020) defends a multidimensional coordinate system for language based on neurophysiological models of sensory processing. With ambitious scope, Martin aims to connect theories of processing and neural implementation, an extraordinarily challenging undertaking. Martin maintains that neural trajectories (of these coordinates) encode sensory, motor, and abstract linguistic states. Gain modulation tunes the path of these trajectories in accordance with behavior. Martin claims that increasingly abstract linguistic structures (from syllables to morphemes to words) are inferred via gain modulation which she defines as “the way neurons combine information from two or more sources.”

Martin (2020) reports how gain modulation underlies “coordinate transform between sensory modalities and between sensory and motor systems.” She cites classic work on cellular dynamics (Salinas and Abbott, 2001), but does not cite other work (e.g., Dayan and Abbott, 2001) where the limitations of gain modulation are stressed, e.g., gaze-dependent gain modulation of retinotopic visual receptive fields is well-reported, but the relevance of gain modulation to higher cognition is not established [(Dayan and Abbott, 2001), p. 17].

Martin (2020) makes an additional claim that “gain modulation also offers a built-in system for predictive coding,” citing Friston (2005); more recent, updated accounts (e.g., Friston, 2018) rely on an interplay of a range of other processes. Moreover, Friston's ventures into language (i.e., Friston et al., 2017) rely on other neural mechanisms (belief propagation in neuronal process theories, phase precession, theta-gamma coupling) employing distinct softmax functions, logarithmic transforms, and linear algebra (implemented via firing rate functions, non-linear postsynaptic responses, and neuronal connectivity, respectively).

Potential advantages of Martin's model are the multiple-realizability of gain modulation and the fact that her model relies on simple changes to a highly conserved, generic neural process—an elegant framework for language evolution.

A Reassessment

Ferguson and Cardin (2020) provide a comprehensive review of gain modulation, pointing to a common set of mechanisms: GABAergic inhibition, synaptically driven fluctuations in membrane potential, and changes in cellular conductance. Ferguson and Cardin (2020) note that diverse cortical functions such as information integration across cognitive, sensory, and motor systems seem to be performed through gain modulation. Further, regulation of neural gain can provide “an integration mechanism whereby information from multiple sources can be non-linearly combined via multiplicative modulation of the cell's response to inputs” [Ferguson and Cardin, 2020, p. 81]. Martin (2020) defends an additive, rather than a tensor-based multiplicative, formulation of linguistic compositionality. Ferguson and Cardin (2020) review how visual properties that must be decoded separately in higher-order areas of non-human primate visual cortex are combined multiplicatively (e.g., object identity), whereas parts that must be integrated (e.g., object sub-parts) are combined additively. It may be that additive-only models can capture integrative language functions, but that structural (hence, syntactic) separation may require multiplicative modulation.

Ferguson and Cardin (2020, p. 88) note that gain modulation may be able to increase information transmission and provide computational efficiency within a given network. Nevertheless, it remains unknown how gain modulation at the single-cell level contributes to population coding that can be read out by downstream targets. The authors conclude that “gain is regulated by a wide range of influences, including attention, learning, locomotion” [(Ferguson and Cardin, 2020), p. 89]. While the role of gain modulation in sensory-to-motor conversions is well-established (evidence that left premotor regions are implicated in language comprehension can be found in Keitel et al., 2018; see also Molinaro and Lizarazu, 2018; Woolnough et al., 2019; Poeppel and Assaneo, 2020), and information transfer between motor and higher-order cortical structures has been shown [crucially via cross-frequency coupling; (Keitel et al., 2018)], gain modulation has not been implicated in higher-order aspects of language in non-motor regions (i.e., components of the cortical language network). As such, it seems premature to use gain modulation as a core component of higher-level linguistic computation.

Ferguson and Cardin (2020, p. 89) also note that “the precise relationship between gain modulation of single neurons and the encoding and transmission of information at the population level is not well-understood,” and that “the reliability and repeatability of gain modulation of single neurons and cortical networks is unknown.” Given this, it seems not well-motivated to stipulate a close connection between gain modulation and oscillatory activity in language. Since gain modulation appears relevant to enabling networks of neurons to produce distributed representations of stimulus features (as opposed to generating inferences to higher abstract structures from such features), it may be that gain modulation goes the way of entrainment: Initially viewed as a crucial mechanism for all manner of linguistic processes (from syllables to sentences) before being relegated by some to a role in (possibly higher-order) speech perception (Keitel et al., 2018; Meyer et al., 2019; Murphy, 2020).

Advancing on and moving away from previous theoretical assumptions (Martin, 2016), Martin (2020) now proposes that cortical oscillations structure speech input into linguistic representations via gain-modulated multiplexing. Multiplexing models of language have been proposed elsewhere, and it is unclear how (Martin, 2020) gain-modulated multiplexing is a step forward. For instance, a range of papers (Murphy, 2015, 2016, 2018, 2020; Murphy and Benítez-Burraco, 2017; Benítez-Burraco and Murphy, 2019) utilize multiplexing as operationalized via cross-frequency coupling, and there is ample empirical support for the involvement of this mechanism in aspects of language processing beyond semantic compositionality, such as prediction and syntactic categorization (see Murphy, 2020 and references therein). In addition, testing the claims in Martin (2020) will require a deeper understanding of the relationship between gain modulation and processes that we can more easily record in the human brain. Lastly, while Martin (2020) presents a list of non-specific predictions pertaining to “low frequency power” increases during “structure” building (an eventuality which would not exclusively support a gain modulation model), Murphy (2020) presents specific predictions about distinct low frequency band increases, suppressions, and coupling dynamics, and can hence be evaluated more clearly.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

My thanks go to the reviewer for their helpful comments which have greatly improved the argumentation of this article.

References

Benítez-Burraco, A., and Murphy, E. (2019). Why brain oscillations are improving our understanding of language. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:190. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayan, P., and Abbott, L. F. (2001). Theoretical Neuroscience: Computational and Mathematical Modeling of Neural Systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Ferguson, K. A., and Cardin, J. A. (2020). Mechanisms underlying gain modulation in the cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 80–92. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0253-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 360, 815–836. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2018). Does predictive coding have a future? Nat. Neurosci. 21, 1019–1021.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Rosch, R., Parr, T., Price, C., and Bowman, H. (2017). Deep temporal models and active inference. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 77, 388–402. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keitel, A., Gross, J., and Kayser, C. (2018). Perceptually relevant speech tracking in auditory and motor cortex reflects distinct linguistic features. PLoS Biol. 16:e2004473. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2004473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, A. (2016). Language processing as cue integration: grounding the psychology of language in perception and neurophysiology. Front. Psychol. 7:120. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, A. E. (2020). A compositional neural architecture for language. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 38, 1–20. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, L., Sun, Y., and Martin, A. E. (2019). Synchronous, but not entrained: exogenous and endogenous cortical rhythms of speech and language processing. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/4s83k

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molinaro, N., and Lizarazu, M. (2018). Delta(but not theta)-band cortical entrainment involves speech-specific processing. Eur. J. Neurosci. 48, 2642–2650. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, E. (2015). The brain dynamics of linguistic computation. Front. Psychol. 6:1515. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, E. (2016). A theta-gamma neural code for feature set composition with phase-entrained delta nestings. UCL Work. Pap. Linguist. 28, 1–23.

Google Scholar

Murphy, E. (2018). “Interfaces (travelling oscillations) + recursion (delta-theta code) = language,” in The Talking Species: Perspectives on the Evolutionary, Neuronal and Cultural Foundations of Language, eds E. Luef, and M. Manuela (Graz: Unipress Graz Verlag), 251–269.

Google Scholar

Murphy, E. (2020). The Oscillatory Nature of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Murphy, E., and Benítez-Burraco, A. (2017). Language deficits in schizophrenia and autism as related oscillatory connectomopathies: an evolutionary account. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 83, 742–764. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poeppel, D., and Assaneo, M. F. (2020). Speech rhythms and their neural foundations. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 322–334. doi: 10.1038/s41583-020-0304-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salinas, E., and Abbott, L. F. (2001). Coordinate transformations in the visual system: how to generate gain fields and what to compute with them. Prog. Brain Res. 130, 175–190. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(01)30012-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolnough, O., Forseth, K. J., Rollo, P. S., and Tandon, N. (2019). Uncovering the functional anatomy of the human insula during speech. eLife 8:e53086. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: gain modulation, neural oscillations, semantics, syntax, compositionality

Citation: Murphy E (2020) Commentary: A Compositional Neural Architecture for Language. Front. Psychol. 11:2101. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02101

Received: 17 June 2020; Accepted: 28 July 2020;
Published: 02 September 2020.

Edited by:

Niels Janssen, University of La Laguna, Spain

Reviewed by:

Nicola Molinaro, Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, Spain

Copyright © 2020 Murphy. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elliot Murphy, elliotmurphy91@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.