Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 24 July 2020
Sec. Movement Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Best Practice Approaches to the Study of Cognitive Functioning and Physical Activity/Sports View all 21 articles

Physical Exercise and Fitness Level Are Related to Cognitive and Psychosocial Functioning in Adolescents

  • 1University of Malaga, Málaga, Spain
  • 2Department of Social Psychology, Social Anthropology Social Work and Social Services, University of Malaga, Málaga, Spain
  • 3Department of Evolutionary Psychology and Education, University of Malaga, Málaga, Spain

The purpose of this study was to analyze the relationships among physical exercise and fitness with selective attention, concentration, processing speed, general self-efficacy, self-rated health, and satisfaction with life. 208 adolescents between 14 and 16 years, from the city of Malaga (Spain), participated in the study. A comparative and predictive design was used to carry out this research. The instruments used for the evaluation were the Tanita® BC-601 Body Composition Monitor, the Eurofit Physical Condition Test Battery, the D2 Test of Attention, the WISC-IV Symbol Search and Coding tests, the General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSE), the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-28), and the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS). Analysis of variance (ANOVA), Kruskal–Wallis test, correlation analysis and linear regression were used to contrast the research objectives. The results indicated that adolescents who practiced more hours of physical exercise per week and were in better physical fitness achieved higher scores in selective attention, concentration, processing speed, general self-efficacy, self-rated health, and satisfaction with life. In addition, cardiorespiratory fitness was the physical fitness variable most closely related to and predictive of cognitive and psychosocial functioning. Cardiorespiratory fitness was predictor of all the variables analyzed, except the factor anxiety and insomnia (self-rated health), and life satisfaction that were predicted by horizontal jump measurements and fat mass, respectively. Thus, the study findings indicate that adolescents who practiced more weekly physical exercise and had a higher level of physical fitness scored better on the cognitive functioning and psychosocial tests evaluated. The data suggest that engaging in physical exercise and fitness in adolescence may be appropriate to improve health and well-being, contributing to better development at this stage.

Introduction

Numerous studies have pointed out the physical, psychological, and social benefits that physical exercise can bring to adolescents (Esteban-Cornejo et al., 2015; Swann et al., 2018). Among them, the relationships between the practice of physical exercise with cognitive, and psychosocial functioning at these ages are relevant (Eime et al., 2013; Lubans et al., 2016; Cooper et al., 2018; Pontifex et al., 2019). On the one hand, adolescents with adequate cognitive functioning improve their adaptation to the environment, increase the likelihood of success in numerous everyday tasks, and contribute to their future mental health (Lubans et al., 2016; Zmyj et al., 2017). Furthermore, cognitive functioning is related to social cognition, which is essential for effective social interactions (Gil et al., 2012). On the other hand, in adolescence identity is formed, interpersonal bonds are established, and psychosocial skills are developed (Viholainen et al., 2014; Wilson et al., 2014). Physical exercise or sport often takes place in contexts of continuous social interaction and comparison, requiring the acquisition of skills necessary for good psychosocial development (Holt, 2008; Swann et al., 2018). Therefore, practicing physical exercise regularly and in appropriate environments could facilitate better adaptation to the environment, contributing to greater well-being and quality of life (Lubans et al., 2016).

In this context of study, it has been highlighted in recent years that only practicing physical exercise does not ensure health benefits. Some research has pointed to the need to achieve an adequate level of physical fitness to develop improvements in cognitive aspects and optimize psychosocial functioning in people (Herting et al., 2014; Ho et al., 2015; Kantomaa et al., 2015; Fraguela-Vale et al., 2016; Reloba-Martínez et al., 2017). So, various studies have suggested in recent years that cardiorespiratory fitness is the best predictor of optimal cognitive and psychosocial functioning (Becerra-Fernández et al., 2013; Chaddock et al., 2014; Herting et al., 2014; Reigal et al., 2014).

Specifically, in recent years much progress has been made in understanding how physical exercise is related to cognitive functioning. Advances in neuroscientific knowledge (Chaddock et al., 2014), making it possible to resolve questions that were unknown decades ago, are supporting the findings in this area. The use of techniques such as electroencephalography, functional magnetic resonance imaging, and magnetoencephalography, together with the study of proteins such as BDNF (brain-derived neurotrophic factor), IGF-1 (insulin-like growth factor-1), and VEGF (vascular endothelial growth factor), are helping to achieve essential milestones for the development of this field of knowledge (e.g., Tari et al., 2019; Voss et al., 2019). For example, there is evidence of structural changes in the brain associated with increases in physical fitness, and this is a reliable indicator of the impact that physical exercise is thought to have on the brain (Esteban-Cornejo et al., 2017; Chaddock et al., 2018). Also, Chaddock et al. (2010) observed positive relationships among aerobic performance with hippocampal volume and the striated dorsal body. Furthermore, Chaddock et al. (2018) found an increase in the white matter microstructure of the genu of the corpus callosum after a program of physical exercise. In another study, Esteban-Cornejo et al. (2017) noted relationship among cardiorespiratory capacity and speed/agility with the volume of gray matter in various areas of the brain.

Thus, some research has highlighted the relationships among physical exercise and variables such as attention, concentration, working memory, inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, processing speed, and language (e.g., Liu et al., 2018; Ludyga et al., 2018; Westfall et al., 2018; Xue et al., 2019). Specifically, attention has been an object of interest in a number of studies (Budde et al., 2008; Vanhelst et al., 2016). The ways in which selective attention and concentration are related to physical exercise and physical condition have been analyzed and positive associations have been found between them (Guiney and Machado, 2013; Tine, 2014; Reloba-Martínez et al., 2017). Likewise, cognitive processing speed has been explored, also indicating positive links with physical performance (Hillman et al., 2005; Pontifex et al., 2011). Both selective attention, which is the ability to attend to a particular series of stimuli and ignore others (Giuliano et al., 2014), and concentration or processing speed are factors that affect the efficiency with which multiple tasks are performed, such as academic or social tasks (Perlman et al., 2014; Rabiner et al., 2016).

Also, other studies have pointed out relationships between physical exercise and psychosocial variables such as life satisfaction, self-efficacy, and perception of health. Self-efficacy constitutes the judgments made about one’s own abilities and their effectiveness in carrying out a task (Bandura, 1986). Its development is complex and is thought to depend on factors such as previous successes, vicarious experience, verbal persuasion, and physiological states (Bandura, 1986, 1997). Many authors suggest that self-efficacy is specific; others, however, argue for the existence of general self-efficacy (Schwarzer, 1992; Schwarzer and Jerusalem, 1995). Perception of health refers to the judgments people make about the level of physical or mental health they possess, and it can be a predictor of levels of mortality and future disease, even in a young population (Bombak, 2013; Kantomaa et al., 2015). Finally, satisfaction with life involves an overall assessment of what life itself is like, the conditions in which it is developed, whether the expectations created have been fulfilled or are being achieved, etc., and it is one of the indicators that are usually evaluated when analyzing subjective well-being (Diener et al., 1985; Miller et al., 2019).

The analysis of well-being has been carried out from different models (Ryff, 1989; Keyes et al., 2002). The hedonic tradition presents well-being in subjective terms and is linked to aspects such as life satisfaction. From the eudaimonic tradition it is linked to human potential and has been studied under the construct called psychological well-being (Ryff, 1989). Specifically, Keyes’s model extends Ryff’s to speak of social well-being, and considers that mental health requires positive psychosocial functioning (Keyes et al., 2002). From this point of view, emphasis has been placed on the so-called psychosocial well-being, which would refer to aspects such as perceptions of themselves, the ability to function effectively in the environment or the perception of health (Pinquart and Silbereisen, 2004). For this reason, the analysis of variables such as self-efficacy, perception of health or life satisfaction are relevant to determine the mental health or well-being in people. Therefore, analyze whether physical exercise could be related to these variables becomes very important, due to the repercussion that it can have on the health and quality of life of adolescents.

So, previous research has revealed positive relationships among physical exercise and physical fitness with self-efficacy (Ho et al., 2015), perception of health (Bombak, 2013; Kantomaa et al., 2015), and life satisfaction (Zullig and White, 2011; Fraguela-Vale et al., 2016). In general, appropriate engagement in physical exercise and sport is considered an effective tool for improving numerous psychological issues in adolescence and other stages of life (Lubans et al., 2016).

The aim of this study was to determine if the weekly physical exercise volumen and physical fitness are related to cognitive and psychpsocial functioning in a sample of adolescents. For this purpose, we first analyzed the differences between various groups divided by hours of weekly physical exercise, and secondly, we evaluated whether there were correlations between the study variables, as well as whether physical fitness could predict measures of cognitive functioning and psychosocial variables studied.

Materials and Methods

Study Design

This is a comparative and predictive study (Ato et al., 2013).

Participants

A total of 208 adolescents (boys = 50.96%, girls = 49.04%) aged between 14 and 16 years from the city of Malaga (Spain) were included in the study [mean (M) ± standard deviation (SD): age = 15.25 ± 0.74 years; height = 167.65 ± 9.67 cm; weight = 63.75 ± 14.76 kg; and body mass index (BMI) = 22.59 ± 4.29 kg/m2]. The initial inclusion criteria was to be between 14 and 16 years old. Those who had significant health problems that did not allow them to carry out the evaluation tests following the specified protocol (e.g., physical injury), failed to provide informed consent, or failed to complete the tests correctly were excluded. Out of 224 possible participants, the sample was finally made up of the 208 indicated. Sampling was not probabilistic, it was chosen for convenience.

Instruments and Measures

(a) D2 Test of Attention (Brickenkamp, 2002). This was used to analyze selective attention and concentration. Performing this test requires discriminating among 47 elements in each of the 14 rows that make up the test (658 elements in total). Each row is completed in 20 s, working from left to right and from top to bottom. The stimuli contain the letters d or p and may be accompanied by one or two dashes located at the top, bottom, or both. The d’s, which are considered relevant stimuli, must be crossed out when they have 2 dashes in any position. The following scores can be obtained: total effectiveness in the test/selective attention index (TOT) and concentration index (CON).

(b) Wechsler Intelligence Scale Coding and Symbol Search tests for children (WISC-IV; Wechsler, 2005). These tests basically assess cognitive processing speed, but also attention, or cognitive flexibility. The Coding test consists of copying a set of symbols, associated with a number, in a certain order. The Symbol Search test involves observing two groups of symbols and indicating whether any of them coincide. The tests have a completion time of 120 s. A Processing Speed Index is obtained from the results.

(c) General Self-Efficacy Scale (Schwarzer and Jerusalem, 1995; Baessler and Schwarzer, 1996; Sanjuán et al., 2000). This consists of 10 items and analyzes the perception of one’s competence to handle a wide range of situations. It is evaluated with scores between 1 (strongly disagree) and 10 (strongly agree). The internal consistency (Cronbach’s Alpha) value for this study was 0.84.

(d) General Health Questionnaire in its 28-item version (GHQ-28; Goldberg, 1978; Lobo et al., 1986). This was initially designed to assess psychiatric disorders in a community setting and in non-psychiatric clinical settings, although it has subsequently been used for other populations. It is composed of 28 items and explores the following 4 dimensions: somatic symptoms, anxiety and insomnia, social dysfunction, and severe depression. It is answered with scores from 0 (absence of health problems) to 3 (presence of health problems). The internal consistency (Cronbach’s Alpha) values for this study were somatic symptoms = 0.83, anxiety and insomnia = 0.74, social dysfunction = 0.78, and severe depression = 0.81.

(e) Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al., 1985; Atienza et al., 2000). This analyzes life satisfaction and is made up of 5 items. It is answered with scores between 1 (strongly disagree) and 7 (strongly agree). The internal consistency (Cronbach’s Alpha) value for this study was 0.81.

(f) Anthropometric and physical fitness measurements. To describe the sample, height and weight were analyzed using a conventional measuring rod and scale, respectively. The fat mass percentage was measured with a bioimpedance meter (Tanita® BC-601 Body Composition Monitor). Explosive power in the lower body was evaluated with the horizontal jump test (Eurofit, 1993). Speed was asssessed using the 5 × 10 meter test (Eurofit, 1993). Maximum oxygen consumption was obtained indirectly with the Course Navette test (Léger et al., 1988; Eurofit, 1993), an incremental round trip test over 20 meters, increasing the speed by 0.5 km/h every minute from 8.5 km/h. Oxygen consumption was calculated using the formula VO2max = 31.025 + 3.238S–3.248A + 0.1536SA, where S is the speed reached in the last completed stage and A is the age of the participant.

Procedure

The sample was obtained in schools fron the city of Malaga (Spain) by contacting each school and requesting permission for adolescents to participate. In addition, informed consent was obtained from the parents or legal guardians of the participants. Throughout the process, the ethical principles set forth in the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013) were respected. In addition, this study is part of a line of research that has been positively evaluated by the Ethics Committee of the University of Malaga (No. 243, CEUMA Registry No.: 18-2015-H).

Anthropometric and physical condition evaluations were carried out in Physical Education classes. Cognitive assessment was conducted in a noise-free classroom and in groups. The questionnaires were self-administered and completed in a group, in a normal classroom. In addition, written data were obtained on the number of hours spent each day in physical exercise, using a simple form. Specifically, information was collected on structured physical activity, but not that which was not (e.g., walking to school, shopping, climbing the stairs, etc.). That is, whether it was federated or not, only the information related to the training of a sport or that activity that was carried out with a specific purpose was collected (e.g., running, playing basketball with friends, etc.). On the basis of this information, the sample was divided into three groups: Group 1, low level of physical exercise (less than 2 h per week); Group 2, moderate level of physical exercise (2 to 4 h per week); and Group 3, high level of physical exercise (more than 4 h per week).

Data Analysis

Descriptive and inferential analyses were performed. The Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was used to analyze the normality of the data. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) were used to assess differences between the groups. Bonferroni statistic would be used to analyze multiple post hoc comparisons if there were significant differences between groups. Cohen’s d was used to estimate the size of the effect between groups. Correlations were assessed with the Pearson and Spearman coefficients. In order to ascertain the predictive capacity of physical condition for the other variables, linear regression analyses (successive steps) were used (Ruiz-Barquín, 2008). The SPSS computer program, version 20.0, was used for statistical processing.

Results

Descriptive Analysis and Normality of Data

Tables 1, 2 show the descriptive statistics and the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test for the total sample and each group, divided by hours of activity per week. The results indicated normality problems in some variables (bold text, Table 2). The ln(x), x2, and 1/x algorithms were used to correct this. All variables were adjusted.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean and standard deviation of physical fitness assessment tests and cognitive and well-being indicators.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Skewness, kurtosis, and Kolmogorov–Smirnov statistic of physical fitness, cognitive, and psychological assessment tests.

Inter-Group Mean Differences

The ANOVAs performed indicated that there were differences between the groups in the variables fat mass percentage, horizontal jump test, VO2max, 5 × 10 speed test, D2-TOT, D2-CON, Symbol Search, Coding, Processing Speed, general self-efficacy, somatic symptoms, anxiety and insomnia, and life satisfaction. There were no differences between groups in social dysfunction, and severe depression (GHQ). Table 3 shows the comparisons between groups (with Bonferroni correction) for each variable. Furthermore, Levene’s test indicated that there was homegeneity between group variances in each case (p < 0.05).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Inter-group (post-hoc) comparisons for each variable with significant differences.

Correlation and Linear Regression Analysis

Tables 4, 5 shows the correlations between measures of physical fitness and cognitive functioning. There were significant relationships among them. Maximum oxygen consumption was the measure of physical fitness that best correlated with the measures of cognitive functioning.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Correlation between measures of physical fitness and cognitive functioning.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Correlation between measures of physical fitness and psychosocial variables.

Table 6 shows the linear regression analyses (successive steps) with which we attempted to identify the physical fitness variables that predict the values of the psychological measures analyzed. The models meet the assumptions of linearity in the relationship between predictor variables and criteria, homoscedasticity, and normal distribution of residuals whose mean value is 0 with a standard deviation of almost 1 (0.99). The Durbin–Watson value was between 1.60 and 2.05, which is appropriate according to Pardo and Ruiz (2005), indicating that it can be assumed that the residuals are independent and the assumption of independence of the independent variables with respect to the dependent variable is met. The models obtained included a single variable, in most cases maximum oxygen consumption (Table 6).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Linear regression analysis (successive steps).

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to analyze how physical exercise and physical fitness are related to certain cognitive and psychosocial functioning variables. The results obtained show relationships between the measures studied and therefore meet the objective of the research. Specifically, the data reveal differences in the measures evaluated in favor of adolescents who performed more hours of physical exercise per week, as well as significant associations of physical fitness with cognitive functioning and the various psychosocial indicators.

Firstly, we observed that adolescents who were physically active for a larger number of hours achieved higher scores in measures of cognitive functioning, specifically selective attention, concentration, and speed of processing. This is in line with previous studies that identify a relationship between physical exercise and these measures in the adolescent population (Hillman et al., 2005; Pontifex et al., 2011; Guiney and Machado, 2013; Tine, 2014; Reloba-Martínez et al., 2017). In addition, it is noteworthy that there were no differences between adolescents with low and moderate levels of physical exercise (Groups 1 and 2). That is, only those who engaged in a high number of hours of activity per week (Group 3) showed significant differences from those with lower levels. This coincides with previous research that draws attention to the need for physical exercise to attain a certain degree of intensity and frequency in order to produce functional brain changes (Herting et al., 2014; Reloba-Martínez et al., 2017).

There is therefore no guarantee that doing a certain number of hours of physical exercise per week or at a certain intensity will explain this phenomenon, so to understand it better we need to monitor the level of physical fitness achieved. In this study, the groups into which the sample was divided had different levels of physical fitness, and the group that engaged in the most hours of physical exercise per week achieved the best results. This is consistent with the arguments of authors such as Chaddock et al. (2018) or Esteban-Cornejo et al. (2017), who consider that improvement in physical fitness is an suitable indicator to explain cognitive changes. It is also in line with other data obtained, which show that measures of physical fitness are significantly correlated with attention, concentration, and processing speed. Specifically, cardiorespiratory fitness, assessed by indirect calculation of maximum oxygen consumption, was a particularly significant value; indeed, it was the main predictor in the linear regression models. This connects with other studies which identity this variable as the one that best explains cognitive functioning (Chaddock et al., 2014; Herting et al., 2014; Reloba-Martínez et al., 2017).

As described in previous research, physical exercise contributes to brain plasticity. In other words, it helps the brain to be more prepared to change its functioning. Possibly only exercise does not produce the change, but it does produce greater sensitivity for it to occur. And for this, a series of physiological phenomena occur so that the brain is more willing to be modified. However, for this to occur a high impact on the body must be produced, which would be reflected in the level of fitness reached by the person (Chaddock et al., 2010, 2018; Esteban-Cornejo et al., 2017). That is why, in recent years, the data supports that physical exercise of moderate and high intensity and frequency would better explain the changes produced in the functioning of the brain. In addition, these studies show that exercises that increase cardiorespiratory fitness, cause greater synthesis of biomolecules such as BDNF or IGF-1, and facilitate volume increase in cortical and subcortical gray matter (Esteban-Cornejo et al., 2017; Tari et al., 2019).

Secondly, differences between the groups in our study according to the amount of physical exercise undertaken indicate that the most active and fittest group achieved the best scores for psychosocial indicators. These results support previous findings that highlight this phenomenon in populations of similar ages (Eime et al., 2013; Lubans et al., 2016), and specifically they are consistent with other work that assesses general self-efficacy, perception of health, and satisfaction with life (Zullig and White, 2011; Bombak, 2013; Ho et al., 2015; Kantomaa et al., 2015; Fraguela-Vale et al., 2016).

Furthermore, this previous research indicates that physical fitness was a determining factor in assessing the effects of physical exercise on these psychological variables. It is pertinent to point out that in our study some dimensions of the GHQ, such as social dysfunction or serious depression, showed no differences between the groups divided according to hours of physical exercise per week. However, when correlation and linear regression analyses were performed, physical fitness was shown to be significantly related to these factors. This coincides with the arguments put forward in other studies, which consider it necessary to assess physical fitness to better understand how physical exercise relates to these types of variables (Reigal et al., 2014). In addition, cardiorespiratory fitness also emerges, in most cases, as the physical fitness factor most closely related to the other variables, which is in line with what has been described in other studies (Becerra-Fernández et al., 2013).

Probably, the relationship between physical exercise, fitness and psychosocial functioning is justified for multiple reasons. Among them, when physical exercise is performed, personal skills are increased. Not only the physical ones, which is obvious, but also those derived from the social context in which it develops. Thus, when doing physical exercise, you have to interact with other people and you have to learn to improve social skills. Therefore, learning would be generated that would affect the perception of personal competence. In addition, there is an improvement in physical health, but also a greater subjective feeling of well-being caused by enjoyment with the activity that is performed.

This work has some limitations. First, the sample was made up of both boys and girls, but the analyses have not been differentiated by gender for each group. Future research should seek to verify whether the results are similar for each gender. Second, the design employed does not allow for the establishment of causal relationships. Thus, quasi-experimental or longitudinal designs could provide valuable information about changes in the variables studied due to the practice of physical exercise. Finally, it would be interesting in subsequent research to assess the relationships between measures of cognitive and psychosocial functioning, given that more complete profiles could be established on how these variables are associated with each other. In any case, the results of this study provide valuable information on the relationships between physical exercise, physical fitness, and cognitive and psychosocial functioning in adolescents, which suggests the need to continue promoting the practice of physical exercise among the young population.

Conclusion

The study findings indicate that adolescents who practiced more weekly physical exercise and had a higher level of physical fitness scored better on the cognitive functioning and psychosocial tests evaluated. Besides, cardiorespiratory fitness was predictor of all the variables analyzed, except the factor anxiety and insomnia (self-rated health), and life satisfaction that were predicted by horizontal jump measurements and fat mass, respectively. Among others, if increasing the level of physical exercise and fitness can affect their cognitive and psychosocial functioning, it would be contributing to having a healthier life and increasing their ability to adapt to the daily demands of life. Thus, adolescents must face multiple situations, such as academic ones, social interactions, etc., that could be favored by the continued practice of physical exercise. For all this, and referring to the evidence collected on this subject, it is necessary to indicate that despite the large amount of leisure time activities availables to adolescents today, active lifestyle habits are likely to be among those that bring the most direct and indirect benefits to their health and well-being. Therefore, any effort to develop this type of behavior will have been worthwhile.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Malaga. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

AH-M, VM-S, RJ-R, and RR participated in the study design and data collection, performed the statistical analyses, contributed to the interpretation of the results, wrote the manuscript, approved the final manuscript as presented, reviewed and provided feedback to the manuscript, and made substantial contributions to the final manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Atienza, F. L., Pons, D., Balaguer, I., and García-Merita, M. (2000). Propiedades psicométricas de la Escala de Satisfacción con la Vida en adolescentes [Psychometric properties of the life satisfaction scale in adolescents]. Psicothema 12, 314–319.

Google Scholar

Ato, M., López-García, J. J., and Benavente, A. (2013). A classification system for research designs in psychology. An. Picol. 29, 1038–1059. doi: 10.6018/analesps.29.3.178511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baessler, J., and Schwarzer, R. (1996). Evaluation of self-efficacy: spanish adaptation of the general self-efficacy scale. Anxiety Stress 2, 1–8.

Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York: Freeman.

Google Scholar

Becerra-Fernández, C. A., Reigal, R. E., Hernández-Mendo, A., and Martín-Tamayo, I. (2013). Relaciones de la condición física y la composición corporal con la autopercepción de salud [Relationship between physical condition and body composition with self-perception of health]. Rev. Int. Cienc. Deporte 9, 305–318. doi: 10.5232/ricyde2013.03401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bombak, A. E. (2013). Self-rated health and public health: a critical perspective. Front. Public Health 1:3401. doi: 10.5232/ricyde2013.03401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brickenkamp, R. (2002). D-2. Attention Task. Madrid: TEA ediciones.

Google Scholar

Budde, H., Voelcker-Rehage, C., Pietraßyk-Kendziorra, S., Ribeiro, P., and Tidow, G. (2008). Acute coordinative exercise improves attentional performance in adolescents. Neurosci. Lett. 441, 219–223. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.06.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaddock, L., Erickson, K. I., Holtrop, J. L., Voss, M. W., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., et al. (2014). Aerobic fitness is associated with greater white matter integrity in children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:584. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaddock, L., Erickson, K. I., Kienzler, C., Drollette, E., Raine, L., Kao, S. C., et al. (2018). Physical activity increases white matter microstructure in Children. Front. Neurosci. 12:950. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaddock, L., Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Kim, J. S., Voss, M. W., VanPatter, M., et al. (2010). A neuroimaging investigation of the association between aerobic fitness, hippocampal volume, and memory performance in preadolescent children. Brain Res. 1358, 172–183. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.08.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, S. B., Dring, K. J., Morris, J. G., Sunderland, C., Bandelow, S., and Nevill, M. E. (2018). High intensity intermittent games-based activity and adolescents’ cognition: moderating effect of physical fitness. BMC Public Health 18:603. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-5514-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Emmons, R., Larsen, R. J., and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eime, R. M., Young, J. A., Harvey, J. T., Charity, M. J., and Payne, W. R. (2013). A systematic review of the psychological and social benefits of participation in sport for children and adolescents: informing development of a conceptual model of health through sport. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 10, 98–98. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-10-98

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esteban-Cornejo, I., Cadenas-Sanchez, C., Contreras-Rodriguez, O., Verdejo-Roman, J., Mora-Gonzalez, J., Migueles, J. H., et al. (2017). A whole brain volumetric approach in overweight/obese children: examining the association with different physical fitness components and academic performance. The ActiveBrains project. NeuroImage 159, 346–354. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esteban-Cornejo, I., Tejero-Gonzalez, C. M., Sallis, J. F., and Veiga, O. L. (2015). Physical activity and cognition in adolescents: a systematic review. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 18, 534–539. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2014.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eurofit (1993). Handbook for the Eurofit Test on Physical Fitness. Strasbourg: Council of Europe.

Google Scholar

Fraguela-Vale, R., Varela-Garrote, L., and Sanz-Arazuri, E. (2016). Ocio deportivo, imagen corporal y satisfacción vital en jóvenes españoles [Sporting leisure, body image and life satisfaction in young Spaniards]. J. Sport Psychol. 25, 33–38.

Google Scholar

Gil, D., Fernández-Modamio, M., Bengochea, R., and Arrieta, M. (2012). Adaptation of the Hinting Task theory of the mind test to Spanish. Rev. Psiquiatr. Salud Ment. 5, 79–88. doi: 10.1016/j.rpsmen.2011.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giuliano, R. J., Karns, C. M., Neville, H. J., and Hillyard, S. A. (2014). Early auditory evoked potential is modulated by selective attention and related to individual differences in visual working memory capacity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 2682–2690. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberg, D. P. (1978). Manual of the General Health Questionnaire. Windsor: NLF Publishing.

Google Scholar

Guiney, H., and Machado, L. (2013). Benefits of regular aerobic exercise for executive functioning in healthy populations. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 73–86. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0345-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herting, M. M., Colby, J. B., Sowell, E. R., and Nagel, B. J. (2014). White matter connectivity and aerobic fitness in male adolescents. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 7, 65–75. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillman, C. H., Castelli, D. M., and Buck, S. M. (2005). Aerobic fitness and neurocognitive function in healthy preadolescent children. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 37, 1967–1974. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000176680.79702.ce

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, F. K. W., Louie, L. H. T., Chow, C. B., Wong, W. H. S., and Ip, P. (2015). Physical activity improves mental health through resilience in Hong Kong Chinese adolescents. BMC Pediatr. 15:48. doi: 10.1186/s12887-015-0365-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt, N. L. (2008). Positive Youth Development Through Sport. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Kantomaa, M. T., Tammelin, T., Ebeling, H., Stamatakis, E., and Taanila, A. (2015). High levels of physical activity and cardiorespiratory fitness are associated with good self-rated health in adolescents. J. Phys. Act. Health 12, 266–272. doi: 10.1123/jpah.2013-0062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keyes, C. L., Shmotkin, D., and Ryff, C. D. (2002). Optimizing well-being: the empirical encounter of two traditions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 1007–1022. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.1007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Léger, L. A., Mercier, D., Gadoury, C., and Lambert, J. (1988). The multistage 20 metre shuttle run test for aerobic fitness. J. Sports Sci. 6, 93–101. doi: 10.1080/02640418808729800

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J. H., Alderman, B. L., Song, T. F., Chen, F. T., Hung, T. M., and Chang, Y. K. (2018). A randomized controlled trial of coordination exercise on cognitive function in obese adolescents. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 34, 29–38. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2017.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lobo, A., Pérez-Echeverría, M. J., and Artal, J. (1986). Validity of escaled versión of de general health questionnaire (GHQ-28) in a Spanish population. Psychol. Med. 16, 135–140. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700002579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubans, D., Richards, J., Hillman, C., Faulkner, G., Beauchamp, M., Nilsson, M., et al. (2016). Physical activity for cognitive and mental health in youth: a systematic review of mechanisms. Pediatrics 138:e20161642. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludyga, S., Gerber, M., Herrmann, C., Brand, S., and Pühse, U. (2018). Chronic effects of exercise implemented during school-break time on neurophysiological indices of inhibitory control in adolescents. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 10, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2017.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, B. K., Zivnuska, S., and Kacmar, K. M. (2019). Self-perception and life satisfaction. Pers. Individ. Differ. 139, 321–325. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pardo, A., and Ruiz, M. A. (2005). Análisis De Datos Con SPSS 13 [BaseData analysis with SPSS 13 Base]. Madrid: McGraw Hill.

Google Scholar

Perlman, S. B., Hein, T. C., and Stepp, S. D. (2014). Emotional reactivity and its impact on neural circuitry for attention–emotion interaction in childhood and adolescence. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 100–109. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinquart, M., and Silbereisen, R. K. (2004). Human development in times of social change: theoretical considerations and research needs. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 28, 289–298. doi: 10.1080/01650250344000406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pontifex, M. B., McGowan, A. L., Chandler, M. C., Gwizdala, K. L., Parks, A. C., Fenn, K., et al. (2019). A primer on investigating the after effects of acute bouts of physical activity on cognition. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 40, 1–22. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.08.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Johnson, C. R., Chaddock, L., Voss, M. W., Cohen, N. J., et al. (2011). Cardiorespiratory fitness and the flexible modulation of cognitive control in preadolescent children. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 1332–1345. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabiner, D. L., Godwin, J., and Dodge, K. A. (2016). Predicting academic achievement and attainment: the contribution of early academic skills, attention difficulties, and social competence. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 45, 250–267. doi: 10.17105/SPR45-2.250-267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reigal, R. E., Becerra-Fernández, C. A., Hernández-Mendo, A., and Martín-Tamayo, I. (2014). Relación del autoconcepto con la condición físicay la composición corporal en una muestra de adolescents [Relationship of self-concept with physical condition and body composition in a sample of adolescents]. An. Psicol. 30, 1079–1085. doi: 10.6018/analesps.30.3.157201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reloba-Martínez, S., Reigal, R. E., Hernández-Mendo, A., Martínez-López, E. J., Martín-Tamayo, I., and Chirosa-Ríos, L. J. (2017). Efectos del ejercicio físico extracurricular vigoroso sobre la atención de escolares [Effects of vigorous extracurricular physical exercise on school attendance]. J. Sport Psychol. 26, 29–36.

Google Scholar

Ruiz-Barquín, R. (2008). portaciones del análisis subdimensional del cuestionario de personalidad BFQ para la predicción del rendimiento en judokas jóvenes de competición [Contributions of the subdimensional analysis of the BFQ personality questionnaire for the prediction of the performance in young competitive judokas]. Cuad. Psicol. Deporte 8, 5–29.

Google Scholar

Ryff, C. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1069–1081. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanjuán, P., Pérez, A. M., and Bermúdez, J. (2000). Escala de autoeficacia general: datos psicométricos de la adaptación para población española [General self-efficacy scale: adaptation psychometric data for the Spanish population]. Psicothema 12(Suppl. 2), 509–513.

Google Scholar

Schwarzer, R. (1992). Self-Efficacy: Thought Control of Action. Berlin: Universidad de Berlin.

Google Scholar

Schwarzer, R., and Jerusalem, M. (1995). “Generalized Self-Efficacy scale,” in Measures in Health Psychology: A User’s Portfolio. Casual and Control Beliefs, eds J. Weinman, S. Wright, and M. Johnston (Windsor: NFER-Nelson), 35–37.

Google Scholar

Swann, C., Telenta, J., Draper, G., Liddle, S., Fogarty, A., Hurley, D., et al. (2018). Youth sport as a context for supporting mental health: adolescent male perspectives. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 35, 55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2017.11.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tari, A. R., Norevik, C. S., Scrimgeour, N. R., Kobro-Flatmoen, A., Storm-Mathisen, J., Bergersen, L. H., et al. (2019). Are the neuroprotective effects of exercise training systemically mediated? Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 62, 94–101. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2019.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tine, M. (2014). Acute aerobic exercise: an intervention for the selective visual attention and reading comprehension of low-income adolescents. Front. Psychol. 5:575. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanhelst, J., Béghin, L., Duhamel, A., Manios, Y., Molnar, D., De Henauw, S., et al. (2016). Physical activity is associated with attention capacity in adolescents. J. Pediatr. 168, 126–131. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.09.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viholainen, H., Aro, T., Purtsi, J., Tolvanen, A., and Cantell, M. (2014). Adolescents’ school-related self-concept mediates motor skills and psychosocial well-being. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 84, 268–280. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, M. W., Soto, C., Yoo, S., Sodoma, M., Vivar, C., and van Praag, H. (2019). Exercise and hippocampal memory systems. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 318–333. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (2005). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-IV). Madrid: TEA Ediciones.

Google Scholar

Westfall, D. R., Gejl, A. K., Tarp, J., Wedderkopp, N., Kramer, A. F., Hillman, C. H., et al. (2018). Associations between aerobic fitness and cognitive control in adolescents. Front. Psychol. 9:1298. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, H. E., Siegle, D., McCoach, D. B., Little, C. A., and Reis, S. M. (2014). A Model of Academic Self-Concept Perceived Difficulty and Social Comparison Among Academically Accelerated Secondary School Students. Gift. Child Q. 58, 111–126. doi: 10.1177/0016986214522858

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Medical Association (2013). World medical association declaration of helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 310, 2191–2194. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xue, Y., Yang, Y., and Huang, T. (2019). Effects of chronic exercise interventions on executive function among children and adolescents: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 53, 1397–1404. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2018-099825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zmyj, N., Witt, S., Weitkämper, A., Neumann, H., and Lücke, T. (2017). Social cognition in children born preterm: a perspective on future research directions. Front. Psychol. 8:455. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zullig, K. J., and White, R. J. (2011). Physical activity, life satisfaction, and self-rated health of middle school students. Appl. Res. Qual. Life 6, 277–289. doi: 10.1007/s11482-010-9129-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: physical exercise, cognitive, psychosocial, quality of life, health

Citation: Reigal RE, Hernández-Mendo A, Juárez-Ruiz de Mier R and Morales-Sánchez V (2020) Physical Exercise and Fitness Level Are Related to Cognitive and Psychosocial Functioning in Adolescents. Front. Psychol. 11:1777. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01777

Received: 29 April 2020; Accepted: 29 June 2020;
Published: 24 July 2020.

Edited by:

Diogo Monteiro, Polytechnic Institute of Santarém, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Filipe Rodrigues, Polytechnic Institute of Santarém, Portugal
Luis Cid, Polytechnic Institute of Santarém, Portugal

Copyright © 2020 Reigal, Hernández-Mendo, Juárez-Ruiz de Mier and Morales-Sánchez. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Verónica Morales-Sánchez, dm9tb3JhbGVzQHVtYS5lcw==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.