Skip to main content

CONCEPTUAL ANALYSIS article

Front. Psychol., 22 July 2020
Sec. Health Psychology

Putting the ‘Art’ Into the ‘Art of Medicine’: The Under-Explored Role of Artifacts in Placebo Studies

  • 1Center for Alcohol and Addiction Studies, School of Public Health, Brown University, Providence, RI, United States
  • 2School of Psychology, University of Plymouth, Plymouth, United Kingdom
  • 3Division of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Psychology, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
  • 4Division of General Medicine, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
  • 5Pain and Psychotherapy Research Lab, University of Koblenz-Landau, Landau, Germany
  • 6Research Unit of General Practice, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark
  • 7School of Psychology, University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland

Research in social psychology demonstrates that physical environmental factors – or “artifacts” such as provider clothing and office décor – can influence health outcomes. However, the role of artifacts in augmenting or diminishing health outcomes is under-explored in the burgeoning discipline of placebo studies. In this paper, we argue that a careful consideration of artifacts may carry significant potential in informing how placebo effects can be maximized, and nocebo effects minimized in clinical settings. We discuss the potential mechanisms, including classical conditioning, response expectancy, and mindsets, by which artifacts might enhance or diminish these effects. Next, we propose testable hypotheses to investigate how placebo and nocebo effects might be elicited by artifacts in care settings, and conclude by providing innovative research designs to advance this novel research agendum.

Introduction

From the soft-furnishings in the waiting area, to the artwork adorning the walls, to the physician’s framed credentials: as soon as a patient enters the clinical environment he or she is unavoidably seeped in a range of artifacts. To date, a range of studies in social and health psychology demonstrates that these physical environmental factors – or “artifacts” – can influence patients’ perceptions about the quality of their care, and, as a result, even their health outcomes (e.g., Arneill and Devlin, 2002; Devlin et al., 2009; Petrilli et al., 2018). In this paper, we propose that artifacts, which have been investigated at some length in psychology, deserve further scrutiny within the burgeoning field of placebo studies. Placebo effects have been recognized for centuries (Findley, 1953; Shapiro, 1959, 1960). Thomas Jefferson referred to placebos as a “pious fraud” (De Craen et al., 1999); according to Shapiro (1968, p. 667), Stanley Hall, the founding president of the American Psychological Association, once exclaimed, “Physicians appeal to the imagination in desperate cases with bread pills and placeboes [sic].” Placebo effects refer to psychobiological processes that give rise to genuine therapeutic effects for a range of conditions (Blease, 2019). In the last three decades research on placebo studies has increased considerably. Scientific findings now show that placebo effects are powerful (Finniss et al., 2010), particularly for “self-appraised symptoms” (Kaptchuk and Miller, 2015). It is now understood that placebo effects are particularly relevant for many commonly presented symptoms and conditions including depression, anxiety, and pain (Kaptchuk and Miller, 2015). Nocebo effects, on the other hand, refer to psychobiological processes that engender adverse health outcomes (Colloca and Barsky, 2020).

Prior research in placebo studies largely focused on (a) what providers say, (b) how they say it, or (c) the nature of treatment in influencing placebo and nocebo effects. With regard to the content of provider disclosures, research suggests that positively valenced information, and a convincing rationale may augment placebo effects (Thomas, 1987; Locher et al., 2017). With regard to the how information is disclosed, considerable research in placebo studies has examined social and environmental factors in the context of the interaction between a patient and caretaker (Kaptchuk et al., 2008; Leibowitz et al., 2019). Finally, studies also show that the contingent features of placebo treatments such as taste, color, size, or invasiveness can also modulate the effect (Branthwaite and Cooper, 1981; De Craen et al., 1996; Kaptchuk et al., 2008; Kam-Hansen et al., 2014; Faasse et al., 2016; Berna et al., 2017).

However, very little attention has been given to non-medical artifacts arising in the proximal clinical environment. Artifacts have been referenced in the history of psychology. Vygotsky (1962, 1978) first discussed the notion of artifacts as culturally charged tools and symbols, theorizing their influence on learning and human behavior. Artifacts are also described in the theoretical framework of Cultural-Historical Activity Theory (Engeström, 1999; Foot, 2014). Our discussion of artifacts is rooted in present-day cognitive science. From this perspective, we also draw on theories about the role of contextual factors in psychotherapy. With regard to the latter Frank and Frank (1991) articulate the importance of the “healing setting” in psychotherapy outcomes which, they argue, includes “symbol[s of] the therapist’s role as a healer” (p. 41). According to Frank and Frank, these symbols can include “bookshelves, (an) impressive desk, couch, and easy chair” (p. 41). By taking a cognitive science perspective – rather, than a semiotics approach – we explore how these contextual factors might influence health outcomes, a consideration that so far has gone largely unexplored in experimental placebo research.

Drawing on well-established psychological findings, we discuss non-medical artifacts that we suggest placebo researchers consider as factors that potentially influence healthcare outcomes. Such artifacts include clinician clothing and office décor. Our goal in this Conceptual Analysis is therefore to propose a novel research direction in placebo studies: namely, to investigate whether non-medical artifacts in the context of care can influence the size of placebo effects.

In what follows we offer an introduction to current findings on the role of non-medical artifacts in healthcare. We focus specifically on clothing, décor, and how artifacts may influence behavior. Next, we provide an overview of research into placebo and nocebo effects. We describe the mechanisms by which placebo and nocebo effects arise, focusing on how artifacts might engender these effects among patients via non-conscious psychological processes. Linking these two distinct bodies of literature, we propose specific hypotheses, and suggest experimental designs aimed at investigating the potential of artifacts to elicit placebo effects. Finally, if artifacts play a role in modulating the size of placebo effects, we note that this will have important ethical implications for clinical practice. In short, putting the “art” into “the art of medicine” may require the medical community to expand beyond current conceptual standards of care to establish appropriate therapeutic aesthetics for health practitioners’ clothing, as well as for décor, natural lighting, and other features of the healthcare environment.

The Influence of Artifacts on Healthcare Outcomes

Clothing

One particular artifact that has received extensive attention in health research is physician attire. For example, Petrilli et al. (2018) surveyed over 4,000 patients in primary care, emergency department, hospital, and surgery settings at 10 United States academic hospitals. Just over half responded that the clothing their physician wears was important to them. Participants were also presented with a series of pictures of male and female doctors with manipulated attire. Overall, participants rated physicians more favorably when they were dressed formally with a lab coat. Similarly, Rehman et al. (2005) found that among 400 patients from an internal medicine outpatient clinic at a US Veterans Administration Center, 76% of respondents expressed a preference for a physician with a white coat. Further, the authors found that this preference for formal attire was associated with patients’ perceptions about physician confidence, trust, as well as their openness in discussing intimate personal problems with their doctor. One systematic review (Petrilli et al., 2015) concluded that, overall, 70% of the 30 studies included in the analysis found clear attire preferences, particularly for formal dress and white coats. The authors also reported that attire preferences were influenced by patients’ age: formal attire preference was slightly higher among older compared to younger patients. Also, in outpatient clinics attire preferences were generally higher than in acute care settings.

Moreover, cultural factors seem to influence attire preferences. Research suggests that formal attire or white coat has been preferred even more consistently in several European countries (including the United Kingdom, Belgium, and Netherlands) than in the United States (Gallagher et al., 2008; Gherardi et al., 2009; Kocks et al., 2010; Hartmans et al., 2013). Studies from Australia (Gooden et al., 2001) and Brazil (Yonekura et al., 2013) revealed a clear preference of patients for white coat, whereas patients from Saudi Arabia preferred formal attire (Al-Ghobain et al., 2012). Interestingly, in Korea, research has shown that although patients expressed a preference for white coat, traditional attire was associated with increased patient comfort with their physician (Chang et al., 2011; Chung et al., 2012).

Although experimental findings have rarely explored objective outcomes among physicians Adam and Galinsky (2012) found that physicians’ attention-span increased when donning white coats. On the other hand, Haque and Waytz (2012) have suggested that the use of white coats might, in part, have a negative impact on healthcare if, “caregivers in hospitals become anonymized a sea of white coats, which subtly diffuses their individual responsibility toward patients” (p. 177). If healthcare provider’s objective behavior changes according to what they wear, it would confound some of the results discussed here with patient perception as the outcome.

Décor

Beyond the question of clothing, there is evidence that healthcare is influenced by aesthetic factors, consistent with the model presented by Wager and Atlas (2015). For instance, Devlin et al. (2009) have conducted a series of studies in the Northeast of the United States suggesting that the physical space where patients spend time can impact perceptions of care. In one study, college students viewed a series of pictures of physician waiting rooms (Arneill and Devlin, 2002). The authors concluded that waiting rooms that were, “nicely furnished, well-lit, contained artwork, and are warm in appearance” (p. 348) were associated with higher perceived quality of care. Similarly, Andrade et al. (2013) reported that objective quality of healthcare environments mediated patients’ perception of care. Another study, this time involving displayed credentials (i.e., diplomas) in therapists’ offices, found that the number of framed certificates on display was positively related to perceived quality of care and perceived friendliness of the therapist; interestingly, family photos did not appear to impact friendliness ratings (Devlin et al., 2009).

Impact of Artifacts on Behavior

Beyond affecting patients’ perception of care, artifacts can influence our behavior via non-conscious processes. For example, following exposure to business-related objects participants negotiated in a more self-interested and competitive manner (Kay et al., 2004). Other research demonstrates that people keep their direct environment cleaner when sitting in a room filled with the scent of a cleaning agent (Holland et al., 2005). Even the natural environment can be considered a relevant artifact. In a naturalistic experiment, surgery patients took fewer analgesics when their hospital room window had a view of trees versus a view of a wall (Ulrich, 1984). Therefore, there is considerable potential to transfer this knowledge to a health-related environment to foster possible benefits for patients (Sheeran et al., 2013).

Placebos and Placebo Effects

A growing body of research shows that placebo effects are therapeutically significant in the treatment of: acute (Benedetti, 2014) and chronic (Vase et al., 2014) pain, migraine (Kam-Hansen et al., 2014), major depressive disorder (Kirsch, 2014, 2019; Deacon and Spielsman, 2017), anxiety disorders (Sugarman et al., 2014), irritable bowel syndrome (Kaptchuk et al., 2008), alcohol dependence (Weiss et al., 2008), Parkinson’s Disease (Lidstone, 2014), intellectual disabilities (Jensen et al., 2017), and binge eating disorder (Blom et al., 2018).

Importantly, the term placebo should be understood in two distinctive senses that are not always distinguished by researchers (Blease, 2018; Blease and Annoni, 2019). First, in randomized controlled trials (“RCTs”), placebos are deployed as controls to test for the efficacy of particular interventions. Second, in contrast to the ethical principle of transparency, placebos are widely used by physicians for the purpose of alleviating symptoms and/or placating patients in clinical contexts (Linde et al., 2018; Bernstein et al., 2020). Placebos in this latter form occasionally constitute cellulose/sugar pills or saline injections, but more often consist of treatments, such as antibiotics or vitamins, that are not expected to have a direct curative effect on the condition being treated (Linde et al., 2018). These placebos are often referred to as “impure.” Considerable evidence demonstrates that physicians and other healthcare professional play a large role in influencing the size of placebo effects, and researchers in the field of placebo studies have begun to investigate how placebo effects might be ethically harnessed or personalized to improve patient outcomes for a variety of prevalent conditions and symptoms, including pain, depression, and irritable bowel syndrome (Enck et al., 2013; Blease et al., 2016; Evers et al., 2018).

Nocebo Effects

Nocebo effects – often conceived as “negative placebo effects” – are usually understood as the amalgam of adverse responses to receiving an inert treatment (Kennedy, 1961). When placebos are used as controls in RCTs, patients sometimes experience side effects in the inert placebo control arm. For example, de la Cruz et al. (2010) examined the frequency of nocebo effects in patients with cancer-related fatigue. They found that 71% of patients who received an inert treatment reported nocebo effects. In RCTs, participants usually receive a list of all the potential side effects of the active drug. This might create negative expectations of treatment outcome (i.e., a nocebo effect). Similarly, patients experiencing specific negative symptoms at baseline are more likely to report side effects of the medication perhaps by misattributing them to the medication as opposed to other factors (de la Cruz et al., 2010).

Like placebo effects, nocebo effects can operate in the absence of a traditional placebo pill and comprise part of everyday treatments (Colloca and Finniss, 2012). In 2012, a systematic review concluded that nocebo responses typically result from unintended negative suggestions by physicians or the nursing staff. Such phrases may, for example, result in an unwanted focus of attention (e.g., “Are you feeling nauseous?”), in trivializing a patient’s legitimate complaint or concern (e.g., “You don’t need to worry”), or in uncertainty (e.g., “Try to take your meds regularly”), (phrases cited from Häuser et al., 2012, p. 461). Thus, the challenge in the clinic is to find a balance between the communication of important clinical information while minimizing negative instructions and a negative therapeutic context.

Proposed Mechanisms of Placebo and Nocebo Effects

Placebo effects are understood to be genuine psychobiological events that engage in cognitive processes to promote healing. The mechanisms behind placebo and nocebo effects are the focus of much theoretical and empirical work. Several potential mechanisms have been suggested. Below, we discuss the three psychological pathways that are currently the most widely discussed as mechanisms of the placebo effect: response expectancies, mindsets, and conditioning. We discuss the way artifacts might harness placebo effects via each of these mechanisms after first introducing the mechanism in greater detail. It is important to note that it is the role of artifacts in eliciting these cognitive mechanisms that, we postulate, engenders health effects rather than the mere presence of artifacts per se. We address the link between artifacts and mindsets and expectancies together, since the latter two concepts are highly related. We emphasize that artifacts might exert effects via other placebogenic mechanisms, including embodiment (Ongaro and Kaptchuk, 2019), social observational learning (Colloca and Benedetti, 2009), the “somatic focus” model (Alfano, 2015), and the Bayesian model of perceptual decision (Geuter et al., 2017). Since these mechanisms have been subject to less sustained empirical research, they lie beyond the scope of this paper.

Response Expectancies

Response expectancies can elicit beneficial effects as a result of a patient’s beliefs that a treatment or intervention will be effective (e.g., Kirsch, 1985, 2018). Some psychologists appear to define response expectancies as consciously held beliefs or expectations. However, recent research implies a broader interpretation of expectations as encompassing non-verbal, implicit, and non-conscious “beliefs” and dispositions (Geers et al., 2005; Wellman and Geers, 2009; Jensen et al., 2012, 2014).

There is a rich body of research on the purported role of response expectancy in placebo studies. One example is anticipating a reduction in pain after the application of a specific cream described as having analgesic properties. If an individual experiences analgesia from this cream, yet the substance is actually inert, it is proposed that response expectancy has mediated the placebo effect. Several experiments investigating the role of response expectancy on placebo effects have been conducted. Benedetti et al. (1999) induced pain in healthy subjects through a subcutaneous capsaicin injection in the participant’s right foot, left foot, right hand, and left hand. When an inert topical cream was applied to the left hand (one experimental condition) or right hand and left foot (another experimental condition), participants expected to experience less pain in the affected areas with no change in the unaffected areas. When pain ratings were taken, actual pain intensity was correlated with these expectations.

Mindsets

More recently, advancing a new research agendum, health psychologists proposed a “new framework for harnessing placebo effects in medicine” (Zion and Crum, 2018). Bearing resemblance to earlier terms in social psychology – “gestalt” and “schema” – and to “paradigms” in social sciences and philosophy, the authors describe mindsets as “lenses or frames of mind that orient individuals to particular sets of associations and expectations” (p. 147) (Zion and Crum, 2018). Differentiating this mechanism from response expectancies, Zion and Crum state that, “Expectations are specific beliefs about future events. Mindsets are a more general psychological construal that orient an individual to a number of mindset-consistent expectations” (p. 147).

Arguing that the provider-patient interaction can influence mindsets, and thereby placebo effects (Zion and Crum, 2018), researchers working from this perspective have constructively focused on perceptions of clinicians’ competence and empathy (see Howe et al., 2019 for a review). This research builds on, and refines, an earlier study by Kaptchuk et al. (2008), which showed that placebo acupuncture for Irritable Bowel Syndrome delivered by an especially warm and empathetic provider resulted in more relief compared to placebo acupuncture delivered by a less empathetic – or “businesslike” – provider. More recently, Howe et al. (2017) examined the role of warmth and competence in influencing placebo effects. The authors induced an allergic reaction in volunteer participants, and applied a sham topical cream. The provider interaction differed on three dimensions: warmth (high vs. low), competence (high vs. low), and expectation (high vs. low). Of particular relevance to this paper, the authors changed artifacts in line with the “competence” manipulation. In the high competence condition, posters with warm images were displayed and the room was “organized, neat and clean” (p. 1076). In the low competence condition, posters were not shown, and the room was disorganized with “papers scattered on floor” (p. 1076). Results showed that participants with positive expectations of allergy relief, and who were treated by a practitioner high in warmth and competence, experienced a reduction in their allergic reaction, as measured by the wheal size. This study provides initial evidence for the notion that artifacts might influence placebo effects. However, in the competence manipulation, other cues that lie outside of artifacts were also varied (e.g., the presence or absence of eye contact, putting the blood pressure cuff on correctly or incorrectly). Thus, the impact of artifacts in this study was confounded with the effects of other cues, and further research is needed to systematically examine the potential contribution of artifacts to placebo effects.

Artifacts and Expectancies and Mindsets

The artifacts discussed in this paper often signal certain attitudes about providers. These attitudes are likely tied to both mindsets and treatment-relevant expectancies. For instance, one might have the mindset: “Well-dressed people are good at their job.” After seeing a formally attired doctor, this could translate to the expectancy: “Dr. X will help me get better.” Similarly, observing professional credentials conspicuously on display might lead to higher expectancies. In terms of nocebo effects, the mindset, “A shabby or uninviting room means the physician is not up to the job” could be linked to the expectancies “My doctor will not know what is wrong with me (incompetence) and/or will not listen attentively (unempathetic)” (additional examples of the proposed relationship between mindsets and expectancies are provided by Zion and Crum, 2018). Teasing apart the role of specific artifacts in mediating perceptions of physician competence and/or empathy, we suggest, would be a valuable task for future empirical research in placebo studies.

Classical Conditioning

Classically conditioned placebo effects occur when symptom reduction in response to a placebo has been learned through pairings. In some experimental designs, placebo conditioning consists of two phases. In the conditioning phase, one cream (the placebo) is presented as an effective analgesic. Afterward, a painful stimulation is provided on both the placebo-treated and the non-treated skin sites. Participants are then informed that the stimulus intensity (e.g., heat pain) will be the same on both sites, whereas in fact it is deliberately reduced for the placebo-treated site in order to reinforce the experience of pain relief. During the final test phase, equivalent levels of painful simulations are performed on both sites. Researchers employing this paradigm have observed analgesia in the placebo-treated region in a number of different studies (e.g., Voudouris et al., 1985; Montgomery and Kirsch, 1997; Amanzio and Benedetti, 1999; Benedetti et al., 2007). Although much of this research does not evaluate classical conditioning fully independently of response expectancies, since participants believe they are receiving an analgesic agent, other work (e.g., Jensen et al., 2015; Ba̧bel et al., 2017) has confirmed that conditioning alone can elicit placebo effects.

Artifacts and Classical Conditioning

How might the influence of artifacts be mediated by classical conditioning? In the West, at least, white coats are synonymous with medical care. Conceivably, they may also play a role in conditioned health responses within clinical encounters. Learned associations between a stethoscope (i.e., conditioned stimulus) and (for example) positive health outcomes, might result in symptom improvement (i.e., a conditioned healthcare response) if the neutral, or conditioned, stimulus evokes an unconscious response as a result of the repeated pairing. The negative counterpart to this – nocebo effects – arise when repeated learned associations between a neutral stimulus and adverse responses occur. For example, in the past, one might have had negative conditioned associations, with physicians wearing white coats or scrubs, or with the distinctive appearance of a particular clinical environment. As a result of this conditioning, the white coat/scrubs/appearance of the current clinical environment, could itself evoke anxiety in the present. To illustrate: while its mechanisms have not been related to nocebo research, “white coat hypertension” might be a nocebo effect that results from conditioned health anxieties when doctors perform medical tests in recognizable professional apparel which, in many health settings, is typically a white coat. It should be emphasized that classical conditioning in these contexts is strictly a result of repeated pairings over-time, consistent with the animal model work from Pavlov (1927) and others (Rescorla, 1988). Some of the placebo conditioning studies mentioned above (e.g., Montgomery and Kirsch, 1997) employed a combination of verbal cues and repeated pairings, which might over-estimate the real-world impact of conditioning in this context. Nonetheless, since analgesic and hyperalgesic conditioning to pain stimuli can occur without conscious awareness (Jensen et al., 2012, 2015), verbal instructions do not seem necessary for conditioned placebo effects.

Next Steps: Exploring the Role of Artifacts and Placebo Effects

To better explore the connections between artifacts and placebo effects, we suggest that future studies investigate whether manipulation of artifacts – such as cues of clothing and aesthetics – influences the size of placebo effects. Based on the conceptual outline summarized above, we suggest that a preliminary experimental framework explore a classical conditioning paradigm to manipulate whether conspicuous artifacts or the apparel of a physician augment or diminish placebo effects. While much of the research we cover focuses on subjective outcomes, research ought to consider incorporating objective measures (e.g., behavior) [e.g., Zech et al. (2019)]. In addition, as we describe in more detail below, we suggest there may be significant promise in focusing on how artifacts influence expectancies and mindsets in clinical encounters. For example, we hypothesize that for certain placebo-effect responsive conditions and symptoms, patients may be more likely to experience beneficial effects if they encounter formally attired, versus casually dressed physicians, and if they encounter higher quality versus lower quality office furnishings and waiting areas.

In order to examine whether the magnitude of placebo effects is influenced by artifacts, we advance three research designs (Table 1). First, future research might investigate whether physician behavior changes naturalistically in response to the manipulations. For instance, if a provider is exposed to enriched artifacts, he or she may non-consciously interact with patients differently, thereby enhancing placebo effects. Second, investigators could provide the same inert1 treatment to all participants (such as sham-acupuncture) and vary the extent to which the use of artifacts is optimized, e.g., positive vs. neutral vs. negative. Based on the hypothesis presented in this article, we predict the following pattern of magnitudes of placebo effects for the three groups: positive use of artifacts > neutral use of artifacts > negative use of artifacts. A final approach would utilize a variation of the Balanced Placebo Design with Enrichment (BPDE) proposed by Kube and Rief (2017). In this design, both the treatment being provided (such as drug vs. placebo) and the therapeutic setting (enriched vs. impoverished) is varied, thus allowing the examination of main and interaction effects. In particular, the treatment could be varied by comparing several conditions (e.g., drug vs. placebo, open-label placebo vs. deceptive placebo, or placebo vs. no treatment/usual care); with respect to artifacts, the therapeutic setting could be varied as described above (e.g., positive use of artifacts vs. neutral). This 2 × 2 design would enable researchers to examine whether a specific treatment “requires” a positive therapeutic setting to be effective. In these studies, at least two experimental conditions are required: one condition in which the use of artifacts is optimized, and one condition with neutral or even negative use of artifacts (e.g., physician being dressed casually or even “inappropriately”). More conditions could also be considered if the goal is to test the presence of a dose-dependent relationship. Formal mediation or moderation analyses could be used across these designs to examine the extent to which these artifacts are mediated or modulated by perceived warmth and competence (Howe et al., 2019). Researchers should also consider employing manipulation checks to verify that artifacts were indeed manipulated in the expected manner. This could be done with participants who were in the primary study, or with a pre-selected group of volunteers viewing images. While the former is preferable, it may not be relevant for unconscious effects.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Artifacts and placebo effects: Future research directions.

Box 1. [b] Key suggestions and findings.

Prior research shows:

• Artifacts in the clinical environment – such as clinician apparel, and office décor – can impact patient perceptions of healthcare providers, and also health outcomes.

• Placebo and nocebo effects are genuine psychobiological events that engage perceptual and cognitive processes to elicit, respectively, positive and negative health changes.

• Mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects are thought to include response expectancies, mindsets, and classical conditioning.

• Research in placebo studies has focused on the nature of treatments, or the role of clinician communication, including information disclosures and socio-emotional cues, in eliciting placebo and nocebo effects.

We suggest that:

• Artifacts in clinical settings may modulate mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects.

• Experimental research could be conducted to better understand how artifacts impact health outcomes, and be ethically harnessed in clinical contexts.

Further considerations that we have not elaborated on relate to mobile Health (or “mHealth”) including apps aimed at helping patients to manage their illnesses or symptoms. Artifactual features of mobile devices, for example, might augment the size of placebo effects (Torous and Firth, 2016; Pontén et al., 2019). Similarly, incidental features in the design of mHealth apps, for example, how sophisticated the imagery or graphic design are – might also be conceived of as non-medical physical artifacts – that might augment (or diminish) placebo effects. This is especially important as there are measurable effects of smartphone-based interventions for several health conditions, such as depression (Firth et al., 2017), and owing to their potential, the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) has already approved some health applications as digital-based drugs (Waltz, 2018). Hence, we suggest that design features of eHeath innovations may influence artifactual placebo effects (Torous and Firth, 2016), a consideration that warrants further exploration.

Ethical Implications

If the theory proposed in this conceptual analysis is borne out by empirical research, patients accessing health centers with upscale décor and furnishings may conceivably experience a boost in their clinical outcomes across a range of conditions and symptoms for which the placebo effect is relevant. In such a scenario, artifactual differences across clinical environments might be responsible for unequal distributions of placebo effects. Dependent on empirical findings, whether any such differences constitute injustice in care, we suggest, will require further ethical analysis.

We acknowledge that it may seem curious to consider aesthetic or artifactual aspects of clinical environments as potentially important features of healthcare. However, the fact that such factors are not currently considered part of the toolkit that ought to be focused on during clinical education does not imply they should not be reclassified as such (Blease, 2012; Friesen and Blease, 2018). Indeed, if further empirical studies suggest that artifacts do play a role in placebo effects, this may prompt other ethical questions. For example, should patients be informed of the remedial effects of their aesthetic surroundings? In fee-for-service systems of healthcare, should clinicians be reimbursed for the therapeutic boost implemented by their beneficial aesthetic taste? Additional questions relate to determining the patient’s values in person-centered care. For example, if artifacts elicit different effects among different demographic or groups, this may incur challenging ethical and practical dilemmas about décor and attire, and whether aesthetic dimensions of clinical encounters can and should be matched to individual patients.

Finally, should aspects of care that make a real difference to patient outcomes be taught to clinicians? We flag up these issues to highlight that non-trivial practice implications that might arise, if research demonstrates that artifacts may modulate the size of placebo and nocebo effects.

Limitations

The artifacts discussed in the present paper focused on décor and clothing. This is not to suggest research should be limited to these aspects of healthcare but rather that they might present an important entry point for considering the role of artifacts in placebo studies. Furthermore, although we have specifically concentrated on recognizable artifacts that might modulate mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects other physical aspects should not be ignored. Ambient features such as lighting, temperature, scent, and sound can certainly contribute to the sum of environmental effects (Harris et al., 2002; Fenko and Loock, 2014), and may also influence placebo and nocebo effects. Additionally, there may be cultural variations in how artifacts elicit effects. Depending on an individual’s background and life experiences, diplomas and particular styles of interior, for example, may evoke different responses (also see Moerman, 2002 for a relevant review).

Conclusion

Artifacts have been studied at some length in social psychology but have not yet been the subject of systematic investigation in the field of placebo studies. Building on findings in psychology, and current research in placebo studies, we suggest that it is fruitful to connect these fields of research (see Box 1). If empirical research supports the hypotheses forwarded in this paper, enhanced consideration of aesthetic features could lead to important benefits for patients. We suggest that even apparently trivial measures to maximize patient outcomes may be worthwhile. Optimization of artifacts in clinical settings represents one such promising possibility.

Author Contributions

CB conceived the manuscript. MB, CB, CL, and TK wrote the first draft. MB, CB, and CL revised the first draft. MB, SB, SS-F, and CB performed the revisions after peer-review. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

MB was funded by the National Institutes of Health K01DA048087. CL was funded by Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF): P400PS_180730. CB was funded by Irish Research Council-Marie Skłodowska-Curie Fellowship and a Keane Scholarship.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr. Alia Crum for feedback on an earlier draft of this manuscript.

Footnotes

  1. ^ One could also examine the impact of artifacts on verum treatment, but our paper is mostly concerned with the way artifacts influence placebo effects.

References

Adam, H., and Galinsky, A. D. (2012). Enclothed cognition. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 918–925. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.02.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alfano, M. (2015). Placebo effects and informed consent. Am. J. Bioethics 15, 3–12. doi: 10.1080/15265161.2015.1074302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Ghobain, M. O., Al-Drees, T. M., Alarifi, M. S., Al-Marzoug, H. M., Al-Humaid, W. A., and Asiry, A. M. (2012). Patients’ preferences for physicians’ attire in Saudi Arabia. Saudi Med. J. 33, 763–767.

Google Scholar

Amanzio, M., and Benedetti, F. (1999). Neuropharmacological dissection of placebo analgesia: expectation-activated opioid systems versus conditioning-activated specific subsystems. J. Neurosci. 19, 484–494. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.19-01-00484.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrade, C. C., Lima, M. L., Pereira, C. R., Fornara, F., and Bonaiuto, M. (2013). Inpatients’ and outpatients’ satisfaction: The mediating role of perceived quality of physical and social environment. Health Place 21, 122–132. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2013.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arneill, A. B., and Devlin, A. S. (2002). Perceived quality of care: the influence of the waiting room environment. J. Environ. Psychol. 22, 345–360. doi: 10.1006/jevp.2002.0274

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ba̧bel, P., Bajcar, E. A., Adamczyk, W., Kicman, P., Lisińska, N., Świder, K., et al. (2017). Classical conditioning without verbal suggestions elicits placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia. PLoS One 12:e0181856. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0181856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedetti, F. (2014). Placebo Effects. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Benedetti, F., Arduino, C., and Amanzio, M. (1999). Somatotopic activation of opioid systems by target-directed expectations of analgesia. J. Neurosci. 19, 3639–3648. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.19-09-03639.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedetti, F., Pollo, A., and Colloca, L. (2007). Opioid-mediated placebo responses boost pain endurance and physical performance: is it doping in sport competitions? J. Neurosci. 27, 11934–11939. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3330-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berna, C., Kirsch, I., Zion, S. R., Lee, Y. C., Jensen, K. B., Sadler, P., et al. (2017). Side effects can enhance treatment response through expectancy effects: an experimental analgesic randomized controlled trial. Pain 158:1014. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, M. H., Locher, C., Stewart-Ferrer, S., Buergler, S., DesRoches, C. M., Dossett, M. L., et al. (2020). Primary care providers’ use of and attitudes towards placebos: an exploratory focus group study with US physicians. Br. J. Health Psychol. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blease, C. (2012). The principle of parity: the ‘placebo effect’and physician communication. J. Med. Ethics 38, 199–203. doi: 10.1136/medethics-2011-100177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blease, C. (2018). Consensus in placebo studies: lessons from the philosophy of science. Perspect. Biol. Med. 61, 412–429. doi: 10.1353/pbm.2018.0053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blease, C., and Annoni, M. (2019). Overcoming disagreement: a roadmap for placebo studies. Biol. Philos. 34:18.

Google Scholar

Blease, C., Colloca, L., and Kaptchuk, T. J. (2016). Are open-label placebos ethical? Informed consent and ethical equivocations. Bioethics 30, 407–414. doi: 10.1111/bioe.12245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blease, C. R. (2019). The role of placebos in family medicine:’Implications of evidence and ethics for general practitioners’. Aust. J. Gen. Pract. 48:700. doi: 10.31128/ajgp-05-19-4939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blom, T. J., Guerdjikova, A. I., and McElroy, S. L. (2018). Placebo response and cessation in binge eating disorder: A pooled analysis of two randomized parallel-group clinical trials. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 27, 421–428. doi: 10.1002/erv.2655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Branthwaite, A., and Cooper, P. (1981). Analgesic effects of branding in treatment of headaches. Br. Med. J. 282, 1576–1578. doi: 10.1136/bmj.282.6276.1576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, D. S., Lee, H., Lee, H., Park, H. J., and Chae, Y. (2011). What to wear when practicing oriental medicine: patients’ preferences for doctors’ attire. J. Alternat. Complement. Med. 17, 763–767. doi: 10.1089/acm.2010.0612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, H., Lee, H., Chang, D. S., Kim, H. S., Lee, H., Park, H. J., et al. (2012). Doctor’s attire influences perceived empathy in the patient–doctor relationship. Patient Educ. Couns. 89, 387–391. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2012.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colloca, L., and Barsky, A. J. (2020). Placebo and nocebo effects. N. Engl. J. Med. 382, 554–561.

Google Scholar

Colloca, L., and Benedetti, F. (2009). Placebo analgesia induced by social observational learning. PAIN§ 144, 28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.01.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colloca, L., and Finniss, D. (2012). Nocebo effects, patient-clinician communication, and therapeutic outcomes. JAMA 307, 567–568.

Google Scholar

De Craen, A. J., Kaptchuk, T. J., Tijssen, J. G., and Kleijnen, J. (1999). Placebos and placebo effects in medicine: historical overview. J. R. Soc. Med. 92, 511–515. doi: 10.1177/014107689909201005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Craen, A. J., Roos, P. J., De Vries, A. L., and Kleijnen, J. (1996). Effect of colour of drugs: systematic review of perceived effect of drugs and of their effectiveness. BMJ 313, 1624–1626. doi: 10.1136/bmj.313.7072.1624

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Cruz, M., Hui, D., Parsons, H. A., and Bruera, E. (2010). Placebo and nocebo effects in randomized double-blind clinical trials of agents for the therapy for fatigue in patients with advanced cancer. Cancer 116, 766–774. doi: 10.1002/cncr.24751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deacon, B. J., and Spielsman, G. I. (2017). “Is the efficacy of “antidepressant” medications overrated?,” in Psychological Science Under Scrutiny: Recent Challenges and Proposed Solutions, eds S. O. Lilienfeld and I. D. Waldman (New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons), 250–270. doi: 10.1002/9781119095910.ch13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devlin, A. S., Donovan, S., Nicolov, A., Nold, O., Packard, A., and Zandan, G. (2009). Impressive?” Credentials, family photographs, and the perception of therapist qualities. J. Environ. Psychol. 29, 503–512. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.08.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enck, P., Bingel, U., Schedlowski, M., and Rief, W. (2013). The placebo response in medicine: minimize, maximize or personalize? Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 12, 190–204.

Google Scholar

Engeström, Y. (1999). Activity Theory and Individual and Social Transformation Perspectives on Activity Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 19–38.

Google Scholar

Evers, A., Colloca, L., Blease, C., Annoni, M., Atlas, L. Y., Benedetti, F., et al. (2018). Implications of placebo and nocebo effects for clinical practice: expert consensus. Psychother. Psychosom. 87, 204–210.

Google Scholar

Faasse, K., Martin, L. R., Grey, A., Gamble, G., and Petrie, K. J. (2016). Impact of brand or generic labeling on medication effectiveness and side effects. Health Psychol. 35, 187–190. doi: 10.1037/hea0000282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fenko, A., and Loock, C. (2014). The influence of ambient scent and music on patients’ anxiety in a waiting room of a plastic surgeon. HERD 7, 38–59. doi: 10.1177/193758671400700304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Findley, T. (1953). The placebo and the physician. Med. Clin. N. Am. 37, 1821–1826. doi: 10.1016/S0025-7125(16)34945-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finniss, D. G., Kaptchuk, T. J., Miller, F., and Benedetti, F. (2010). Biological, clinical, and ethical advances of placebo effects. Lancet 375, 686–695. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(09)61706-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Firth, J., Torous, J., Nicholas, J., Carney, R., Pratap, A., Rosenbaum, S., et al. (2017). The efficacy of smartphone-based mental health interventions for depressive symptoms: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. World Psychiatry 16, 287–298. doi: 10.1002/wps.20472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foot, K. A. (2014). Cultural-historical activity theory: Exploring a theory to inform practice and research. J. Hum. Behav. Soc. Environ. 24, 329–347. doi: 10.1080/10911359.2013.831011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank, J. D., and Frank, J. B. (1991). Persuasion and Healing: A Comparative Study of Psychotherapy. Baltimore, MD: JHU Press.

Google Scholar

Friesen, P., and Blease, C. (2018). Placebo effects and racial and ethnic health disparities: an unjust and underexplored connection. J. Med. Ethics Medethics 2018:104811.

Google Scholar

Gallagher, J., Waldron, F. L., Stack, J., and Barragry, J. (2008). Dress and address: patient preferences regarding doctor’s style of dress and patient interaction. Ir. Med. J. 101, 211–213.

Google Scholar

Geers, A. L., Weiland, P. E., Kosbab, K., Landry, S. J., and Helfer, S. G. (2005). Goal activation, expectations, and the placebo effect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 89:143. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.2.143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geuter, S., Koban, L., and Wager, T. D. (2017). The cognitive neuroscience of placebo effects: concepts, predictions, and physiology. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 40, 167–188. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gherardi, G., Cameron, J., West, A., and Crossley, M. (2009). Are we dressed to impress? A descriptive survey assessing patients’ preference of doctors’ attire in the hospital setting. Clin. Med. 9:519. doi: 10.7861/clinmedicine.9-6-519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gooden, B. R., Smith, M. J., Tattersall, S. J., and Stockier, M. R. (2001). Hospitalised patients’ views on doctors and white coats. Med. J. Australia 175, 219–222. doi: 10.5694/j.1326-5377.2001.tb143103.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haque, O. S., and Waytz, A. (2012). Dehumanization in medicine: Causes, solutions, and functions. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 176–186. doi: 10.1177/1745691611429706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, P. B., McBride, G., Ross, C., and Curtis, L. (2002). A place to heal: environmental sources of satisfaction among hospital patients 1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 32, 1276–1299. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb01436.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartmans, C., Heremans, S., Lagrain, M., Van Asch, K., and Schoenmakers, B. (2013). The doctor’s new clothes: professional or fashionable? Prim. Health Care 3, 1–9.

Google Scholar

Häuser, W., Hansen, E., and Enck, P. (2012). Nocebo phenomena in medicine. Dtsch. Arztebl. Int. 109, 459–465.

Google Scholar

Holland, R. W., Hendriks, M., and Aarts, H. (2005). Smells like clean spirit: Nonconscious effects of scent on cognition and behavior. Psychol. Sci. 16, 689–693. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01597.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howe, L., Leibowitz, K. A., and Crum, A. (2019). When Your Doctor “Gets It” and “Gets You”: The Critical Role of Competence and Warmth in the Patient–Provider Interaction Table_1.docx. Front. Psychiatry 10:475. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howe, L. C., Goyer, J. P., and Crum, A. J. (2017). Harnessing the placebo effect: exploring the influence of physician characteristics on placebo response. Health Psychol. 36, 1074–1082. doi: 10.1037/hea0000499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, K., Kaptchuk, T. J., Kirsch, I., Raicek, J., Lindstrom, K. M., Berna, C., et al. (2012). Nonconscious activation of placebo and nocebo pain responses. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 15959–15964. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1202056109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, K., Kirsch, I., Odmalm, S., Kaptchuk, T. J., and Ingvar, M. (2015). Classical conditioning of analgesic and hyperalgesic pain responses without conscious awareness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 7863–7867. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1504567112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, K., Kirsch, I., Pontén, M., Rosén, A., Yang, K., Gollub, R. L., et al. (2017). Certainty of genuine treatment increases drug responses among intellectually disabled patients. Neurology 88, 1912–1918. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000003934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, K. B., Kaptchuk, T. J., Chen, X., Kirsch, I., Ingvar, M., Gollub, R. L., et al. (2014). A neural mechanism for nonconscious activation of conditioned placebo and nocebo responses. Cereb. Cortex 25, 3903–3910. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kam-Hansen, S., Jakubowski, M., Kelley, J. M., Kirsch, I., Hoaglin, D. C., Kaptchuk, T. J., et al. (2014). Altered placebo and drug labeling changes the outcome of episodic migraine attacks. Sci. Transl. Med. 6, ra215–ra218.

Google Scholar

Kaptchuk, T. J., Kelley, J. M., Conboy, L. A., Davis, R. B., Kerr, C. E., Jacobson, E. E., et al. (2008). Components of placebo effect: randomised controlled trial in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. BMJ 336, 999–1003.

Google Scholar

Kaptchuk, T. J., and Miller, F. G. (2015). Placebo effects in medicine. N. Engl. J. Med. 373, 8–9.

Google Scholar

Kay, A. C., Wheeler, S. C., Bargh, J. A., and Ross, L. (2004). Material priming: The influence of mundane physical objects on situational construal and competitive behavioral choice. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 95, 83–96. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, W. P. (1961). The nocebo reaction. Med. World 95, 203–205.

Google Scholar

Kirsch, I. (1985). Response expectancy as a determinant of experience and behavior. Am. Psychol. 40, 1189–1202. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.40.11.1189

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirsch, I. (2014). “The emperor’s new drugs: medication and placebo in the treatment of depression,” in Placebo, eds F. Benedetti, P. Enck, E. Frisaldi, and M. Schedlowski (New York, NY: Springer), 291–303. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-44519-8_16

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirsch, I. (2018). Response Expectancy and the Placebo Effect International Review of Neurobiology, Vol. 138. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 81–93.

Google Scholar

Kirsch, I. (2019). The placebo effect in the treatment of depression and anxiety. Front. Psychiatry 10:407. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kocks, J. W., Lisman-van, Y. L., and Berkelmans, P. G. (2010). Clothing make the doctor–patients have more confidence in a smartly dressed GP. Ned. Tijdschr. Geneeskd. 154, A2898–A2898.

Google Scholar

Kube, T., and Rief, W. (2017). Are placebo and drug-specific effects additive? Questioning basic assumptions of double-blinded randomized clinical trials and presenting novel study designs. Drug Discov. Today 22, 729–735. doi: 10.1016/j.drudis.2016.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leibowitz, K. A., Hardebeck, E. J., Goyer, J. P., and Crum, A. J. (2019). The role of patient beliefs in open-label placebo effects. Health Psychol. 38, 613–622. doi: 10.1037/hea0000751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lidstone, S. C. (2014). “Great expectations: the placebo effect in Parkinson’s disease,” in Placebo, eds F. Benedetti, P. Enck, E. Frisaldi, and M. Schedlowski (New York, NY: Springer), 139–147. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-44519-8_8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linde, K., Atmann, O., Meissner, K., Schneider, A., Meister, R., Kriston, L., et al. (2018). How often do general practitioners use placebos and non-specific interventions? Systematic review and meta-analysis of surveys. PLoS One 13:e0202211. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0202211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Locher, C., Nascimento, A. F., Kirsch, I., Kossowsky, J., Meyer, A., and Gaab, J. (2017). Is the rationale more important than deception? A randomized controlled trial of open-label placebo analgesia. Pain 158, 2320–2328. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moerman, D. E. (2002). Meaning, Medicine, and the “Placebo Effect”. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Cambridge.

Google Scholar

Montgomery, G. H., and Kirsch, I. (1997). Classical conditioning and the placebo effect. Pain 72, 107–113. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(97)00016-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ongaro, G., and Kaptchuk, T. J. (2019). Symptom perception, placebo effects, and the Bayesian brain. Pain 160:1. doi: 10.1097/00006396-900000000-98882

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes. London: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Petrilli, C., Mack, M., Petrilli, J. J., Hickner, A., Saint, S., and Chopra, V. (2015). Understanding the role of physician attire on patient perceptions: a systematic review of the literature—targeting attire to improve likelihood of rapport (TAILOR) investigators. BMJ Open 5:e006578. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-006578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrilli, C. M., Saint, S., Jennings, J. J., Caruso, A., Kuhn, L., Snyder, A., et al. (2018). Understanding patient preference for physician attire: a cross-sectional observational study of 10 academic medical centres in the USA. BMJ Open 8, e021239. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-021239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pontén, M., Ljótsson, B., and Jensen, K. (2019). Shaping placebo analgesic responses on the Internet: a randomized experimental trial. Pain Rep. 4:e698. doi: 10.1097/pr9.0000000000000698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rehman, S. U., Nietert, P. J., Cope, D. W., and Kilpatrick, A. O. (2005). What to wear today? Effect of doctor’s attire on the trust and confidence of patients. Am. J. Med. 118, 1279–1286. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2005.04.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning: It’s not what you think it is. Am. Psychol. 43:151. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.43.3.151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, A. K. (1959). The placebo effect in the history of medical treatment implications for psychiatry. Am. J. Psychiatry 116, 298–304. doi: 10.1176/ajp.116.4.298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, A. K. (1960). A contribution to a history of the placebo effect. Syst. Res. Behav. Sci. 5, 109–135. doi: 10.1002/bs.3830050202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, A. K. (1968). Semantics of the placebo. Psychiatr. Q. 42, 653–695. doi: 10.1007/bf01564309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheeran, P., Gollwitzer, P. M., and Bargh, J. A. (2013). Nonconscious processes and health. Health Psychol. 32:460. doi: 10.1037/a0029203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugarman, M. A., Loree, A. M., Baltes, B. B., Grekin, E. R., and Kirsch, I. (2014). The efficacy of paroxetine and placebo in treating anxiety and depression: a meta-analysis of change on the Hamilton Rating Scales. PLoS One 9:e106337. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, K. B. (1987). General practice consultations: is there any point in being positive? Br. Med. J. 294, 1200–1202. doi: 10.1136/bmj.294.6581.1200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torous, J., and Firth, J. (2016). The digital placebo effect: mobile mental health meets clinical psychiatry. Lancet Psychiatry 3, 100–102. doi: 10.1016/s2215-0366(15)00565-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, R. S. (1984). View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science 224, 420–421. doi: 10.1126/science.6143402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vase, L., Petersen, G. L., and Lund, K. (2014). “Placebo effects in idiopathic and neuropathic pain conditions,” in Placebo, eds F. Benedetti, P. Enck, E. Frisaldi, and M. Schedlowski (Berlin: Springer), 121–136. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-44519-8_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voudouris, N. J., Peck, C. L., and Coleman, G. (1985). Conditioned placebo responses. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 48, 47–53. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.48.1.47

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and Language trans. E. Hanfmann, and G. Vakar, (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).

Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Interaction between learning and development. Read. Dev. Child. 23, 34–41.

Google Scholar

Wager, T. D., and Atlas, L. Y. (2015). The neuroscience of placebo effects: connecting context, learning and health. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 403–418. doi: 10.1038/nrn3976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waltz, E. (2018). Pear approval signals FDA readiness for digital treatments. Nat. Biotechnol. 36, 481–482. doi: 10.1038/nbt0618-481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, R. D., O’Malley, S. S., Hosking, J. D., LoCastro, J. S., and Swift, R. (2008). Do patients with alcohol dependence respond to placebo? Results from the COMBINE Study. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 69, 878–884. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2008.69.878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wellman, J. A., and Geers, A. L. (2009). Rebel without a (conscious) cause: Priming a nonconscious goal for psychological reactance. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 31, 9–16. doi: 10.1080/01973530802659711

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yonekura, C. L., Certain, L., Karen, S. K. K., Alcântara, G. A. S., Ribeiro, L. G., Rodrigues-Júnior, A. L., et al. (2013). Perceptions of patients, physicians, and Medical students on physicians’ appearance. Rev. Assoc. Méd. Bras. 59, 452–459. doi: 10.1016/s2255-4823(13)70504-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zech, N., Seemann, M., Grzesiek, M., Breu, A., Seyfried, T. F., and Hansen, E. (2019). Nocebo effects on muscular performance–an experimental study about clinical situations. Front. Pharmacol. 10:219. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2019.00219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zion, S. R., and Crum, A. J. (2018). Mindsets matter: A new framework for harnessing the placebo effect in modern medicine. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 138, 137–160. doi: 10.1016/bs.irn.2018.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: placebo, placebo effects, expectancy, mindset, psychology, classical conditioning

Citation: Bernstein MH, Locher C, Kube T, Buergler S, Stewart-Ferrer S and Blease C (2020) Putting the ‘Art’ Into the ‘Art of Medicine’: The Under-Explored Role of Artifacts in Placebo Studies. Front. Psychol. 11:1354. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01354

Received: 30 September 2019; Accepted: 22 May 2020;
Published: 22 July 2020.

Edited by:

Changiz Mohiyeddini, Oakland University William Beaumont School of Medicine, United States

Reviewed by:

Victoria J. Madden, University of Cape Town, South Africa
Ernil Hansen, University Medical Center Regensburg, Germany

Copyright © 2020 Bernstein, Locher, Kube, Buergler, Stewart-Ferrer and Blease. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michael H. Bernstein, TWljaGFlbF9CZXJuc3RlaW5AQnJvd24uZWR1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.