Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 08 May 2020
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Application of the Chinese Version of the BIS/BAS Scales in Participants With a Substance Use Disorder: An Analysis of Psychometric Properties and Comparison With Community Residents

\r\nQingqing Che,,&#x;Qingqing Che1,2,3†Peiwen Yang,,&#x;Peiwen Yang1,2,3†Huiyuan Gao,,Huiyuan Gao1,2,3Meizhu Liu,,Meizhu Liu1,2,3Jun Zhang*Jun Zhang4*Taisheng Cai,,*Taisheng Cai1,2,3*
  • 1Medical Psychological Center, Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
  • 2Medical Psychological Institute, Central South University, Changsha, China
  • 3National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders, Changsha, China
  • 4Hunan Judicial Police Vocational College, Changsha, China

Carver and White developed the Behavioral Inhibition/Behavioral Activation Scales (the BIS/BAS Scales) based on Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory proposed by Gray. Subsequent studies proposed that substance abuse was closely related to Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) and Behavioral Activation System (BAS). However, researches on the psychometric properties of the BIS/BAS scales in clinical samples are scarce. The present study was conducted to analyze the applicability of the BIS/BAS scales in a sample suffering from a substance use disorder (SUD) and undergoing treatment in compulsory detoxification institutions (n = 1117). Meanwhile, 822 community residents were selected for comparison. Confirmatory factor analysis was carried out to examine the construct validity and the results showed that the five-factor model was the best fit for people with a substance use disorder’ data. Besides, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the total scale was 0.808, indicating the satisfactory internal consistency reliability. Analysis of the correlation coefficient of the questionnaire with the corresponding personality traits showed that BAS was more associated with the impulsive trait. Surprisingly, participants with a substance use disorder showed more insensitivity for the reward dimension compared with that of community residents and the result of comparison between two samples supported joint subsystems hypothesis. Generally, the BIS/BAS scales showed good reliability and validity. These findings provide more direct evidence on the personality traits of people with a substance use disorder and should form the basis for further research.

Introduction

Drug abuse is a public health and social issue worldwide. Studies have shown that drugs cause serious harm to individuals. They not only lead to deficits in response inhibition (Bell et al., 2014; Smith et al., 2014), but also damage the execution function and make people more impulsive (Dolan et al., 2008). Neuroimaging results showed that the prefrontal cortex and other structures involved in higher-order executive function (such as self-control) of individuals with drug addiction were damaged by compulsive drug taking (Goldstein and Volkow, 2011). Drugs damage the physiological structure of drug users as well as affecting their mental health. Relevant surveys found that most substance users suffer from marked mental pressure (Andreas et al., 2015), and were prone to psychological problems (Schuckit and Hesselbrock, 1994; Gilbert, 2000), such as anxiety and depression. Therefore, anxious and impulsive personality traits were considered as risk factors for drug addiction (Belin et al., 2008; Ersche et al., 2012).

The personality characteristics of anxiety and impulsivity have been studied widely. The reinforced sensitivity theory proposed by Gray (1982, 1987) was one of the most influential biological personality theories (Yu et al., 2011) in this field. The theory postulated that anxiety and impulsivity were the two main dimensions of personality, which are in an orthogonal relationship and are regulated by the behavioral Inhibition scale (BIS) and behavioral Activation scales (BAS), respectively. BIS is responsible for dealing with the processing of target conflicts. When a stimulus that triggers the target conflict is presented, this system is activated and is accompanied by the generation of anxiety. Then, the system will suppress the originally dominant behavior in the conflict and make people evaluate the gains and losses to find the best way, in memory and context, to resolve the conflict. BAS is sensitive to all appetitive stimuli (conditional and unconditional). When presenting reward signals or withdrawing punishment signals, BAS is activated, accompanied by the generation of positive emotions to promote ongoing behavior (Gray and McNaughton, 2000). Carver and White (1994) developed the BIS/BAS scales based on Gray’s theory, which is the most widely used scales based on the RST (Carver and White, 1994).

To date, the BIS/BAS scales have been widely used in different countries. For instance, Caci et al. found that the BIS/BAS scales were a reliable measure in French college students (Caci et al., 2007). Beck et al. demonstrated that the BIS/BAS scales had a satisfactory construct validity and acceptable internal consistency in an analysis of German eating disordered outpatients (Beck et al., 2009). Beside these, the BIS/BAS scales have been shown to have excellent psychometric properties among other samples, such as normal school children (Muris et al., 2005) and community samples (Jorm et al., 1999). In terms of factor structure, most previous studies have supported the view that the BIS/BAS scales have a four-factor structure, named the behavior inhibition, the fun-seeking, the reward responsiveness, and the drive respectively (Carver and White, 1994). Meanwhile, many scholars have proposed certain new structures. Johnson et al. proposed a five-factor structure (Johnson et al., 2003), which was verified in a large sample of offenders (Caci et al., 2007). Heym et al. proposed another five-factor structure (Heym et al., 2008), which was confirmed in heroin-dependent participants (Dissabandara et al., 2012). The two five-factor structures mentioned above share the same three dimensions of the fun-seeking, the reward responsiveness and the drive, with different BIS-fear and the BIS-anxiety dimensions. Besides, previous studies have also analyzed one-factor structure (Müller and Wytykowska, 2005) and two-factor structure (Ross et al., 2002). The two-factor structure included two dimensions: the Behavioral Approach System and the Behavioral Inhibition System.

Current research indicates that BIS and BAS might be associated with substance abuse. Franken et al. found that the number of college students’ illegal substances use correlated positively with BAS and negatively with BIS personality characteristics (Franken and Muris, 2006). Franken et al. also found that the BAS scores were significantly higher in clinically referred drug addicts than in the control group (Franken et al., 2006). Stenason et al. found that behavioral inhibition correlated negatively with substance use (Stenason and Vernon, 2016). Meanwhile, many scholars found that substance addicts have higher impulsivity levels (Wong et al., 2013). Studies also showed that subjects with a history of drug-dependence were more likely to be impulsive than subjects with no drug-dependence history (Holdich et al., 1998). Thus, we hypothesized that people with a substance use disorder could have higher levels of BAS and lower levels of BIS than community residents. To prove this hypothesis, we compared those two personality traits between participants with a substance use disorder and a community sample on the dimensions of the scale. In addition, there should be people with four combinations of high and low BIS and BAS sensitivity within a given population according to previous studies (Carver and White, 1994) and joint subsystems hypothesis (Corr, 2002). Therefore, we further compared the difference in the number of people between the four groups in the two samples.

Previous studies have provided important information about BIS and BAS among people from different countries (Cooper et al., 2007), different ages (Yu et al., 2011), and even those with different diseases (Beck et al., 2009); however, relatively few studies have focused on people with a substance use disorder who were undergoing treatment in compulsory detoxification institutions in China and in those studies, the samples were not sufficiently representative (Franken et al., 2006). The present study is the first to test the suitability of the BIS/BAS scales in a large SUD sample in China. The authors also hope to further clarify the relationship among BIS, BAS, impulsivity and self-control ability. In general, the results of the present study could not only enrich our understanding of BIS and BAS, but also reveal the differences between two groups in terms of personality traits.

Materials and Methods

Samples

The inclusion criteria were as follows: The subjects met Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-V) criteria for substance use disorders and completed acute detoxification treatment; had at least 4 years of formal education; and demonstrated drug addiction. The exclusion criteria were as follows: The subjects suffered from a serious infectious disease, somatic disease, or serious mental illness; their educational level was too low to complete the scale independently; or they were unwilling to participate. The study was conducted using the convenient sampling method. The participants were people with a substance use disorder from six compulsory detoxification centers in Hunan Province. To compare the differences between the different samples on the BIS/BAS scales, community residents were also tested. Finally, we obtained valid data for 1117 participants with a substance use disorder and 822 community residents after eliminating invalid questionnaires (missing answers or false answers). In addition, we randomly selected 362 participants with a substance use disorder to complete the SPSRQ, the SCS, and the SUPPS-P. Retesting was performed after 6 weeks. 163 questionnaires were completed effectively.

Procedure

Before the test, illiteracy was eliminated by inquiry and self-reporting, then the content, purpose, and requirement of this study was explained to each participant. Data was only collected after signed, informed consent was obtained. We respected the participants’ wishes and they could quit at any time. The study was approved by the Local Ethics Committee of our institution.

Measures

The Behavioral Inhibition/Behavioral Activation Scales

The BIS/BAS scales were developed by Carver and White in 1994 with a total of 24 self-reported items and four filler items, without scoring. The scale was divided into the behavior inhibition scale (items 2, 8, 13, 16, 19, 22, 24) and the behavior activation scale. BAS comprises three subscales: The fun-seeking subscale (items 3, 9, 12, 21), the reward responsiveness subscale (items 5, 10, 15, 20), and the drive subscale (items 4, 7, 14, 18, 23). The scales were scored on a four point Likert scale (1 indicating strongly agree and 4 indicating strongly disagree). Item 2 and item 22 were reverse scoring. The lower the score, the greater the sensitivity levels of BIS or BAS. Studies have shown that the BIS/BAS scales have good reliability and validity. The internal consistency coefficient of the BIS scale and the three subscales of BAS scale ranged from 0.66 to 0.76. The corresponding test-retest reliabilities were 0.66, 0.66, 0.59, and 0.69 (Carver and White, 1994).

First, two bilingual graduate students (one each from the Psychology and English Departments) translated the BIS/BAS scales from English into Mandarin. Second, with no prior knowledge of the English version of the BIS/BAS scales, a psychologist and a linguist translated the preliminary Chinese version back into English. Third, the research team compared and discussed each item of the Chinese manuscript and the English manuscript to form the first draft of a Chinese translation. Finally, the back-translated version was reviewed by a psychology doctor with an English background to form the final draft. We obtained permission for this process from the original author.

The Punishment and Reward Sensitivity Scales (the SPSRQ Scales)

The questionnaire was developed by Torrubia et al. (2001) and comprises a 48-item questionnaire designed to assess punishment sensitivity and reward sensitivity. The odd items formed the punishment sensitivity subscale and the even items formed the reward sensitivity subscale. The scale was scored on a two-point scale (1 indicating yes and 2 indicating no). The internal consistency coefficients of the two subscales were 0.83 and 0.78 and the retest reliabilities of the three-month interval were 0.89 and 0.87 respectively. The results of the validity study were good. In the current study, the Cronbach α of the total scale was 0.861.

The Short Version of the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale (the SUPPS-P Scale)

The short version of the UPPS-P impulsive behavior scale has five dimensions, including feeling seeking, lack of persistence, lack of predictability, negative urgency, and positive urgency (Cyders et al., 2014). The Cronbach α ranged from.74 to.85, suggesting acceptable to good internal consistency for the various subscales. The SUPPS-P scale has been translated and introduced into French (Billieux et al., 2012), Italian (D’Orta et al., 2015), and other versions. The results of these studies confirmed that it had good reliability and validity, and a good predictive performance for alcohol abuse and drug abuse. The Cronbach α in this study was 0.769.

The Self-Control Scale (SCS)

The self-control scale published by Tangney et al. in 2004 is divided into a full version and a short version (Tangney et al., 2004). We chose to use the full version, which includes 36 items and five dimensions. Those dimensions were self-discipline, non-impulsive action, healthy habits, work ethic, and reliability. Alpha values and test-retest reliability for the total self-control scale were both.89. Using this scale, studies found that impaired self-control ability was one of the characteristics of substance addicts (Michael, 2000). In the current study, the Cronbach α of the total scale was 0.764.

Statistical Methods

Data analysis was conducted in SPSS version 22.0 and Mplus version 7.0. The significance level was set to p < 0.05. The BIS/BAS scales were evaluated for reliability using internal consistency, split-half reliability, and test-retest reliability. Internal consistency was evaluated using Cronbach’s alpha coefficient. The split-half reliability and test-retest reliability were evaluated using Spearman correlation coefficients. The construct validity was analyzed using confirmatory factor analysis. We used the chi-squared, comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) to evaluate the fit of each model. CFI and TLI greater than 0.90, and RMSEA and SRMR less than 0.08 indicate an acceptable model fit (Sun, 2005). All items obtained significant skewness and kurtosis values (p < 0.001) in the Kolmogorov-Smirnov normality test, which meant the data did not fit the normal distribution; therefore, so the robust maximum likelihood with standard error and mean adjustments (MLM) estimator was used in all confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs) (DiStefano, 2002). It is acceptable for the factor loading of the item to be greater than 0.3 (Wang, 2014). The convergent and discriminant validity and subscale inter-correlations were evaluated using the Spearman correlation coefficients, then the difference in correlation coefficients was analyzed using cocor, a free software package for the R programming language (Birk et al., 2015). The Mann-Whitney U test and independent sample ratio difference significance test (Zhang and Xu, 2015) were used to evaluate the differences between the two groups. The data did not conform to normal distribution; therefore, it was presented as rank mean when performed the Mann-Whitney U test.

Results

Basic Demographic Data

The average age of the people with a substance use disorder and community residents were 35.88 (SD: 8.19 years) and 38.06 (SD: 11.35 years), and 66.2% and 46.00% of them were male, respectively. The unemployment rate among the participants with a substance use disorder was much higher than among community participants. Most participants with a substance use disorder were educated to the primary and middle levels. A total of 907 participants with a substance use disorder reported using ice drug, and polydrug abuse was common. See Table 1 for specific information.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic data of people with a substance use disorder (n = 1117) and community residents (n = 822) in this study.

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the BIS/BAS Scales

We performed a confirmatory factor analysis on the four-factor model of Carver and White, as well as the one-factor model (Müller and Wytykowska, 2005) and two-factor model (Ross et al., 2002), five-factor model (Johnson et al., 2003; Heym et al., 2008). The five-factor model proposed by Johnson et al. used items 2 and 22 as the fear subscale and the remaining items in the BIS as the anxiety subscale. The five-factor model proposed by Heym et al. used items 2, 22, and 16 as the fear subscale and the remaining items in the BIS as the anxiety subscale. The remaining dimensions were consistent with the four-factor model proposed by Carver and White. The results showed that the five-factor model fitted to the data. The results are shown in Table 2. Factor loadings for CFA of factor models are shown in Table 3.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Fit indices of existing models of the BIS/BAS scales in people with a substance use disorder (n = 1117).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Factor loadings for Confirmatory factor analysis of five factor models.

Inter-Factor Correlations and Reliability

The inter-scale correlation coefficient of the scale was between -0.216 and 0.519. There were significant positive correlations between the three subscales of BAS and also between two subscales of BIS. Three subscales of BAS were significantly negatively correlated with BIS-fear and positively correlated with BIS-anxiety. The internal consistency coefficient ranged from 0.387 to 0.824, both of which were within the acceptable range, and only the internal consistency coefficient of BIS-fear was relatively low. The test-retest reliability of the total scale was 0.532. The split-half reliability was 0.652. Specific information is illustrated in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Inter-factor correlations and internal consistency of BIS/BAS scales on five-factor model in participants with a substance use disorder (n = 1117).

Convergent and Discriminant Validity

The correlation coefficient between SR and three subscales of BAS was markedly larger than that between two subscales of BIS (p < 0.05). The correlation coefficient between SP and two subscales of BIS was markedly larger than that between three subscales of BAS (p < 0.05). In addition, we also found that the correlation coefficient between three subscales of BAS and SR was markedly larger than that between SP (p < 0.05). The correlation coefficient between two subscales of BIS and SP was markedly larger than that between SR (p < 0.05). The SUPPS-P correlated more substantially with three subscales of BAS than that with two subscales of BIS (p < 0.05, only the difference of correlation coefficient between BAS-reward and BIS-fear was not significant). The fun seeking dimension correlated moderately with the SCS. The results are shown in Table 5.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Spearman correlations among BIS/BAS scales and other measures in people with a substance use disorders (n = 362).

Comparison Between Participants With a Substance Use Disorder and Community Residents

Based on the BIS and BAS scores, 27% of the people at each end were allocated into four groups: BIS high and BAS high score group; BIS high and BAS low score group; BIS low and BAS high score group; and BIS low and BAS low score group. In the comparison of the five dimensions, the score for people with a substance use disorder in the fun seeking dimension was significantly lower than that of the community residents. Second, the score for people with a substance use disorder in the reward responsiveness and BIS-fear dimensions were significantly higher than that of the community residents. There were no significant differences in the remaining dimensions. In the comparison of the four groups, there were significantly fewer people with high BAS activity and low BIS activity among the people with a substance use disorder than among the community residents. There were significantly more people with high BAS activity and high BIS activity among the people with a substance use disorder than among the community residents. There were no significant differences among the remaining groups. The results are shown in Table 6.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Comparisons of participants with a substance use disorder and community residents of four subscales.

Discussion

Since Carver et al. developed the BIS/BAS scales in 1994, its factor structure has ignited controversy. The four-factor structure proposed by Carver et al. was validated in most CFA studies (Moreira et al., 2015; Maack and Ebesutani, 2018). Meanwhile, many scholars have proposed some new structures, such as the five-factor structure mentioned above. In the present study, the five-factor model was the best fit for people with a substance use disorder; however, it failed to meet the psychometric standard. We correlated items 3 and 9; items 10 and 15; and items 4 and 5 to modified model. All of the modifications were based on the MI applied by Mplus and had sufficient theoretical meaning because the two pairs of correlated items have similar directionality and were loaded on the same factor in our study. We found the descriptions of item 3 and item 9, item 10 and item 15 were similar, meanwhile they were both loaded on drive and fun-seeking dimensions, respectively. For item 4 and item 5, both of them expressed individual’s willingness of doing something. In previous studies, these two items have also been correlated (Yu et al., 2011). People who were undergoing treatment in compulsory detoxification institutions usually had poor education and might confuse the meaning of two items. The factor loading of each item ranged from 0.438 to 0.669, which was within the acceptable range. It also indicated that the five-factor model was more suitable for people with a substance use disorder.

The factor structure of BIS has been controversial in previous research. Some researchers have confirmed that BIS consists of BIS-fear and BIS-anxiety dimensions (Caci et al., 2007), while other scholars support the one-factor structure of BIS (Muris et al., 2005). This question was controversial for the following reasons (Maack and Ebesutani, 2018). Firstly, a single factor is simpler and easier to explain than two factors; secondly, the appearance of BIS-fear factor was caused by a lack of modeling method effects or reverse scoring. Generally, our study provided empirical support for the substantive significance of BIS-fear factor and two-factor structure of BIS, which was consistent with johnson et al.’s study (Johnson et al., 2003).

The result of inter-factor correlation was consistent with previous studies (Heubeck et al., 1998; Caci et al., 2007). The internal consistency coefficient was only low for BIS-fear dimension. Correlation analysis found that the correlation coefficient between two items was relatively low (0.235). Dissabandara also reported low alpha levels for the BIS-Fear scale in heroin-dependent samples [α = 0.17, (Dissabandara et al., 2012)]. Low test-retest reliability might be associated with a series of negative symptoms experienced during substance withdrawal, such as restlessness, anxiety (Benowitz, 2008) and increased response time (Snyder et al., 1989). These symptoms might have some influence on the questionnaire filling.

SPSRQ, like the BIS/BAS scales, is a self-reporting scale that measures RST (Torrubia et al., 2001). The correlation between those two scales was consistent with the results of previous studies (Caseras et al., 2003), and indicated that the BIS/BAS scales had good convergent and discriminant validity. Previous studies always associated impulsivity with BAS activity. For example, Caseras et al. found that all three BAS subscales were associated with measures of impulsivity, among which the fun-seeking subscale was the most prominent. We had similar findings in the present study. The relationship between BAS and impulsivity has always been an issue worth studying. BAS was initially proposed as the causal basis of impulsivity (Smillie et al., 2006), while others argued that impulsive behavior did not stem intrinsically from BAS (Patock-Peckham et al., 2001). No causal conclusion can be drawn in the present study, and its further mechanism depends on future research. The relationship between self-control and the fun seeking dimension could be explained by the negative effects of the use of self-control (Hagger et al., 2010). Exercising self-control is a difficult thing. In particular, when people do not receive the corresponding reward, they are likely to pursue more enjoyable behavior. Another possible explanation for this results centers on the temporary decrease in the sensitivity of behavioral inhibition system caused by self-control (Inzlicht and Gutsell, 2007). BAS may play a leading role in this situation. In summary, the above results indicated that BIS was different from BAS. BIS was more sensitive to punishment and BAS was more closely related with reward sensitivity and impulsivity.

Surprisingly, we found participants with a substance use disorder were significantly insensitive compared with the community residents in the reward responsiveness dimension. As typically found, people with a substance use disorder have a heightened sensitivity to cues associated with BAS (Dissabandara et al., 2012). This could be explained by the interaction between BAS and BIS. Some participants with a substance use disorder might be less sensitive in the reward responsiveness dimension than the community residents; however, their BIS might be more insensitive. Therefore, BIS, which has the function of suppressing prepotent conflicting behaviors and resolving goal conflict (Corr, 2004), may be not work properly, making it difficult for people with a substance disorder to weigh the pros and cons and correctly resolve the conflict. The association between BIS and substance use was observed to be inconsistent in previous studies. Previous studies had failed to find significant differences in BIS dimensions between people with heroin or cocaine addiction and the healthy control group (Franken et al., 2006; Dissabandara et al., 2012), while Loxton et al. found that club-drug users had significantly lower BIS scale scores than non-drug users (Loxton et al., 2008). These different findings may be related to different types of drugs used.

Contrary to our expectations, we found that there were significantly fewer people with a substance use disorder than community residents in BIS low and BAS high sensitivity group. The first thing to note is that this does not mean that the community residents had higher levels of BAS and lower levels of BIS than people with a substance use disorder. According to the joint subsystems hypothesis (Corr, 2002), reactivity also changes because of the nature of the stimulus. Corr found that low anxiety/high impulsivity participants were least reactive to unpleasant slides. Therefore, it can be inferred that community residents have a less emotional response and decreased risk of taking impulsive behavior to conflicts than people with a substance use disorder. Considering the huge difference in their living environment, we thought that it would be sensible to analyze the sensitivity of BIS and BAS in combination with the effect of learning and experience. There was evidence that the activation levels of both systems was affected by learning (Smillie et al., 2007). BAS-reactive individuals should display superior learning when their good behavior is rewarded (compared to those with a less reactive BAS). Winning experience might affect the promotion of BAS, which takes charge of the experience of positive feelings and the hindrance of BIS, which is responsible for the experience of negative feelings (Kim and Lee, 2011). Thus, community residents with good experience would more likely to choose behaviors that conform to social expectations. In the high anxiety/high impulsivity group, Corr put proposed that participants showed attenuated fear potentiation. Analyzing the characteristics of BIS and BAS in people with a substance use disorder was more complex. This involved not only the euphoria of the drug (aversive conditioned stimuli), but also a sense of failure to control oneself (appetitive conditioned stimuli). That is to say, attenuated fear potentiation might be the result of mutual interplay of the BAS and BIS under two stimuli of different properties. Corr also found that participants high anxiety/high impulsivity were more reactive to negative stimuli than to positive stimuli, which revealed that people with a substance use disorder were more likely to make high-risk choices. Generally, the above results supported joint subsystems hypothesis. Influenced by the nature of stimulation and daily experience, there were differences in personality traits and behavioral tendencies between the two groups.

Conclusion

The present study found that the BIS/BAS scales had good applicability in Chinese participants with a substance use disorder who were undergoing treatment in compulsory detoxification institutions. In addition, this study supported joint subsystems hypothesis and indicated that people with a substance use disorder were more likely to take impulsive and risky behavior than community residents. However, this study also had some shortcomings. First, this study failed to distinguish the differences in BIS and BAS levels caused by different drugs. Second, the two groups of subjects could not be completely matched demographically because of certain practical reasons, such as participants under compulsory rehabilitation usually having poor education and low income, and there were always more male than female participant with a substance use disorder. In addition, this study did not conduct long-term follow-up surveys of the subjects and could not provide dynamic data. In short, this study proved that BIS/BAS scales have satisfactory psychometric properties in people with a substance use disorder and enriched our knowledge of the personality characteristics of people with a substance use disorder and general population.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University (Changsha, China). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

QC, PY, HG, JZ, and TC conceived and designed the study. QC, PY, and ML organized and supervised collection and input of data. QC and PY drafted the manuscript, organized and supervised the data analysis. HG, ML, JZ, and TC provided critical comments on various drafts of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by grants from the National Key Research and Development Project (Grant No. 2016YFC0800908).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

The BIS/BAS scales, the Behavioral Inhibition/Behavioral Activation Scales; BIS, Behavioral Inhibition system; BAS, Behavioral Activation system; SPSRQ, The punishment and reward sensitivity scales; SCS, The Self-control scale; SUPPS-P, The Short version of the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale.

References

Andreas, B. J., Lauritzen, G., and Nordfjærn, T. (2015). Co-occurrence between mental distress and poly-drug use: a ten year prospective study of patients from substance abuse treatment. Addict. Behavi. 48, 71–78. doi: 10.1177/0049124192021002005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, I., Smits, D. J. M., Claes, L., Vandereycken, W., and Bijttebier, P. (2009). Psychometric evaluation of the behavioral inhibition/behavioral activation system scales and the sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward questionnaire in a sample of eating disordered patients. Persona. Individ. Differ. 47, 407–412. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belin, D., Mar, A. C., Dalley, J. W., Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (2008). High impulsivity predicts the switch to compulsive cocaine-taking. Science 320, 1352–1355. doi: 10.1126/science.1158136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, R. P., Garavan, H., and Foxe, J. J. (2014). Neural correlates of craving and impulsivity in abstinent former cocaine users: towards biomarkers of relapse risk. Neuropharmacology 85, 461–470. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benowitz, N. (2008). Clinical pharmacology of nicotine: implications for understanding, preventing, and treating tobacco addiction. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 83, 531–541. doi: 10.1038/clpt.2008.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Billieux, J., Rochat, L., Ceschi, G., Carré, A., Offerlin-Meyer, I., Defeldre, A. C., et al. (2012). Validation of a short french version of the UPPS-P impulsive behavior scale. Compr. Psychiatry 53, 609–615. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birk, D., Jochen, M., and Jake, O. (2015). cocor: a comprehensive solution for the statistical comparison of correlations. Plos One 10:e0121945. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caci, H., Deschaux, O., and Baylé, F. J. (2007). Psychometric properties of the french versions of the BIS/BAS scales and the SPSRQ. Personal. Individ. Differ. 42, 987–998. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., and White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 319–333. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caseras, X., Àvila, C., and Torrubia, R. (2003). The measurement of individual differences in behavioural inhibition and behavioural activation systems: a comparison of personality scales. Personal. Individ. Differ. 34, 999–1013. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(02)00084-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, A., Gomez, R., and Aucote, H. (2007). The behavioural inhibition system and behavioural approach system (BIS/BAS) scales: measurement and structural invariance across adults and adolescents. Personal. Individ. Differ. 43, 295–305. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corr, P. J. (2002). J. A. Gray’s reinforcement sensitivity theory: tests of the joint subsystems hypothesis of anxiety and impulsivity. Personal. Individ. Differe. 33, 511–532.

Google Scholar

Corr, P. J. (2004). Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 317–332.

Google Scholar

Cyders, M. A., Littlefield, A. K., Coffey, S., and Karyadi, K. A. (2014). Examination of a short English version of the UPPS-P impulsive behavior scale. Addict. Behav. 39, 1372–1376. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dissabandara, L. O., Loxton, N. J., Dias, S. R., Mark, D., and Alfreda, S. (2012). Testing the fear and anxiety distinction in the BIS/BAS scales in community and heroin-dependent samples. Personal. Individ. Differ. 52, 888–892. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DiStefano, C. (2002). The impact of categorization with confirmatory factor analysis. Struc. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 9, 327–346.

Google Scholar

Dolan, S. L., Antoine, B., and Nathan, P. E. (2008). Executive dysfunction as a risk marker for substance abuse: the role of impulsive personality traits. Behav. Sci. Law 26, 799–822. doi: 10.1002/bsl.845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Orta, I., Burnay, J., Aiello, D., Niolu, C., Siracusano, A., Timpanaro, L., et al. (2015). Development and validation of a short Italian UPPS-P impulsive behavior scale. Addict. Behav. Rep. 2, 19–22. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2015.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ersche, K. D., Turton, A. J., Chamberlain, S. R., Ulrich, M., Bullmore, E. T., and Robbins, T. W. (2012). Cognitive dysfunction and anxious-impulsive personality traits are endophenotypes for drug dependence. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 926–936. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franken, I. H. A., and Muris, P. (2006). BIS/BAS personality characteristics and college students’ substance use. Personal. Individ. Differ. 40, 1497–1503. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franken, I. H. A., Muris, P., and Georgieva, I. (2006). Gray’s model of personality and addiction. Addict. Behav. 31, 399–403.

Google Scholar

Gilbert, P. (2000). The relationship of shame, social anxiety and depression: the role of the evaluation of social rank. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 7, 174–189.

Google Scholar

Goldstein, R. Z., and Volkow, N. D. (2011). Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 652–669. doi: 10.1038/nrn3119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: an enquiry into the function of the septo-hippocampal system. Behav. Brain Sci. 5, 469–484. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x00013066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, J. A. (1987). “The neuropsychology of emotion and personality,” in Cognitive Neurochemistry, eds S. D. I. S. M. Stahl and E. C. Goodman (Oxfor: Oxford University Press), 171–190.

Google Scholar

Gray, J. A., and McNaughton, N. (2000). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry Into the Functions of The Septo-Hippocampal System, 2nd Edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Hagger, M., Wood, C., Stiff, C., and Chatzisarantis, N. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 136, 495–525. doi: 10.1037/a0019486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heubeck, B. G., Wilkinson, R. B., and Cologon, J. (1998). A second look at Carver and White’s (1994) BIS/BAS scales. Personal. Individ. Differ. 25, 785–800. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(98)00124-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heym, N., Ferguson, E., and Lawrence, C. (2008). An evaluation of the relationship between Gray’s revised RST and Eysenck’s PEN: distinguishing BIS and FFFS in Carver and White’s BIS/BAS scales. Personal. Individ. Differ. 45, 709–715. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.07.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holdich, R. G., Cumming, I. W., Smith, I. D., Allen, T. J., Moeller, F. G., Rhoades, H. M., et al. (1998). Impulsivity and history of drug dependence. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 50, 137–145. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(98)00023-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inzlicht, M., and Gutsell, J. N. (2007). Running on empty: neural signals for self-control failure. Psychol. Sci. 18, 933–937. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02004.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, S. L., Turner, R. J., and Iwata, N. (2003). BIS/BAS levels and psychiatric disorder: an epidemiological study. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Asses. 25, 25–36.

Google Scholar

Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Henderson, A. S., Jacomb, P. A., Korten, A. E., and Rodgers, B. (1999). Using the BIS/BAS scales to measure behavioural inhibition and behavioural activation: factor structure, validity and norms in a large community sample. Personal. Individ. Differ. 26, 49–58.

Google Scholar

Kim, D. Y., and Lee, J.-H. (2011). Effects of the BAS and BIS on decision-making in a gambling task. Personal. Individ. Differ. 50, 1131–1135. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.01.041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loxton, N. J., Wan, V. L.-N., Ho, A. M.-C., Cheung, B. K.-L., Tam, N., Leung, F. Y. K., et al. (2008). Impulsivity in Hong Kong-Chinese club-drug users. Drug Alcohol. Depend 95, 0–89. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maack, D. J., and Ebesutani, C. (2018). A re-examination of the BIS/BAS scales: Evidence for BIS and BAS as unidimensional scales. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 27:e1612. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michael, L. (2000). \“Loss of control\” in alcoholism and drug addiction: a neuroscientific interpretation. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 8, 225–249. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.8.2.225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreira, D., Almeida, F., Pinto, M., Segarra, P., and Barbosa, F. (2015). Data concerning the psychometric properties of the behavioral inhibition/behavioral activation scales for the portuguese population. Psychol. Assess. 27, 1117–1122. doi: 10.1037/pas0000108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, J. R. M., and Wytykowska, A. M. (2005). Psychometric properties and validation of a Polish adaptation of Carver and White’s BIS/BAS scales. Personal. Individ. Differ. 39, 795–805. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muris, P., Meesters, C., Kanter, E. D., and Timmerman, P. E. (2005). Behavioural inhibition and behavioural activation system scales for children: relationships with Eysenck’s personality traits and psychopathological symptoms. Personal. Individ. Differ. 38, 831–841.

Google Scholar

Patock-Peckham, J. A., Cheong, J. W., Balhorn, M. E., and Nagoshi, C. T. (2001). A social learning perspective: a model of parenting styles, self-regulation, perceived drinking control, and alcohol use and problems. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 25, 1284–1292.

Google Scholar

Ross, S. R., Millis, S. R., Bonebright, T. L., and Bailley, S. E. (2002). Confirmatory factor analysis of the behavioral inhibition and activation scales. Personal. Individ. Differ. 33, 861–865.

Google Scholar

Schuckit, M. A., and Hesselbrock, V. (1994). Alcohol dependence and anxiety disorders: what is the relationship? Am. J. Psychiatry 151:1723. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.12.1723

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smillie, L. D., Dalgleish, L. I., and Jackson, C. J. (2007). Distinguishing between learning and motivation in behavioral tests of the reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality. Pers. Soc. Psychol Bull. 33, 476–489. doi: 10.1177/0146167206296951

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smillie, L. D., Jackson, C. J., and Dalgleish, L. I. (2006). Conceptual distinctions among Carver and White’s (1994) BAS scales: a reward-reactivity versus trait impulsivity perspective. Personal. Individ. Differ. 40, 1039–1050.

Google Scholar

Smith, J. L., Mattick, R. P., Jamadar, S. D., and Iredale, J. M. (2014). Deficits in behavioural inhibition in substance abuse and addiction: a meta-analysis. Drug. Alcohol. Dependence. 145, 1–33. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, F. R., Davis, F. C., and Henningfield, J. E. (1989). The tobacco withdrawal syndrome: performance decrements assessed on a computerized test battery. Drug Alcohol. Dependence 23, 259–266. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(89)90090-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stenason, L., and Vernon, P. A. (2016). The dark triad, reinforcement sensitivity and substance use. Personal. Individ. Differ. 94, 59–63.

Google Scholar

Sun, J. (2005). Assessing goodness of fit in confirmatory factor analysis. Meas. Eval. Counsel. Dev. 37, 240–256.

Google Scholar

Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., and Boone, A. L. (2004). High Self?ontrol predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J. Personal. 72, 271–324. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torrubia, R., Ávila, C., Moltó, J., and Caseras, X. (2001). The sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray’s anxiety and impulsivity dimensions. Personal. Individ. Differ. 31, 837–862. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M. (2014). Latent Variable Modeling Using Mplus. Chongqing: Chongqing University Press, 85.

Google Scholar

Wong, C. F., Silva, K., Kecojevic, A., Schrager, S. M., Bloom, J. J., Iverson, E., et al. (2013). Coping and emotion regulation profiles as predictors of nonmedical prescription drug and illicit drug use among high-risk young adults. Drug Alcohol. Dependence 132, 165–171. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, R., Branje, S. J., Keijsers, L., and Meeus, W. H. (2011). Psychometric characteristics of carver and White’s BIS/BAS scales in dutch adolescents and their mothers. J. Personal. Assess. 93, 500–507. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2011.595745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, H., and Xu, J. (2015). Modern Psychology and Education Statistics.Beijing: Beijing normal university press, 255–256.

Google Scholar

Keywords: Behavioral Inhibition, Behavioral Activation, people with a substance use disorder, psychometric properties, impulsivity, self-control ability

Citation: Che Q, Yang P, Gao H, Liu M, Zhang J and Cai T (2020) Application of the Chinese Version of the BIS/BAS Scales in Participants With a Substance Use Disorder: An Analysis of Psychometric Properties and Comparison With Community Residents. Front. Psychol. 11:912. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00912

Received: 20 January 2020; Accepted: 14 April 2020;
Published: 08 May 2020.

Edited by:

Laura Badenes-Ribera, University of Valencia, Spain

Reviewed by:

Mengcheng Wang, Guangzhou University, China
Stephan Arndt, The University of Iowa, United States

Copyright © 2020 Che, Yang, Gao, Liu, Zhang and Cai. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Taisheng Cai, caitaisheng@csu.edu.cn; Jun Zhang, zhangjun888aaa@163.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.