- 1Department of English and American Studies, Humboldt University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- 2Department of Linguistics, Universität Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany
- 3School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, United Kingdom
This work explores gender agreement attraction in comprehension. Attraction occurs when an agreement error (such as, “the key to the cabinets are rusty”) goes unnoticed, leading to the illusion of grammaticality due to a mismatch between the value of the head and the value of a local intervening phase (attractor). According to retrieval accounts, these errors occur during cue retrieval from memory and predict illusions of grammaticality. Alternatively, representational accounts predict that the errors occur due to the faulty representation of certain features, thus, illusions of ungrammaticality are also expected. In four experiments we explore: (a) whether gender agreement attraction occurs in Greek and the strategy/-ies employed, (b) the role of the agreement target, (c) the timing of gender agreement attraction, (d) the role of phonological matching between the nominal inflectional morphemes of the attractor and the agreement target, and (e) participants’ sensitivity to agreement when there is no conflict from the attractor. In all four experiments, the grammaticality of the sentence and the attractor value (match or mismatch with the head) and also the phonological matching between the attractor and the agreement target in ungrammatical sentences were manipulated. The agreement target was either an adjectival predicate or an object-clitic and the gender value of the head was feminine or neuter. Attraction was found in all measures during the time-course of adjectival predicates (Experiment 1) and object-clitics (Experiment 2), and in timed (Experiment 3), and untimed (Experiment 4) judgments. Even more, both gender values showed attraction and the results mainly suggest that participants experience illusions of grammaticality, confirming retrieval accounts. Phonological matching did not modulate attraction in any of the experiments, suggesting that the similarity in the morphophonological realization between the agreement target and the attractor does not increase attraction. Furthermore, participants were sensitive to gender agreement violations in the absence of gender mismatch between the head and the attractor, suggesting that they respect agreement rules and have both neuter and feminine available in their feature content repertoire, although with some tendency in favor of neuter in feminine agreement contexts. The impact of these findings is discussed within the concept of attraction and sensitivity to agreement violations.
Introduction
Agreement is one of the core linguistic operations where a head noun and its corresponding agreement target have to accord in their agreement feature specification. Agreement checking can be modulated by interference from a local and syntactically unavailable constituent, a phenomenon which is called attraction. Research has shown that the parser is highly accurate in certain structures with highly complex constraints, but less accurate in the implementation of some other simpler constraints (Phillips et al., 2011; Felser et al., 2017). Specifically, in attraction errors, the parser performs less accurately and experiences an “illusion effect,” during which these errors may fleetingly go unnoticed in comprehension. However, the parser appears to be rather selective on the types of interference it is vulnerable to. Thus, studying the cases under which the parser becomes prone to errors can provide psycholinguistic research with knowledge of how users encode and navigate complex linguistic representations in real-time (Phillips et al., 2011, p. 37).
Attraction errors, such as the sentence “The key to the cabinets are unsurprisingly rusty” (Bock and Miller, 1991), may arise due to the presence of a local intervening phrase (“the cabinets”), often called the attractor. Research has shown that people are prone to attraction errors in production, not only in spontaneous speech but also in well-edited texts and academic writing (see Pfau, 2009 for a corpus study), as well as in comprehension. More recently, it has been shown that number in subject-verb agreement is not the only case of attraction and that other features and agreement targets are subject to attraction as well (e.g., Badecker and Kuminiak, 2007; Franck et al., 2008; Slioussar and Malko, 2016).
In this paper, we explore gender agreement attraction at the nominal domain, a less explored case of attraction. Studying attraction errors is of great theoretical importance in the psycholinguistic literature because it is relevant to discussions on the relation between the parser and the grammar and whether they constitute one cognitive system (e.g., grammar constraints are implemented within noisy general memory architecture) or two separate cognitive systems or “snapshots” (see Lewis and Phillips, 2015 and Mancini, 2018 for discussion). We explore gender agreement attraction in Greek comprehension in real-time as well as in timed and untimed judgments in two different agreement targets.
Background
Agreement Attraction Accounts
Currently two major groups of accounts have been formulated to explain attraction: representational accounts and retrieval accounts. Representational accounts comprise models which argue that attraction occurs due to faulty or ambiguous representations (e.g., Nicol et al., 1997; Franck et al., 2002; Eberhard et al., 2005; Brehm and Bock, 2013) and can be divided into two distinct categories: models where attraction occurs due to feature movement/percolation (Vigliocco et al., 1995; Eberhard, 1997; Franck et al., 2002) and continuous valuation models (Eberhard et al., 2005) where attraction occurs due to spreading activation. In the percolation model, the marked feature (e.g., plural) of a phrase (attractor, i.e., “to the cabinets”) intervening between the subject and the verb, as in the sentence “The key to the cabinets are rusty,” can erroneously migrate upward to the subject noun phrase during the computation of the subject NP and then can be copied onto the verb. The first studies of agreement attraction in comprehension confirmed this account (e.g., Nicol et al., 1997; Pearlmutter et al., 1999). In the Marking and Morphing model (Eberhard et al., 2005), the hypothesis is that the more plural (conceptually) the subject NP is, the more likely it is for attraction to occur. Attraction takes place at stage two (morphing stage). Similarly to percolation accounts, the Marking and Morphing model predicts that in comprehension, both grammatical and ungrammatical sentences will be affected due to the representation of the complex NP and there is evidence confirming this prediction (e.g., Solomon and Pearlmutter, 2004; Hammerly et al., 2019). At the same time, representational accounts would also expect attraction to occur only when the attractor is morphologically marked, which has also been confirmed in certain studies (Bock and Miller, 1991; Nicol et al., 1997; Staub, 2010; Santesteban et al., 2017).
Alternatively, according to retrieval accounts (e.g., Lewis and Vasishth, 2005; Van Dyke and McElree, 2006), attraction is an error of process of the memory retrieval system, according to which the cues of a certain head should be retrieved on the agreement region and during retrieval the parser might select the wrong NP if there is a partial feature match. Retrieval accounts were initially proposed for comprehension but they were later extended to production. Despite the differences among different retrieval models, what they all seem to have in common is their prediction that interference effects arise when there is a match between multiple encodings in memory and certain cues that should be retrieved. The most well-known retrieval model of attraction is the one proposed by Wagers et al. (2009). The underlying hypothesis is that encountering the agreement target initiates a search through memory to retrieve the head. Misretrieval of the attractor might occur only when an ungrammatical sentence is encountered, such as “the key to the cabinets are rusty,” due to partial cue-matches between the agreement target and the previously identified NPs (e.g., Head noun: NOM, attractor: PL). On the contrary, grammatical sentences are predicted to remain unaffected (but see Cunnings and Sturt, 2018 and Jäger et al., 2017 for inhibitory interference in grammatical sentences with long-distance dependencies) given that the verb is not specified for number and the first NP offers a full match. Certain studies on comprehension have confirmed this hypothesis, mostly for number (e.g., Wagers et al., 2009; Lago et al., 2015, 2018; Slioussar, 2018).
The two groups of accounts make different predictions for attraction. Representational accounts predict attraction to affect both grammatical and ungrammatical sentences, while retrieval accounts predict that attraction affects only ungrammatical sentences. At the same time, the markedness of the attractor can be more easily explained under a representational account.
Gender Agreement Attraction: Theoretical Considerations and Experimental Evidence
The above accounts have been based largely on number attraction. However, as it has been pointed out in the literature, they should be reconsidered for gender attraction because gender differs from number with respect to the involvement or non-involvement of meaning (Bock, 2004, p. 116): the gender of a noun in grammatical-gender languages can be either conceptual or grammatical, whereas number on most nouns has semantic motivation. Eberhard et al. (2005, p. 553) discuss the extent to which their Marking and Morphing model could account for gender attraction and they are open to the idea that this model is able to account for gender attraction when the noun bears a conceptual gender. When it is not, the Marking stage is cancelled (see also Acuña-Fariña et al., 2014; Slioussar and Malko, 2016). However, Eberhard et al. (2005) also argue that the model might be relevant to grammatical gender, although this relevance is restricted to the lexical level (p. 553-4). Taking the latter under consideration, it seems that they do not necessarily exclude grammatical gender from their model, although it is not clear how exactly gender agreement attraction would be explained.
Other researchers suggest alternative accounts for agreement attraction with grammatical gender. Slioussar and Malko (2016) point out that (number and/or gender) attraction is still possible without any semantic effects, and consequently, attraction should stem from a different process (e.g., from the formal properties of features). In a similar vein, Acuña-Fariña et al. (2014) claim for grammatical gender that only competing percolation (of form) inside a complex NP could explain gender attraction. They further argue that in percolation/copying models there are many reasons why the agreement mechanism could fail, such as it might simply select the gender feature of the wrong noun in the complex NP to pass to the predicate. Thus, percolation accounts could predict gender attraction even with inanimate nouns which hold arbitrary gender where a copying mechanism would apply lexically. Percolation would occur at the level of form, reflecting that the feature of the wrong NP has been selected. Thus, both grammatical and ungrammatical sentences are expected to be influenced by gender attraction and also the marked attractor is expected to cause (more) attraction.
On the other hand, retrieval accounts could apply for gender agreement attraction in both production and comprehension (e.g., Badecker and Kuminiak, 2007; Wagers et al., 2009) irrespective of the conceptual and grammatical gender distinction. Under this group of models, gender attraction is expected to influence ungrammatical sentences only, similarly to number, while both marked and unmarked attractors are expected to show attraction.
Most studies on gender attraction have focused on production and have reported the existence of the phenomenon in languages with a bipartite or tripartite gender distinction. These studies revealed that marked attractors do not significantly cause more attraction in a bipartite system (e.g., Vigliocco and Franck, 1999 for French and Italian). However, marked attractors seem to influence languages with a tripartite system, such as Slovak (Badecker and Kuminiak, 2007) and Russian (Slioussar and Malko, 2016, Experiment 1).
In comprehension, attraction studies are scarce. For example, there are currently only two studies testing gender attraction in a language with a tripartite gender distinction (Slioussar and Malko, 2016 for Russian; Tucker et al., 2016 for Arabic). Slioussar and Malko (2016) found no markedness effects in Russian comprehension and no attraction in grammatical sentences, confirming retrieval accounts of agreement attraction. In Arabic, Tucker et al. (2016) found markedness effects; however, attraction affected ungrammatical sentences only. The authors conclude that their results seem to be better explained under retrieval accounts of agreement attraction and that the effect of markedness may be more relevant to agreement itself.
There are also six attraction studies in Spanish that consistently found no markedness effects (Martin et al., 2012, 2014; Acuña-Fariña et al., 2014) or no attraction at all (Fuchs et al., 2015). However, results are mixed with respect to whether attraction influences grammatical sentences (e.g., Martin et al., 2012, 2014; Acuña-Fariña et al., 2014) or not (e.g., Cunnings et al., 2017). An important aspect that needs to be taken into consideration, though, is the fact that the structures and/or the factor of animacy of the head/attractor differed across these studies. Additionally, most studies have only measured the time-course of attraction. Only Fuchs et al. (2015) and Cunnings et al. (2017) (Experiment 1) measured gender attraction in untimed acceptability judgments and they failed to find attraction, reflecting that (gender) attraction may be dependent on timing. One methodology that has not been used with gender attraction so far is speeded judgments which are considered to be more sensitive to representational effects (Wagers et al., 2009). For French and Italian, Villata et al. (2018) and Villata and Franck (2019) found that grammatical sentences are affected by marked (feminine) attractors and they attribute their findings to predictive processing as well as retrieval.
Another important aspect which influences attraction is the type of attractor. It has been shown that certain manipulations on the attractor play an important role. One of those is the morphophonological similarity between the attractor and the head (Hartsuiker et al., 2003; Slioussar, 2018). However, some other manipulations of the attractor do not influence attraction in comprehension (e.g., notional number and plausibility of the attractor, Schlueter et al., 2018). To our knowledge, such attractor manipulations have only been scarcely explored for gender agreement attraction in comprehension (e.g., Fuchs et al., 2015). In our study, we do so by focusing on another morphophonological manipulation, this time between the attractor and the agreement target. We namely explore whether phonological matching (Agathopoulou et al., 2010) between the nominal inflectional morphemes of the attractor and the agreement target influences gender attraction; i.e., the agreement target not only shares the same gender value with the attractor but also the exact same morphophonological realization for this gender value, as in example (2). Agreement attraction occurs during certain syntactic computations (e.g., at the feature-checking stage). Thus, whether attraction is influenced by certain morphophonological encodings provides us with a window into the modularity vs. interactivity of language (see Franck et al., 2008 for discussion).
Furthermore, recent studies explore the role of agreement target itself in attraction. This body of research (e.g., Phillips et al., 2011; Dillon et al., 2013) has shown that agreement targets are not all equally vulnerable to attraction (e.g., reflexive pronouns are less prone to attraction due to the application of syntactic rather than morphological cues during retrieval). In the present study we test gender agreement attraction not only with adjectival predicates but also with object-clitics targets. Our main motivation is to better understand which agreement targets are vulnerable to gender attraction. To that end, we explore whether across all different measures employed in this study, clitic dependencies are prone to gender attraction effects similarly to those found for verbs and adjectival predication. To date, only one study has tested object-clitics in sentence comprehension and found number attraction (Santesteban et al., 2017).
Apart from attraction, a large part of the psycholinguistic literature has also focused on studying simple agreement violation paradigms. This body of research (e.g., Fuchs et al., 2015; Bañón and Rothman, 2016; Kaltsa et al., 2016) focused on the sensitivity to agreement rules during real-time processing and provided useful evidence regarding the structural organization of feature values within a certain language (see Fuchs et al., 2015 for discussion).
Gender Agreement in Greek
Greek is a language with grammatical gender. At the same time, phonological criteria seem to partially predict gender values (Alexiadou, 2004; Alexiadou et al., 2007), while declension classes do not provide a safe criterion on the categorization of nouns into different gender values. Greek bears a three-gender distinction of masculine, feminine, and neuter with various agreement targets, such as determiners, attributive and predicate adjectives, adjectival participles, personal, demonstrative, definite, indefinite, interrogative, relative pronouns, and inflected numerals. However, there is no one-to-one match between a certain suffix and a gender value (see dromos, harakas, fortistis in (1a), many suffixes are distributed across (all or two) values on nouns (see dromos, isodos, edafos in (1a) as well as agreement targets, such as attributive adjectives (1b), adjectival predicates (1c), and object-clitic pronouns (1d). Additionally, number and case are marked on the same suffix with gender in both head nouns and targets (e.g., Anastasiadi-Simeonidi and Chila-Markopoulou, 2003; Ralli, 2003). Interestingly, the features of these suffixes can also interact with each other within the inflectional system leading to morphophonological ambiguities/syncretism (1b).
Another issue which is relevant to the present work is gender markedness in Greek. Theoretical accounts (e.g., Anagnostopoulou, 2017) as well as experimental data (e.g., Kazana, 2011) show that the default gender which holds unmarked properties in Greek is mediated by animacy; neuter is the default gender in inanimate nouns and masculine in animate nouns. Focusing on neuter, the majority of nouns and neologisms are neuter in Greek (Anastasiadi-Simeonidi and Chila-Markopoulou, 2003). Neuter is also the index of metalinguistic use as well as the value of impersonal structures and it also modifies genderless elements, such as sentences, prepositions, and “mentioned” words. Greek children overgeneralize neuter with inanimate nouns when errors occur, especially with feminine nouns (see Tsimpli and Hulk, 2013 and references therein). Neuter is the most ambiguous value, bearing the same marking on the determiner and the noun itself for NOM and ACC (to kastro “the castle”) while determiners disambiguate case in feminine nouns (i limni vs. ti limni, “the lake”). In cases of subject co-ordination with two inanimate nouns, feminine agreement occurs when the nouns are both feminine (but neuter may still be considered an option by some Greek speakers even in this case, see Kazana, 2011, p.79), while neuter agreement is the elsewhere condition when there is conflict between the gender values of the two inanimate nouns (Kazana, 2011; Anagnostopoulou, 2017; Manasis, 2019).
The Present Research
The present research explores gender agreement attraction in Greek sentence comprehension in real-time (Study 1) as well as in timed and untimed acceptability judgments (Study 2) across two structurally different configurations, i.e., adjectival predicates and object-clitics. Thus, we investigate not only how participants process sentences in real-time but also how they judge the acceptability of sentences with or without time pressure. This is the first study to investigate gender attraction across two structurally different configurations by using similar methods and materials, and the first attraction study in Greek. The current experiments address the following questions:
(a) Does Greek show attraction during real-time sentence comprehension? (Experiment 1)
(b) Are object-clitics prone to gender agreement attraction? (Experiment 2)
(c) Does gender agreement attraction occur under time pressure, i.e., in timed (speeded) grammaticality judgments? (Experiment 3)
(d) Is gender attraction evident in the absence of time pressure, i.e., in untimed acceptability judgments? (Experiment 4).
Across all four experiments, the same topics are explored, that is the processing strategies the parser aligns with during the comprehension of agreement attraction: namely, the existence of attraction in grammatical vs. ungrammatical sentences, the markedness of the attractor, phonological matching, and the sensitivity to gender agreement rules in the absence of attraction.
Study 1
Study 1 consisted of Experiment 1 and Experiment 2.
Experiment 1
Experiment 1 investigated gender attraction in adjectival predicates.
Methodology
Participants
Fifty-two healthy adult native speakers of Greek (mean age = 23.2, range = 18–33, 23 females) completed Experiment 1. They were born and raised in Greece, they had never lived abroad, and both of their parents were Greek. In this and all other experiments reported, all participants provided informed consent and they received a fee of 10 Euros/hour for their participation. The participants were recruited via mailing lists, and they were tested individually in a quiet office of the Aristotle University of Thessaloniki in Greece.
Materials and Design
Sentences including a set of feminine and a set of neuter head nouns with their agreement targets were tested; the following variables were manipulated for each set in a 2 × 2 within-subjects design: (a) the Grammaticality of the sentence (grammatical, ungrammatical), depending on the gender value of the predicate; and (b) the Attractor (match, mismatch), depending on whether the gender value of the attractor matches or mismatches the value of the head. Table 1 shows a complete item for feminine and neuter heads.
Table 1. A complete item for feminine and neuter heads in Experiment 1. R stands for different regions.
Twenty-four items (6 per condition, 24 for each gender value) were distributed across four lists in a Latin Square design after being combined with 48 grammatical fillers, which appeared in pseudo-random order. Consequently, 25% of the trials within each list were ungrammatical. Additionally, eight grammatical practice trials were also created and none of them included the target configuration. Feminine and neuter heads were chosen to test for markedness asymmetries in inanimate nouns between the most marked value (feminine) and the least marked one (neuter), see section “Gender agreement attraction: theoretical considerations and experimental evidence.” Sentences of the type Introduction - Verb - Head - Attractor - Agreement target - Adverb - PP - Modifier were constructed, where the Head is a DP in ACC, the Attractor is an intervening PP including an NP in ACC licensed by the preposition ja (“for”) and it is syntactically unavailable for agreement, and Agreement target is a past-participle. The structure of a small clause was selected because a configuration in which a noun intervenes between the head noun and the agreement target and in which all the elements are inflected for gender was needed. This configuration with the head noun in the object position - instead of the subject position - was selected because Greek bears morphological case, which could decrease attraction due to morphological disambiguation between ACC (i.e., attractor) and NOM (i.e., head) (Hartsuiker et al., 2003; Slioussar, 2018). Additionally, the ja-PP was intentionally selected because it is less ambiguous compared to other PPs with respect to its attachment point (Katsika, 2009). As for the suffixes of the heads, feminine heads ended in -a and -i, and neuter heads ended in -o and -i and -ma. Thus, both sets of heads included at least one suffix with a strong phonological predictive value (predictive values: feminine -a = 0.97, feminine -i = 0.45, neuter-o = 98, neuter-i = 0.55, neuter-ma = 0.75, Mastropavlou, 2006, p.134). Half of the suffixes of the attractors were creating a phonological match with the suffix of the agreement target in the ungrammatical conditions to test the role of phonological realization (Phonological matching: matching, mismatching) on attraction (2), i.e., whether phonological matching exhibits more attraction.
There were three verbs in past-tense and perfective aspect, equally distributed across items, and they consisted of two-syllables: βρκε “found,” εχε “had” and εδε “saw,” The agreement target was followed by certain prepositions which were also used an equal number of times. Frequency, length (in syllables), and plausibility were also taken under consideration. The length was between 2-4 syllables for heads, 2-3 syllables for attractors, 3-4 syllables for agreement targets, and 2 syllables for the adverbs following the target. Given that different nouns for attractors between the matching (attractor match) and mismatching (attractor mismatch) conditions were used, frequency was also measured by extracting and comparing the lemma frequency from the National Hellenic Corpus. The difference in frequency between the matching and mismatching attractors was not significant (Supplementary Material S1). Additionally, careful consideration was taken to normalize the items with respect to plausibility of head noun - attractor - agreement target between the match and mismatch conditions. This was done by receiving feedback for the naturalness of the items from both linguists as well as naïve native speakers.
All trials were followed by a yes or no comprehension question (with theta-role reversal or lexical replacement) and were never referring to the head, the attractor, or the agreement target. Grammatical sentences were recorded in a sound booth by a female native speaker of Greek in such a way to avoid co-articulation between the words that belonged to different segments. The comprehension questions were recorded by a male native speaker of Greek. The ungrammatical versions of the sentences were created by splicing out the agreement target and replacing it with the target from a grammatical sentence (Ferreira et al., 1996; Marinis, 2010; Papadopoulou et al., 2014).
Procedure
In Experiment 1, the self-paced listening methodology was employed (Ferreira et al., 1996; Marinis, 2010) and reaction-time and accuracy data were collected. In this and all other experiments of the present work, the presentation of the stimuli and the recording of end-of-sentence responses and/or reaction times across regions were controlled by the E-prime software (Psychology Software Tools Inc. [E-Prime 3.0], 2016 Pittsburgh, PA, United States). The stimuli were auditorily presented. We chose the auditory modality because we were interested in exploring the influence of attraction in auditory comprehension, which is underexplored and forms a more unplanned and spontaneous modality in daily communication compared to the visual (reading) one. Both the auditory method of Experiments 1 and 2 (see section “Experiment2”) as well as of Experiment 4 (see section “Experiment 4”) have been used extensively and successfully in the previous literature (see Fuchs et al., 2015 and also Papadopoulou et al., 2014 for discussion) and are also highly important for research in young children and heritage speakers (Paspali, 2019a, b) and/or for languages without a writing system. Also note, that the reading method of Experiment 3 (see section “Experiment 3”) showed similar results with the other two methods.
The sentences were divided in eight regions as indicated in the example (3) below; R3 shows the Region of the Head, R4 the Region of the Attractor, and R5 the Region of the agreement target. The duration of the sound files did not differ on the Attractor between the match and the mismatch condition and the same applies to the duration of the agreement target (Region 5) between the grammatical and the ungrammatical agreement target (Supplementary Materials S2.1.1, S2.2.1).
Participants were tested individually. They were seated comfortably at a desk and they listened to the sentences on a laptop via headphones. In each trial, they listened to a sentence region by region by pressing a button on a button box. Participants were instructed to listen to each region for comprehension and to press the button as soon as they were ready to listen to the next region. To proceed to the comprehension question, participants had to press the same button and then had to press the yes/no button to respond to the comprehension question. They received feedback on their accuracy in each trial; the LED lights on the button box flashed green three times for a correctly answered question and red for an incorrectly answered question. The underlying rationale of the task is that longer RTs for a certain condition on a specific region indicate relatively higher processing difficulty compared to a control condition. The experiment lasted approximately 40 min.
Predictions
If gender attraction occurs, a facilitation in RTs in the ungrammatical mismatch condition is expected on the region (R5) of the agreement target (or the spill-over regions) compared to the ungrammatical match condition. However, if grammatical sentences are also affected, an inhibition in the grammatical mismatch condition compared to the grammatical match condition is also expected with longer RTs for the Attractor region (R4) due to the representation of the complex NP itself (or the downstream regions), indicating processing difficulties due to mismatch1. Additionally, attraction is expected to be (more) evident with marked attractors (neuter heads with feminine attractors) than with unmarked attractors (feminine heads with neuter attractors) if markedness of the attractor plays a role. Moreover, if participants exhibit sensitivity to gender agreement violations in real-time, a main effect of Grammaticality is expected on the agreement region and/or the two post-critical regions (longer RTs in the ungrammatical match condition compared to the grammatical match one). Finally, if phonological matching plays a role in regulating attraction, an interaction between Attractor and Phonological matching is expected with shorter RTs in sentences with phonological matching compared to sentences without phonological mismatching.
Analysis
A 70% threshold of accuracy was set for participants in the filler sentences and for items in the experimental sentences. Thus, two items were removed in Experiment 1, and the data from one participant and three items in Experiment 2 (one from the dataset of the feminine heads, two from the dataset of the neuter heads). For the RT analysis, only trials in which participants had correctly responded to the comprehension question remained in the dataset. RTs were calculated by subtracting the duration of each region (sound file) from the raw RTs for each item to obtain the Difference Time (Ferreira et al., 1996, p. 327; Marinis and Saddy, 2013, p. 169; Papadopoulou et al., 2014, p. 60). Extreme values below 150 ms and above 2,500 ms were excluded from the analysis based on previous literature (Marinis and Saddy, 2013; Parker et al., 2015; Ratcliff, 1993; Schlueter et al., 2018). Additionally, values below or above 2.5 SD by condition were treated as outliers and deleted (Häussler, 2009; Franck et al., 2015; Ahn and Jiang, 2018). Overall, the data cleaning did not affect more than 15% of the data (Ratcliff, 1993) in Experiment 1 (Supplementary Material S3). The regions analyzed were R3 (the region of the Head/pre-critical region), R4 (the region of the Attractor/first critical region), R5 (the region of the agreement target/second critical region), and the post-critical regions, namely R6 and R7 (for potential spillover effects), similar to previous attraction studies (e.g., Wagers et al., 2009; Lago et al., 2015; Slioussar and Malko, 2016; Parker and An, 2018). RTs were log-transformed in all models.
The data analysis for this and all other experiments was conducted in R (R Development Core Team, 2017) in the library languageR with the lme4 package. For accuracy data, logistic mixed models were fit, and in the case of the log-transformed Reaction Times (RTs) linear mixed-effects models were fit. Grammaticality and Attractor were modeled as fixed effects, using effects coding with orthogonal contrasts (Grammaticality: grammatical = −0.5, ungrammatical = 0.5, Attractor: match = −0.5, mismatch = 0.5), as well as their interaction. Following Dillon et al. (2013) and Parker and An (2018), additional linear mixed-effects models were fit to focus on the effect of attraction (i.e., the amount of facilitation for ungrammatical sentences with attractor mismatch relative to ungrammatical sentences with attractor match) represented as “Attraction model” in Tables 2, 4. Effects coding was also used here for Attractor (match = −0.5 mismatch = 0.5). For comparison, analyses on the raw untrimmed data have also been conducted (Supplementary Material S8), given that certain studies have shown that data transformations may obscure attraction effects (e.g., Staub, 2010; Tucker and Almeida, 2017), and both analyses show similar results. To test whether phonological matching influences attraction in ungrammatical sentences, a model with Phonological matching (phonological matching = −0.5, phonological mismatching = 0.5) and Attractor (match = −0.5, mismatch = 0.5) as fixed effects as well as their interaction was also fit for ungrammatical sentences. Furthermore, all pairwise comparisons reported in this manuscript were conducted using similar contrasts (effects coding) and models (lmer/glmer) as the corresponding omnibus analysis. The initial random effects structure of all models included random intercepts and slopes for both participants and items. When the models failed to converge, the maximal random effects structure was gradually simplified following the recent literature (Barr et al., 2013) until convergence was reached.
Results
No significant differences by condition were found in accuracy to comprehension questions (Supplementary Material S6). Thus, we proceeded with the RT analyses. Figure 1 shows participants’ mean RTs and the standard error of the mean by condition and region in both feminine and neuter heads (see Supplementary Material S4 for table with the means). Table 2 reports the results of the mixed-effects models in the log transformed RTs for Regions 4–7.
Figure 1. Mean reaction times by condition in feminine (A) and neuter (B) heads in Experiment 1. Error bars indicate the standard error of the mean across participants. R on the x-axis stands for Region. Region 3: Head, Region 4: Attractor, Region 5: Agreement target (adjectival predicate), Region 6: first post-critical region, Region 7: second post-critical region, and Region 8: sentence-final region.
Feminine heads
The pre-critical Region 3 (Supplementary Material S7) and Region 4 (Attractor) did not show significant effects. This demonstrates that attractor mismatch conditions do not show increased RTs compared to the match ones due to mismatch. In Region 5 (agreement target), a main effect of Attractor was observed reflecting shorter RTs in the attractor mismatch conditions compared to the attractor match conditions. Crucially, the interaction between Grammaticality and Attractor and the attraction model were both significant, reflecting a facilitation in RTs in the ungrammatical mismatch condition. Pairwise comparisons (significance level adjusted to p = 0.013 using Bonferroni correction) showed that an ungrammatical match received longer RTs than a grammatical match (β = 0.127, SE = 0.033, t = 3.83, p < 0.001), while this was not the case for the mismatch conditions (grammatical mismatch vs. ungrammatical mismatch: β = 0.009, SE = 0.032, t = 0.30, p = 0.767). Crucially, the attraction effect only occurred in the ungrammatical sentences, as Table 2 shows, with a facilitation in RTs for the ungrammatical mismatch compared to the ungrammatical match. The grammatical sentences were not affected by attractor mismatch (β = −0.013, SE = 0.049, t = −0.27, p = 0.793) confirming attraction in ungrammatical sentences only, as predicted by retrieval accounts. On Region 6 (first post-critical region), an effect of Grammaticality was found, such that ungrammatical sentences overall exhibited longer RTs compared to the grammatical ones. Since feminine heads with neuter attractors exhibited attraction on Region 5 (Agreement target), the role of phonological matching was also explored. Figure 2 shows mean RTs by condition.
Figure 2. Mean reaction times in ungrammatical sentences by Attractor (match, mismatch) and Phonological matching (phonological matching/mismatching) in feminine heads on Region 5 (Agreement target) in Experiment 1. Error bars indicate the standard error of the mean across participants.
The model detected a main effect of Attractor (β = −0.144, SE = 0.035, t = −4.117, p < 0.001) confirming the initial model of attraction. However, Phonological matching (β = 0.101, SE = 0.072, t = 1.40, p = 0.176), and, crucially, the interaction between Phonological matching and Attractor (β = −0.025, SE = 0.072, t = 0.10, p = 0.725) were not significant, reflecting that phonological matching between the attractor and the agreement target did not lead to greater facilitation in RTs when attraction occured.
Neuter heads
Figure 1 shows mean RTs and standard error of the mean by condition and region. In the pre-critical Region 3 (Head region), there was a significant effect of Attractor, such that attractor match conditions had shorter RTs than attractor mismatch conditions (Attractor: β = 0.080, SE = 0.032, t = 2.53, p = 0.016). Region 4 showed that this effect was facilitated by an interaction with Grammaticality. Pairwise comparisons confirmed this finding (grammatical match vs. ungrammatical match: β = −0.167, SE = 0.036, t = −4.63, p < 0.001; grammatical mismatch vs. ungrammatical mismatch: β = 0.049, SE = 0.037, t = 1.34, p = 0.180; grammatical match vs. grammatical mismatch: β = −0.064, SE = 0.038, t = −1.70, p = 0.089; ungrammatical match vs. ungrammatical mismatch: β = 0.140, SE = 0.036, t = 3.86, p < 0.001). Region 5 (Agreement target) showed a similar significant interaction too, which, however, was toward the opposite direction of attraction (facilitation in the ungrammatical match instead of the ungrammatical mismatch). Region 6 showed a main effect of Grammaticality demonstrating that the ungrammatical conditions exhibited longer RTs compared to the grammatical ones on this region. Region 7 also showed a main effect of Attractor showing that the mismatch conditions received longer RTs compared to the match ones.
Discussion
The results confirm the presence of attraction at the nominal domain in Greek comprehension. Furthermore, attraction affected only ungrammatical sentences confirming retrieval accounts. Moreover, the fact that attraction occurred with the unmarked value (neuter) could not be easily captured under a representational account which predicts attraction only with marked/feminine attractors. When looking at neuter heads, the results are less clear because the significant effect on the pre-critical region (Region 3) and the interaction in the Attractor region (Region 4) were unexpected. This interaction on Region 4 remained significant on Region 5 (agreement target) too, and its direction was the opposite one from what was predicted if attraction had occurred. Given that Region 3 occurs before the critical regions (Region 4 and Region 5) and that ungrammaticality occurs only when participants reach Region 5, no differences of Attractor/Grammaticality for Region 3 and of Grammaticality for Region 4 were expected. Consequently, it is likely that the effects found on these regions are spurious (Patson and Husband, 2016, p. 956) and possibly responsible for the absence of attraction on the critical region.
Experiment 2
Methodology
Participants
The same participants who participated in Experiment 1 also participated in Experiment 2, however, in a different session. The order of experiments by session was counterbalanced and there was at least a 2-week interval between the two sessions.
Materials and Design
Experiment 2 had a similar design and materials to Experiment 1. The crucial modification here was the target of the agreement. In this experiment, agreement on pronominal reference was tested by using object-clitic targets. In this experiment, the verb of the introductory sentence was in past-tense and imperfective aspect (three verbs were equally distributed across items: epsahne, anazituse, jireve “was looking for”) and the verb following the object-clitic target was in past-tense and perfective aspect (five verbs were equally distributed across items: vrike “found,” eide “saw,” entopise “found,” anaklipse “discovered,” adikrise “countered”). The same variables as in Experiment 1 were manipulated. The object-clitic was presented in the same region with the verb because they form one phonological unit. Table 3 shows a complete item across conditions for feminine and neuter heads, respectively.
Procedure
Experiment 2 had the same procedure as Experiment 1. The sentences were also divided into eight regions, as in example (4). The duration of the sound files did not differ on the Attractor between the match and the mismatch condition and the same applies to the duration of the agreement target (Region 5) between the grammatical and ungrammatical agreement target (Supplementary Materials S2.1.2, S2.2.2). Experiment 2 lasted approximately 40 min.
Predictions
The same predictions holding for Experiment 1 hold also for Experiment 2.
Analysis
The same analyses used for Experiment 1 were also used for Experiment 2.
Results
No significant differences by condition were found in accuracy to the comprehension questions (Supplementary Material S6). Thus, we proceeded with the RT analyses. Figure 3 shows participants’ mean RTs and standard error of the mean by condition and region in both feminine and neuter heads. Table 4 reports the results of the mixed-effects models in the log-transformed RTs for Regions 4–7.
Figure 3. Mean reaction time by condition in feminine (A) and neuter (B) heads in Experiment 2. Error bars indicate the standard error of the mean across participants. R on the x-axis stands for Region. Region 3: Head, Region 4: Attractor, Region 5: Agreement target (past-participle), Region 6: first post-critical region, Region 7: second post-critical region and Region 8: sentence-final region.
Feminine heads
Region 3 (pre-critical region), Region 4 (Attractor), and Region 5 (Agreement target) did not show any significant effects. However, in Region 6 (post-critical 1) the interaction between Grammaticality and Attractor was approaching significance and the attraction model was significant, reflecting a facilitation in RTs in the ungrammatical mismatch condition. Grammatical sentences were not affected by attractor mismatch (grammatical match vs. grammatical mismatch: β = 0.001, SE = 0.027, t = 0.05, p = 0.957) confirming attraction only in ungrammatical sentences, as predicted by retrieval accounts. In Region 7 (post-critical 2), a main effect of Grammaticality showed that the ungrammatical sentences received longer RTs than the grammatical ones, reflecting that participants were sensitive to ungrammaticality. The role of Phonological matching in modulating the attraction pattern was also explored. Figure 4A shows mean RTs in ungrammatical match/mismatch conditions by Phonological matching.
Figure 4. Mean reaction times in ungrammatical sentences by Attractor (match, mismatch) and Phonological matching (phonological matching/mismatching) in feminine heads, Region 6 (A), neuter heads, Region 5 (B), neuter heads, Region 6 (C), and neuter heads, Region 7 (D) in Experiment 2. Error bars indicate the standard error of the mean across participants.
There was an effect of Attractor (β = −0.078, SE = 0.033, t = −2.39, p = 0.021). However, Phonological matching (β = −0.070, SE = 0.048, t = −1.46, p = 0.156) and the interaction between Phonological matching and Attractor (β = 0.097, SE = 0.068, t = 1.44, p = 0.157) were not significant; Phonological matching did not facilitate RTs in the ungrammatical mismatch condition.
Neuter heads
No differences were found in Regions 3 and 4. In Region 5, there was a main effect of Attractor, such that attractor match received longer RTs than mismatch. Crucially, the attraction models were significant on Regions 5, 6, and 7, reflecting attraction in ungrammatical sentences (facilitation in RTs for the ungrammatical mismatch). Crucially, grammatical mismatch sentences did not show increased RTs compared to grammatical match sentences (Region 5: = −0.042, SE = 0.036, t = −1.56, p = 0.248, Region 6: = −0.031, SE = 0.031, t = −1.01, p = 0.312; Region 7: = −0.033, SE = 0.038, t = −0.86, p = 0.389). Since attraction was found, the role of Phonological matching was also explored. Figures 4B–D shows mean RTs in ungrammatical match/mismatch condition by Phonological matching. There was an effect of Attractor (Region 5: β = −0.113, SE = 0.044, t = −2.55, p = 0.011, Region 6: β = −0.094, SE = 0.030, t = −3.104, p = 0.003, Region 7: β = −0.120, SE = 0.037, t = −3.242, p = 0.001) confirming what the comparisons above had captured. Phonological matching (Region 5: β = −0.068, SE = 0.069, t = −0.986, p = 0.336, Region 6: β = 0.009, SE = 0.055, t = 0.162, p = 0.873, Region 7: β = 0.090, SE = 0.076, t = 1.181, p = 0.252) and crucially the interaction between Phonological matching and Attractor were not significant (Region 5: β = 0.027, SE = 0.089, t = 0.306, p = 0.760, Region 6: β = 0.105, SE = 0.061, t = 1.718, p = 0.090, Region 7: β = 0.014, SE = 0.076, t = 0.189, p = 0.850).
Discussion
The results of Experiment 2 showed that attraction occurs with feminine heads in line with what was found in Experiment 1, as well as with neuter heads. Thus, similarly to adjectival predicates, attraction on object-clitics occurred only in ungrammatical sentences in line with retrieval accounts, and both feminine and neuter heads were affected.
Discussion Study 1
Overall, Study 1 showed that agreement attraction occurs in Greek during real-time sentence comprehension. Furthermore, most comprehension studies with gender attraction have focused on the verbal domain (e.g., Slioussar and Malko, 2016). Our study shows that the nominal domain is also prone to gender agreement attraction throughout the time course of sentence comprehension. Our study also shows that object-clitics are prone to gender attraction in comprehension as well (see Santesteban et al. (2017) for number attraction with clitics in Basque). Overall, our findings suggest that grammatical gender in inanimate nouns can induce attraction too, in line with previous studies (e.g., Slioussar and Malko, 2016). The fact that an uninterpretable feature (gender in inanimate nouns), i.e., without semantic content, can cause attraction speaks directly to the stage of processing where grammatical illusions arise, namely at the feature-checking stage. Consequently, our results suggest that attraction does not arise at the conceptual level and exhibits itself even in the absence of this level (e.g., Acuña-Fariña et al., 2014; Slioussar and Malko, 2016). The fact that phonological matching did not influence attraction reflects that the similarity of morphophonological cues between the attractor and the agreement target is not decisive in attraction. Finally, adult native speakers of Greek were sensitive to gender agreement violations in the ungrammatical match condition with both adjectival predicates (in both feminine and neuter heads) and object-clitics (in feminine heads), demonstrating that they process gender on both agreement targets.
Study 2
Study 2 targeted participants’ timed and untimed judgments in order to test whether gender agreement attraction influences end-of-sentence measurements in comprehension. In the following experiments, the same materials as in Study 1 were used for reasons of consistency and comparability.
Experiment 3
Experiment 3 was a speeded Grammaticality Judgement task (s-GJT) and targeted participants’ ability in judging sentences with gender agreement attraction under time pressure.
Methodology
Participants
The participants’ pool consisted of 37 healthy adult native speakers of Greek (mean age = 21.6, age range = 18–32, 18 female).
Materials and Design
The materials included the experimental sentences from Experiment 1 (adjectival predicates) and Experiment 2 (object-clitics). The design, manipulations, and number of items were all similar to Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 (Tables 1, 2). Thus, 48 out of 96 experimental sentences were ungrammatical. Additionally, 104 filler sentences were constructed; half of them were ungrammatical including a variety of violations. The agreement target was in sentence-final position following the previous literature of s-GJTs (e.g., Tanner et al., 2014; Lago et al., 2018; Hammerly et al., 2019) and an adverb (Supplementary Material S5) was also added before the agreement target to increase the processing demands of the task (“John was looking for the recipe for the bread and (he) eventually found it”).
Procedure
All participants were tested individually in a quiet room at the Aristotle-University of Thessaloniki. Participants were told that their task was to read and judge sentences in Greek fast. Then they read the written instructions on the computer screen. They were also instructed to judge the sentences as fast and accurate as they could. There were six practice trials at the beginning of the task, half of them ungrammatical. The practice trials did not include violations of gender agreement and participants were receiving feedback on their accuracy only during the practice trials. The stimuli were visually presented on a laptop. The sentences were presented one word at a time at the center of the screen. Each word remained on the screen for 250 ms plus 25 ms further for each character to compensate for length differences. The participants had to press the space button on the keyboard to start reading a sentence whenever they were ready. A fixation cross (remaining on the screen for 1,500 ms) was preceding the first word to warn participants that a new trial was about to start. After the end of the sentence, a question mark appeared at the center of the screen and participants had to indicate whether the sentence was grammatical or ungrammatical by pressing the green (grammatical) or the red (ungrammatical) button on the keyboard (K and D keys, respectively). If participants failed to respond within 1,700 ms from the end of a trial, a warning appeared on the screen to let them know that they were too slow. During the task, no feedback was given for their accuracy. Overall, the session lasted approximately 30 min.
Predictions
Attractor mismatches between the gender value of the head and the attractor are expected to influence accuracy scores if attraction occurs. If attraction affects ungrammatical sentences only, lower accuracy in rejecting the ungrammatical mismatch condition is expected compared to the ungrammatical match condition. Alternatively, if attraction affects grammatical sentences too, lower accuracy in accepting the grammatical mismatch condition compared to the grammatical match condition is also expected. Moreover, if gender markedness modulates attraction patterns, then attraction is more likely to occur with marked attractors (e.g., neuter heads-feminine attractors) than with unmarked attractors (feminine heads-neuter attractors). Also, based on the findings of Experiment 1, phonological matching should not modulate attraction.
Analysis
A 70% accuracy threshold was set as an inclusion criterion. All participants scored above this threshold. Additionally, trials in which participants exceeded the response deadline (RTs > 1,700 ms) were deleted, influencing less than 3.6% of the dataset (Supplementary Material S3). In all cases, following the current literature on agreement attraction we report (a) the results from the logistic regression models with Grammaticality and Attractor as fixed effects with orthogonal contrasts (Grammaticality: grammatical = 0.5, ungrammatical = −0.5, Attractor: match = 0.5, mismatch = −0.5) as well as their interaction, and (b) planned pairwise comparisons employing logistic regression models between the grammatical match and the grammatical mismatch conditions and between the ungrammatical match and the ungrammatical mismatch conditions, given that the theoretical questions of interest crucially depend on whether attraction influences both grammatical and ungrammatical sentences or only ungrammatical sentences (e.g., Lago et al., 2015; Hammerly et al., 2019;, p. 12; Tanner et al., 2014, p. 12). Attractor was coded as a fixed effect in the pairwise comparisons, and the significance level was corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction (significance level adjusted to p = 0.025). A model of Phonological matching was also fit, when attraction in ungrammatical sentences was significant, including Attractor (match = 0.5, mismatch = −0.5) and Phonological matching (phonological matching = 0.5, phonological mismatching = −0.5) as well as their interaction.
Results
The mean accuracy in the filler sentences was 89% (SD = 31, range = 78–97). Table 5 shows participants’ accuracy by condition and Table 6 reports the results of the mixed-effects models.
Table 5. Participants’ mean accuracy and standard error in adjectival predicates and object-clitics in Experiment 3.
Table 6. Linear mixed-effects model results of participants’ accuracy in adjectival predicates and object-clitics in Experiment 3.
Adjectival predicates
Feminine heads
The logistic regression model and the pre-planned pairwise comparisons did not detect any significant differences.
Neuter heads
The model revealed a main effect of Grammaticality, such that participants were more accurate in rejecting ungrammatical sentences than accepting grammatical sentences and a main effect of Attractor, demonstrating that attractor mismatch decreased accuracy. The planned pairwise comparisons confirmed attraction in ungrammatical sentences; accuracy in ungrammatical sentences decreased due to attractor mismatch (β = 1.646; SE = 0.509; z = 3.23; p = 0.001), reflecting that participants incorrectly gave more “grammatical” responses for the ungrammatical mismatch condition. In grammatical sentences, accuracy lowered due to attractor mismatch by 7% (β = 0.636; SE = 0.299; z = 2.13; p = 0.033), although the difference was not significant (p > 0.025). Table 7 shows mean accuracy in ungrammatical match conditions grouped by Phonological matching.
Table 7. Participants’ mean accuracy and standard error in Experiment 3 grouped by Phonological matching.
The model of Phonological matching in ungrammatical sentences revealed a main effect of Attractor (β = 1.860; SE = 0.61; z = 3.05; p = 0.002) confirming the existence of attraction. However, no effect of Phonological matching was detected (β = 0.873; SE = 0.657; z = 1.33; p = 0.184) and no interaction with Attractor (β = 1.169; SE = 1.219; z = 0.96; p = 0.338), i.e., Phonological matching did not increase attraction errors.
Object-clitics
Feminine heads
The model revealed a main effect of Grammaticality demonstrating that participants were less accurate in rejecting ungrammatical sentences than accepting grammatical sentences. There was a main effect of Attractor showing a decrease in the participants’ accuracy in the mismatch conditions. In the planned pairwise comparisons the ungrammatical sentences seemed to be modulated by attractor mismatch (β = 0.857; SE = 0.257; z = 3.34; p < 0.001), but this was not the case in grammatical sentences (β = 0.767; SE = 0.398; z = 1.99; p = 0.054). Table 7 shows mean accuracy in ungrammatical match conditions grouped by Phonological matching. The model of Phonological matching in ungrammatical sentences revealed a main effect of Attractor (β = 0.924; SE = 0.403; z = 2.29; p = 0.022) confirming the existence of attraction. However, no effect of Phonological matching was detected (β = −0.312; SE = 0.352; z = −0.89; p = 0.375) and no interaction with Attractor (β = −0.275; SE = 0.618; z = −0.45; p = 0.657).
Neuter heads
The model revealed a main effect of Attractor demonstrating that the mismatch condition lowered participants’ accuracy. Crucially, the effect was modulated by a significant interaction with Grammaticality. Planned pairwise comparisons revealed that attractor mismatch decreased the accuracy rate in the ungrammatical condition but not in the grammatical one (ungrammatical: (β = 1.52; SE = 0.359; z = 4.24; p < 0.001, grammatical: β = 0.216; SE = 0.337; z = 0.64; p = 0.521). Table 7 shows mean accuracy in the ungrammatical match conditions grouped by Phonological matching. The model of Phonological matching in ungrammatical sentences revealed a main effect of Attractor (β = 1.926; SE = 0.667; z = 2.89; p = 0.004) confirming the existence of attraction. However, no effect of Phonological matching was detected (β = 0.655; SE = 0.553; z = 1.19; p = 0.236) and no interaction with Attractor (β = 0.682; SE = 0.887; z = 0.77; p = 0.442).
Discussion
The results of Experiment 3 showed that gender attraction occurs in timed judgments, in both feminine and neuter heads, demonstrating that both marked and unmarked attractors cause attraction in line with Experiment 2. Attraction affected only ungrammatical sentences in line with retrieval accounts and with what was found in Study 1. Although grammatical sentences seemed to be affected by attractor mismatches (e.g., participants’ tendency to reject grammatical sentences with mismatch in the neuter heads), this effect was not statistically reliable. Thus, attractor mismatches affected ungrammatical sentences only, confirming retrieval accounts.
Experiment 4
Experiment 4 was an auditory scaled Acceptability Judgment Task (AJT), similar to the one used by Fuchs et al. (2015) and tested whether gender attraction is evident in untimed scaled judgments.
Methodology
Participants
The participants’ pool consisted of 32 healthy adult native speakers of Greek (mean age = 20.4, age range = 18–24, 14 female).
Materials and design
Similarly to Experiment 3, the materials consisted of the materials of Experiment 1 and 2 (plus regions 6–8). Given that the task measured scaled judgments, some new marginally ungrammatical fillers were created for balance (e.g., nominal ellipsis, weak and strong islands, etc.). Overall, there were 201 trials, 1/3 were ungrammatical and 1/3 marginally ungrammatical. The design, the materials, and the number of experimental items were all the same as in Experiment 3.
Procedure
All participants were tested in a quiet room at the Aristotle-University of Thessaloniki. The sentences were auditorily presented via headphones. The participants had to press the space button on the keyboard to listen to each trial. During the auditory presentation of a trial, the screen remained blank. After the end of the trial, a question mark appeared on the screen and participants had to judge the sentences on a 1–7 Likert scale by pressing one of the relevant keys on the keyboard (1 not acceptable/bad - 7 completely acceptable/very good). The task was untimed and participants could spend as much time as they wanted on their response and no feedback was given for their judgments. Participants were instructed from the experimenter that their task was to listen to and judge sentences in Greek on a 1–7 scale. Participants then listened to the prerecorded instructions. There were 6 practice trials at the beginning of the task, 2 grammatical, 2 ungrammatical, 2 marginally ungrammatical. The practice trials did not include violations of gender agreement and no feedback was given to the participants for the way they rated the sentences. Overall, the session lasted 40 min approximately.
Predictions
If attraction occurs, attractor mismatch should increase acceptability ratings in the ungrammatical mismatch condition compared to the ungrammatical match condition. If attraction influences grammatical sentences too, then attractor mismatch should decrease ratings in the grammatical mismatch condition compared to the grammatical match condition. Additionally, if participants are sensitive to gender agreement violations overall (i.e., ungrammatical match condition), they are expected to give overall significantly higher acceptability ratings to grammatical than ungrammatical sentences. Based on the findings of Experiment 1, Phonological matching is not expected to modulate attraction with adjectival predicates.
Analysis
For participants’ ratings linear mixed effects models were fit as in Experiment 1. Analyses and contrast coding were similar to Experiment 3.
Results
The mean ratings in the fillers was 6.2 (SD = 1.58, range = 3–7) for grammatical sentences, 4.5 (SD = 2.2, range = 2.8–5.6) for marginally ungrammatical sentences, and 2.96 (SD = 1.9, range = 1.3–5) for ungrammatical sentences. Table 8 reports the mean acceptability ratings by condition in feminine and neuter heads for both adjectival predicates and object-clitics and Table 9 reports the results of the mixed-effects models by gender value and structure. An additional analysis on the z-transformed ratings was also conducted showing similar results (Supplementary Material S9).
Table 8. Participants’ mean ratings and standard error in adjectival predicates and object-clitics in Experiment 4.
Table 9. Linear mixed-effects model results of participants’ ratings in adjectival predicates and object-clitics in Experiment 4.
Adjectival predicates
Feminine heads
The model showed a main effect of Grammaticality; the difference between the grammatical and ungrammatical conditions was significant. The effect of Attractor and the interaction between Grammaticality and Attractor were not significant. The pairwise comparisons did not show any differences either.
Neuter heads
The model revealed a main effect of Grammaticality with significantly higher ratings for grammatical than ungrammatical sentences and no significant interaction between Grammaticality and Attractor. The pairwise comparisons did not show any significant differences either (p > 0.025).
Object-clitics
Feminine heads
The model revealed a main effect of Grammaticality with significantly higher ratings for grammatical than ungrammatical sentences. The interaction between Grammaticality and Attractor was marginally significant, reflecting higher acceptability ratings in the ungrammatical mismatch condition than the ungrammatical match condition. The pairwise comparisons confirmed this tendency: only the ungrammatical sentences seemed to be modulated by attractor mismatch (β = −0.351; SE = 0.114; t = −3.09; p = 0.002), with higher ratings for the ungrammatical mismatch condition compared to the ungrammatical match condition. The ratings in the grammatical mismatch condition were not significantly lower compared to the grammatical match condition (β = 0.041; SE = 0.125; t = 0.33; p = 0.748), indicating attraction only in ungrammatical sentences. Table 10 shows ratings in ungrammatical conditions grouped by Phonological matching.
Table 10. Participants’ mean ratings and standard error in Experiment 4 grouped by Phonological matching.
The model of Phonological matching in ungrammatical sentences revealed a main effect of Attractor (β = −0.334; SE = 0.115; z = −2.89; p = 0.006), confirming the existence of attraction. However, no effect of Phonological matching was detected (β = −0.0002; SE = 0.149; z = −0.0001; p = 0.999) and no interaction with Attractor was seen (β = −0.059; SE = 0.247; z = −0.24; p = 0.811).
Neuter heads
The model revealed a main effect of Grammaticality and a main effect of Attractor. The interaction did not reach significance. The pairwise comparisons confirmed attraction only in ungrammatical sentences (higher ratings in the ungrammatical mismatch compared to the ungrammatical match condition; β = −0.361; SE = 0.137; t = −2.64; p = 0.009). Grammatical mismatch did not show lower ratings than grammatical match (β = −0.134; SE = 0.135; t = −0.99; p = 0.331). Table 10 shows acceptability ratings in ungrammatical conditions grouped by Phonological matching. The model of Phonological matching in ungrammatical sentences revealed a main effect of Attractor (β = −0.360; SE = 0.137; z = −2.63; p = 0.009), confirming the existence of attraction. However, no effect of Phonological matching was detected (β = −0.404; SE = 0.280; z = −1.44; p = 0.163) and neither was any interaction with Attractor (β = −0.159; SE = 0.282; z = −0.57; p = 0.573).
Discussion
Experiment 4 shows gender agreement attraction in untimed judgments in Greek comprehension. Object-clitics revealed attraction in ungrammatical sentences only in both feminine and neuter heads (as in Experiments 2 and 3), confirming retrieval accounts. On the other hand, adjectival predicates were more resistant to attraction and only some small numerical trends were detected. This reflects that agreement target may play a role in end-of-sentence assessments where the sentence-level acceptability is measured. Additionally, the ungrammatical sentences were rated significantly lower than the grammatical ones in both gender values and in both structures.
Discussion Study 2
Overall, Study 2 shows that attraction occurs in end-of-sentence measurements of sentence comprehension both in timed and untimed judgments. Attraction was found in both feminine and neuter heads for object-clitics but only for neuter heads in adjectival predicates. The analyses also revealed that attraction significantly affected ungrammatical sentences, creating illusions of grammaticality in line with retrieval accounts. Attraction also affected grammatical sentences but only numerically and only in timed judgments (see section “General discussion”). Another finding was that the similarity in the morphophonological realization of gender between the attractor and the agreement target did not increase attraction. Finally, adult native speakers of Greek are sensitive to gender agreement violations and they seem to react to violations with both agreement targets and with both gender values. However, we observe a numerical tendency such that ungrammatical neuter agreement targets are overall more accepted (with feminine heads) in object-clitics (match and mismatch conditions) than ungrammatical feminine targets (with neuter heads). Specifically, focusing only on the attractor match conditions in Experiment 3, the participants were 10% less accurate with feminine heads when combining with ungrammatical neuter object clitics (error rate: 16%) compared to neuter heads combining with ungrammatical feminine object-clitics (error rate: 6%). At the same time, the equivalent differences in the error rates in adjectival predicates were much lower (feminine heads with feminine predicates vs. with ungrammatical neuter targets: 5%, neuter heads with neuter targets vs. with ungrammatical feminine targets: 2%). A similar pattern was also observed in the acceptability judgments (Experiment 4). Feminine heads with ungrammatical neuter object-clitics were accepted with a 3.03 rating out of 7, a difference of 3.12 from the grammatical counterpart (feminine heads with feminine object-clitics), while at the same time, neuter heads with ungrammatical feminine object-clitics were accepted with a 2.88 rating out of 7, a difference of 3.0 from the grammatical counterpart (neuter heads with ungrammatical feminine object-clitics). The equivalent differences in the ratings in adjectival predicates were much lower (feminine heads with feminine predicates vs. with ungrammatical neuter targets: 2.99, neuter heads with neuter targets vs. with ungrammatical feminine targets: 2.77).
General Discussion
This is the first gender attraction study in Greek that investigated attraction across two structurally different configurations, i.e., adjectival predicates and object-clitics, using similar methods and materials. Study 1 used real-time comprehension tasks to address whether Greek shows attraction during real-time sentence comprehension (Experiment 1) and whether object-clitics are prone to gender agreement attraction (Experiment 2). Study 2 targeted end-of-sentence comprehension, employing timed (Experiment 3) and untimed judgment (Experiment 4) tasks in order to investigate whether gender agreement attraction occurs under time pressure as well as in the absence of time pressure. This allowed us to address not only how participants process sentences in real-time but also how they judge the acceptability of sentences with attraction manipulations. The four experiments explored the processing strategy the parser aligns with during the comprehension of agreement attraction (the existence of attraction in grammatical vs. ungrammatical sentences, the markedness of the attractor, phonological matching, and the sensitivity to gender agreement rules in the absence of attraction).
Gender Agreement Attraction: Representations and Processes
The results from the current studies add Greek to the cross-linguistic puzzle of gender agreement attraction in comprehension (Spanish: Martin et al., 2012, 2014; Acuña-Fariña et al., 2014; Fuchs et al., 2015; Cunnings et al., 2017;, Russian: Slioussar and Malko, 2016, Arabic: Tucker et al., 2016, French: Villata and Franck, 2019). The current findings support the idea that attraction is a universal phenomenon occurring cross-linguistically irrespective of the morphological richness of its language (Lago et al., 2015). Our present studies mainly support the existence of attraction in all measures of comprehension. This is important because previous work primarily tested the real-time processing of the phenomenon and little has been known for the impact of gender attraction in other measures of sentences comprehension (e.g., speeded judgments). Specifically, our work shows that adjectival predicates show attraction in real-time and in timed judgments but not in untimed judgments, as in Fuchs et al. (2015). On the other hand, clitics consistently show attraction in timed and untimed measures. Thus, these results suggest that object-clitics may be overall more vulnerable to attraction. We believe that there are two main reasons for that: first, our sentences with object-clitics are not compatible (as opposed to adjectival predicates) with predictive processing - participants could not expect earlier that an object-clitic is coming, and thus, they were more vulnerable to attraction. Second, the position of the gender cue in clitics is not so prominent as compared to adjectival predicates (Tables 1, 3): gender is immediately before the verb in which the clitic is phonologically attached to, while adjectival predicates mark gender on the suffix. On the other hand, predictive processing in adjectival predicates may decrease the chances of attraction. In any case, this work adds object-clitics in the agreement targets modulated by gender attraction in comprehension (see Santesteban et al., 2017 for number attraction with clitics).
Focusing now on the attraction patterns observed, the picture seems to be highly informative. Our findings are in line with previous studies on gender agreement attraction in comprehension; first, markedness of the attractor does not seem to play a role (Martin et al., 2012, 2014; Acuña-Fariña et al., 2014; Slioussar and Malko, 2016). Furthermore, the results mainly suggest that attraction occurs only in ungrammatical sentences. This is in line with retrieval accounts (e.g., cue-based retrieval account: Wagers et al., 2009). Regarding adjectival predicates, attractor mismatch overall affected ungrammatical sentences only, as in object-clitics. However, in speeded judgments (Experiment 3) attraction numerically affected grammatical sentences too. This numerical pattern is in line with representational accounts of agreement attraction, but also with predictive processing which has an impact on grammatical sentences too (e.g., Martin et al., 2012, 2014; Villata et al., 2018; for gender). However, we would not like to overinterpret this finding, given that this pattern was observed once and numerically only across all four experiments. Meanwhile, the previous body of work on gender attraction in comprehension has shown a mixed picture. Structures with ellipsis (Martin et al., 2012, 2014) and adjectival predicates (Acuña-Fariña et al., 2014) showed that attraction affected both grammatical and ungrammatical sentences, while only ungrammatical sentences were affected on the verb (Slioussar and Malko, 2016; Tucker et al., 2016). As Wagers et al. (2009) point out, attraction in grammatical sentences possibly stems from a different source and reflects some later processes; its effect is delayed and smaller than in ungrammatical sentences and end-of-sentence measures are particularly sensitive to such late processes of agreement attraction.
With respect to the presence of attraction, one of the reviewers asked why the presence of attraction is reversed in Study 1 vs. Experiment 3. The results are not completely reversed: Object-clitics do show attraction with both gender values in both Experiment 2 and Experiment 3. As for adjectival predicates, they show attraction with feminine heads in Experiment 1, and attraction with neuter heads in Experiment 3. There are two possible reasons for this reversed pattern: first, attraction may have been hidden in Experiment 1 due to the presence of the spurious effect in neuter heads, and second, the overall good performance on ungrammatical sentences in Experiment 3 may have lowered the attraction error rates in feminine heads.
Another implication of the current work is the lack of phonological matching effects of the attractor in attraction configurations, suggesting that attraction has syntactic grounds and is not influenced by the morphophonological similarity between the suffix of an unavailable syntactic constituent (attractor) and the suffix of the agreement target. In line with the argument in Slioussar and Malko (2016), the head noun seems to be more important than the attractor and only certain features of the latter play a role in modulating attraction: crucially, these features mostly affect attraction when they are similar to certain features of the head noun (e.g., syncretism/case-disambiguation) and not the agreement target which may be related with agreement per se, as the study by Marinis et al. (2017) shows for monolingual and bilingual children.
Overall, adult native speakers of Greek showed attraction across the different tasks employed, in real-time, speeded judgments, and untimed acceptability measurements, suggesting that although the effects of attraction might be greater or smaller depending on the task, the structure, and/or the time-pressure, the existence of attraction is not expected to be necessarily dependent on time. This is in line with what was recently found in the production of number attraction (Linzen and Leonard, 2018).
Sensitivity to Gender Agreement Violations
The findings of the two studies suggest that adult native speakers of Greek are sensitive to gender agreement violations in all measures of sentence comprehension. Both RTs (Study 1) and end-of-sentence judgments (Study 2) demonstrate that speakers use their implicit linguistic knowledge of gender agreement to perform these tasks and they respect agreement rules, at least when there is no conflict between the gender cues of the head and the gender cues of the attractor. These results are in line with previous studies on gender agreement in comprehension (e.g., Spanish: Sagarra and Herschensohn, 2011; Acuña-Fariña et al., 2014; Fuchs et al., 2015; Russian: Slioussar and Malko, 2016). Focusing on Greek, Tzovara (2017) found that native speakers of Greek are sensitive to gender agreement rules with adjectival predicates by means of a self-paced reading task combined with end-of-sentence yes/no judgments. Tzovara reports accuracy and RTs from judgments, while she does not report RTs from the critical region (agreement target). The current studies converge with what Tzovara found for gender agreement sensitivity on adjectival predicates in participants’ judgments, while at the same time they also capture this sensitivity throughout the time course of sentence processing.
Another finding was that, although participants were highly accurate in their speeded judgments (when there was no interference from the attractor), they were more accurate in rejecting an ungrammatical sentence than accepting a grammatical one when the agreement target was an adjectival predicate. Notice that participants did not show this effect in object-clitics; instead, the effect in object-clitics was toward the opposite direction. Both structures were presented in each single experiment and the item presentation was pseudo-randomized. One explanation for the fact that participants were overall more accurate in rejecting ungrammatical sentences than accepting grammatical sentences could be that they were alert to violations since they were asked to do so; the task was explicit since the instructions were to judge the acceptability of the sentence (by a yes/no answer) under time constraints. However, the fact that the opposite effect was observed for object-clitics within the same experiment is puzzling, although it is possible that overall, object-clitics impose more burden to the parser and this is perhaps why the opposite effect was found in the latter configuration. A similar pattern has been also found in Häussler (2009; Experiment 1) for number agreement on the verb in German by means of the same method. Furthermore, Tanner et al. (2014) also found a similar numerical pattern (11% mean difference) for number agreement in English. In order to avoid an over-interpretation of the data at this point, we leave this issue open to further investigation.
Overall, as expected, the two studies show that adult native speakers of Greek are sensitive to gender agreement violations in comprehension in real-time and in end-of-sentences measurements of grammatical sensitivity (timed and untimed). However, numerically we observe that ungrammatical neuter agreement targets (with feminine heads) tend to be more accepted in the object-clitic conditions (match and mismatch conditions) than ungrammatical feminine targets (with neuter heads), which was also observed in Bañón and Rothman (2016) and Fuchs et al. (2015) when the agreement target bears the default gender value. A similar pattern has also been observed in production errors during the acquisition of Greek object-clitics (Mastropavlou, 2006, p. 279–281). This tendency is perhaps related to the fact that object-clitics are also a spell-out of the phi-features (Tsimpli and Stavrakaki, 1999) where neuter bears a default value and may not trigger agreement checking or may impose delays in the parser (i.e., more time for agreement checking to be completed). This is also supported by the fact that object-clitics share the same forms with definite articles in Greek and are, thus, ambiguous at the level of form. Overall, it seems that both gender values are activated in Greek across all measures of comprehension.
Conclusion
Study 1 and 2 share evidence for gender agreement attraction in Greek. The pattern found is primarily predicted by retrieval accounts of attraction; in ungrammatical sentences participants exhibited a facilitation in RTs due to attractor mismatch, lower accuracy in rejecting these sentences in speeded binary judgments, and also higher acceptability ratings in untimed acceptability judgments. Additionally, similar morphophonological cues between the agreement target and the attractor do not increase attraction, reflecting that the morphophonological realization of the attractor relative to the agreement target does not matter. Finally, both feminine and neuter seem to be available in the feature content repertoire of Greek adults, since participants are sensitive to these violations. However, the agreement target also seems to play a role in regulating this sensitivity with ungrammatical neuter object-clitics being relatively more acceptable, reflecting the default and underspecified status of neuter in Greek inanimate nouns.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.
Ethics Statement
All participants gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study has been reviewed by the Ethics Committee of the Deutsche Gesellschaft für Sprachwissenschaft (German Linguistics Association) and has been given a favorable ethical opinion for conduct.
Author Contributions
Both authors conceived and designed the study and wrote the manuscript. AP transcribed and coded the data, oversaw the implementation of the study, collected the data, and conducted the statistical analyses of the data.
Funding
This work was funded from AL 554/8-1, DFG Gottfried Wilhelm Leibniz Preis 2014 awarded to Artemis Alexiadou.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors are grateful to the members of the RUESHeL group, and the audiences of ICGL 13, 26th ConSOLE, and AMLaP 2018 for helpful comments and discussion. Part of this work has been published in the Proceedings of ICGL 13 and the doctoral dissertation of the AP. We acknowledge support by the German Research Foundation (DFG) and the Open Access Publication Fund of Humboldt-Universit t zu Berlin for the publication costs.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00717/full#supplementary-material
Footnotes
- ^ Note that under inhibitory interference in the similarity-based interference accounts, the opposite direction is predicted here, as a reviewer points out, i.e., longer RTs for the grammatical match condition compared to the ungrammatical match condition. However, this prediction has not been confirmed with empirical findings or even in recent metanalyses for agreement dependencies as the ones we test here [see Jäger et al. (2017) and Cunnings and Sturt (2018) for discussion]. Rather, the prediction is more relevant to long-distance dependencies combined with processes and cues, such as, agent identification and animacy.
References
Acuña-Fariña, J. C., Meseguer, E., and Carreiras, M. (2014). Gender and number agreement in comprehension in Spanish. Lingua 143, 108–128. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2014.01.013
Agathopoulou, E., Papadopoulou, D., and Zmijanjac, K. (2010). Noun-Adjective agreement in L2 Greek and the effect of input-based instruction. J. Appl. Linguist. 24, 9–33. (Annual Edition of the Greek Applied Linguistics Association)
Ahn, S., and Jiang, N. (2018). Automatic semantic integration during L2 sentential reading. Bilingualism 21, 375–383. doi: 10.1017/S1366728917000256
Alexiadou, A. (2004). “Inflection class, gender and DP internal structure,” in Exploration in Nominal Inflection, eds G. Müller et al. (New York, NY: Mouton de Gruyter), 21–49.
Alexiadou, A., Haegeman, L., and Stavrou, M. (2007). Noun Phrase in the Generative Perspective. New York, NY: Mouton de Gruyter.
Anagnostopoulou, E. (2017). “A schrift to fest kyle johnson,” in Linguistics Open Access Publications. 1, eds L.-C. Nicholas, K. Moulton, and A.-M. Tessier (Amherst, MA: Amherst University), 23–31. Available online at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/linguist_oapubs/1/
Anastasiadi-Simeonidi, A., and Chila-Markopoulou, D. (2003). “Synchronic and diachronic tendencies on gender in Greek: a theoretical proposal (in Greek),” in Gender, eds A. Anastasiadi-Simeonidi et al. (Athens: Ekdosis Pataki), 13–56.
Badecker, W., and Kuminiak, F. (2007). Morphology, agreement and working memory retrieval in sentence production: evidence from gender and case in Slovak. J. Mem. Lang. 56, 65–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.08.004
Bañón, J. A., and Rothman, J. (2016). The role of morphological markedness in the processing of number and gender agreement in Spanish: an event-related potential investigation. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 31, 1273–1298. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1218032
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., and Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. J. Mem. Lang. 68, 255–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
Bock, J. K., and Miller, C. A. (1991). Broken agreement. Cogn. Psychol. 23, 45–93. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(91)90003-7
Bock, K. (2004). “Psycholinguistically speaking: some matters of meaning, marking and morphing,” in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, ed. B. H. Ross (San Diego, CA: Elsevier), 109–144. doi: 10.1016/s0079-7421(03)44004-8
Brehm, L., and Bock, K. (2013). What counts in grammatical number agreement? Cognition 128, 149–169. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.009
Cunnings, I., González Alonso, J., Miller, D., and Rothman, J. (2017). “Gender attraction in Spanish comprehension,” in Proccedings of the 30th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Comprehension, Cambridge, MA.
Cunnings, I., and Sturt, P. (2018). Retrieval interference and semantic interpretation. J. Mem. Lang. 102, 16–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2018.05.001
Dillon, B., Mishler, A., Sloggett, S., and Phillips, C. (2013). Contrasting intrusion profiles for agreement and anaphora: experimental and modeling evidence. J. Mem. Lang. 69, 85–103. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.04.003
Eberhard, K. M. (1997). The marked effect of number on subject–verb agreement. J. Mem. Lang. 36, 147–164. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1996.2484
Eberhard, M., Cutting, C., and Bock, K. (2005). Making syntax of sense: number agreement in sentence production. Psychol. Rev. 112, 531–559. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.3.531
Felser, C., Phillips, C., and Wagers, M. (2017). Editorial: encoding and navigating linguistic representations in memory. Front. Psychol. 8:164. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00164
Ferreira, F., Anes, M., and Horine, M. (1996). Exploring the use of prosody during language comprehension using the auditory moving window technique. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 25, 273–290. doi: 10.1007/BF01708574
Franck, J., Colonna, S., and Rizzi, L. (2015). Task-dependency and structure-dependency in number interference effects in sentence comprehension. Front. Psychol. 6:349. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00349
Franck, J., Vigliocco, G., Antón-Méndez, I., Collina, S., and Frauenfelder, U. H. (2008). The interplay of syntax and form in sentence production: a crosslinguistic study of form effects on agreement. Lang. Cogn. Process. 23, 329–374. doi: 10.1080/01690960701467993
Franck, J., Vigliocco, G., and Nicol, J. (2002). Subject-verb agreement errors in French and English: the role of syntactic hierarchy. Lang. Cogn. Process. 17, 371–404. doi: 10.1080/01690960143000254
Fuchs, Z., Polinsky, M., and Scontras, G. (2015). The differential representation of number and gender in Spanish. Linguist. Rev. 32, 703–737.
Hammerly, C. M., Staub, A., and Dillon, B. (2019). The grammaticality asymmetry in agreement attraction reflects response bias: experimental and modeling evidence. Cognit. Psychol. 110, 70–104. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2019.01.001
Hartsuiker, R. J., Schriefers, H. J., Bock, K., and Kikstra, G. M. (2003). Morphophonological influences on the construction of subject-verb agreement. Mem. Cogn. 31, 1316–1326. doi: 10.3758/BF03195814
Häussler, J. (2009). The Emergence of Attraction Errors During Sentence Comprehension. Doctoral dissertation, University of Konstanz, Konstanz.
Jäger, L., Engelmann, F., and Vasishth, S. (2017). Similarity-based interference in sentence comprehension: literature review and Bayesian meta-analysis. J. Mem. Lang. 94, 316–339. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2017.01.004
Kaltsa, M., Tsimpli, I. M., Marinis, T., and Stavrou, M. (2016). Processing coordinate subject-verb agreement in L1 and L2 Greek. Front. Psychol. 7:648. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00648
Katsika, K. (2009). Sentence Processing Strategies in Adults and Children. PP Attachement in Corpora and Psycholinguistic Experiments. Doctoral dissertation, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki.
Kazana, D. (2011). Agreement in Modern Greek Coordinate Noun Phrases. Doctoral dissertation, University of Essex, Colchester.
Lago, S., Gracanin-Yuksek, M., Safak, D. F., Demir, O., Kırkıcı, B., and Felser, C. (2018). Straight from the horse’s mouth: agreement attraction effects with Turkish possessors. Linguist. Approaches Biling. 9, 398–426. doi: 10.1075/lab.17019.lag
Lago, S., Shalom, D., Sigman, M., Lau, E., and Phillips, C. (2015). Agreement attraction in Spanish comprehension. J. Mem. Lang. 82, 133–149. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2015.02.002
Lewis, R. L., and Vasishth, S. (2005). An activation-based model of sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval. Cogn. Sci. 29, 375–419. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_25
Lewis, S., and Phillips, C. (2015). Aligning grammatical theories and language processing models. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 44, 27–46. doi: 10.1007/s10936-014-9329-z
Linzen, T., and Leonard, B. (2018). “Distinct patterns of syntactic agreement errors in recurrent networks and humans,” in Proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (CogSci 2018), Cognitive Science Society, eds C. Kalish, M. Rau, J. Zhu, and T. T. Rogers (Austin, TX; Curran Associates, Inc), 692–697.
Manasis, V. (2019). Gender Agreement in Structures with Conjoined Subjects: Experimental Evidence from Greek (in Greek). MA thesis, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki.
Mancini, S. (2018). When grammar and parsing agree. Front. Psychol. 9:336. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00336
Marinis, T. (2010). “On-line sentence processing methods in typical and atypical populations,” in “Experimental Methods in Language Acquisition Research, eds S. Unsworth and E. Blom (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), 139–162.
Marinis, T., Chondrogianni, V., Vasic, N., Weerman, F., and Blom, E. (2017). “The impact of transparency and morpho-phonological cues in the acquisition of grammatical gender in sequential bilingual children and children with Specific Language Impairment: a cross-linguistic study,” in Cross-Linguistic Influence in Biligualism: In Honor of Aafke Hulk, eds E. Blom, L. Cornips, and J. Schaeffer (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), 153–180. doi: 10.1075/sibil.52.08mar
Marinis, T., and Saddy, D. (2013). Parsing the passive: comparing children with Specific Language Impairment to sequential bilingual children. Lang. Acquis. 20, 155–179. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2013.766743
Martin, A., Nieuwland, M., and Carreiras, M. (2012). Event-related brain potentials index cue-based retrieval interference during sentence comprehension. Neuroimage 59, 1859–1869. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.057
Martin, A., Nieuwland, M., and Carreiras, M. (2014). Agreement attraction during comprehension of grammatical sentences: ERP evidence from ellipsis. Brain Lang. 135, 42–51. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.05.001
Mastropavlou, M. (2006). The Role of Phonological Salience and Feature Interpretability in the Grammar of Typically Developing and Language Impaired Children. Doctoral dissertation, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki.
Nicol, J. L., Forster, K. I., and Veres, C. (1997). Subject–verb agreement processes in comprehension. J. Mem. Lang. 36, 569–587. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1996.2497
Papadopoulou, D., Tsimpli, I.-M., and Amvrazis, N. (2014). “Self-paced Listening,” in Research Methods in Second Language Psycholinguistics, eds J. Jegerski and B. van Patten (London: Routledge), 50–68.
Parker, D., and An, A. (2018). Not all phrases are equally attractive: new evidence for selective agreement attraction effects in comprehension. Front. Psychol. 9:1566. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01566
Parker, D., Lago, S., and Phillips, C. (2015). Interference in the processing of adjunct control. Front. Psychol. 6:1346. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01346
Paspali, A. (2019a). Gender Agreement in Native and Heritage Greek: an Attraction Study. Doctoral dissertation, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin.
Paspali, A. (2019b). “Processing gender agreement in Heritage Greek: an attraction study,” in Proceedings of the 13th International Conference on Greek Linguistics, eds V. Chondrogianni, S. Courtenage, H. Geoffrey, A. Arvaniti, and M. Tsimpli (London: University of Westminster), 418–427.
Patson, N. D., and Husband, E. M. (2016). Misinterpretations in agreement and agreement attraction. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 69, 950–971. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.992445
Pearlmutter, N. J., Garnsey, S. M., and Bock, K. (1999). Agreement processes in sentence comprehension. J. Mem. Lang. 41, 427–456. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2653
Pfau, R. (2009). Grammar as Processor. A Distributed Morphology Account of Spontaneous Speech errors. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Phillips, C., Wagers, M. W., and Lau, E. F. (2011). Grammatical illusions and selective fallibility in real-time language comprehension. Exp. Interfaces 37, 147–180. doi: 10.1163/9781780523750_006
Psychology Software Tools Inc. [E-Prime 3.0] (2016). Available online at: https://pstnet.com/e-prime-publications/
R Development Core Team (2017). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Ralli, A. (2003). “O kathorismos tou gramatikou genous sta ousiastika tis neas elinikis [Gender determination on nouns in Modern Greek],” in To Genos [Gender], eds A. Anastasiadi-Simeonidi, A. Ralli, and D. Chila-Markopoulou (Athens: Patakis), 57–99.
Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychol. Bull. 114, 510–532. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.510
Sagarra, N., and Herschensohn, J. (2011). Proficiency and animacy effects on L2 gender agreement processes during comprehension. Lang. Learn. 61, 80–116. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00588.x
Santesteban, M., Zawiszewski, K., Erdocia, K., and Laka, I. (2017). On the nature of clitics and their sensitivity to number attraction effects. Front. Psychol. 8:1470. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01470
Schlueter, Z., Williams, A., and Lau, E. (2018). Exploring the abstractness of number retrieval cues in the computation of subject-verb agreement in comprehension. J. Mem. Lang. 99, 74–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2017.10.002
Slioussar, N. (2018). Forms and features: the role of syncretism in number agreement attraction. J. Mem. Lang. 101, 51–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2018.03.006
Slioussar, N., and Malko, A. (2016). Gender agreement attraction in Russian: production and comprehension evidence. Front. Psychol. 7:1651. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01651
Solomon, E. S., and Pearlmutter, N. J. (2004). Semantic integration and syntactic planning in language production. Cogn. Psychol. 49, 1–46. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2003.10.001
Staub, A. (2010). Response time distributional evidence for distinct varieties of number attraction. Cognition 114, 447–454. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.003
Tanner, D., Nicol, J., and Brehm, L. (2014). The time-course of feature interference in agreement comprehension: multiple mechanisms and asymmetrical attraction. J. Mem. Lang. 76, 195–215. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2014.07.003
Tsimpli, I. M., and Hulk, A. (2013). Grammatical gender and the notion of default: insights from language acquisition. Lingua 137, 128–144. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2013.09.001
Tsimpli, I. M., and Stavrakaki, S. (1999). The effects of a morphosyntactic deficit in the determiner system: the case of a Greek SLI child. Lingua 108, 31–85. doi: 10.1016/S0024-3841(98)00041-2
Tucker, M. A., and Almeida, D. (2017). “The complex structure of agreement errors: evidence from distributional analyses of agreement attraction in Arabic,” in Proceedings of the 47th Meeting of the North East Linguistics Society, eds A. Lamont and K. Tetzloff (Amherst, MA: GLSA), 45–54. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00347
Tucker, M. A., Idrissi, A., and Almeida, D. (2016). Attraction Effects for Verbal Gender and Number are Similar But not Identical: Self-Paced Reading Evidence from Modern Standard Arabic. Ms. (submitted), NYU Abu Dhabi and Qatar University, Abu Dhabi.
Tzovara, P. (2017). Gender Assignment and Agreement in Modern Greek as L2: A Psycholinguistic Study. Doctoral dissertation, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki.
Van Dyke, J. A., and McElree, B. (2006). Retrieval interference in sentence comprehension. J. Mem. Lang. 55, 157–166. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.007
Vigliocco, G., Butterworth, B., and Semenza, C. (1995). Constructing subject-verb agreement in speech: the role of semantic and morphological factors. J. Mem. Lang. 34, 186–215. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1995.1009
Vigliocco, G., and Franck, J. (1999). When sex and syntax go hand in hand: gender agreement in language production. J. Mem. Lang. 40, 455–478. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2624
Villata, S., and Franck, J. (2019). Similarity-based interference in agreement comprehension and production: evidence from object agreement. J. Exp. Psychol. 46, 170–188. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000718
Villata, S., Tabor, W., and Franck, J. (2018). Encoding and retrieval interference in sentence comprehension: evidence from agreement. Front. Psychol. 9:2. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00002
Keywords: gender attraction, Greek gender agreement, agreement processing, phonological matching, gender violations
Citation: Paspali A and Marinis T (2020) Gender Agreement Attraction in Greek Comprehension. Front. Psychol. 11:717. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00717
Received: 27 October 2019; Accepted: 24 March 2020;
Published: 29 April 2020.
Edited by:
Matthew Wagers, University of California, Santa Cruz, United StatesReviewed by:
Sandra Villata, University of Connecticut, United StatesSimona Mancini, Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, Spain
Copyright © 2020 Paspali and Marinis. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Anastasia Paspali, cGFzcGFsaWFAaHUtYmVybGluLmRl