- 1Department of Education, Psychology, Philosophy, University of Cagliari, Cagliari, Italy
- 2Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Pontificia de Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain
Aim of the present study is to support the multidimensional and hierarchical nature of the Spanish version of Questionnaire for Eudaimonic Wellbeing (QEWB) and to analyze its psychometric properties through the exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM) framework. Results of the analyses carried out in a sample of university students (N = 589, 161 males and 428 females), supported the hypothesized bifactor-ESEM solution, composed by a global eudaimonic wellbeing factor and three specific factors (Sense of Purpose, Purposeful Personal Expressiveness and Effortful Engagement). Specifically, the global factor is relatively well defined by most of the 21 items; moreover, two of the specific factors (Purposeful Personal Expressiveness, Effortful Engagement) keep their own meaningful specificity apart from that explained by the global factor, suggesting that they add information to the eudaimonic wellbeing construct. Regarding criterion-related validity of the QEWB, the global factor was positively correlated with self-esteem. Finally, the scale showed adequate levels of composite reliability and measurement invariance over gender. Differences in latent means showed that girls report higher positive Purposeful Personal Expressiveness and Effortful Engagement than boys, whereas no significant differences were found in relation to global eudaimonic wellbeing. Theoretical implications about the nature of eudaimonic wellbeing are considered.
Introduction
In subjective wellbeing literature two perspectives have been developed: the edonic perspective, which focuses on the subjective experiences of pleasure (Diener, 1984; Kahneman et al., 1999), and the eudaimonic one, which refers to factors that support fulfillment of human potential and personal growth (Waterman, 1993; Deci and Ryan, 2008).
On one hand, the edonic perspective considers subjective wellbeing as a multidimensional construct characterized by affective factors (e.g., pleasant or unpleasant affective experiences, Bradburn, 1969) and cognitive components including domains such as satisfaction and a global subjective evaluation of life (e.g., Diener et al., 1999). On the other hand, eudaimonia is a more complex concept and it has been operationalized in terms of purpose, autonomy, competence, meaningfulness, social connectedness, self-realization and self-acceptance (e.g., Ryan and Deci, 2001; Baumeister and Vohs, 2002; Huta and Ryan, 2010).
Although both edonia and eudaimonia are positive subjective states, they are distinct constructs (e.g., Waterman, 1993; Delle Fave et al., 2011), and in order to measure wellbeing as a factor associated to contemporary philosophical perspectives of eudaimonia, Waterman et al. (2010) developed the Questionnaire for Eudaimonic Wellbeing (QEWB). The six inter-related components considered by Waterman et al. (2010) to define the items of the questionnaire (“self-discovery, perceived development of one’s best potentials, a sense of purpose and meaning in life, intense involvement in activities, investment of significant effort, and enjoyment of activities as personally expressive,” p. 44) included both the aspects of eudaimonic functioning (e.g., self-realization and the pursuit of excellence) and the personal experiences of eudaimonia (e.g., feelings of personal expressiveness).
Consistently with eudaimonic wellbeing literature (e.g., Ryff, 1989; Ryan and Deci, 2001), Waterman et al. (2010) included various aspects of eudaimonic functioning; however, they conducted analyses to confirm a unidimensional structure using a questionable parceling strategy (Marsh et al., 2013), without specifying the theoretical dimensions to which the items belong (see Waterman et al., 2010 for additional details on the scale). Schutte et al. (2013), in a multicultural South African students’ sample, using exploratory factor analysis at the item level supported instead, a multidimensional 3- factor structure characterized by the factors Sense of Purpose, Purposeful Personal Expressiveness, Effortful Engagement, and a 4-factor structure characterized by the factors Sense of Purpose, Effortful Engagement, Engagement in Rewarding Activities, Living from Beliefs; both 3- and 4-factor structures might reflect presumably Waterman et al. (2010)’ criteria used to develop the instrument. However, Schutte et al. found multiple large cross-loadings that could suggest the presence of a possible single overarching dimension.
To clarify the factor structure of the QEWB, Fadda et al. (2017), in an Italian sample of high school students, contrasted the a priori one-factor solution suggested by Waterman et al. (2010) and tested using a parceling approach, with the 3- and 4-factor solutions found with exploratory factor analyses by Schutte et al. (2013). The authors also proposed the examination of a bifactor solution through the comparison between confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM) frameworks. Results supported a bifactor-ESEM model characterized by a global component of eudaimonic wellbeing (G-factor), but also some specific aspects were found (S-factors: Sense of Purpose, Purposeful Personal Expressiveness, Effortful Engagement).
The ESEM represents an integration of CFA, SEM, and EFA, and it provides a way to use the advantages of exploratory factor analysis for confirmatory and predictive purposes, solving some limitations of CFA models in multidimensional scales that produce biased parameter estimates. Indeed, in the CFA, items are allowed to load only on their pertinent factors, not taking into account multiple cross-loadings present when researchers are interested to assess multifaceted constructs (Morin et al., 2016a). ESEM instead, remains unbiased and produces, in presence of cross-loadings, more exact estimates of factor correlations (Asparouhov et al., 2015). Moreover, bifactor models provide a more flexible alternative than higher-order factor models to adequately reflect the hierarchical nature of multidimensional constructs (Gignac, 2016; Morin et al., 2016a).
Consistent with Schutte et al. (2013) study, Sense of Purpose dimension concerns key aspects of eudaimonic wellbeing including having a purpose in life and self-knowledge (Ryff, 1989; Steger et al., 2006). Purposeful Personal Expressiveness refers to full engagement in activities considered important to the pursuit of personally objectives and it is related to intrinsic motivation (Ryan et al., 2008) and a virtue ethics perspective on eudaimonia (Fowers, 2012). Effortful Engagement reflects the level of effort invested in life’s activities and it could be related to optimal experience and flow constructs (Nakamura and Csikszentmihalyi, 2009).
The factorial structures proposed by Schutte et al. (2013) and Waterman et al. (2010) were compared with an explorative six-factor model in a sample of Spanish adolescents (Salavera and Usán, 2019). Results supported a model with six factors that explained a cumulative variance of 57.84%. The authors also carried out a CFA of the six-factor model in an additional sample.
The Present Study
Aim of this study is to support the multidimensional and hierarchical nature of the QEWB (Waterman et al., 2010) in a Spanish sample through the application of the ESEM framework in line with Fadda et al. (2017). In particular, we carry out an ESEM model composed by three correlated dimensions (Sense of Purpose, Purposeful Personal Expressiveness, Effortful Engagement) and a bifactor-ESEM model, composed by a global eudaimonic wellbeing construct and three specific dimensions of wellbeing (see Figure 1). The ESEM allows to integrate the strengths of exploratory and confirmative factor analysis approach, useful for scale validation; bifactor models produce a more accurate representation of the distinct nature of the various eudaimonic well-being dimensions incorporated in this model.
Figure 1. Simplified conceptual representations of estimated models. ESEM cross-loadings (in the ESEM models all items are allowed to cross-load on all of the specific-factors) are not included in this figure to avoid cluttering, but simply illustrated for Factor 1. F1, sense of purpose; F2, purposeful personal expressiveness; F3, effortful engagement; GF, global eudaimonic wellbeing factor.
Consistent with previous studies (Waterman et al., 2010; Fadda et al., 2017), we expect positive correlations with indicators of positive psychological functioning like self-esteem. Finally, to examine factorial invariance across gender and to examine differences in mean levels of eudaimonic wellbeing, we carried out multiple group analyses.
Materials and Methods
Participants
A sample of university students from the departments of Psychology (N = 497; mean age = 20.5; female = 80.2%), Nursing (N = 343; mean age = 20.3; female = 79%) and Education (N = 123; mean age = 20.9; female = 91%) of the Pontifical University of Salamanca, Spain, was involved in this study (N = 589; 161 males and 428 females; mean age = 19.17; s.d. = 1.81). During academic hours, the questionnaires were administrated in 30-min group sessions on students who gave their express authorization; the study was conducted in compliance with APA ethical standards, oral informed consent was requested, confidentiality were guaranteed, and the students could withdraw at any time without justification. The protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the Pontifical University of Salamanca (annexed IV, act 13/02/2019).
Measures
A Spanish version of the QEWB (Waterman et al., 2010) was developed in accordance with back translation procedures.
(1) The QEWB items were translated into Spanish by two independent experts in the field of psychological evaluation. The Italian version was used as reference for the translation in addition to the English version, since Italy and Spain are more similar in terms of culture, language and history.
(2) The items were back-translated into Italian by other translators with an adequate linguistic knowledge and compared to the original questionnaire.
(3) This preliminary version of the Spanish questionnaire was evaluated using a small pilot sample to determine clarity of the items.
(4) To achieve the final version of questionnaire no relevant change to the items was necessary.
QEWB includes 21 items with 7 negatively worded items. Consistent with the Italian version of the instrument, a 6-point Likert response scale (from 1 = strongly disagree to 6 = completely agree) was used to increase variability in the responses. The Spanish version is available in the Supplementary Material.
Participants also completed the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Inventory (RSEI; Rosenberg, 1965; Spanish version, Atienza et al., 2000), composed by 10 items (4-point response scale, ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 4 = completely agree). Cronbach’s alpha value for RSEI in the present sample was 0.865.
Analysis
Item distribution analysis showed adequate values for kurtosis (range from −0.10 to 2.60) and univariate skewness (range from −0.015 to −1.59; see Supplementary Material). We used, on standardized data, Mplus 7.3 (Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2014) with robust maximum likelihood (MLR), to estimate ESEM and bifactor-ESEM models. In order to deal with the small number of missing data at the item level (0 to 1.5%, M = 0.3%), we used Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML). The ESEM solution was specified with oblique target rotation (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2009); for the bifactor-ESEM solution we used a bifactor orthogonal target rotation (Reise, 2012), allowing for the estimation of a G-factor. Contrary to the Italian version of the instrument, no correlated uniqueness was necessary for the Spanish adaptation of the QEWB.
To examine factorial invariance across gender we carried out multiple group analyses testing hypothesis more restrictive at every step (Byrne, 2004). Specifically, we examined configural invariance, followed by weak and strong measurement invariance, and finally latent means invariance. Configural invariance requires the same number of factors as well as the same pattern of relations between items and factors over genders. Weak measurement invariance requires all factor loadings to be constrained to be invariant across groups. To compare group means, strong measurement invariance requires, in addition to factor loadings, the same item intercepts over gender. Indeed, to test latent means, strong measurement invariance needs to be assured. Finally, to examine criterion related validity, the latent correlations between dimensions of the QEWB and self-esteem were estimated.
We used several fit indices to evaluate results: the chi square (χ2) test of exact fit, the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), the comparative fit index (CFI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and the RMSEA 90% confidence interval. Finally, as recommended by Morin et al. (2016b), we reported model-based omega coefficients of composite reliability (McDonald, 1970).
Results
We tested alternative representations of the QEWB: the 3-factor ESEM and the bifactor-ESEM model including one global factor of eudaimonic wellbeing. Fit indices for the bifactor-ESEM model considerably improved in comparison to the simple ESEM solution. Specifically, lower RMSEA and changes in CFI/TLI ≥ 0.010 (see Table 1).
These results are similar to those found in the Italian study (ESEM: χ2 = 273.576, df = 149, CFI = 0.919, TLI = 0.886, RMSEA = 0.044, RMSEA CI = [0.035/0.052]; bifactor-ESEM: χ2 = 210.064, df = 131, CFI = 0.949, TLI = 0.918, RMSEA = 0.037, RMSEA CI = [0.028/0.046]) and support the need to take into account a global overarching construct of eudaimonic wellbeing, over and above the specific components.
ESEM Solution
As shown in Table 2, the factors were well-defined by the presence of target loadings greater than 0.300, with the sole exception of item 1 (λ = 0.167) for Sense of Purpose, items 4 (λ = 0.277) and 5 (λ = 0.264) for Purposeful Personal Expressiveness and item 7 (λ = 0.278) for Effortful Engagement. It should be noted that these items are the weakest of their respective factors in the Italian study as well (item1: λ = 0.245; item 4: λ = 351; item 5: λ = 0.279; item 7: λ = 0.276).
Multiple cross-loadings were present, but they do not seem to interfere with the interpretation of the factors. Only item1 (I find I get intensely involved in many of the things I do each day; Siento que me implico intensamente en muchas de las cosas que hago cada día) loaded more on Purposeful Personal Expressiveness than on Sense of Purpose.
Intercorrelations showed positive relations between Sense of Purpose and Purposeful Personal Expressiveness (r = 0.355; s.e. = 0.049); Sense of Purpose and Effortful Engagement (r = 0.226; s.e. = 0.044); and Purposeful Personal Expressiveness and Effortful Engagement (r = 0.298; s.e. = 0.052).
Bifactor-ESEM
Fit indices for the bifactor-ESEM solution were adequate (see Table 1); the analysis of standardized factor loading showed a G-factor relatively well defined by the items (| λ| = 0.159 to 0.717, M = 0.349, ω = 0.972) with the exception of items 4, 8, and 13 that showed weak factor loadings (≤0.200) on the G-factor (see Table 3).
Concerning S-factors, the Purposeful Personal Expressiveness (|λ| = 0.201 to 0.527, M = 0.334, ω = 0.944) and the Effortful Engagement (|λ| = 0.164 to 0.616, M = 0.405, ω = 0.925) S-factors retained their own specificity in addition to that accounted for by the G-factor, suggesting that they add information to eudaimonic wellbeing G-factor.
In contrast, the Sense of Purpose dimension, and especially two items concerning the perceived development of one’s best potentials (item 6, 16), appeared to retain a weaker specificity if the variance explained by the G-factor was taken into account (|λ| = 0.076 to 0.688, M = 0.311, ω = 0.842). Moreover, most of the items of this factor had higher factor loadings on the G-factor (|λ| = 0.268 to 0.717, M = 0.490) than the S-factor. This result does not minimize the role of these items; indeed, they mostly serve to reflect students’ global levels of eudaimonic wellbeing, thus supporting the need for a bifactor representation.
Criterion-Related Validity
To the assessment of the criterion related validity, latent CFA representation of self-esteem was added to the bifactor-ESEM solution (see Table 1).
In line with literature, the global eudaimonic wellbeing (G-factor) was positively correlated with self-esteem measure (r = 0.736, s.d. = 0.047). With respect to S-factors, Purposeful Personal Expressiveness, Sense of Purpose and Effortful Engagement, no correlation with self-esteem was found.
Gender Invariance
A lack of decrement in fit indices respect to the recommended cutoff scores (ΔCFI and ΔTLI < 0.01, ΔRMSEA < 0.015), moving from configural to weak and then strong invariance, confirmed the invariance of the QEWS over gender (see Table 1).
Concerning latent means, invariance constraints imposed to the latent means led to a decrement in fit indices, suggesting la presence of not invariant latent means among male and female. The analysis showed that, when males latent means were fixed to zero, females’ latent means were significantly higher on Purposeful Personal Expressiveness (M = 0.316, p < 0.05) and Effortful Engagement (M = 0.663, p < 0.01). No significant gender differences appeared on Sense of Purpose and the G-factor latent means.
Discussion
This study was aimed to support the multidimensional and hierarchical nature of the QEWB in a Spanish sample of university students using a stronger methodological approach. Answers to this Spanish version of QEWB were examined to this purpose using the ESEM framework. Indeed, ESEM integrates the strengths of EFA and CFA approach (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2009); moreover, we also applied bifactor-ESEM, which allows items to load simultaneously on specific (S-factors) components and on a overarching construct (G-factor; Morin et al., 2018).
Results have supported adequate levels of composite reliability and have showed an improved level of fit to the data for the bifactor-ESEM representation of QEWB, in comparison to the ESEM model. In particular, the combination of three specific factors (Sense of Purpose, Purposeful Personal Expressiveness, Effortful Engagement), identified by Schutte et al. (2013) in South African university students, coexisting with a global overarching construct of eudaimonic wellbeing has been confirmed as previously found by Fadda et al. (2017) in an Italian high school students’ sample.
This structure is largely in line with Fadda et al. (2017) study’s results, although some differences appeared, probably due to differences in participants’ age. Specifically, contrary to the Italian version, in the Spanish version, the Purposeful Personal Expressiveness subscale concerning the full engagement in activities subjectively meaningful (e.g., Item 18: It is important to me that I feel fulfilled by the activities that I engage in; Es importante para mí sentirme realizado en las actividades que llevo a cabo) appears well-defined, whereas two items forming the Sense of Purpose did not retain a meaningful specificity when the global levels of eudaimonic wellbeing were considered. This is not unusual for bifactor models because the specific dimensions reflect the residual covariance of items after that their shared covariance has been extracted by the G-factor (Morin et al., 2016a). Therefore, our results suggest that Sense of Purpose and in particular items 6 (I believe I know what my best potentials are and I try to develop them whenever possible; Creo que soy consciente de cuáles son mis mejores cualidades e intento desarrollarlas en la medida de lo posible) and 16 (I am confused about what my talents really are; No tengo claro cuáles son mis verdaderos talentos) concerning the perceived development of one’s best potentials, mostly assess the participants’ global levels of eudaimonic wellbeing. As stated by Waterman et al. (2010) “It is one thing to have identified one’s talents and skills, but it is another to have decided toward what life goals those talents and skills are to be directed. In order to experience EWB, individuals must find ways for putting their skills and talents to use in the pursuit of personally meaningful objectives (p. 45).” Probably, in our sample, composed of university rather than high school students, participants have already found the way to pursuit one’s purpose in living and therefore the Sense of Purpose factor lost its specificity, but not the importance of its items in the assessment of the G-factor of eudaimonic wellbeing.
Bifactor-ESEM also allows to assess the criterion-related validity of both the global and specific dimensions (Howard et al., 2018). In the bifactor models, the global and specific factors are orthogonal, therefore, both can freely predict the outcomes, leading a distinct contribute in terms of prediction (Litalien et al., 2017). In line with previous research, our results supported a stronger correlation between the G-factor and self-esteem, whereas specific factors did not presented relationships with this construct once the effect of global eudaimonic wellbeing was considered. Indeed, self-esteem that can be considered the individual’s general evaluation as the global sense of self-worth, is closely related to wellbeing and psycho-social adaptation in adults and adolescents despite differences in culture or nationality (Diener and Diener, 1995; DeNeve and Cooper, 1998).
Finally, tests of gender differences showed that the structure of the QEWB was similar for male and female students. Differences in latent means showed that girls have higher positive Purposeful Personal Expressiveness and Effortful Engagement than boys, whereas no significant differences were found regarding the global eudaimonic wellbeing. Therefore, girls seem to have higher levels on dimensions related to intrinsic motivation and to active engagement in activities considered meaningful (Purposeful Personal Expressiveness), and to work hard into issues, more o less difficult (Effortful Engagement), than boys. However, to fully test generalizability further research is needed to include other age groups and expand the subsample of males. Moreover, a wider array of positive and negative psychological functioning variables are necessary to test convergent and discriminant validity.
Conclusion
In conclusion, our study supported the idea of a multidimensional representation of the QEWB that reflects probably Waterman et al. (2010)’ criteria to develop the instrument. The authors, in fact, according to eudaimonic wellbeing literature, selected items reflecting six inter-related components of this construct. Moreover, results showed that these items are hierarchically organized around of a global eudaimonic wellbeing component, in line with hierarchical representations of other psychological constructs (e.g., intelligence, self-esteem). This global factor co-exists indeed, with a series of specific dimensions that keep their independence from a more general level of eudaimonic wellbeing.
Therefore, the adoption of this new representation, as captured by the application of the bifactor-ESEM framework, provides a way to reflect a more comprehensive representation of eudaimonic well-being construct.
Author’s Note
1. We also tested: (1) a one-factor model suggested by Waterman et al. (2010) at the item level in order to avoid parcels, which have been heavily criticized in the absence of a reasonable first-order structure at the item level (Marsh et al., 2013). Results showed inadequate fit indices (χ2 = 1039.449, df = 189, CFI = 0.566, TLI = 0.517, RMSEA = 0.087, RMSEA CI = [0.082/0.093]) and factors loadings (range from 0.182 to 0.629, M = 0.398); (2) a six-factor solution proposed by Salavera and Usán (2019) based on Waterman et al. (2010) eudaimonic well-being theory. Both, the CFA and the ESEM solutions showed convergence issues.
2. To test whether our linguistic adaptation preserves the psychometric properties found in the Italian validation study of the QEWB, we examined the measurement invariance of the bifactor-ESEM model (which was the best in the Italian study), comparing our sample with 197 university students from Italy (46 males and 151 females; mean age = 21.28; s.d. = 1.86). Our results supported invariance of the factor loadings (χ2 = 556.026, df = 332, CFI = 0.926, TLI = 0.907, RMSEA = 0.041, RMSEA CI = [0.035/0.047]), item thresholds (χ2 = 557.971, df = 349, CFI = 0.931, TLI = 0.917, RMSEA = 0.039, RMSEA CI = [0.033/0.045]) and latent means (χ2 = 607.038, df = 383, CFI = 0.926, TLI = 0.919, RMSEA = 0.039, RMSEA CI = [0.033/0.044]) across the Italian and Spanish samples.
3. Although ESEM overcomes restrictions associated with CFA (non-target loadings fixed to zero), on request of one of the paper’s reviewers, we tested a confirmatory solution with three dimensions and a second order factor. As expected, CFA showed inadequate fit indices (χ2 = 2167.859, df = 210, CFI = 0.806, TLI = 0.780, RMSEA = 0.059, RMSEA CI = [0.053/0.065]) in presence of complex constructs.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics committee of the Pontifical University of Salamanca. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.
Author Contributions
All authors contributed equally to the theoretical and empirical aspects of the study. MQ-A and MB collected the data. DF analyzed the data and wrote the initial draft of the manuscript. LS supervised the analyses and contributed to the writing of the final version of this manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by the Open Access Publishing Fund of the University of Cagliari, with the funding of the Regione Autonoma della Sardegna – L.R. n. 7/2007.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00422/full#supplementary-material
References
Asparouhov, T., and Muthén, B. (2009). Exploratory structural equation modeling. Struc. Equ. Model. 16, 397–438. doi: 10.1080/10705510903008204
Asparouhov, T., Muthén, B. O., and Morin, A. J. S. (2015). Bayesian structural equation modeling with cross-loadings and residual covariances. J. Manag. 41, 1561–1577. doi: 10.1177/0149206315591075
Atienza, F. L., Moreno, Y., and Balaguer, I. (2000). Análisis de la dimensionalidad de la Escala de Autoestima de Rosenberg en una muestra de adolescentes valencianos. Rev. Psicol. Univ. Tarraconensis 23, 29–42.
Baumeister, R. F., and Vohs, K. D. (2002). “The pursuit of meaningfulness in life,” in Handbook of Positive Psychology, eds S. J. Lopez, and C. R. Snyder, (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 608–618. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195187243.001.0001
Byrne, B. M. (2004). Testing for multigroup invariance using AMOS graphics: a road less traveled. Struct. Equ. Model. 11, 272–300. doi: 10.1207/s15328007sem1102_8
Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2008). Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: an introduction. J. Happiness Stud. 9, 1–11. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9018-1
Delle Fave, A., Brdar, I., Freire, T., Vella-Brodrick, D., and Wissing, M. (2011). The eudaimonic and hedonic components of happiness: qualitative and quantitative findings. Soc. Indic. Res. 100, 185–207. doi: 10.1007/s11205-010-9632-5
DeNeve, K. M., and Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: a meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychol. Bull. 124, 197–229. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.124.2.197
Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychol. Bull. 95, 542–575. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.95.3.542
Diener, E., and Diener, M. (1995). Cross-cultural correlates of life satisfaction and self-esteem. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 653–663. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.4.653
Diener, E., Suh, M., Lucas, E., and Smith, H. (1999). Subjective well-being: three decades of progress. Psychol. Bul. 125, 276–302. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.125.2.276
Fadda, D., Scalas, L. F., Meleddu, M., and Morin, A. J. S. (2017). A bifactor-ESEM representation of the Questionnaire for Eudaimonic Wellbeing. Personal. Individ. Differ. 116, 216–222. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.062
Fowers, B. J. (2012). An Aristotelian framework for the human good. J. Theor. Philos. Psychol. 32, 10–23. doi: 10.1037/a0025820
Gignac, G. E. (2016). The higher-order model imposes a proportionality constraint: that is why the bifactor model tends to fit better. Intelligence 55, 57–68. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2016.01.006
Howard, J. L., Gagné, M., Morin, A. J. S., and Forest, J. (2018). Using bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling to test for a continuum structure of motivation. J. Manag. 44, 2638–2664. doi: 10.1177/0149206316645653
Huta, V., and Ryan, R. M. (2010). Pursuing pleasure or virtue: the differential and overlapping well-being benefits of hedonic and eudaimonic motives. J. Happiness Stud. 11, 735–762. doi: 10.1007/s10902-009-9171-4
Kahneman, D., Diener, E., and Schwarz, N. (1999). Well-Being: The foundations of hedonic Psychology. New York, NY: Russell Sage.
Litalien, D., Morin, A. J. S., Gagné, M., Vallerand, R. J., Losier, G. F., and Ryan, R. M. (2017). Evidence of a continuum structure of academic self-determination: a two-study test using a bifactor-ESEM representation of academic motivation. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 51, 67–82. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.06.010
Marsh, H. W., Lüdtke, O., Nagengast, B., Morin, A. J. S., and Von Davier, M. (2013). Why item parcels are (almost) never appropriate: two wrongs do not make a right camouflaging misspecification with item parcels in CFA models. Psychol. Methods 18, 257–284. doi: 10.1037/a0032773
McDonald, R. P. (1970). Theoretical foundations of principal factor analysis, canonical factor analysis, and alpha factor analysis. Br. J. Math. Stat. Psychol. 23, 1–21. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8317.1970.tb00432.x
Morin, A. J. S., Arens, A. K., and Marsh, H. W. (2016a). A bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling framework for the identification of distinct sources of construct-relevant psychometric multidimensionality. Struc. Equ. Model. 23, 116–139. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.961800
Morin, A. J. S., Arens, A. K., Tran, A., and Caci, H. (2016b). Exploring sources of construct-relevant multidimensionality in psychiatric measurement. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 25, 277–288. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1485
Morin, A. J. S., Myers, N. D., and Lee, S. (2018). “Modern factor analytic techniques: Bifactor models, exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM) and bifactor-ESEM,” in Handbook of Sport Psychology, 4th Edn, eds G. Tenenbaum, and R. C. Eklund, (New York, NY: Wiley), doi: 10.1002/9781118270011
Nakamura, J., and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2009). “Flow theory and research,” in Oxford Library of psychology. Oxford Handbook of Positive Psychology, eds C. R. Snyder, and S. J. Lopez, (New York, NY: Oxford University), 195–206.
Reise, S. P. (2012). The rediscovery of bifactor measurement models. Multivariate Behav. Res. 47, 667–696. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2012.715555
Ryan, M. R., and Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 141–166. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141
Ryan, R. M., Huta, V., and Deci, E. L. (2008). Living well: a self-determination theory perspective on eudaimonia. J. Happiness Stud. 9, 139–170. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-5702-8_7
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1069–1081. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069
Salavera, C., and Usán, P. (2019). Propiedades psicométricas del cuestionario QEWB de bienestar eudaimónico en adolescentes. Psicol. Educ. 25, 139–146. doi: 10.5093/psed2019a3
Schutte, L., Wissing, M., and Khumalo, I. (2013). Further validation of the questionnaire for eudaimonic well-being. Psychol. Well Being Theory Re. Pract. 3, 1–22. doi: 10.1186/2211-1522-3-3
Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S., and Kaler, M. (2006). The meaning in life questionnaire: assessing the presence of and search for meaning in life. J. Counseling Psychol. 53, 80–93. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.53.1.80
Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: contrasts of personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 64, 678–691. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.678
Keywords: wellbeing, eudaimonia, ESEM, bifactor analysis, gender invariance
Citation: Fadda D, Quevedo-Aguado MP, Benavente Cuesta MH and Scalas LF (2020) The Multidimensional and Hierarchical Nature of the Questionnaire for Eudaimonic Wellbeing: A Bifactor-ESEM Representation in a Spanish Sample. Front. Psychol. 11:422. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00422
Received: 10 October 2019; Accepted: 24 February 2020;
Published: 11 March 2020.
Edited by:
Alessandro Giuliani, Higher Institute of Health (ISS), ItalyReviewed by:
Leonardo Becchetti, University of Rome Tor Vergata, ItalyMashhood Ahmed Sheikh, Oslo University Hospital, Norway
Copyright © 2020 Fadda, Quevedo-Aguado, Benavente Cuesta and Scalas. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: L. Francesca Scalas, lfscalas@unica.it