- 1USC Business, Faculty of Arts, Business and Law, University of the Sunshine Coast, Maroochydore, QLD, Australia
- 2Department of Tourism, Sport and Hotel Management, Griffith University, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
- 3Sustainability Research Centre, University of the Sunshine Coast, Sippy Downs, QLD, Australia
In cognitive psychology novelty is an antecedent of attention, emotion, memory, and behavior. However, the relationship between novelty and experience memorability remains conceptually underdeveloped in tourism. This research applies cognitive appraisal theory (CAT) to explore the contribution of novelty and emotion to memorable tourism experiences (MTEs). Seventy-five novel travel episodes were identified through semi-structured interviews. Analysis focused on the antecedent and consequent conditions of novelty. Novel experiences, whether positive or negative, were identified as critical to experience memorability. Novelty could be segmented into trip-related and event-related dimensions. Novelty contributes to how spatial, temporal, and contextual details of tourism experiences are remembered and reconstructed due to the elicitation of intense emotions. Analysis revealed negative experiences deemed as novel were found to be re-evaluated and often remembered as a positive experience. A conceptual model titled “cognitive appraisal of novelty in memorable tourism experiences” is presented for consideration in future research. By applying a retrospective and prospective approach the conceptual model explores the role of novelty through the process of cognitive appraisal, identifying goals, attention, and prior experiences as central for the experience of novelty. Future research should consider the application of recent advance in CAT to advance inquiry on tourism experiences as a psychological phenomenon.
Introduction
Experiences are an area of strong academic and practical interest (Kim, 2010; Tung and Ritchie, 2011a; Volo, 2013; Kirillova et al., 2017). Tourists' seek memorable tourism experiences (MTEs) (Scott et al., 2017), which have the propensity to generate destination loyalty (Chen and Rahman, 2018; Zhang H. et al., 2018), increase satisfaction (Kim, 2018) and promote emotional engagement (Michalkó et al., 2015). However, the creation of memorable experiences requires an understanding of the mental processes which occur at different stages of a tourist experience, including the antecedent and consequent conditions (Knobloch et al., 2017).
Previous studies of MTEs have focused on concepts such as motivation (Gnoth and Matteucci, 2014; Prayag et al., 2017; Yoo et al., 2018; Passafaro, 2019), expectations and satisfaction (Tynan and McKechnie, 2009; Kim, 2018), well-being and quality of life (Uysal et al., 2012), as well as emotions and memories (Chandralal and Valenzuela, 2015; Moyle et al., 2019). Psychological antecedents of MTEs include hedonism, involvement, knowledge, refreshment, meaningfulness, and novelty (Chandralal and Valenzuela, 2013; Kim, 2014). Recent studies indicate the importance of novelty in elicitation of emotions and a connection to memorability (Ma et al., 2017; Mitas and Bastiaansen, 2018). Although the importance of novelty has been established, there is a dearth of research on the relationship between novelty and MTEs.
A plausible reason for this discrepancy may be the dominance of behavioral rather than cognitive psychology in previous studies on tourism experiences (Skavronskaya et al., 2017a,b). Studies have found that emotions are related to increased memorability of a tourism experiences (Bastiaansen et al., 2019; Hosany et al., 2019), but do not provide adequate insights into the theoretical mechanisms by which these concepts are connected (Scott, 2020). Cognitive psychology is the field that examines the mechanisms by which our brain experiences and interprets external stimuli and provides insights into the mental processes connecting perception of stimuli with behavior (Neisser, 2014). Despite its relevance few studies have applied cognitive psychology to research on tourism experiences (Manthiou et al., 2014; Chandralal and Valenzuela, 2015).
This research applies cognitive appraisal theory (CAT) (Arnold, 1960; Frijda et al., 1989; Lazarus, 1991; Roseman, 2013) to explore the intricate connection between novelty and the memorability of tourism experiences in order to add value to the extant behavioral studies on the tourism experience phenomenon. Semi-structured interviews with 25 respondents were used to identify 75 unforgettable travel experiences involving novelty. Analysis of the results provides a conceptual cognitive appraisal model of the role of novelty in MTEs (Figure 1). The model explores the influence of novelty in the formation of MTEs through the process of cognitive appraisal. Connected to this, the intersection between novelty with cognitive processes such as emotions, goals, attention, expectations, prior experiences (memory), and fantasies (prospection) is explored.
Cognitive Psychology and Tourism Research
Tourism is an applied field that adopts theory from parent disciplines, including psychology (Weiler et al., 2012, 2018; Ruhanen et al., 2015). Most psychological theory applied in tourism is from the behavioral or socio-psychological subfields of psychology. The subfield of cognitive psychology has potential to advance conceptual understanding of the phenomenon of tourism, specifically the mental processes which underpin memorable experiences (Skavronskaya et al., 2017a). Cognitive psychology is focused on understanding the mechanisms by which our brain experiences and interprets the world (Scott, 2020).
Traditional cognitive psychology is based on an information processing or input-output model of the brain but focuses on the mental processes that connect perception of stimuli and subsequent behavior (Neisser, 1967). Cognition refers to all processes by which the sensory input is transformed, reduced, elaborated, stored, recovered, and used (Lachman et al., 2015) including attention, learning, consciousness, memory, and emotion (Neisser, 2014). The application of cognitive psychology theory to tourism experiences is an emerging area of research (Larsen, 2007; Agapito et al., 2013; Skavronskaya et al., 2017a; Campos et al., 2018; Scott, 2020).
For instance, the concept of attention may be used to better understand co-creation and mindful tourist experiences (Campos et al., 2016; Chen et al., 2017). The emotion of delight is elicited through high levels of the cognitive appraisal dimensions of goal interest and importance (Ma et al., 2017). The concept of a memory schema can help to study destination image change through manipulation of schema congruity (Zhang R. et al., 2018). Due to the dominance of behavioral paradigm in psychological studies on the tourism experiences, the literature considering experiences as a cognitive process are scarced. Therefore, CAT of emotions elicitation presenting a prominent theory with a capacity to add value to extant tourism experiences research. This research seeks to explore the cognitive process that connects novelty through emotion and memorability drawing on CAT (Arnold, 1960; Lazarus, 1991; Roseman, 2013).
Cognitive Appraisal Theory
Following the cognitive processes conceptualized in CAT emotions are elicited after an individual evaluates their experience on a limited number of appraisal dimensions relevant to their personal goals at the time of the experience (Bagozzi et al., 1999; Roseman, 2001; Johnson and Stewart, 2005). Johnson and Stewart (2005) define an appraisal as the “implications of the situation for the interests and goals of the individual, and thereby determine the form that emotional reaction takes in a given situation” (p. 4). An appraisal process is defined as evaluation of an event in the environment (Oatley and Johnson-Laird, 2014). In cognitive psychology CAT is often applied in the context of negative emotions, such as fear or anxiety (So et al., 2016) but more recently to positive emotions (Ma et al., 2017; Manthiou et al., 2017).
An emotional response is a reaction to the appraisal of an event in terms of the positive or negative fit to a goal or plan based on a limited number of appraisal dimensions (Massara et al., 2010). Key cognitive appraisal dimensions include outcome desirability or goal congruency, agency, certainty, attention, novelty, unexpectedness, and coping potential (Watson and Spence, 2007). CAT emphasizes the importance of a person's interpretation of an event, rather than the event itself, in determining emotional response. CAT proposes a sequential mechanism consisting of perception of a particular situation (experience), followed by an evaluation (appraisal) based on appraisal dimensions, from which a particular emotion is elicited with the potential to influence behavior (action) (Cassidy, 2013). Importantly, the emotional appraisal of a particular situation may or may not stimulate a behavioral outcome (Roseman, 2013).
A number of appraisal dimensions such as novelty, goal congruence, goal realization, and goal relevance have been identified (Ma et al., 2013). Manthiou et al. (2017) used dimensions of goal congruence and certainty to determine emotional valence of experiences on a luxury cruise. Hosany (2012) found the dimensions of goal congruence, pleasantness, and internal self-compatibility influence elicitation of joy, love, and positive surprise. Novelty is an important appraisal dimension in CAT and higher novelty is related to higher emotional arousal (Ma et al., 2013). The present research applies CAT to study how the novelty of an experience influences emotional responses that in turn enhance memory of an event.
Defining Novelty
Novelty is a process of experiencing or encountering something different to the objects regularly encountered (Barto et al., 2013). In our daily routine, the words “new” or “unusual” are the most common synonyms of novelty. Novelty is a complex and subjective psychological concept (Mandler, 1997) with debate about its precise definition (Skavronskaya et al., 2019a).
Novelty has been defined as the extent to which a stimulus is discrepant or familiar for the individual compared to the typical information that a person is possesses (Cohen, 1993; Mak, 2015), or between what is known and what is discovered (Mather, 2013). In neuroscience, novelty is considered as a variable associated with activity in response to stimulation (Cloninger et al., 1994). Novelty is found to increase attention to a focal stimuli, promote and encode memory and modify goal-directed behavior (Bunzeck et al., 2012). Novel stimuli are found in a wide variety of environments and detection of novelty plays a key role in learning through redirection of attention to unknown and potentially important phenomena (Goldberg, 1994). It is also important for cognitive development and performance (Tournier et al., 2012).
In tourism, novelty has been defined as the “extent to which an experience departs from an individual's expectation” (Ma, 2013, p. 54). Novel tourism experiences are associated with unexpectedness, delight, surprise, thrill, and enjoyment (Ortony et al., 1988; Scherer, 1993; Roseman et al., 1996; Mitas and Bastiaansen, 2018). Novelty is considered as a motivational precursor of behavior (Berlyne, 1960, 1970; Dunman and Mattila, 2005), and associated with varied, complex, and intense feelings with a potential to achieve peak experiences (Cloninger et al., 1994).
The related concept of novelty-seeking is used to understand consumer behavior (Farias et al., 2014), tourists' typology (Cohen, 1972), and travel motivations (Dann, 1977; Crompton, 1979; Chon, 1989). Novelty-seeking is central in the understanding of tourists' destination choice (Petrick, 2002) and motivation (Dann, 1977; Crompton, 1979; Chon, 1989; Lee and Crompton, 1992). Novelty-seeking characterized by search for novel, varied, complex, and intense feelings, and a readiness to take physical, social, legal, and financial responsibility to achieve novel experiences (Cloninger et al., 1994).
Novelty is considered to be intricately connected to the experience of pleasure through experiences of flow, mindfulness, and creativity (Csikszentmihalyi, 1997; Filep et al., 2016). Novelty is related to emotional arousal through the alleviation of boredom and hedonic and eudemonic well-being (Iso-Ahola and Weissinger, 1990; Filep and Laing, 2019; Vada et al., 2019).
Novelty Experience, Emotion, and Memory
Novelty is fundamental to tourism and travel (Mitas and Bastiaansen, 2018), and critical for MTEs (Kim J. H et al., 2012). Novelty in a pleasurable tourism experience is associated with suddenness or unexpectedness and with emotions such delight, surprise, and enjoyment (Ortony et al., 1988; Scherer, 1993; Roseman et al., 1996). Emotions are considered as critical for the delivery of MTEs (Bastiaansen et al., 2019; Skavronskaya et al., 2019b). Strong emotions resulting from a novel experience result in vivid memories, created through the secretion of chemicals including dopamine (Moyle et al., 2019). Recent studies on psychological antecedents of MTEs acknowledge novelty as one of the main dimensions underlying experience memorability (Chandralal and Valenzuela, 2013; Kim, 2014).
While the relationship between novel experiences, emotion, and memory has been previously discussed, the cognitive processes by which these concepts are related require further exploration (Tung and Ritchie, 2011a; Scott, 2020). There is need to explore the process by which novelty, memory and emotions are connected in the context of MTEs from cognitive psychology perspective due to dominance of behavioral paradigm in extant tourism experiences research. Furthermore, recent tourism literature has a tendency to focus predominantly on the antecedents of positive memorable experiences due to the hedonic nature of tourism (Tung and Ritchie, 2011a). However, the neuroscience literature indicates that novelty is independent from emotional valence (Förster et al., 2010), suggesting novelty is connected with both positive and negative emotions in MTEs.
Overall, novelty is a complex and subjective phenomenon fundamental for tourism (Mitas and Bastiaansen, 2018) with lack of comprehensive, structured theory (Witt, 2009a,b). How novelty connects with emotions and memories is not well-explored in tourism (Skavronskaya et al., 2019b). Thus, the present research draws on cognitive psychology to explore the connection between novelty and experience memorability in tourism.
Method
Analysis of existing studies revealed quantitative methods, often using experimental approaches, dominated conceptually related studies in psychology and tourism (Li et al., 2015; Veal, 2017; Weiler et al., 2018). Furthermore, extant quantitative studies on MTEs predominantly focus on correlations between external tourism stimuli with consequent conditions, such as emotions or behavioral intentions, assuming that external stimuli have direct effect on outcome. The prevalence of quantitative studies in the tourism field is further demonstrated by studies on the antecedents and consequences of MTEs (Kim, 2014; Chandralal and Valenzuela, 2015; Coudounaris and Sthapit, 2017; Chen and Rahman, 2018). Results confirm the importance of novelty for eliciting emotions in tourism (Mitas and Bastiaansen, 2018). Therefore, quantitative studies on MTEs acknowledge various components in the process of cognitive appraisal as mediating factors connecting tourism stimuli with behavior (Ma et al., 2013, 2017; Campos et al., 2016; Kim, 2018).
Although drawing on cognitive psychology the results of these studies are also often interpreted in behavioral terms (Mari and Poggesi, 2013; Manthiou et al., 2017). Consequently, while informed by theory grounded in cognitive psychology, analysis is yet to explore, in-depth, how the process of cognitive appraisal contributes to MTEs in tourism. Exploring novelty through the process of cognitive appraisal has the potential to provide in-depth insights to further inform quantitative studies by enriching conceptual understanding of the mental processes which underpin experience memorability. Subsequently, this research utilized qualitative approach to study novelty in MTEs designed to advance conceptual understanding of the tourism experience as a psychological phenomenon, as suggested by previous studies (Tung and Ritchie, 2011a; Coelho et al., 2018). Therefore, to address the outlined gap, this research is applying qualitative approach with CAT as a conceptual framework to study MTEs and merging tourism with an area of cognitive psychology where quantitative methods dominate existing scholarly inquiry. Studying MTEs with CAT as a theoretical framework provide additional in-depth perspective on an organism mental state prior—during and after traveling, that quantitative studies are not able to cover. Thus, qualitative studies on the antecedent conditions of mental processes occurring prior and during the tourism experiences will help to better understand tourist behavior.
In-depth semi structured interviews were undertaken with 25 participants that were asked to recall three episodes of unforgettable and unexpected travel experiences. As a result, 75 episodes of memorable and novel travel experiences were collected and identified. Interviews were conducted in Russian in Saint Petersburg, Krasnodar, and Moscow between April and May 2018. Each interview took place in a public setting, usually, a café, with the duration of each interview between 30 and 60 min. Respondents were selected using convenience sampling (Ali et al., 2014). This sampling method is suitable as the core goal of the research is not generalizability, rather an in-depth exploration of novelty and memorability.
Interview questions designed utilizing core tenants from CAT depicted in previous studies focused on tourism experiences (Hosany, 2012; Ma et al., 2013; Manthiou et al., 2017). Probing questions explicitly designed to unearth the mental process that intricately connects novelty with experience memorability through emotions (See Table 1). Pilot interviews with two participants was applied to ensure the questions were appropriately structured and framed, resulting in minor modifications to the instrument prior to final launch. Particular attention was given to how novel experiences resulted in emotional responses and memorability. Episodes of novelty episodes were identified utilizing the criteria of a “first time” occurrence of a particular event or experience during traveling and a mismatch between expectations and actual experience (Meyer et al., 1991).
Interviews were conducted in Russian, the participants' native language, to develop rapport with respondents who encouraged to speak openly and in a relaxed and engaged manner (Welch and Piekkari, 2006). The interview processes allowed the researcher to understand the in-depth meanings expressed by participants, as well as their cultural and social background (Chen et al., 2017). Interviews were recorded using a digital voice recorder, specifically a “Dictaphone” app, after the permission from each participant had been granted. Pseudonyms are used to preserve the anonymity of respondents. Table 2 shows the characteristics of respondents. Prior to analysis, all interviews were transcribed. Following Hall (2004), to improve the credibility and trustworthiness of the findings the transcripts from the interviews were double-blind translated to English and compared.
CAT provided a theoretical structure to the enquiry and a grounded theory approach was used for analysis, specifically the coding and interpretation of the data (Corbin and Strauss, 1990; Matteucci and Gnoth, 2017). Following Sharma and Sarmah (2019), this study applied NVivo 12 to increase the efficiency of data analysis. Interviews were coded for themes, using open, axial, and selective coding as previously applied in Kennelly et al. (2013); Hillman et al. (2018), and Moyle et al. (2018).
Open coding enabled to identify and record the common emergent themes, such as the episodes of novelty, surprise, and unexpectedness in positively and negatively remembered tourism experiences. Axial coding ensured that common emergent themes has been grouped into categories such as antecedent and consequent conditions of novelty in the context of tourism experiences. An interceder reliability check was performed on the emergent themes from two coders independent to the research, resulting in 82 per cent agreement between three coders, which is above the level suggested by Miles and Huberman (1994).
Results
Episodes of Novelty in the Tourism Experiences
The findings from analysis of the novelty episodes revealed that there are two broad contexts where people experience novelty while traveling: a whole trip (trip-related) or an explicit event during travel (event-related). One main difference of these two contexts is the amount of detail remembered. Trip related novelty was often associated with first time travel experiences, such as “I'm going to talk about my trip to Cuba first. It was a first trip to that region. It was a last-minute decision….” [RES_17]. In a contrast, event-related novelty focused on a situation within a trip “In Salerno we met a wandering artist with a dog. He was two meters tall, very skinny and spoke all European languages, including Russian.” [RES_8].
Episodes involving novelty were identified as “memorable” [RES_24], “unforgettable” [RES_15] and “happening for the first time” [RES_14]. According to participants responses, novelty of the tourism experiences is something that they had “never experienced before” [RES_10], which happened “unexpectedly” [RES_25] and was “far from ordinary” [RES_3]. Novelty for participants was described as a sense of being in an “unfamiliar” [RES_18] or a “different” [RES_3] environment. Novelty was associated with either an “unforgettable trip from beginning to end” [RES_8] or a particularly “significant moment” within a trip [RES_2]. For instance, a participant mentioned that “this trip was the first time when I was traveling for so long,” highlighting the unusual duration of the trip that she “never previously experienced” [RES_14]. Related to a novelty associated with a whole trip, [RES_22] noted that “visiting an Asian country for the first time in my life” made his travel experience “memorable,” largely due to culture shock. In contrast to a novelty of a whole trip, [RES_12] highlights a particular event within a trip to Andorra when “it was first time in my life I saw the mountains of the enormous scale.” Whereas [RES_16] pinpointed an exact moment that made the trip novel “remembering every second of that moment when I saw the Eiffel tower, which I was dreaming about.”
Factors Influencing Novelty in Tourism
Dreams and desires have been identified as a significant theme in reported episodes of tourism novelty. A trip to Egypt to see the Pyramids and the Sphinx for [RES_24] “was a childhood dream” and once achieved, he found this novel experience “significant” and “more memorable” than other trips. Elaborating on how he felt after a first-time trip to Byron Bay, Australia to learn how to surf, [RES_22] reported that “my dream came true” and that his “next trip would be somewhere where I can surf .” A “childhood dream” for [RES_19] was go on a cruise and he reported that the “fact that I'm going to a cruise made me feel so excited!” Another respondent reported overcoming an obstacle to achieve her dream to see the Great Wall of China. “I always had a desire to visit the Great Wall of China, but I was scared of the crowds. The pictures of thousands of tourists climbing the Wall was stopping me for achieving my dream” [RES_2]. Once achieved, she said the trip “has overcome my expectations” and now she thinks it was one of the “peak experiences' in her life, and unexpectantly there was a lack of people which meant we had the place all to ourselves!” [RES_4] mentioned that her trip to Lake Baikal in Russia is the one she “remember the most” because her future husband “really wanted it to happen' and “was dreaming about it.”
Goals and intentions to visit a particular novel destination were another significant theme. To illustrate this, [RES_5] mentioned that a “trip to Bali was in a list of the life goals I wants to achieve before I die.” Life/ travel goals and intentions category appears as something that this participant “really want it to happen” [RES_25]. A participant identified her first visit to Xian as “unforgettable,” and that she was “prepared to go” because “it was my goal … and I made a list of the Xian sights that I really want to see and just follow the list” [RES_14]. One of the participants mentioned they “really wanted to visit Japan,” but also that they wanted to go “everywhere where I haven't been yet” [RES_1].
Recollections of novelty of an event were described by respondents with richer contextual details compared to a novelty of the whole trip. For instance, while reconstructing a trip to China [RES_2] mentioned they still remembered an exact phrase her friend told her on the Great Wall of China—“if you shared a special moment like this with someone, you are bonded together for the rest of your life.” While reconstructing “a moment remembered the most” [RES_5] mentioned that “the famous Norway's Trollfjord is 1000 meters high and you have to walk for 11 km to reach the peak.” A participant describing event-related novelty reported being able to “remember … every second of that moment” [RES_16], which they will “never forget” [RES_12].
Outcomes of Novelty in the Tourism Experiences
Novel experiences described by respondents had a strong emotional component, both pleasurable and unpleasurable. First in life experiences were found to possess a highly sentimental tone. For instance, [RES_8] described her first hitchhiking trip with “the sky became a roof over your head, and the road becomes a thread to home.” [RES_11] stated that her “first trip to America was like the first love, that cannot be overshadowed by anything.” [RES_12] the moment when she saw mountains for the first time in life: “still have a feeling that a part of me is still there – in Andorra's mountains.” Another participant [RES_22] mentioned that the experience of surfing on his first trip to Byron Bay in Australia was “like a good coffee – it takes time and then you are finding yourself addicted to it.”
Participants described novel experiences as having embedded positive emotions and feeling a “sense of absolute freedom” [RES_8], an ability to “break free” [RES_4] and a sense of being “lost in time” [RES_2]. One of the participants mentioned that seeing Mongolian yurts for the first time was: “so exciting!” [RES_14]. Some unforgettable experiences shared by respondents were very positive. For instance, [RES_17] said she “was happy almost every second of that trip.” Along with “being happy,” [RES_6] mentioned that he experienced happiness when “share[ing] these emotions and experiences” with other people.
Surprise was also identified as accompanying novelty in the tourism experiences. For instance [RES_10] reported: “It was my first overseas trip…I remember, I asked a local person in a shop: how much (in English)? and he answered me in Chinese. I was so surprised! It seems that everyone around the world nowadays speaks in Mandarin.” [RES_1] said his first yacht trip “delightful and happy atmosphere.” [RES_15] described a surprising event during the unforgettable trip as a very “touching moment.”
Episodes of tourism novelty were associated with nostalgic feelings. Participants “wanted to come back” [RES_14] to places they had visited for “the first time” and where they “felt so good” [RES_20]. [RES_14] said: “I want to go back to certain places in Italy and to see certain people.… I want to go back to the same hotel, I want to go back to the same sea shore, to the same cafe. I feel so nostalgic.”
Respondents recalled both positive and negative unexpected events during travels. Negative novel experiences were described as “unlucky,” “unsuccessful” [RES_16], and as “far from what was expecting” [RES_17]. While describing her first banana boat experience [RES_10] mentioned being “scared” and “horrified” as she can't swim, and nobody explained that she has to do it. Interestingly, one of the respondents, re-evaluated a negative first-time travel experience with her boyfriend as positive, even though their baggage was stolen, and the experience was “difficult” and not “impressive at all.” This respondent mentioned that “challenges…were really critical for our future relationships…Difficulties make couple stay together or separate. We went through this together as a team… Tenerife by itself didn't impressed me at all” [RES_17].
Experience sharing emerged as another significant theme. According to participants, sharing unforgettable travel experiences with other people makes an experience “have sense” [RES_6], “meaningful” [RES_23], and “memorable” [RES_9]. Sharing an emotional event in a trip to new destination was extremely important for one of the participants who mentioned that happiness from visiting Malaysia came from an ability to “share these emotions and experiences with others” [RES_11].
Another theme was labeled vivid and detailed memories. Thus [RES_2] described details of a visit to the Great Wall of China, including words she still remembered, for instance “he called me ‘sweetie', no one called me like this before.” A respondent recalled her peak experience during a trip 10 years before: “I will never forget Italy. In Salerno we met a wandering artist with a dog. He was two meters tall, very skinny and spoke all European languages, including Russian. He was from Slovenia and his name was Ivan, and the dog's name was Doris…” [RES_8].
Discussion
This research explore the intersection between novelty and the memorability of tourism experiences. Data included recalled and reconstructed tourism experiences, with analysis designed to focus explicitly on the episodes of novelty during travels, factors influencing novelty, as well as the outcomes of novelty in the tourism experiences. This research to identified the antecedents and consequent conditions of novelty in the tourism experiences. Additionally, in-depth investigation on the role of novelty in the process of cognitive appraisal was considered. The following section connects the findings in this research with tourism and cognitive psychology. Findings of this research bridge a perceived gaps in existing literature on MTEs, Antecedent and consequent conditions of novelty in tourism are integrated into a conceptual framework of cognitive appraisal of novelty in MTEs (Figure 1).
Reconstruction of the Episodes of Novelty in the Tourism Experiences
Results indicate that novelty influences how spatial, temporal, contextual, and emotional details of tourism experiences are remembered and reconstructed. Novelty influences the reconstruction and re-evaluation of experiences, which is defined as memorability. A connection between experiencing novelty and the ability to recall novelty episodes with more accuracy, particularly for specific events was identified. Episodes of novelty were identified to be focused on recollections of an entire trip (trip-related novelty) or narrowed in on a single event (event-related novelty) within a trip. The main criteria of difference between these two contexts is the amount of detail recalled and reconstructed. Trip-related novelty was found to be associated with comparably low levels of emotional and contextual details during recollection and reconstruction. The reconstruction of trip-related novelty in tourism included broader spatial and temporal details of the trip, such as novel geographical location, new duration of the trip or unusual distance to the destination, while event-related novelty focused on a specific situation within a trip. Event-related novel experiences within a trip were recollected by respondents with richer contextual and emotional details compared to a novelty of the whole trip.
Recall of detailed trip and event related experiences was noted and explained through increased attention identified by respondents when recalling MTEs. This is consistent with studies in the cognitive psychology and neuroscience literature on autobiographical memory (Radvansky and Zacks, 2011), memory retrieval (Tulving et al., 1996), flashbulb memories (Winograd and Neisser, 1992), as well as the studies on stimulus novelty and orienting response (Kishiyama and Yonelinas, 2003). This research confirmed that novelty facilitates recall of the tourism experience with more accuracy through attention since those unusual and new activities to which participants paid attention are more likely to be remembered. Thus, extant studies on autobiographical and flashbulb memories explained that novelty linked to intense emotions and are more likely to be remembered.
Antecedents of Novelty in the Tourism Experiences
Thinking about the past (retrospection) and future (prospection) will be used as the lens to identify and critically analyze the antecedents and consequences of novelty in tourism experiences. This research found that retrospective antecedents are involved the process of recollection, while prospective antecedents on novelty in the tourism experiences associated with anticipation of future events as reflected on the model of cognitive appraisal of novelty in MTEs (see Figure 1).
Retrospective thinking about past travel influences how novelty in tourism is discussed. Retrospection involves detailed reconstruction of an event from memory with an accompanying sense of self (Shiffman et al., 1997; Miller et al., 2010; Sadeh et al., 2014). This research indicates the process of recollection of memories of prior travel experiences is associated with reconstructed emotion as well as memories consistent with CAT.
Utilizing retrospective thinking to analyze antecedent conditions of novelty in tourism, this research identified that prior experiences can be interpreted as a broader retrospective antecedent of novelty in tourism, which could be further explained through the concepts of mental schema, emotions, and memories about past trips, including nostalgia. Prior experience identified as an important condition of novelty as it forms traces of similar experiences in the past which allows participants to experience novelty in the present and allows potential recollection and reconstruction in the future.
This research has also identified that apart from recollection, experience of novelty in tourism also involves the process of anticipation of the future events which links novelty to the concept of prospection. Prospection refers to a prototypical biased vision of the future toward pleasurable experiences, and grounded in schemas, stereotypes, personal goals, and other mental representations of what person is typically like (Kane et al., 2012). Concomitantly, prospection is similar to affective forecasting or imagining what our future travels will be like under various scenarios (Skavronskaya et al., 2017a). Overall, prospective thinking about the antecedents of MTEs connects novelty with the concepts such as goals, expectations, dreams, and fantasies as reflected on the proposed Figure 1.
The concepts of anticipation and prospection enhances the conceptual understanding of the integral role of fantasies and goals in the cognitive appraisal of novelty in tourism. In this research respondents recalled travel episodes from the past, including the recollection of the feelings prior the trip which involves the excitement. In turn, anticipation of future travel was intertwined with concepts such as interest and goals for the trip which in turn stimulated obsessive thinking about future travel, which is fantasy (Le et al., 2019). Following this cognitive process, during an actual trip, a person had stronger emotional response due to achievement of the trip-related goals.
Overall, antecedents of novelty in tourism included retrospective and prospective components. The prospective component involves anticipation of future events, expectations, and fantasies about future travels. Stronger goals mean that greater expectations and stronger emotion and memorability when novelty related to a goal is experienced. Recollection of novelty requires recreation of memories and of past experiences. These memories of past events are stronger and hence easier to recall when an event involves novelty.
Consequences of Novelty in the Tourism Experiences
The consequences of novelty in tourism can be divided into immediate and long-term. To acknowledge the difference between long-term and immediate consequences of novelty in tourism, relevant section has been included on the conceptual framework of cognitive appraisal of novelty in MTEs (see Figure 1).
Immediate consequences of novelty in the tourism experiences include intense emotions and feelings, and relay memories related to the novel experiences with greater potential to be transferred into long-term memory. Neuropsychological studies suggest that information about novel experiences is encoded and transferred into long-term memory (Tulving et al., 1994; Tulving and Kroll, 1995). As has been previously mentioned, novelty is also connected to enhanced attention. Greater attention to novel experiences increases the amount information that is encoded and stored in memory (Dijksterhuis and Aarts, 2010; Van Kesteren et al., 2012). Short-term memory is able to store information for only a short period of time and with limited capacity, whereas long-term memory has greater capacity and duration (Atkinson and Shiffrin, 1968). Novelty is considered a necessary condition for encoding information in long-term memory (Tulving and Kroll, 1995) and enhances the capacity for interactions between the different memory systems (Henson and Gagnepain, 2010).
Long-term consequences of novelty include enhanced memorability and ease of recall (reminiscence) (Webster, 1994; Tung and Ritchie, 2011b). Reminiscence or recall and reprocessing of past events novelty leads to re-evaluation and the change of meaning associated with the event. An example may be the re-evaluation of an unexpected and unpleasant travel experiences (e.g., stolen luggage) as positive if person's related goals were achieved. Thus, re-evaluation involved reassessment based on the consistency of the particular travel experiences with the persons' goals. This finding is consistent with the previous tourism literature on role of emotions and goals in cognitive appraisal process in tourism (Hosany, 2012; Ma et al., 2017; Campos et al., 2018). Goals can also guide behavior by direction of attention to salient objects (Dijksterhuis and Aarts, 2010). Novelty of an object salient to a goal captures attention.
This research found that intense emptions is a consequent condition of novelty in recalled events. Previous studies recognize that emotions related to the memorability of the tourism experiences (Bastiaansen et al., 2019). Findings of the present research indicate that novelty may lead to positive as well as negative emotions in the tourism experiences (Kim J. H et al., 2012; Lin et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2017). Thus, novelty is independent of emotional valence (Förster et al., 2010). Emotional valence is instead determined by appraisals of goal congruence and novelty associated with goal congruent experience will result in pleasurable emotions (Mitas and Bastiaansen, 2018).
Novel pleasurable events were often associated freedom (Moore et al., 1995) surprise, happiness, excitement, and delight (Ma et al., 2017). Interestingly, it has been found that positive vivid memories of novel travel experiences are related to nostalgia and intention to revisit a destination (Hwang and Hyun, 2013; Bergs et al., 2019). Nostalgia is included in the retrospective part of the conceptual framework (Figure 1) and the relationship about the effect of novelty on nostalgia represents a prominent area for future scholarly inquiry. Participants also shared negatively remembered tourism experiences involving novelty, as a result of unexpected illness, injury and other losses, or heightened periods of emotional stress. As a result of unpleasant travel experiences, participants feel disappointment, unexpectedness, fear, horror (Petzer et al., 2012; Weaver et al., 2018).
Conclusion, Implications, and Future Research
Novelty is a cognitive appraisal dimension with the capacity to enhance attention and emotions and hence create lasting vivid memories. Novelty influences the intensity of emotions, stronger memory and therefore affects the ease of the experience reconstruction (memorability) and reminiscence. This research developed a conceptual model that explores the role of novelty as a cognitive appraisal dimension in the formation of MTEs through the elicitation of emotions, but also associations with cognitive processes such as goals, attention, and expectations through retrospective and prospective approach to imagined and recalled tourism experiences (see Figure 1).
This research contributes to the tourism field by exploring how theory from cognitive psychology can be utilized to provide a more nuanced understanding of the tourism experience and vice-versa (Tung and Ritchie, 2011a; Kim and Brown, 2012; Kim J. H et al., 2012; Kim K. et al., 2012; Pearce and Packer, 2013; Torland et al., 2015; Scott et al., 2017; Moyle et al., 2019; Huang et al., 2020). Specifically, this research adds value to the studies on mental processes associated with tourism experiences (Ma et al., 2013, 2017; Campos et al., 2016, 2018; Skavronskaya et al., 2019a) and contributes to a better understanding of the role of novelty both an antecedent to, and consequence of, MTEs (Chandralal and Valenzuela, 2015; Mitas and Bastiaansen, 2018; Skavronskaya et al., 2019a).
While focused on tourism, this research is also designed to stimulate debate in cognitive psychology surrounding the efficacy of tourism for advancing knowledge within the subdiscipline. Traditional cognitive psychology uses experimental approaches, often in a laboratory setting and these methods and techniques are available to study internal processes associated with tourism and travels (Li et al., 2015; Hadinejad et al., 2019; Scott, 2020). However, tourism provides an opportunity outside of laboratory settings where cognitive theories can be applied. Understanding of the mental processes that underpins tourism experiences help better understanding of tourists' behavior.
The value of novelty for tourism industry is demonstrated through enhanced tourism experience design. For instance, by implementing novel, unexpected and surprising components for designing tourism experiences and marketing campaigns, tourism practitioners can increase re-visitation, form loyalty and generate tourist's satisfaction (Chen and Rahman, 2018; Kim, 2018). Understanding novelty in tourism can increase emotional engagement with tourists at the destination (Michalkó et al., 2015), presenting an opportunity for entrepreneurs to operationalize novelty to engage tourists at a deep and profound level. Additionally, due to the rapid expansion of cutting-edge technologies there is an avenue to further explore, interpret, and implement novelty to MTEs through technologies, such as virtual and augmented reality (Bec et al., 2019).
Further conceptualization of novelty in tourism is required as a number of theoretical gaps has been identified. For instance, in the extant tourism literature on emotional and MTEs (Petrick, 2002; Tung and Ritchie, 2011a; Ma et al., 2017) the words novelty, unexpectedness and surprise are often used interchangeably, and the concepts are not distinguished from one another. However, the concepts have been expensively explained in cognitive and neuroscience (Plutchik, 1980; Meyer et al., 1991; Barto et al., 2013). Therefore, there is a need to distinguish between novel, surprising, and unexpected tourism experiences. Furthermore, extant tourism studies are largely focused on novelty seeking behavior (Kim and Kim, 2015), rather than novelty as appraisal dimension. Thus, the differences between novelty and novelty-seeking in tourism experiences should be examined further. Due to the hedonic nature of the tourism experiences, most studies on the role of novelty in positively remembered tourism experiences (Ma et al., 2013, 2017; Mitas and Bastiaansen, 2018). The role of novelty as an appraisal dimension in tourism experiences perceived as negative is not addressed in the tourism literature.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Griffith University—2017/751. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.
Author Contributions
The outcomes of this research are based on LS PhD dissertation titled (Novelty in Memorable Tourism Experiences). LS played a key role in conceptualization, data collection, analysis, and drafting of the manuscript. BM and NS played a critical role in the conceptualization of the research, with further guidance provided by BM focused on the method and analysis. All parties contributed to the writing and reviewed the final draft several times prior to submission.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Agapito, D., Mendes, J., and Valle, P. (2013). Exploring the conceptualization of the sensory dimension of tourist experiences. J. Destin. Market. Manag. 2, 62–73. doi: 10.1016/j.jdmm.2013.03.001
Ali, F., Hussain, K., and Ragavan, N. A. (2014). Memorable customer experience: examining the effects of customers experience on memories and loyalty in Malaysian resort hotels. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 144, 273–279. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.07.296
Atkinson, R. C., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). “Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes,” in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation:Advances in Research and Theory, Vol. 2, eds K. W. Spence and J. T. Spence (London: Academic Press), 103–106.
Bagozzi, R. P., Gopinath, M., and Nyer, P. U. (1999). The role of emotions in marketing. J. Acad. Market. Sci. 27, 184–206. doi: 10.1177/0092070399272005
Barto, A., Mirolli, M., and Baldassarre, G. (2013). Novelty or surprise? Front. Psychol. 4:907. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00907
Bastiaansen, M., Lub, X. D., Mitas, O., Jung, T. H., Ascenção, M. P., Han, D. I., et al. (2019). Emotions as core building blocks of an experience. Int. J. Contemp. Hosp. Manag. 31, 651–668. doi: 10.1108/IJCHM-11-2017-0761
Bec, A., Moyle, B., Schaffer, V., Timms, K., Skavronskaya, L., and Little, C. (2019). Management of immersive heritage tourism experiences: a conceptual model. Tour. Manag. 72, 117–120. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2018.10.033
Bergs, Y., Mitas, O., Smit, B., and Nawijn, J. (2019). Anticipatory nostalgia in experience design. Curr. Issues Tour. 1–13. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2019.1669539
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). “Novelty, uncertainty, conflict, complexity,” in McGraw-Hill Series in Psychology. Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity, ed. D. E. Berlyne (New York, NY: McGraw-Hill), 18–44. doi: 10.1037/11164-002
Berlyne, D. E. (1970). Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value. Percept. Psychophys. 8, 279–286. doi: 10.3758/BF03212593
Bunzeck, N., Doeller, C. F., Dolan, R. J., and Duzel, E. (2012). Contextual interaction between novelty and reward processing within the mesolimbic system. Hum. Brain Map. 33, 1309–1324. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21288
Campos, A. C., Mendes, J., Valle, P. O., and Scott, N. (2016). Co-creation experiences: attention and memorability. J. Travel Tour. Market. 33, 1309–1336. doi: 10.1080/10548408.2015.1118424
Campos, A. C., Mendes, J., Valle, P. O., and Scott, N. (2018). Co-creation of tourist experiences: a literature review. Curr. Issues Tour. 21, 369–400. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2015.1081158
Cassidy, T. (2013). Environmental Psychology: Behaviour and Experience in Context. London, UK: Psychology Press. doi: 10.4324/9780203940485
Chandralal, L., and Valenzuela, F. (2015). Memorable tourism experiences: scale development. Contemp. Manag. Res. 11, 291–310. doi: 10.7903/cmr.13822
Chandralal, L., and Valenzuela, F. R. (2013). Exploring memorable tourism experiences: antecedents and behavioural outcomes. J. Econ. Bus. Manag. 1, 177–181. doi: 10.7763/JOEBM.2013.V1.38
Chen, H., and Rahman, I. (2018). Cultural tourism: an analysis of engagement, cultural contact, memorable tourism experience and destination loyalty. Tour. Manag. Perspect. 26, 153–163. doi: 10.1016/j.tmp.2017.10.006
Chen, I.-L., Scott, N., and Benckendorff, P. (2017). Mindful tourist experiences: a Buddhist perspective. Ann. Tour. Res. 64, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2017.01.013
Chon, K. (1989). Understanding recreational travellers' motivation, attitude and satisfaction. Tour. Rev. 44, 3–7. doi: 10.1108/eb058009
Cloninger, C. R., Przybeck, T. R., and Svrakic, D. M. (1994). The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A Guide to Its Development and Use. Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University, St. Louis, MO, United States. doi: 10.1037/t03902-000
Coelho, M., Gosling, M., and Almeida, A. (2018). Tourism experiences: core processes of memorable trips. J. Hosp. Tour. Manag. 37, 11–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jhtm.2018.08.004
Corbin, J. M., and Strauss, A. (1990). Grounded theory research: procedures, canons, and evaluative criteria. Qual. Sociol. 13, 3–21. doi: 10.1007/BF00988593
Coudounaris, D. N., and Sthapit, E. (2017). Antecedents of memorable tourism experience related to behavioral intentions. Psychol. Market. 34, 1084–1093. doi: 10.1002/mar.21048
Crompton, J. L. (1979). Motivations for pleasure vacation. Ann. Tour. Res. 6, 408–424. doi: 10.1016/0160-7383(79)90004-5
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Flow and the Psychology of Discovery and Invention. New York, NY: HarperPerennial.
Dann, G. M. (1977). Anomie, ego-enhancement and tourism. Ann. Tour. Res. 4, 184–194. doi: 10.1016/0160-7383(77)90037-8
Dijksterhuis, A., and Aarts, H. (2010). Goals, attention, and (un) consciousness. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 61, 467–490. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100445
Dunman, T., and Mattila, A. S. (2005). The role of affective factors on perceived cruise vacation value. Tour. Manag. 26, 311–323. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2003.11.014
Farias, S. A., Aguiar, E. C., and Melo, F. V. S. (2014). Store atmospherics and experiential marketing: a conceptual framework and research propositions for an extraordinary customer experience. Int. Bus. Res. 7, 87–99. doi: 10.5539/ibr.v7n2p87
Filep, S., and Laing, J. (2019). Trends and directions in tourism and positive psychology. J. Travel Res. 58, 343–354. doi: 10.1177/0047287518759227
Filep, S., Laing, J., and Csikszentmihalyi, M., (eds.). (2016). “Synthesising positive tourism,” in Positive Tourism (London: Routledge), 205–214. doi: 10.4324/9781315707129
Förster, J., Marguc, J., and Gillebaart, M. (2010). Novelty categorization theory. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 4, 736–755. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00289.x
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., and Ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 212–228. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.2.212
Gnoth, J., and Matteucci, X. (2014). A phenomenological view of the behavioural tourism research literature. Int. J. Cult. Tour. Hosp. Res. 8, 3–21. doi: 10.1108/IJCTHR-01-2014-0005
Hadinejad, A., Moyle, B. D., Scott, N., and Kralj, A. (2019). Emotional responses to tourism advertisements: the application of FaceReader™. Tour. Recreat. Res. 44, 131–135. doi: 10.1080/02508281.2018.1505228
Hall, M. (2004). “Reflexivity and tourism research,” in Qualitative Research in Tourism: Ontologies, Epistemologies and Methodologies, eds L. Goodson and J. Phillimore (London: Routledge), 137–155.
Henson, R. N., and Gagnepain, P. (2010). Predictive, interactive multiple memory systems. Hippocampus 20, 1315–1326. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20857
Hillman, P., Moyle, B. D., and Weiler, B. (2018). Application of visual methods to perceptions of tourism development. Int. J. Cult. Tour. Hosp. Res. 12, 124–129. doi: 10.1108/IJCTHR-06-2017-0073
Hosany, S. (2012). Appraisal determinants of tourist emotional responses. J. Travel Res. 51, 303–314. doi: 10.1177/0047287511410320
Hosany, S., Buzova, D., and Sanz-Blas, S. (2019). The influence of place attachment, ad-evoked positive affect, and motivation on intention to visit: imagination proclivity as a moderator. J. Travel Res. 2019:1–19. doi: 10.1177/0047287519830789
Huang, D., Coghlan, A., and Jin, X. (2020). Understanding the drivers of Airbnb discontinuance. Ann. Tour. Res. 80:102798. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2019.102798
Hwang, J., and Hyun, S. S. (2013). The impact of nostalgia triggers on emotional responses and revisit intentions in luxury restaurants: the moderating role of hiatus. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 33, 250–262. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhm.2012.09.001
Iso-Ahola, S. E., and Weissinger, E. (1990). Perceptions of boredom in leisure: conceptualization, reliability and validity of the leisure boredom scale. J. Leis. Res. 22, 1–17. doi: 10.1080/00222216.1990.11969811
Johnson, A. R., and Stewart, D. W. (2005). A reappraisal of the role of emotion in consumer behavior: traditional and contemporary approaches. Rev. Market. Res. 1, 3–33. doi: 10.1108/S1548-6435(2004)0000001005
Kane, J., Van Boven, L., and McGraw, A. P. (2012). Prototypical prospection: future events are more prototypically represented and simulated than past events. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 42, 354–362. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1866
Kennelly, M., Moyle, B., and Lamont, M. (2013). Constraint negotiation in serious leisure: a study of amateur triathletes. J. Leis. Res. 45, 466–484. doi: 10.18666/jlr-2013-v45-i4-3895
Kim, A. K., and Brown, G. (2012). Understanding the relationships between perceived travel experiences, overall satisfaction, and destination loyalty. Anatolia 23, 328–347.
Kim, J. (2014). The antecedents of memorable tourism experiences: the development of a scale to measure the destination attributes associated with memorable experiences. Tour. Manag. 44, 34–45. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2014.02.007
Kim, J. (2018). The impact of memorable tourism experiences on loyalty behaviors: the mediating effects of destination image and satisfaction. J. Travel Res. 57, 856–870. doi: 10.1177/0047287517721369
Kim, J. H. (2010). Determining the factors affecting the memorable nature of travel experiences. J. Travel Tour. Market. 27, 780–796. doi: 10.1080/10548408.2010.526897
Kim, J. H., Ritchie, J. B., and McCormick, B. (2012). Development of a scale to measure memorable tourism experiences. J. Travel Res. 51, 12–25. doi: 10.1177/0047287510385467
Kim, K., Hallab, Z., and Kim, J. N. (2012). The moderating effect of travel experience in a destination on the relationship between the destination image and the intention to revisit. J. Hosp. Market. Manag. 21, 486–505. doi: 10.1080/19368623.2012.626745
Kim, S., and Kim, H. (2015). Moderating effects of tourists' novelty-seeking tendencies on the relationship between satisfaction and behavioral intention. Tour. Anal. 20, 511–522. doi: 10.3727/108354215X14411980111415
Kirillova, K., Lehto, X., and Cai, L. (2017). What triggers transformative tourism experiences? Tour. Recreat. Res. 42, 498–511. doi: 10.1080/02508281.2017.1342349
Kishiyama, M. M., and Yonelinas, A. P. (2003). Novelty effects on recollection and familiarity in recognition memory. Mem. Cogn. 31, 1045–1051. doi: 10.3758/BF03196125
Knobloch, U., Robertson, K., and Aitken, R. (2017). Experience, emotion, and eudaimonia: a consideration of tourist experiences and well-being. J. Travel Res. 56, 651–662. doi: 10.1177/0047287516650937
Lachman, R., Lachman, J. L., and Butterfield, E. C. (2015). Cognitive Psychology and Information Processing: An Introduction. New York, NY: Psychology Press. doi: 10.4324/9781315798844
Larsen, S. (2007). Aspects of a psychology of the tourist experience. Scand. J. Hosp. Tour. 7, 7–18. doi: 10.1080/15022250701226014
Le, D., Scott, N., and Lohmann, G. (2019). Applying experiential marketing in selling tourism dreams. J. Travel Tour. Market. 36, 220–235. doi: 10.1080/10548408.2018.1526158
Lee, T., and Crompton, J. (1992). Measuring novelty seeking in tourism. Ann. Tour. Res. 19, 732–751. doi: 10.1016/0160-7383(92)90064-V
Li, S., Scott, N., and Walters, G. (2015). Current and potential methods for measuring emotion in tourism experiences: a review. Curr. Issues Tour. 18, 805–827. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2014.975679
Lin, Y., Kerstetter, D., Nawijn, J., and Mitas, O. (2014). Changes in emotions and their interactions with personality in a vacation context. Tour. Manag. 40, 416–424. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2013.07.013
Ma, J. (2013). Emotions derived from theme park experiences: the antecedents and consequences of customer delight (Doctoral dissertation). University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia.
Ma, J., Gao, J., Scott, N., and Ding, P. (2013). Customer delight derived from theme park experiences: the antecedents of delight based on cognitive appraisal theory. Ann. Tour. Res. 42, 359–381.
Ma, J., Scott, N., Ding, P., and Gao, J. (2017). Delighted or satisfied? Positive emotional responses derived from theme park experiences. J. Travel Tour. Market. 34, 1–19. doi: 10.1080/10548408.2015.1125824
Mak, A. H. (2015). “Novelty, tourism,” in Encyclopedia of Tourism, eds J. Jafari and H. Xiao (London: Routledge), 319. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-01669-6_460-1
Manthiou, A., Kang, J., and Hyun, S. S. (2017). An integration of cognitive appraisal theory and script theory in the luxury cruise sector: the bridging role of recollection and storytelling. J. Travel Tour. Market. 34, 1071–1088. doi: 10.1080/10548408.2016.1277575
Manthiou, A., Lee, S., Tang, L., and Chiang, L. (2014). The experience economy approach to festival marketing: vivid memory and attendee loyalty. J. Serv. Market. 28, 22–35. doi: 10.1108/JSM-06-2012-0105
Mari, M., and Poggesi, S. (2013). Servicescape cues and customer behavior: A systematic literature review and research agenda. Serv. Industries J. 33, 171–199.
Massara, F., Liu, S. S., and Melara, R. D. (2010). Adapting to a retail environment: modeling consumer-environment interactions. J. Bus. Res. 63, 673–681. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2009.05.004
Mather, E. (2013). Novelty, attention, and challenges for developmental psychology. Front. Psychol. 4:491. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00491
Matteucci, X., and Gnoth, J. (2017). Elaborating on grounded theory in tourism research. Ann. Tour. Res. 65, 49–59. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2017.05.003
Meyer, W. -U., Niepel, M., Rudolph, U., and Schützwohl, A. (1991). An experimental analysis of surprise. Cogn. Emot. 5, 295–311. doi: 10.1080/02699939108411042
Michalkó, G., Irimiás, A., and Timothy, D. J. (2015). Disappointment in tourism: perspectives on tourism destination management. Tour. Manag. Perspect. 16, 85–91. doi: 10.1016/j.tmp.2015.07.007
Miles, M. B., and Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative Data Analysis: An Expanded Sourcebook. New York, NY: Sage.
Miller, C. J., Newcorn, J. H., and Halperin, J. M. (2010). Fading memories: retrospective recall inaccuracies in ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 14, 7–14. doi: 10.1177/1087054709347189
Mitas, O., and Bastiaansen, M. (2018). Novelty: a mechanism of tourists' enjoyment. Ann. Tour. Res. 72, 98–108. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2018.07.002
Moore, K., Cushman, G., and Simmons, D. (1995). Behavioral conceptualization of tourism and leisure. Ann. Tour. Res. 22, 67–85. doi: 10.1016/0160-7383(94)00029-R
Moyle, B. D., Moyle, C. J., and Bec, A. (2018). The coexistence of tourism and mining: a strategic framework for cross-sectoral interaction. Curr. Issues Tour. 21, 1966–1987. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2016.1238884
Moyle, B. D., Moyle, C. L., Bec, A., and Scott, N. (2019). The next frontier in tourism emotion research. Curr. Issues Tour. 22, 1393–1399. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2017.1388770
Neisser, U. (2014). Cognitive Psychology: Classic Edition. New York, NY: Psychology Press. doi: 10.4324/9781315736174
Oatley, K., and Johnson-Laird, P. (2014). Cognitive approaches to emotions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 134–140. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.004
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., and Collins, A. (1988). The Cognitive Structure of Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511571299
Passafaro, P. (2019). Attitudes and tourists' sustainable behavior: an overview of the literature and discussion of some theoretical and methodological issues. J. Travel Res. doi: 10.1177/0047287519851171. [Epub ahead of print].
Pearce, P. L., and Packer, J. (2013). Minds on the move: new links from psychology to tourism. Ann. Tour. Res. 40, 386–411. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2012.10.002
Petrick, J. F. (2002). An examination of golf vacationers' novelty. Ann. Tour. Res. 2, 384–400. doi: 10.1016/S0160-7383(01)00040-8
Petzer, D. J., De Meyer, C. F., Svari, S., and Svensson, G. (2012). Service receivers' negative emotions in airline and hospital service settings. J. Serv. Market. 26, 484–496. doi: 10.1108/08876041211266413
Plutchik, R. (1980). “A general psychoevolutionary theory of emotion,” in Theories of Emotion, eds R. Plutchik and H. Kellerman (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 3–33. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-558701-3.50007-7
Prayag, G., Hosany, S., Muskat, B., and Del Chiappa, G. (2017). Understanding the relationships between tourists' emotional experiences, perceived overall image, satisfaction, and intention to recommend. J. Travel Res. 56, 41–54. doi: 10.1177/0047287515620567
Radvansky, G. A., and Zacks, J. M. (2011). Event perception. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2, 608–620. doi: 10.1002/wcs.133
Roseman, I. J. (2001). “A model of appraisal in the emotion system: integrating theory, research, and application,” in Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research, eds K.R. Scherer, A. Schorr, and T. Johnston (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 68–91.
Roseman, I. J. (2013). Author reply: on the frontiers of appraisal theory. Emot. Rev. 5, 187–188. doi: 10.1177/1754073912469592
Roseman, I. J., Antoniou, A. A., and Jose, P. E. (1996). Appraisal determinants of emotions: constructing a more accurate and comprehensive theory. Cogn. Emot. 10, 241–277. doi: 10.1080/026999396380240
Ruhanen, L., Weiler, B., Moyle, B. D., and McLennan, C. L. J. (2015). Trends and patterns in sustainable tourism research: a 25-year bibliometric analysis. J. Sustain. Tour. 23, 517–535. doi: 10.1080/09669582.2014.978790
Sadeh, T., Ozubko, J. D., Winocur, G., and Moscovitch, M. (2014). How we forget may depend on how we remember. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 26–36. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.008
Scherer, K. R. (1993). Studying the emotion-antecedent appraisal process: an expert system approach. Cogn. Emot. 7, 325–355. doi: 10.1080/02699939308409192
Scott, N. (2020). Cognitive psychology and tourism - surfing the “cognitive wave”: a perspective article. Tourism Rev. doi: 10.1108/TR-06-2019-0217. [Epub ahead of print].
Scott, N., Gao, J., and Ma, J. (2017). “Introduction: creating memorable experiences,” in Visitor Experience Design, eds N. Scott, N. J. Gao, and J. Ma (Oxfordshire: CABI Publishing), 3–12.
Sharma, N., and Sarmah, B. (2019). Consumer engagement in village eco-tourism: a case of the cleanest village in Asia-Mawlynnong. J. Global Schol. Market. Sci. 29, 248–265. doi: 10.1080/21639159.2019.1577692
Shiffman, S., Hufford, M., Hickcox, M., Paty, J. A., Gnys, M., and Kassel, J. D. (1997). Remember that? A comparison of real-time versus retrospective recall of smoking lapses. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 65, 292–308. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.65.2.292.a
Skavronskaya, L., Dung, L., Moyle, B. D., and Scott, N. (2017b). “Experiential decision choice,” in Visitor Experience Design, eds N. Scott, N. J. Gao, and J. Ma (Oxfordshire: CABI Publishing), 68–90. doi: 10.1079/9781786391896.0068
Skavronskaya, L., Moyle, B., Scott, N., and Kralj, A. (2019a). The psychology of novelty in memorable tourism experiences. Curr. Issues Tour. 1–16. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2019.1664422
Skavronskaya, L., Moyle, B., Scott, N., and Schaffer, V. (2019b). Collecting memorable tourism experiences: how do 'WeChat'? J. China Tour. Res. 1–23. doi: 10.1080/19388160.2019.1656131
Skavronskaya, L., Scott, N., Moyle, B., Le, D., Hadinejad, A., Zhang, R., et al. (2017a). Cognitive psychology and tourism research: state of the art. Tour. Rev. 72, 221–237. doi: 10.1108/TR-03-2017-0041
So, J., Kwang, K., and Cho, H. (2016). Reexamining fear appeal models from cognitive appraisal theory and functional emotion theory perspectives. Commun. Monogr. 83, 120–144. doi: 10.1080/03637751.2015.1044257
Torland, M., Weiler, B., Moyle, B. D., and Wolf, I. D. (2015). Are your ducks in a row? External and internal stakeholder perceptions of the benefits of parks in New South Wales, Australia. Manag. Sport Leis. 20, 211–237. doi: 10.1080/23750472.2015.1028428
Tournier, I., Mathey, S., and Postal, V. (2012). The association between routinization and cognitive resources in later life. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 74, 143–161. doi: 10.2190/AG.74.2.c
Tulving, E., and Kroll, N. (1995). Novelty assessment in the brain and long-term memory encoding. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2, 387–390. doi: 10.3758/BF03210977
Tulving, E., Markowitsch, H., Craik, F., Habib, R., and Houle, S. (1996). Novelty and familiarity activations in PET studies of memory encoding and retrieval. Cereb. Cortex 6, 71–79. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.1.71
Tulving, E., Markowitsch, H. J., Kapur, S., Habib, R., and Houle, S. (1994). Novelty encoding networks in the human brain: positron emission tomography data. Neuroreport 5, 2525–2528.
Tung, V. W. S., and Ritchie, J. B. (2011a). Exploring the essence of memorable tourism experiences. Ann. Tour. Res. 38, 1367–1386. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2011.03.009
Tung, V. W. S., and Ritchie, J. B. (2011b). Investigating the memorable experiences of the senior travel market: an examination of the reminiscence bump. J. Travel Tour. Market. 28, 331–343. doi: 10.1080/10548408.2011.563168
Tynan, C., and McKechnie, S. (2009). Experience marketing: a review and reassessment. J. Market. Manag. 25, 501–517. doi: 10.1362/026725709X461821
Uysal, M., Perdue, R., and Sirgy, M. J. (Eds.). (2012). Handbook of Tourism and Quality-of-Life Research: Enhancing the Lives of Tourists and Residents of Host Communities. New York, NY: Springer Science and Business Media. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-2288-0
Vada, S., Prentice, C., and Hsiao, A. (2019). The influence of tourism experience and well-being on place attachment. J. Retail. Cons. Serv. 47, 322–330. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2018.12.007
Van Kesteren, M. T., Ruiter, D. J., Fernández, G., and Henson, R. N. (2012). How schema and novelty augment memory formation. Trends Neurosci. 35, 211–219. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.02.001
Volo, S. (2013). “Conceptualizing experience: a tourist based approach,” in Marketing of Tourism Experiences, eds N. Scott, E. Laws, and P. Boksberger (London: Routledge), 19–34.
Wang, S., Kirillova, K., and Lehto, X. (2017). Reconciling unsatisfying tourism experiences: message type effectiveness and the role of counterfactual thinking. Tour. Manag. 60, 233–243. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2016.12.008
Watson, L., and Spence, M. T. (2007). Causes and consequences of emotions on consumer behaviour. Eur. J. Market. 41, 487–511. doi: 10.1108/03090560710737570
Weaver, D., Tang, C., Shi, F., Huang, M. F., Burns, K., and Sheng, A. (2018). Dark tourism, emotions, and postexperience visitor effects in a sensitive geopolitical context: a Chinese case study. J. Travel Res. 57, 824–838. doi: 10.1177/0047287517720119
Webster, J. D. (1994). Predictors of reminiscence: a lifespan perspective. Can. J. Aging 13, 66–78. doi: 10.1017/S0714980800006565
Weiler, B., Moyle, B., and McLennan, C. L. (2012). Disciplines that influence tourism doctoral research: the United States, Canada, Australia and New Zealand. Ann. Tour. Res. 39, 1425–1445. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2012.02.009
Weiler, B., Torland, M., Moyle, B. D., and Hadinejad, A. (2018). Psychology in tourism doctoral research. Tour. Recreat. Res. 43, 277–288. doi: 10.1080/02508281.2018.1460081
Welch, C., and Piekkari, R. (2006). Crossing language boundaries: qualitative interviewing in international business. Manag. Int. Rev. 46, 417–437. doi: 10.1007/s11575-006-0099-1
Winograd, E., and Neisser, U. (1992). Affect and Accuracy in Recall: The Problem of “flashbulb” Memories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511664069
Witt, U. (2009a). Propositions about novelty. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 70, 311–320. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2009.01.008
Witt, U. (2009b). Novelty and the bounds of unknowledge in economics. J. Econ. Methodol. 16, 361–375. doi: 10.1080/13501780903339269
Yoo, C. K., Yoon, D., and Park, E. (2018). Tourist motivation: an integral approach to destination choices. Tour. Rev. 73, 169–185. doi: 10.1108/TR-04-2017-0085
Zhang, H., Wu, Y., and Buhalis, D. (2018). A model of perceived image, memorable tourism experiences and revisit intention. J. Destin. Market. Manag. 8, 326–336. doi: 10.1016/j.jdmm.2017.06.004
Zhang, R., Scott, N., and Gardiner, S. (2018). “The role of image schemas in developing the beach destination image of Hainan island,” in CAUTHE 2018: Get Smart: Paradoxes and Possibilities in Tourism, Hospitality and Events Education and Research, eds Y. Tamara, S. Paul, M. Gabrielle (Newcastle), 380–383
Keywords: novelty, tourism experience, cognitive appraisal theory, emotions, memory, psychology
Citation: Skavronskaya L, Moyle B and Scott N (2020) The Experience of Novelty and the Novelty of Experience. Front. Psychol. 11:322. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00322
Received: 17 July 2019; Accepted: 23 January 2020;
Published: 26 February 2020.
Edited by:
Claudia Chiarolanza, Sapienza University of Rome, ItalyReviewed by:
Anja Pabel, Central Queensland University, AustraliaMaria Paola Piccini, Università Pontificia Salesiana, Italy
Copyright © 2020 Skavronskaya, Moyle and Scott. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Liubov Skavronskaya, bGl1Ym92LnNrYXZyb25za2F5YSYjeDAwMDQwO3Jlc2VhcmNoLnVzYy5lZHUuYXU=