Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 13 December 2019
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Progress in Computer Gaming and Esports: Neurocognitive and Motor Perspectives View all 13 articles

Taking the First Steps Toward Integrating Testing and Training Cognitive Abilities Within High-Performance Athletes; Insights From a Professional German Football Club

\nAdam Beavan
Adam Beavan1*Jan SpielmannJan Spielmann1Jan Mayer,Jan Mayer1,2
  • 1TSG ResearchLab gGmbH, Zuzenhausen, Germany
  • 2TSG 1899 Hoffenheim, Zuzenhausen, Germany

Introduction to Executive Functions

Executive functions (EFs) are higher-level cognitive functions which refer to the family of top-down mental processes that sub serve goal-directed behavior (Miller and Cohen, 2001), and are relevant in situations that require a fast and flexible adjustment of behavior to the changing demands of the environment (Zelazo et al., 2003). There is a general agreement that the three core EFs are: inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility (Diamond, 2013). Previous research has proposed that EFs play an essential role in sport, connecting successful athletes with greater cognitive abilities (Jacobson and Matthaeus, 2014). Therefore, academics and practitioners alike are implementing EF testing batteries as an additional measure of performance. During the planning and implementation of EF assessments, there are obstacles that may be encountered by practitioners and coaches throughout all levels of play (i.e., amateur to professional leagues), such as the choice of assessments, the financial and opportunity costs, how to convey the data into meaningful results to the team, and what assumptions can currently be made from the data that is supported by research. By using the experience that we have gained by testing and training EFs for over 5 years at a professional 1st division football (Association Football) club in Germany, we aim to share our opinion on how to tackle these issues. We also aim to discuss the remaining barriers in EF research in hope of having more researchers and practitioners working together to collectively overcome them.

Setting Up a Protocol

The choice of assessments is the foundation that all future assumptions are based upon, and it is recommended to have a test measuring each EF independently. However, a large hurdle that clubs will inevitably encounter is the financial cost of implementing new assessment tasks. Despite some companies marketing their cognitive testing for upwards of $30,000 (i.e., CANTAB), this does not mean that cognitive batteries should only be implemented by the teams with larger budgets. Assessments such as the Design Fluency task to assess cognitive flexibility, and the Digit Symbol Substitution task to assess working memory can both be completed by pen and paper, whereas the N-back task to assess working memory and the Stroop Task to assess response inhibition can be created using PowerPoint. Furthermore, establishing mutualistic relationships with universities can provide opportunities for teams to either use the psychological tools owned by the university in exchange of participants' data for research purposes, and/or to help develop their own EF assessments.

Data Collection

Our club assesses every players' EFs once during the pre-season and once halfway through the season. We further recommend collecting contextual information about each player such as: birthdate, birth quartile and birthplace, intelligence quotient (IQ) or a similar academic grading score, history questionnaires on their years of experience playing both their main sport and any additional sports participation, and hours of training per week both in structured and unstructured playing environments (Mann et al., 2017). Contextual information can help improve our understanding of whether high-level athletes display better EFs than their lower-level and non-athletic counterparts because they were either born with greater cognitive abilities (i.e., nature), or whether their higher cognitive abilities are sport/environmentally-induced (i.e., nurture; Scharfen and Memmert, 2019).

Communicating the Results

Measuring EFs requires multiple complex assessments in a psychological domain and ensuring that the results are understood by the intended audience can be difficult (Eisenmann, 2017). Some strategies have been recommended in the literature. For example, Sakamoto et al. (2018) created a composite score by changing the results of each individual test into a z-score and then adding the z-scores together. In a practical sense, the idea of creating a single number that encompasses different scales for each variable can make the results easier to interpret and can be relatively easy to implement. However, caution should be taken in this approach as it may under-power the changes for each test. A more complex method that this club developed is an “EF sum score,” which combines all results into one total value (Beavan et al., 2019). Figure 1 displays the practicality of the sum score to provide a smoother translation of the relevant results to the intended audience (Buchheit, 2017).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mock data demonstrating how the results from individual assessments with various units can be combined in one summary graph. Values were normalized and then used in a two factor analyses to develop a total executive function sum score (EF sum score) for all players. A new EF variable was developed using each individual item's factor loading as a weighting system, so each change in one unit of performance was equal across each assessment. Our equation unique to our assessments (and serves only as an example) are: EF sum score = (0.720*Response Time Congruent Inverse) + (0.699*Response Time Incongruent Inverse) + (0.756*Determination Test Number of Correct Answers) + (0.828*Determination Test Response Time Inverse) + (0.853*SSRT Inverse) + (0.766*Response Inhibition Time Inverse).

What Inferences Can Currently Be Made from EF Results?

We evaluate our players' EF performance relative to their age group norms. However, there is a high variation between players on EFs across each age group, and this has similarly been reported in another study (Sakamoto et al., 2018). Our practitioners report that players who are the true elite academy players that yield the potential to make it to the adult professional level are also the players who are outperforming their age-group in the EF assessments, and the variation is caused by the many athletes that do not yet hold this “elite” status amongst coaches. Longitudinal data is needed in order to support this standpoint, but an interesting next step is to relate EFs to a panel of coaches' ratings of skill and potential of each player to reach the professional level rather than their age. Furthermore, various levels of motivation may influence the attainment of scores across the battery, specifically in EF assessments where athletes may be disinterested in performing non-sport specific testing. Although this remains difficult to account for, the underlying principle is that practitioners should consider the subjective contextual variables when interpreting their team's EF results. Variables such as the presence of a coach, motivation, players' contract situations, wellness, and whether the participants understand why they are doing the assessments all may influence the test results (Stiroh, 2007); but these variables have remained difficult to account for objectively in scientific research that may help explain the observed variance.

Despite these obstacles, a recent meta-analysis reported that a positive finding exists regarding the importance of EFs in football (Scharfen and Memmert, 2019). Higher-level athletes demonstrated better EFs than lower level athletes, and it has therefore been advocated that EF testing could play a role within the talent identification process (Huijgen et al., 2015; Sakamoto et al., 2018; Montuori et al., 2019). Yet additional research outside of football did not confirm the generalization of better EFs linked with expertise, where no differences between different levels of expertise in tennis (Kida et al., 2005), ice hockey (Lundgren et al., 2016), or basketball (Nakamoto and Mori, 2008) were reported. Therefore, there is a lack of agreement in the literature on whether EFs as a prognostic tool for football talent has practical validity. Albeit, previous research has attempted to understand if EFs could help identify talent. For example, Sakamoto et al. (2018) reported that players who were accepted into an academy exhibited better EFs than players who were rejected. Yet we need to assess whether these statistically significant differences are also practically relevant. The between-group difference on a Stroop task was on average +3 correct answers out of 100 (rejected group: 31.3 ± 9.6; approved group: 34.5 ± 8.6; p = 0.001; effect size = 0.35). In other words, the groups that were accepted and not accepted into the academy overlapped by about 86% (Magnusson, 2014). Although the differences reached statistical significance, whether they are large enough to help a coach distinguish between a player that should be accepted or rejected from an academy remains questionable.

To date, longitudinal studies in athletic populations are lacking, leaving only weak generalizations of the EF developmental trajectories from existing longitudinal studies in general populations. Therefore, longitudinal research is needed to understand if assessments of EFs are able to help practitioners predict talent in young athletes. For example, does assessing EFs have more value to help in the detection of potential talent from a heterogenous cohort that does not yet compete at a high-level (i.e., a large group of school kids), or in the identification of the best performers within a homogenous cohort of already competing athletes (i.e., high-level academy players likely to become adult-professionals)? Currently, no study has yet to demonstrate that athletes with higher EF scores become more successful in their sport.

Cognitive Training

The cognitive training approach stems from the “broad training hypothesis” which states that training basic cognitive skills could improve EFs and would therefore translate into better performances when utilizing EFs (Walton et al., 2018). For example, 10 sessions of cognitive training in a laboratory improved football players' on field passing decision-making accuracy by 15% (Romeas et al., 2016) and Ducrocq et al. (2016) reported the possibility to enhance sporting performance by improving the inhibitory control of tennis athletes. Importantly, there remains a large debate on whether training with computer-based cognitive tasks can broadly transfer into real-world performances. An extensive review by Simons et al. (2016) conveyed that no compelling evidence currently exists showing a true positive transfer of cognitive training interventions to real-world tasks. Recently, Harris et al. (2018) mentioned that although the lack of evidence across the literature is not an encouraging sign that it would work for athletes, only one study directly examined the benefits of a cognitive training program on sporting transfer task. Seemingly, if academics and practitioners are wanting to overcome this paucity of knowledge directly in sport, they are recommended to read Walton et al. (2018) who provides recommendations of how to best explore the link between cognitive and sporting abilities.

It is important that if clubs decide to invest in training EFs, the staff should further discuss both the financial cost of purchasing the equipment and the opportunity cost of spending the time and money on a different task (Simons et al., 2016). In order to reduce the opportunity cost, we emphasize the importance of cognitive training toward players who are: (i) regressing from their previous EF scores, (ii) wanting to engage in cognitive training, (iii) injured, and (iv) scoring in the lowest third within their age norms. The reason behind training players who performed in the lowest third is based on previous non-sporting literature reporting that a threshold effect may exist with natural abilities and expertise, where any improved ability beyond the requirement to compete at a high-level (i.e., the threshold) may not further improve performance (Terman and Oden, 1959). Contrastingly, this also means that players who are under the threshold may yield the potential to enhance their performance by improving their EFs (Diamond, 2016). Diamond (2016) advocated that training EFs are important to the future success of individuals and should begin as young as possible; especially in individuals that yield the lowest scores to ensure that their deficiencies are not enlarged over the coming years. Although the threshold hypothesis is still relatively new when explaining the role of EFs in sport, it has been used to explain differential correlations between intelligence (i.e., IQ), creativity (Jauk et al., 2013), and future career achievement (Terman and Oden, 1959; Baird, 1985; Gagné, 2004).

Conclusion

Practitioners are commonly the first to apply and test new methods with science following in attempt to examine their practices. The measurement and training of cognitive abilities such as EFs are becoming a popular new approach in sporting clubs. However, with EF research being a relatively young area of research, the importance of EFs in sport remains widely unknown, and it remains unclear if the measurement and training of EFs are justifiable to help predict future talent. Pending further research, a current focus on EF development in the lower achieving athletes may be a more suitable use. Being well-informed of the scientific literature will help in overcoming the delicate balancing act between administering good scientific practice methodology and what is functional for the club with respect to the aforementioned hurdles of implementing the testing and training of EFs. Therefore, new research-practitioner relationships are a cornerstone to furthering our understanding of the role that EFs play in sport. Collectively, a promising opportunity exists to help overcome the limitations in the literature if research informed protocols are put in place with a purpose of improving the support toward the testing and training EFs.

Author Contributions

AB, JS, and JM contributed toward the conception and ideas presented of the opinion piece. AB wrote the majority of the manuscript. JS wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the manuscript revisions, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by TSG 1899 Hoffenheim Fußball-Spielbetriebs GmbH.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Dr. Job Fransen, Ludwig Ruf, and the reviewers for their valuable comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

References

Baird, L. L. (1985). Do grades and tests predict adult accomplishment? Res. High. Educ. 23, 3–85. doi: 10.1007/BF00974070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beavan, A., Spielmann, J., Mayer, J., Skorski, S., Meyer, T., and Fransen, J. (2019). Age-related differences in executive functions within high-level youth soccer players. Braz. J. Motor Behav. 13, 64–75. doi: 10.20338/bjmb.v13i2.131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchheit, M. (2017). Houston, we still have a problem. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 12, 1111–1114. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.2017-0422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2016). “Why improving and assessing executive functions early in life is critical,” in Executive Function in Preschool-Age Children: Integrating Measurement, Neurodevelopment, and Translational Research (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 11–43.

Google Scholar

Ducrocq, E., Wilson, M., Vine, S., and Derakshan, N. (2016). Training attentional control improves cognitive and motor task performance. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 38, 521–533. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2016-0052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenmann, J. (2017). Translational gap between laboratory and playing field: new era to solve old problems in sports science. Transl. J. Am. Coll. Sports Med. 2, 37–43. doi: 10.1249/TJX.0000000000000032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagné, F. (2004). Transforming gifts into talents: the DMGT as a developmental theory. High Abil. Stud. 15, 119–147. doi: 10.1080/1359813042000314682

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, D. J., Wilson, M. R., and Vine, S. J. (2018). A systematic review of commercial cognitive training devices: implications for use in sport. Front. Psychol. 9:709. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huijgen, B. C., Leemhuis, S., Kok, N. M., Verburgh, L., Oosterlaan, J., Elferink-Gemser, M. T., et al. (2015). Cognitive functions in elite and sub-elite youth soccer players aged 13 to 17 years. PLoS ONE 10:e0144580. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0144580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, J., and Matthaeus, L. (2014). Athletics and executive functioning: how athletic participation and sport type correlate with cognitive performance. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 15, 521–527. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jauk, E., Benedek, M., Dunst, B., and Neubauer, A. C. (2013). The relationship between intelligence and creativity: new support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence 41, 212–221. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kida, N., Oda, S., and Matsumura, M. (2005). Intensive baseball practice improves the Go/Nogo reaction time, but not the simple reaction time. Cogn. Brain Res. 22, 257–264. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundgren, T., Högman, L., Näslund, M., and Parling, T. (2016). Preliminary investigation of executive functions in elite ice hockey players. J. Clin. Sport Psychol. 10, 324–335. doi: 10.1123/jcsp.2015-0030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnusson, K. (2014). Interpreting Cohen's D Effect Size, an Interactive Visualization. Available online at: https://rpsychologist.com/d3/cohend/.

Mann, D. L., Dehghansai, N., and Baker, J. (2017). Searching for the elusive gift: advances in talent identification in sport. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 16, 128–133. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montuori, S., D'Aurizio, G., Foti, F., Liparoti, M., Lardone, A., Pesoli, M., et al. (2019). Executive functioning profiles in elite volleyball athletes: preliminary results by a sport-specific task switching protocol. Hum. Mov. Sci. 63, 73–81. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2018.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamoto, H., and Mori, S. (2008). Effects of stimulus–response compatibility in mediating expert performance in baseball players. Brain Res. 1189, 179–188. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romeas, T., Guldner, A., and Faubert, J. (2016). 3D-multiple object tracking training task improves passing decision-making accuracy in soccer players. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 22, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2015.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakamoto, S., Takeuchi, H., Ihara, N., Ligao, B., and Suzukawa, K. (2018). Possible requirement of executive functions for high performance in soccer. PLoS ONE 13:e0201871. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0201871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scharfen, H. E., and Memmert, D. (2019). Measurement of cognitive functions in experts and elite-athletes: a meta-analytic review. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 33, 843–860. doi: 10.1002/acp.3526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simons, D. J., Boot, W. R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S. E., Chabris, C. F., Hambrick, D. Z., et al. (2016). Do “brain-training” programs work? Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 17, 103–186. doi: 10.1177/1529100616661983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stiroh, K. J. (2007). Playing for keeps: pay and performance in the NBA. Econ. Inq. 45, 145–161. doi: 10.1111/j.1465-7295.2006.00004.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terman, L. M., and Oden, M. H. (1959). Genetic Studies of Genius. Vol. V. The Gifted Group at Mid-Life. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Walton, C. C., Keegan, R. J., Martin, M., and Hallock, H. (2018). The potential role for cognitive training in sport: more research needed. Front. Psychol. 9:1121. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., and Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood: I. The development of executive function. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 68, 7–137. doi: 10.1111/j.0037-976X.2003.00261.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: executive functions, game intelligence, computer testing, soccer, talent identification

Citation: Beavan A, Spielmann J and Mayer J (2019) Taking the First Steps Toward Integrating Testing and Training Cognitive Abilities Within High-Performance Athletes; Insights From a Professional German Football Club. Front. Psychol. 10:2773. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02773

Received: 07 September 2019; Accepted: 25 November 2019;
Published: 13 December 2019.

Edited by:

Mark J. Campbell, University of Limerick, Ireland

Reviewed by:

Adam Mark Bruton, University of Roehampton London, United Kingdom
John Toner, University of Hull, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2019 Beavan, Spielmann and Mayer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Adam Beavan, adam.beavan@uni-saarland.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.