Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 27 March 2019
Sec. Developmental Psychology

Spanish Validation of the PICCOLO (Parenting Interactions With Children: Checklist of Observations Linked to Outcomes)

  • 1Department of Cognition, Development and Educational Psychology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 2Department of Psychobiology and Methodology of the Behavioral Sciences, University of Málaga, Málaga, Spain
  • 3Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
  • 4Department of Psychology, Universitat Abat Oliba-CEU, Barcelona, Spain
  • 5Center for Persons with Disabilities, Utah State University, Logan, UT, United States

Background/Objective: The aim of this study was to explore the psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the Parenting Interactions with Children: Checklist of Observations Linked to Outcomes (PICCOLO; Roggman et al., 2013a). This observational measure is composed of 29 items that assess the quality of four domains of parenting interactions that promote child development: affection, responsiveness, encouragement, and teaching.

Methods: The sample included 203 mother-child dyads who had been video-recorded playing together. Fifty-six percent of the children were male, and 44% were female, aged from 10 to 47 months. Video-recorded observations were rated using PICCOLO items.

Results: Confirmatory factor analysis supported that the instrument has four first-order factors corresponding to the hypothesized domains of parenting behaviors, and a second-order factor corresponding to a general factor of positive parenting. Construct validation evidence was compiled by examining the relationship between PICCOLO scores and child age. As expected, teaching domain and total PICCOLO scores were positively correlated with child age. The Spanish PICCOLO also demonstrated good inter-rater reliability (ranging from 0.69 to 0.84) and internal consistency reliability (ranging from 0.59 to 0.88) for the four domain scores and the total parenting score. Concurrent criterion-related validity was examined via correlations between parenting scores and child cognitive, language and motor skills outcomes, measured using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development.

Conclusion: The Spanish version of the PICCOLO meets the criteria for a reliable and valid observational measurement of parenting interactions with children. The psychometric properties of the instrument make it appropriate for general research purposes, but also for program evaluation of Early Intervention and other parenting-support interventions. This measure, focused on parent strengths, could be used to facilitate family-centered practices in early intervention and other programs that have parenting as an outcome.

Introduction

Early positive parent-child interactions are important for a child’s positive development in both normally developing children (Love et al., 2005; Blair et al., 2014; Vargas-Rubilar and Arán-Filippetti, 2014) and those with developmental delay and disabilities (Spiker et al., 2002; Innocenti et al., 2013). Early positive parenting interactions promote child development, and specific parenting behaviors that lead to better developmental outcomes have been identified (Bradley and Corwyn, 2002; Mahoney, 2009; Warren et al., 2010). Those parental behaviors that promote child development, primarily studied in mothers but also in fathers in some cases (Anderson et al., 2013; Cabrera et al., 2017), are labeled parenting or positive parenting. Although we use the term parenting, this refers to the child’s caregiver, which may or may not be the parent.

Specific aspects of early parenting have been related to different developmental outcomes in children. On the one hand, parents’ emotional warmth and affection have been related to the child’s social emotional development and secure attachment (Laible et al., 2000; Zhou et al., 2002; Caspi et al., 2004; Sanders et al., 2004). On the other hand, responsive behaviors have also been linked to children’s developmental outcomes. Parental responsive behaviors are quick and contingent responses to the child, adjusted to his/her initiative and interests. Responsiveness has been linked to emotional, social, cognitive and linguistic development (Landry et al., 2001, 2006; Crouter and Head, 2002; Davidov and Grusec, 2006; Hirsh-Pasek and Burchinal, 2006; Bernier et al., 2010; Roseberry et al., 2013; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2001, 2014). Further, parental behaviors that promote some degree of autonomy, on the part of the child, and that are adjusted to the child’s competencies, setting limits and demanding maturity according to age, have been associated with social and cognitive development and readiness for school (Hubbs-Tait et al., 2002; Joussemet et al., 2005; Bernier et al., 2010, 2012; Graziano et al., 2011; Fay-Stammbach et al., 2014). Finally, cognitive and linguistic stimulation (e.g., explanations, asking the child questions, promoting the child’s participation in adult-child joint activities and conversation) have also been related to children’s cognitive, linguistic, and social emotional development as well as to their emergent literacy skills (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2001; Hubbs-Tait et al., 2002; Kim-Cohen et al., 2004; Bingham, 2007; Farah et al., 2008).

Different tools have been used to measure the quality of parent-child interactions or positive parenting, most relying on parent reports. Typically, parent reports address parental abilities or competences in a broad sense, and parents respond to different items in terms of their consciousness about their own most frequent behaviors and attitudes about their child’s rearing and education. Good examples of this kind of instrument are the Alabama Parenting Questionnaire (Essau et al., 2006), the Baby Care Questionnaire (Winstanley and Gattis, 2013), and those developed by Gómez and Muñoz (2014) in Chile, and by Vázquez et al. (2016) in Spain. Although these tools can be useful for gaining knowledge about parental competences, they have some limitations that are common to all measures based on self-reporting, which include limitations on validity. When filling out self-report questionnaires, different parents can interpret items differently, not remember well, want to give a good image of themselves… And, even though well-administered questionnaires can offer valuable and useful information, the direct observation of interaction processes between parents and children offers relevant and complementary information.

Some authors have pointed out that direct observation of parental interactions by qualified coders can provide more accurate data about parenting in face-to-face daily interactions (Roggman et al., 2013b). The HOME (Home Measure of the Environment; Caldwell and Bradley, 2003), probably the most broadly used instrument for assessing the quality of family as a developmental context, includes some direct observations of parent-child interactions but is primarily conducted through interview. In Spain, Trenado and Cerezo (2007) developed a sequential coding system of early caregiver-child interaction in real time (CITMI-R). This coding system provides microanalytic measurements of the early dyadic interaction in a free play situation and has adequate reliability in Spanish and English samples (Trenado et al., 2014). Also, in Spain, Velasco et al. (2014) developed another family context assessment instrument (Etxadi-Gangoiti scale), which also includes some items of direct observation of parenting interactions. Although these direct observation measures tend to have good reliability and validity they are time intensive to code and few have been used cross-culturally. For more information about these tools see the review by Alcantud et al. (2015).

Parenting interaction characteristics depend on both personal and cultural factors. Parenting practices are diverse among cultures and reflect cultural child rearing expectations as well as cultural beliefs and values about child development. This does not mean that parenting is intergenerationally transmitted in an automatic way. Every parent conducts his/her parenting behaviors according to personal characteristics and beliefs, individual experience, cultural background and contextual demands. Although cross-cultural studies have been conducted (e.g., Jackson-Newsom et al., 2008; Burchinal et al., 2010; Bornstein, 2013; Lansford et al., 2014), these studies do not typically use a common measure across cultural groups. To better understand the global context of parenting, we need a single tool that could be used among different countries and cultural populations. A tool that is based on direct observation and has potential for cross-cultural use is the PICCOLO (Roggman et al., 2013b). PICCOLO was developed following an in-depth review of previous literature about parental behaviors related to children’s developmental outcomes and the authors’ own studies, and it is composed of 29 items in four behavioral domains. These domains are called affection, responsiveness, encouragement and teaching (Roggman et al., 2013a). As mentioned above, emotional warmth and affection (affection), responsiveness or responsivity (responsiveness), parental control and promotion of autonomy (encouragement), and cognitive and linguistic stimulation (teaching) are the main dimensions of parental behaviors during parent-child interactions that literature has related to the optimization of child development. Each domain identifies specific kinds of developmentally supportive parenting practices that predict children’s outcomes (Innocenti et al., 2013; Roggman et al., 2013a).

The PICCOLO has been shown to have strong reliability and validity in multiple ethnic groups in the United States (788 European American, 792 African American, and 468 Latino American families) and appears applicable across the developmental spectrum from 10 to 47 months. The PICCOLO has been translated into many languages using a process that includes back translation while working with the authors to ensure content validity (Innocenti and Roggman, 2018). This procedure has been used for translations into Spanish for use in Chile and Spain, and into German, Chinese, Italian, Turkish, Brazilian-Portuguese and Dutch (personal communication with the PICCOLO authors). However, only two studies have analyzed psychometric properties of PICCOLO adaptations to other cultures: The Turkish validation of the PICCOLO with a sample of 130 mother-child dyads (Bayoğlu et al., 2013), and the Brazilian-Portuguese validation with a sample of 156 mother-child (18-month-old) dyads (Schneider, 2018).

In Spain, a tool for assessing parenting both for research purposes (e.g., to gain general knowledge about parent-child interactions and their association with developmental outcomes, or to conduct intercultural comparisons about parenting) and for applied purposes in early intervention programs (e.g., to identify parenting strengths as part of an intervention) is needed. It should be an easy-to-administer and easy-to-score observational tool that can provide accurate data about parent-child interactions, be sensitive to changes in response to intervention, and be useful for practitioners who aim to improve parental interactions and thus child development (Gardner, 2000; Aspland and Gardner, 2003; Roggman et al., 2013b; Zaslow et al., 2006). The Spanish validation of the PICCOLO is a contribution in this regard, as the PICCOLO appears to meet these requirements. Unlike other tools mentioned above, the PICCOLO requires little administration time (10 min of audiovisual recording and 20–30 min of coding), and its international use in different ethnic backgrounds opens the way to sharing results between researchers and carrying out intercultural studies.

The purpose of this study was to test the psychometric properties of the PICCOLO in a large sample of Spanish mothers and children aged from 10 to 47 months, in order to validate the tool for use in the Spanish population, for research or applied purposes.

Materials and Methods

Instruments

The Parenting Interactions with Children: Checklist of Observations Linked to Outcomes (PICCOLO; Roggman et al., 2013a) is a checklist of 29 observable behaviors used to assess parenting interactions with children in four domains: affection, responsiveness, encouragement, and teaching. This instrument is a reliable and valid measure of parent-child interactions for parents with children between the ages of 10 and 47 months. The PICCOLO was developed in the United States based on a sample of over 2,048 low-income families from diverse ethnic groups with at least one child between 10 and 47 months of age, from the archive of the Early Head Start Research and Evaluation Project (Roggman et al., 2013a). Parent-child dyads were observed during 10 min of free play using three bags containing, respectively, a book, toys for pretend play and manipulative toys (Fuligni and Brooks-Gunn, 2013). The 29 items reflect parent interaction behaviors and are scored according to their frequency as 0 (absent, no behavior observed), 1 (barely, minor or emerging behavior) and 2 (clearly, definitive, strong and frequent behavior). They are grouped into four dimensions: (a) Affection (seven items), which involves physical and verbal expression of affection, positive emotions, positive evaluation and positive regard; (b) Responsiveness (seven items), which includes reacting sensitively to a child’s cues and expressions of needs or interests and reacting positively to the child’s behavior; (c) Encouragement (seven items), which considers parents’ support of children’s efforts, exploration, independence, play, choices, creativity, and initiative; and (d) Teaching (eight items), which includes cognitive stimulation, explanations, conversation, joint attention, and shared play. The instrument generates a score for each dimension between 0 and 14 (and 0 to 16 for teaching dimension) and a total score between 0 and 58 (by summing all the items).

The original PICCOLO reliability is good, with an average of 0.77 inter-rater reliability correlations between pairs of observers for total score (0.80 for affection, 0.76 for responsiveness, 0.73 for encouragement and 0.69 for teaching). The correlation of total score between observers of different ethnicities averaged 0.80 (0.78 for affection, 0.68 for responsiveness, 0.66 for encouragement and 0.75 for teaching). The analysis of Cronbach’s Alpha for the total instrument was 0.91 (0.78 for affection, 0.75 for responsiveness, 0.77 for encouragement and 0.80 for teaching) and the instrument had good results for construct and predictive validity (Roggman et al., 2013a). The PICCOLO has also been used to observe parents interacting with children with a disability, showing both reliability and validity (Innocenti et al., 2013).

Child development was assessed using the Spanish version of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development - III (BSID-III; Bayley, 2015). BSID-III scales are widely used to assess development between 1 and 42 months of age. Cognitive, Expressive Language, Receptive Language, Fine Motor, and Gross Motor Subscales were applied. While not used in the present study, the BSID-III also includes a Social-Emotional Scale and Adaptive Behavior Scale, which are questionnaires for caregivers. Cognitive, motor and linguistic percentiles were calculated. The Bayley Scales are standardized and have good inter-rater reliability and are valid for predicting current and future development.

Translation and Adaptation

Consent was obtained from the authors of the original PICCOLO (Roggman et al., 2013a) and publisher (Brookes Publishing) to develop the Spanish version. Two native speakers of Spanish, both experts in Developmental Psychology, translated the original scale from English into Spanish. An English native speaker back-translated it into English. The back-translated form was evaluated by two of the PICCOLO authors (Roggman and Innocenti), who provided suggestions for refinements. The final adapted Spanish version was produced after incorporating their suggestions.

Participants

Participants were recruited from pediatric centers, nurseries and Community Family Centers. The following criteria were used for inclusion of children in the study: (a) child’s age between 10 and 47 months; (b) child’s birth weight of 2.5 kg (5 pounds 8 ounces) or more; (c) not having complications in childbirth; and (d) no hospitalizations prior to enrollment in the study. Written informed consent was obtained from the participants of this study and from the parents/legal guardians of all non-adult participants.

The Spanish sample included 203 mother-child dyads who had been video recorded playing together. Fifty-six percent of the children were male, and 44% were female, aged from 10 to 47 months (M = 27.1, SD = 9.8). Three percent of children were less than 1 year old (10 to 11 months), 38% were 1 year old (12 to 23 months), 36% were 2 years old (24 to 35 months), and 23% were 3 years old (36 to 47 months). The mothers were aged between 22 and 47 years (M = 34.6, SD = 4.3). The majority of mothers were married or living with a partner (95.1%). Most of them (69.1%) had a university degree, 24.1% had completed high school, and 6.8% had received only elementary schooling. They were either full (59.5%) or partially employed (27.2%), or cared for their children and were fully responsible for housework (13.3%). Almost a third of the sample (29%) had a monthly family net income between €1,313 and €2,451, considered an average income in Spain. The monthly income was lower than €1,313 for 6% of the families, and higher than €2,451 for the rest (65%).

Training of the Raters

A university research group scored the Spanish version of the PICCOLO. All scorers were psychologists and specialists in child development. The first author, who was trained by the authors of the original PICCOLO, trained the raters for this study. Observer trainees read about the content and purpose of the measure (during a 3-h session) and watched and discussed four video recordings (3 h). At the end of the training sessions, the observers watched and coded four to six additional video-recorded interactions to establish reliability (3–6 h). The observers were considered to have completed their training satisfactorily when the percentage inter-rater agreement was equal to or above 80%.

Procedure

Initially, ethical approval was obtained from the University of Barcelona’s Bioethics Commission (CBUB), according to the International Ethical Guidelines for Health-related Research Involving Humans prepared by the Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences (CIOMS) in collaboration with the World Health Organization (WHO), and the WMA Declaration of Helsinki - Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects.

Later on, pediatric centers, nurseries and Community Family Centers were contacted by letter and telephone and informed of the study. We contacted the coordinators of the centers to request their collaboration in recruiting families for the study. Families were informed that their participation would be entirely voluntary and anonymous. Families were mailed questionnaire packages containing a newsletter with information about the study and a brief guide about how to video record at home, an informed consent to sign, and a demographic questionnaire to complete. Mothers were asked to record a video of an approximately 10-minute (between 8 and 10 min) play session with their child at home, with the following instruction: “Interact and play with your children as you typically do.” Mothers recorded themselves and then they sent the video tape to us by mail or we collected them. The videos used in this research were those in which the parent followed the directions of the researchers. The most frequent toys that mothers and children used to play together were books, toy animals, kitchens, little dolls, building blocks… So, the selected toys were very similar to those used by Roggman et al. (2013b) in their original study. Twelve video recordings had to be excluded, either because only the child appeared on the tape, or the audio was not clear enough or because they were shorter than 8 min. The PICCOLO was then used to score parent-child interactions from these approximately 10-minute-long video recordings.

To test criterion-related validity, a subsample of 64 children were randomly selected and were assessed using the cognitive, motor and linguistic subscales of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID-III; Bayley, 2015). The smaller number of participants for this subsample was due to the high cost of applying the Bayley Scales to the children. The PICCOLO domain scores for each observation were correlated with the Bayley scores.

Data Analysis

According to the original version of the PICCOLO, video observations rated independently by two coders (N = 61) were used to estimate inter-rater reliability via percentage observer agreement for each item, and intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) for each domain and total scores.

Video observations rated by one of the two coders (N = 203) were used to analyze the different psychometric properties of the instrument. Cronbach’s α coefficient was estimated to determine the internal consistency reliability of the instrument. For element analyses, we calculated Cronbach’s alpha if an item was deleted, and discrimination indexes, obtained as the corrected correlation of the item score with that of the corresponding domain.

Dimensionality of the instrument was examined through confirmatory factor analysis. Since items were rated on a 3-point ordinal scale, diagonally weighted least squares (DWLS) were used to estimate the model parameters. DWLS is considered a robust estimator for ordinal data and small samples and in cases of violations of normality (Forero et al., 2009). Goodness of fit of the model was evaluated according to the following criteria: (a) relative chi-square (χ2/df): a good fit is indicated by a value lower than 2; (b) comparative fit index (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis index (TLI): a ≥ 0.90 value indicates an acceptable fit, while a ≥ 0.95 value is an indicator of a good fit; and (c) root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA): a value of ≤ 0.08 RMSEA is indicative of an acceptable fit, while a value of ≤ 0.05 RMSEA is indicative of a good fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999).

Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient was used to analyze the relationship between age (in months) and PICCOLO scores. For independent samples, the Student’s t test was used to compare mean Spanish PICCOLO scores according to child gender. Discriminant validity was analyzed by obtaining the Pearson correlation coefficients among different PICCOLO domains.

Finally, concurrent criterion-related validity was examined (for a subsample of 64 participants) via Pearson’s correlations between mothers’ PICCOLO scores and children’s scores on the Bayley scales of infant development. Using a two-sided test, with 5% significance and a power of 80%, the required sample size to detect a correlation ≥ 0.32 is approximately 74.

Confirmatory factor analysis was performed using lavaan, an R package for Structural Equation Modeling, version 0.5–12 (Rossel, 2012). The rest of the statistical analyses were carried out using IBM SPSS Statistics, version 24.0.

Results

Reliability

Video observations rated independently by two coders (N = 61, 30,05%) were used to estimate inter-rater reliability. Item-level observer agreement ranged from 54 to 95%. Only item 5 in the encouragement domain (Verbally encourages child’s efforts) showed a low inter-rater agreement (<60%). Averaging across items, agreement was 83% for the affection domain, 83% for the responsiveness domain, 78% for the encouragement domain, 77% for the teaching domain, and 80% for all the items of the Spanish PICCOLO. Inter-rater reliability was also estimated via ICCs for each domain and total scores. ICCs were 0.83 for the affection domain, 0.69 for the responsiveness domain, 0.81 for the encouragement domain, 0.80 for the teaching domain, and 0.84 for the total Spanish PICCOLO score. These results demonstrate that there was high agreement between the scores given to each item by the two observers and, consequently, domain and total scores also showed high inter-rater reliability.

Video observations rated by one of the two coders (N = 203) were used to analyze the scale’s internal consistency reliability. Internal consistency of the scale was indicated by a Cronbach’s α coefficient of 0.59 for the affection domain, 0.75 for the responsiveness domain, 0.79 for the encouragement domain, 0.68 for the teaching domain, and 0.88 for the total Spanish PICCOLO score. All domain and total scores showed a satisfactory alpha coefficient exceeding an acceptable minimum of 0.65 (DeVellis, 2016), except the alpha coefficient for the affection domain. While a value of 0.65 is generally agreed to be an acceptable value, some researchers (e.g., Cho and Kim, 2015) caution against applying any arbitrary or automatic cutoff criteria. Rather, it is suggested than any minimum value should be determined on an individual basis based on the purpose of the research, the number of items in the scale, and/or the stage of research (i.e., exploratory, basic, or applied). The alpha coefficient for the affection domain was very close to 0.60, which may be considered as acceptable given that the score is composed of only seven items. For item analyses, we calculated Cronbach’s alpha if an item was deleted, and discrimination indexes, obtained as the corrected correlation of the item score with that of the corresponding domain (Table 1). Except for two items, the alpha coefficient of each domain decreased if any of the items was deleted, indicating that all of them contribute to increasing the domain’s internal consistency. In addition, all items (except two) showed discrimination indexes above the recommended minimum of 0.30 (DeVellis, 2016). Item 5 in the affection domain (Uses positive expressions with child), and item 6 in the teaching domain (Does activities in a sequence of steps) did not contribute to the α scale, and their discrimination index was below the recommended minimum of 0.30. Indeed, as shown in Table 1, if item 5 was deleted from the affection domain, the alpha coefficient would be 0.65 instead of 0.59, and if item 6 was deleted from the teaching domain, the alpha coefficient would be 0.71 instead of 0.68.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Item analysis of the Spanish version of the PICCOLO (N = 203) (both in English and Spanish).

Factor Analysis

Because the Spanish PICCOLO was developed using a model based on the original PICCOLO domains (Roggman et al., 2013a), the dimensionality of the instrument was examined using confirmatory factor analysis. We tested a model with four first-order factors corresponding to the four hypothesized domains of parenting behaviors (affection, responsiveness, encouragement, and teaching), and a second-order factor corresponding to a general factor of positive parenting interactions with children. Figure 1 shows the path diagram of the confirmatory factor analysis with items loading on one of the four domains, and domains loading on the general factor.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Path diagram of the confirmatory factor analysis. Aff, Affection domain; Resp, Responsiveness domain; Enc, Encouragement domain; Teach, Teaching domain. Affl indicates Item 1 in the affection domain; this shorthand is used throughout. Error terms are omitted.

All factor loadings exceeded the desired threshold of 0.40, and were statistically significant (p < 0.001), except item 5 loading on the affection domain, and item 6 loading on the teaching domain. The model’s overall goodness of fit indices were as follows: χ2 = 617.65 (df = 373, N = 203); relative chi-square (χ2/df) = 1.66; CFI = 0.970; TLI = 0.967; and RMSEA = 0.057 (90% CI:0.049 -0.065). These results suggest a good (χ2/df < 2, CFI > 0.95 and TLI > 0.95) or, at least, acceptable (RMSEA < 0.08) fit of the model.

Spanish PICCOLO Scores by Child Age and Gender

Table 2 shows the mean and standard deviation for the Spanish PICCOLO scores according to age of the child. Pearson’s correlation coefficients between child age (in months) and Spanish PICCOLO scores were computed. Statistically significant correlations were found between child age and scores for the teaching domain (r = 0.28; p < 0.001) and, to a lesser extent, between child age and total PICCOLO scores (r = 0.15, p = 0.033), indicating that teaching and positive parenting interactions were higher with older children, as also found in the original PICCOLO sample (Roggman et al., 2013b) and in a Turkish sample (Bayoğlu et al., 2013). In the Turkish sample, a statistically significant negative correlation was also found between child age and scores for the affection domain (r = -0.32, p < 0.001), which means that affection was higher with younger children. However, in the Spanish sample, the obtained negative correlation between those variables was not significant (r = -0.06, p = 0.431).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Spanish PICCOLO scores by child age.

With respect to child gender, the Student’s t test for independent samples found no statistically significant differences between boys (n = 114) and girls (n = 89) either for each domain or for the mean total PICCOLO scores. This result shows that, as expected based on previous studies (Bayoğlu et al., 2013; Roggman et al., 2013b), the positive parenting interactions observed in the Spanish mothers were not associated with child gender.

Correlations Among PICCOLO Domains

Pearson’s correlation coefficients among Spanish PICCOLO domains were obtained. As shown in Table 3, the lowest correlation coefficient was found between the affection and teaching domains (r = 0.30), and the highest between responsiveness and encouragement (r = 0.66). Therefore, the PICCOLO domains were moderately to highly correlated with one another, although not at a level that would suggest that they measure the same construct. Indeed, all of the correlation coefficients between two different domains met the recommended criterion for discriminant validity, which requires a correlation between two constructs of less than 0.85 (Kline, 2015).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Pearson’s correlation coefficients among Spanish PICCOLO domains (N = 203).

Concurrent Validity

For a subsample of 64 participants, Spanish PICCOLO domain and total scores were examined in relation to child cognitive, language and motor skills outcomes, measured using the Bayley scales of infant development (BSID-III). For this analysis, Pearson’s correlations between mothers’ PICCOLO scores and children’s BSID-III scores were computed.

As shown in Table 4, some significant positive correlations (p < 0.05) were found between PICCOLO scores and some Bayley subscale scores. In particular, child cognitive Bayley scores were positively associated with mothers’ PICCOLO encouragement scores. These results indicated that the more a mother demonstrates encouragement behaviors with her child, the higher the child’s cognitive level.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Pearson’s correlations between PICCOLO scores and children’s BSID-III scores (N = 64).

Child language Bayley scores were also positively associated with mothers’ PICCOLO responsiveness, encouragement, teaching, and total scores. That is, the higher a mother’s parenting scores (except for the affection domain), the higher the child’s language level. However, children’s motor skills were not associated with any of the mothers’ positive parenting domains.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to test the psychometric properties of the PICCOLO, an observational measure of parenting interactions with children that was developed in the United States (Roggman et al., 2013a), for use in practice and research on the Spanish population. The reliability and validity of the instrument was explored using a large sample of Spanish mothers and children aged from 10 to 47 months. The mean scores in all domains were similar between 1, 2 and 3 years of age, as shown in Table 2, with Responsiveness being the most constant dimension among ages. Affection decreased slightly from 1 to 2 years, and Encouragement showed a slight increase between the same ages. Teaching was the most variable dimension, increasing from 1 to 2 years and from 2 to 3. This could be interpreted as showing a mother’s tendency to adjust to the child’s interests and needs at all ages, a slight tendency to show more affection to younger children and to promote autonomy in older children, and a progressive increase in teaching behaviors with child’s age.

The analyses of the 29 original PICCOLO items showed that two items should be eliminated due to discrimination indices below the recommended limits. However, in item analysis it is important to keep in mind the main purpose of the instrument. The PICCOLO was designed as a useful parenting measure that predicts positive child outcomes. Indeed, the items contained in the PICCOLO were selected because they represented positive parenting interactions that research and theory suggested were related to children’s development (i.e., the items are causal indicators of children’s outcomes). As Bradley (2015) pointed out, using procedures that are standard practice in constructing scales (e.g., dropping items with low item-to-total correlations or weak factor loadings) can be a mistake for measures composed of causal indicators. Accordingly, we decided to include all of the original PICCOLO items for the Spanish version in order to maximize content validity and potential criterion-related validity, at the expense of a lower reliability of the measures.

Our findings showed good inter-rater reliability and internal consistency reliability of the Spanish PICCOLO domain and total scores, with similar values to those found in samples from Turkey (Bayoğlu et al., 2013) and the United States (Roggman et al., 2013a). Other observational measures of parenting that require complex coding or rating scales often require substantial training time for observers to accurately use the measures (Fuligni and Brooks-Gunn, 2013). In contrast, PICCOLO is a checklist of observable behaviors that can be learned relatively quickly to achieve accurate ratings of parenting interactions with children.

In relation to the dimensionality of the instrument, the results of the confirmatory factor analysis demonstrated that the instrument has a four-factor structure of first order domains that collapses into a single, second-order factor of parenting. The four first-order factors corresponded to the theoretical constructs (affection, responsiveness, encouragement, and teaching), which the original authors hypothesized in developing the instrument (Roggman et al., 2013a). Indeed, the original authors recommend scoring all four domains of the PICCOLO with families “in order to be (a) Positive: Most parents have strengths in more than one of the four domains, (b) Practical: Practitioners can see more parenting strengths if they are watching for all four domains, (c) Culturally sensitive: Different cultures emphasize different domains, and (d) Sensible: Using all four domains helps practitioners see the whole picture” (Roggman et al., 2013a, p. 23). Furthermore, the fact that these four domains collapse into a general second-order factor also justifies the use of a total score of positive parenting interactions in the evaluation. The fit of the model, for the confirmatory factor analysis, was acceptable, which implies construct validation evidence for the Spanish version of the PICCOLO.

Further construct validation evidence was compiled by examining the association between the PICCOLO scores and expected parenting-related constructs, such as child age and child gender. In this study, teaching domain and total PICCOLO scores were positively correlated with child age, as also found in the original PICCOLO sample (Roggman et al., 2013b) and the Turkish sample (Bayoğlu et al., 2013). In the Turkish sample, a statistically significant negative correlation was also found between child age and scores for the affection domain, which means that affection was higher with younger children. However, in the Spanish sample, the correlation between those variables was not significant. These differences may reflect different cultural values about parenting or about the parents’ goals for their children’s developmental outcomes (Tuttle et al., 2012). With respect to the relationship between positive parenting interactions and child gender, our results showed no statistically significant differences between boys and girls for the mean PICCOLO scores, as expected based on previous studies (Bayoğlu et al., 2013; Roggman et al., 2013a).

Concurrent validity between the PICCOLO and the Bayley scales, assessed using a subsample of 64 participants from the general validation sample, showed that children’s cognitive development was positively associated with mothers’ PICCOLO score in encouragement. These results are consistent with those of Hubbs-Tait et al. (2002); Bernier et al. (2010, 2012), and Fay-Stammbach et al. (2014). On the other hand, the children’s language Bayley scores were positively associated with the mothers’ PICCOLO responsiveness, encouragement, teaching, and the total scores. Our results are consistent with those of Hirsh-Pasek and Burchinal (2006) and Tamis-LeMonda et al. (2001, 2014). As expected, most of the very clear relations established in previous studies were found: linguistic development was strongly associated with responsiveness and teaching, and also encouragement; cognitive development was associated with encouragement, but not teaching, as expected. As also expected, parenting, as assessed using the PICCOLO, was not associated with motor skills. The literature about the parental behaviors assessed using the PICCOLO during play situations, such as book-reading, symbolic play, block constructions and so on, does not report any associations between these kinds of parental behaviors and motor skills.

Affection was not associated with any Bayley measure. This was not a totally expected result because, although the literature reports more conclusive data about the relationships between affection and social emotional development (Laible et al., 2000; Zhou et al., 2002; Caspi et al., 2004; Sanders et al., 2004), affection is especially relevant for the development of a secure attachment, and this in turn constitutes a good basis for general development (Mikulincer and Shaver, 2007; Thompson, 2008). Nevertheless, a larger sample of participants is needed to confirm these results.

It would also be interesting to explore predictive validity, correlating parenting measures with later measures of child development at time 2, as done by Roggman et al. (2013a,b). The authors found, among other results, that PICCOLO total and domain scores at ages 1 and 2 years were significantly associated with children’s cognitive development measured using the Bayley scales at age 3 years, and with linguistic development assessed at ages 3 and 5 using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III (Dunn and Dunn, 1997). In future studies, this aspect will be analyzed in a sample of about 100 normally developing children aged between 18 and 30 months, evaluating parenting at time 1 and relating this variable to developmental outcomes, assessed using the Bayley Scales, as already done in a small sample (Rivero et al., 2017).

This study has some limitations. First, our participants were all mothers; thus, our results might not be generalizable to other caregivers such as fathers or grandparents. In future studies the PICCOLO should be validated in other populations since gender differences in parental behaviors have been established (Cabrera et al., 2017). Secondly, our results come from observing mother-child interactions at home, so other settings where teacher interactions with young children take place should be tested as well (Jump Norman and Christiansen, 2013).

Furthermore, this study was conducted in families mostly belonging to the middle and upper middle classes, with a monthly family income and study level both higher than the average of the Spanish population. In comparison with the other published studies on validation of the PICCOLO -the original study from the United States and one from Turkey- our Spanish sample scored higher in all dimensions and at all ages, with the only exception of Affection in Turkey when children were 1 and 2 years of age. This could be explained by the higher level of maternal education in our sample, compared to the United States (Roggman et al., 2013b) and Turkey (Bayoğlu et al., 2013). A higher maternal educational level has been associated with parental practices, as mothers with a higher educational level show more parental behaviors that promote their children’s development (Bornstein et al., 2007; Brophy-Herb et al., 2012). Having a low income or limited education may certainly affect parenting behaviors (Roubinov and Boyce, 2017). The Brazilian-Portuguese validation is not comparable, since the sample included only children aged 18 months old.

Finally, although this study specifically concerned validation of the scale within a sample from Spain, replication of the validation and reliability procedures in other Spanish-speaking samples could extend the utility of this scale. We expect that the Spanish PICCOLO would be useful in other countries with substantial Spanish speaking populations.

Conclusion

For infants and toddlers, a responsive home environment that includes good parenting and positive parent-child interactions is important for child development. The Spanish version of the PICCOLO, an observational measure of parent-child interactions for infants and toddlers, meets the criteria for a reliable and valid measurement instrument of mothers’ parenting interactions with children aged from 10 to 47 months. The psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the PICCOLO make it appropriate for general research purposes but also for program evaluation of home-visiting and other parenting-support interventions in Spain. For early intervention professionals in Spain, an observational measure such as the PICCOLO may be very useful and can help to better establish and expand a family-centered approach in Early Interventions programs (Mas et al., 2016; Vilaseca et al., 2017). Following a collaborative model of working together with parents, it is possible to identify what parents are already doing well to support their children’s development through their daily routines (McWilliam, 2010, 2016) and practitioners can build on those strengths by helping parents to increase support for children’s development at home, both in those children at-risk for environmental reasons or in children with established disabilities.

Data Availability

The datasets for this study are not made publicly available because parents have allowed to our research team to video record their children, but not to publish any data or image.

Author Contributions

All authors RV, MR, RB, EN-P, MJC, FF, CVV, MI, and LR made substantial contributions to conception and design, and/or acquisition of data, and/or analysis and interpretation of data; and participated in drafting the article or revising it critically for important intellectual content; and gave final approval of the version to be submitted.

Funding

This research was supported by a grant from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness and the European Regional Development Fund (Project PSI2015-63627-R). The funding bodies have not imposed any restrictions on free access to or publication of the research data.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all participants, parents and families, and collaborating staff who took part in this research.

References

Alcantud, F., Alonso, Y., and Rico, D. (2015). Herramientas de cribado para la detección de retrasos o trastornos en el desarrollo: una revisión sistemática de la literatura. Revista Española de Discapacidad 3, 7–26. doi: 10.5569/2340-5104.03.02.01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, S., Roggman, L. A., Innocenti, M. S., and Cook, G. A. (2013). Dads’ parenting interactions with children: checklist of observations linked to outcomes (PICCOLO-D). Infant Ment. Health J. 34, 339–351. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21390

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aspland, H., and Gardner, F. (2003). Observational measures of parent-child interaction: an introductory review. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 8, 136–143. doi: 10.1111/1475-3588.00061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayley, N. (2015). Escalas Bayley de desarrollo Infantil (Bayley-III), 3 Edn. Madrid: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Bayoğlu, B., Unal,Ö, Elibol, F., Karabulut, E., and Innocenti, M. S. (2013). Turkish validation of the PICCOLO (Parenting interactions with children: checklist of observations linked to outcomes). Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 34, 330–338. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21393

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., Deschênes, M., and Matte-Gagne, C. (2012). Social factors in the development of early executive functioning: a closer look at the caregiving environment. Dev. Sci. 15, 12–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01093.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., and Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Dev. 81, 326–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bingham, G. E. (2007). Maternal literacy beliefs and the quality of mother-child book-reading interactions: associations with children’s early literacy development. Early Educ. Dev. 18, 23–49. doi: 10.1080/10409280701274428

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C., Raver, C. C., and Berry, D. J. (2014). Two approaches to estimating the effect of parenting on the development of executive function in early childhood. Dev. Psychol. 50, 554–565. doi: 10.1037/a0033647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bornstein, M., Hendricks, C., Haynes, O. M., and Painter, K. (2007). Maternal sensitivity and child responsiveness: associations with social context, maternal characteristics, and child characteristics in a multivariate analysis. Infancy 12, 189–223. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00240.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bornstein, M. H. (2013). Parenting and child mental health: a cross-cultural perspective. World Psychiatry 12, 258–265. doi: 10.1002/wps.20071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, R. H. (2015). Constructing and adapting causal and formative measures of family settings: the home inventory as illustration. J. Fam. Theory Rev. 7, 381–414. doi: 10.1111/jftr.12108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, R. H., and Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 371–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brophy-Herb, H., Stansbury, K., Bocknek, E., and Horodynski, M. (2012). Modeling maternal emotion-related socialization behaviors in a low-income sample: relations with toddlers’ self-regulation. Early Child Res. Q. 27, 352–364. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.11.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burchinal, M., Skinner, D., and Reznick, J. S. (2010). European american and african american mothers’ beliefs about parenting and disciplining infants: a mixed-method analysis. Parent. Sci. Pract. 10, 79–96. doi: 10.1080/15295190903212604

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabrera, J. N., Karberg, E., Malin, J. L., and Aldoney, D. (2017). The magic of play: low income mothers’ and fathers’ playfulness and children’s emotion regulation and vocabulary skills. Infant Ment. Health J. 38, 757–771. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caldwell, B. M., and Bradley, R. H. (2003). Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment: Administration Manual. Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas.

Google Scholar

Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Morgan, J., Rutter, M., Taylor, A., Arseneault, L., et al. (2004). Maternal expressed emotion predicts children’s antisocial behavior problems: using monozygotic-twin differences to identify environmental effects on behavioral development. Dev. Psychol. 40, 149–161. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, E., and Kim, S. (2015). Cronbach’s coefficient alpha: well known but poorly understood. Organ. Res. Methods 18, 207–230. doi: 10.1177/1094428114555994

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crouter, A. C., and Head, M. R. (2002). “Parental monitoring and knowledge of children,” in Handbook of Parenting 3: Being and Becoming a Parent, ed. M. H. Bornstein (New Jersey, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 461–483.

Google Scholar

Davidov, M., and Grusec, J. E. (2006). Untangling the links of parental responsiveness to distress and warmth to child outcomes. Child Dev. 77, 44–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00855.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVellis, R. F. (2016). Scale Development: Theory and Applications, 4th Edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Dunn, L. M., and Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd Edition) (PPVT-III). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

Google Scholar

Essau, C. A., Sasagawa, S., and Frick, P. J. (2006). Psychometric properties of the alabama parenting questionnaire. J. Child Fam. Stud. 15, 595–614. doi: 10.1007/s10826-006-9036-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farah, M. J., Betancourt, L., Shera, D. M., Savage, J. H., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L., et al. (2008). Environmental stimulation, parental nurturance and cognitive development in humans. Dev. Sci. 11, 793–801. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00688.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fay-Stammbach, T., Hawes, D. J., and Meredith, P. (2014). Parenting influences on executive function in early childhood: a review. Child Dev. Perspect. 8, 258–264. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forero, C. G., Maydeu-Olivares, A., and Gallardo-Pujol, D. (2009). Factor analysis with ordinal indicators: a monte carlo study comparing DWLS and ULS estimation. Struct. Equ. Model. 16, 625–641. doi: 10.1080/10705510903203573

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuligni, A. S., and Brooks-Gunn, J. (2013). Mother-child interactions in early head start: age and ethnic differences in low-income dyads. Parent. Sci. Pract. 13, 1–26. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2013.732422

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, F. (2000). Methodological issues in the direct observation of parent-child interaction: do observational findings reflect the natural behavior of participants? Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 3, 185–198. doi: 10.1023/A:1009503409699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gómez, E., and Muñoz, M. M. (2014). Manual de la Escala de Parentalidad Positiva. Santiago de Chile: Fundación Ideas para la Infancia.

Google Scholar

Graziano, P. A., Calkins, S. D., and Keane, S. P. (2011). Sustained attention development during the toddlerhood to preschool period: associations with toddlers’ emotion regulation strategies and maternal behaviour. Infant Child Dev. 20, 389–408. doi: 10.1002/icd.731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsh-Pasek, K., and Burchinal, M. (2006). Mother and caregiver sensitivity over time: predicting language and academic outcomes with variable and person-centered approaches. Merrill Palmer Q. 52, 449–485. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2006.0027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hubbs-Tait, L., Culp, A. M., Culp, R. E., and Miller, C. E. (2002). Relation of maternal cognitive stimulation, emotional support, and intrusive behavior during head start to children’s kindergarten cognitive abilities. Child Dev. 73, 110–131. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Innocenti, M. S., and Roggman, L. A. (2018). Understanding parent-child interaction globally: a model using the PICCOLO assessment tool. Zero Three 38, 60–63.

Google Scholar

Innocenti, M. S., Roggman, L. A., and Cook, G. A. (2013). Using the PICCOLO with parents of children with a disability. Infant Ment. Health J. 34, 307–318. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21394

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson-Newsom, J., Buchanan, C. M., and McDonald, R. M. (2008). Parenting and perceived maternal warmth in european american and african american adolescents. J. Marriage Fam. Couns. 70, 62–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00461

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joussemet, M., Koestner, R., Lekes, N., and Landry, R. (2005). A longitudinal study of the relationship of maternal autonomy support to children’s adjustment and achievement in school. J. Pers. 73, 1215–1236. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00347.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jump Norman, V., and Christiansen, K. (2013). Validity of the PICCOLO tool in child care settings: can it assess caregiver interaction behaviors? Infant Ment. Health J. 34, 319–329. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21391

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim-Cohen, J., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., and Taylor, A. (2004). Genetic and environmental processes in young children’s resilience and vulnerability to socioeconomic deprivation. Child Dev. 75, 651–668. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00699.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and Practice of Estructural Equation Modeling, 4th Edn. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Laible, D. J., Carlo, G., and Raffaelli, M. (2000). The differential relations of parent and peer attachment to adolescent adjustment. J. Youth Adolesc. 29, 45–59. doi: 10.1023/A:1005169004882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., and Swank, P. R. (2006). Responsive parenting: establishing early foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills. Dev. Psychol. 42, 627–642. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landry, S. H., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A., Smith, K. E., and Vellet, S. (2001). Does early responsive parenting have a special importance for children’s development or is consistency across early childhood necessary? Dev. Psychol. 37, 387–404. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.37.3.387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lansford, J. E., Sharma, C., Malone, P. S., Woodlief, D., Dodge, K. A., Oburu, P., et al. (2014). Corporal punishment, maternal warmth, and child adjustment: a longitudinal study in eight countries. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 43, 670–685. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.893518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Love, J. M., Kisker, E. E., Ross, C., Raikes, H., Constantine, J., Boller, K., et al. (2005). The effectiveness of early head start for 3-year-old children and their parents: lessons for policy and programs. Dev. Psychol. 41, 885–901. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.6.885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahoney, G. (2009). Relationship focused intervention (RFI): enhancing the role of parents in children’s developmental Intervention. Int. J. Early Child. Spec. Educ. 1, 79–94.

Google Scholar

Mas, J., Giné, C., and McWilliams, R. (2016). The adaptation process of families with children with intellectual disabilities in Catalonia. Infants Young Child 29, 335–351. doi: 10.1097/IYC.0000000000000077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McWilliam, R. A. (2010). Routines-Based Early Intervention. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.

Google Scholar

McWilliam, R. A. (2016). “Birth to three early intervention,” in Handbook of Early Childhood Special Education, eds B. Reichow, B. Boyd, E. Barton, and S. Odom (New York, NY: Springer), 75–88.

Google Scholar

Mikulincer, M., and Shaver, P. R. (2007). Boosting attachment security to promote mental health, prosocial values and inter-group tolerance. Psychol. Inq. 18, 139–156. doi: 10.1080/10478400701512646

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivero, M., Vilaseca, R., Bersabé, R. M., Cantero López, M. J., Navarro-Pardo, E., Valls-Vidal, C., et al. (2017). “Relations between parenting (PICCOLO) an developmental outcomes,” in Proceedings of the International Meeting: PICCOLO Across Cultures & Languages, Barcelona.

Google Scholar

Roggman, L. A., Cook, G. A., Innocenti, M. S., Jump Norman, V., Christiansen, K., and Anderson, S. (2013a). Parenting Interactions with Children: Checklist of Observations Linked to Outcomes (PICCOLO) User’s Guide. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.

Google Scholar

Roggman, L. A., Cook, G. A., Innocenti, M. S., Jump Norman, V., and Christiansen, K. (2013b). Parenting interactions with children: checklist of observations linked to outcomes (PICCOLO) in diverse ethnic groups. Infant Ment. Health J. 34, 290–306. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21389

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roseberry, S., Hish-Pasek, K., and Golinkoff, R. M. (2013). Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Dev. 85, 956–970. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: An R package for Structural Equation Modeling and More. Version 0.5-12 (BETA). Available at: https://users.ugent.be/∼yrosseel/lavaan/lavaanIntroduction.pdf

Google Scholar

Roubinov, D. S., and Boyce, W. T. (2017). Parenting and SES: relative values or enduring principles? Curr. Opin. Psychol. 15, 162–167. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanders, M. R., Mazzucchelli, T. G., and Studman, L. J. (2004). Stepping stones triple P: the theoretical basis and development of an evidence-based positive parenting program for families with a child who has a disability. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 29, 265–283. doi: 10.1080/13668250412331285127

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, A. (2018). Cross-Cultural Adaptation and Validation of Strengths-Based Parenting Measures in Brazil: PICCOLO and Cognitive Sensitivity Scale. Doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto, Toronto.

Google Scholar

Spiker, D., Boyce, G. C., and Boyce, L. K. (2002). Parent-child interactions when young children have disabilities. Int. Rev. Res. Ment. Retard. 25, 35–70. doi: 10.1016/S0074-7750(02)80005-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., and Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Dev. 72, 748–767. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y., and Song, L. (2014). Why is infant language learning facilitated by parental responsiveness? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23, 121–126. doi: 10.1177/0963721414522813

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, R. A. (2008). “Early attachment and later development: Familiar questions, new answers,” in Handbook of attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Applications, 2nd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 348–365.

Google Scholar

Trenado, R. M., and Cerezo, M. A. (2007). Codificación de la Interacción Temprana Materno Infantil en su versión revisada, CITMI-R. Unpublished document, Universitat de València, Spain.

Google Scholar

Trenado, R. M., Pons-Salvador, G., and Cerezo, M. A. (2014). Interacción temprana: evaluación de la fiabilidad del sistema observacional CITMI-R, versión inglesa/early interaction: reliability study of the citmi-r coding system, english version. Revista Electrónica de Metodología Aplicada 19, 29–42.

Google Scholar

Tuttle, A. R., Knudson-Martin, C., and Kim, L. (2012). Parenting as relationship: a framework for assessment and practice. Fam. Process. 51, 73–89. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2012.01383.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vargas-Rubilar, J., and Arán-Filippetti, V. (2014). The importance of parenthood for the child’s cognitive development: a theoretical revision. Revista Latinoamericana de Ciencias Sociales, Niñez y Juventud 12, 171–186. doi: 10.11600/1692715x.1219110813

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vázquez, N., Molina, M. C., Ramos, P., and Artazcoz, L. (2016). Validación de un instrumento en español para medir habilidades parentales promovidas en una intervención de educación parental. REIRE Revista d’Innovació i Recerca en Educació 9, 30–47. doi: 10.1344/reire2016.9.2923

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Velasco, D., Sánchez de Miguel, M. S., Egurza, M., Arranz, E., Aranbarri, A., Fano, E., et al. (2014). Family context assessment in a públic health study. Gac. Sanit. 28, 356–362. doi: 10.1016/j.gaceta.2014.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vilaseca, R., Rivero, M., Ferrer, F., Bersabé, R. M., Navarro-Pardo, E., Cantero, M. J., et al. (2017). “Using PICCOLO to promote a positive parenting that leads to better outcomes in children with disabilities and improves the emotional well-being of families,” in Proceedings of the Eurlyaid (European Association on Early Childhood Intervention) Conference 2017, Belgrade.

Google Scholar

Warren, S. F., Brady, N., Sterling, A., Fleming, K., and Marquis, J. (2010). Maternal responsivity predicts language development in young children with Fragile X Syndrome. Am. J. Intell. Dev. Disabil. 115, 54–75. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-115.1.54

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winstanley, A., and Gattis, M. (2013). The baby care questionnaire: a measure of parenting principles and practices during infancy. Infant Behav. Dev. 36, 762–775. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaslow, M., Halle, T., Martin, L., Cabrera, N., Calkins, J., Pitzer, L., et al. (2006). Child outcome measures in the study of child care quality. Eval. Rev. 30, 577–610. doi: 10.1177/0193841X06291529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., Losoya, S. H., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Guthrie, I. K., et al. (2002). The relations of parental warmth and positive expressiveness to children’s empathy related responding and social functioning: a longitudinal study. Child Dev. 73, 893–915. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: PICCOLO, Spanish version, parenting interactions, observational scale, psychometric properties

Citation: Vilaseca R, Rivero M, Bersabé RM, Navarro-Pardo E, Cantero MJ, Ferrer F, Valls Vidal C, Innocenti MS and Roggman L (2019) Spanish Validation of the PICCOLO (Parenting Interactions With Children: Checklist of Observations Linked to Outcomes). Front. Psychol. 10:680. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00680

Received: 08 October 2018; Accepted: 11 March 2019;
Published: 27 March 2019.

Edited by:

Gabrielle Strouse, University of South Dakota, United States

Reviewed by:

Kristel Thomassin, University of Guelph, Canada
Lorenzo Giusti, Eugenio Medea (IRCCS), Italy

Copyright © 2019 Vilaseca, Rivero, Bersabé, Navarro-Pardo, Cantero, Ferrer, Valls Vidal, Innocenti and Roggman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Esperanza Navarro-Pardo, esperanza.navarro@uv.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.