Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 21 December 2018
Sec. Eating Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic Eating Behavior Research in Children’s and Adolescents' Naturalistic Environment View all 7 articles

Stress and Eating Behavior: A Daily Diary Study in Youngsters

  • 1Department of Developmental, Personality and Social Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
  • 2Department of Public Health, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium

Background: Overweight and obesity are growing problems, with more attention recently, to the role of stress in the starting and maintaining process of these clinical problems. However, the mechanisms are not yet known and well-understood; and ecological momentary analyses like the daily variations between stress and eating are far less studied. Emotional eating is highly prevalent and is assumed to be an important mechanism, as a maladaptive emotion regulation (ER) strategy, in starting and maintaining the vicious cycle of (pediatric) obesity.

Objectives: The present study aims to investigate in youngsters (10 – 17 years) the daily relationship between stress and the trajectories of self-reported eating behavior (desire to eat motives; hunger eating motives and snacking) throughout 1 week; as well as the moderating role of emotion regulation and emotional eating in an average weight population.

Methods: Participants were 109 average weighted youngsters between the age of 10 and 17 years (Mage = 13.49; SD = 1.64). The youngsters filled in a trait-questionnaire on emotion regulation and emotional eating at home before starting the study, and answered an online diary after school time, during seven consecutive days. Desire to eat motives, hunger eating motives and snacking were assessed daily for seven consecutive days.

Results: Using multilevel analyses results revealed that daily stress is significantly associated with trajectories of desire to eat motives and hunger eating motives. No evidence was found for the moderating role of maladaptive ER in these relationships; marginally significant evidence was found for the moderating role of emotional eating in the trajectories of desire to eat and snacking.

Discussion: These results stress the importance of looking into the daily relationship between stress and eating behavior parameters, as both are related with change over and within days. More research is needed to draw firm conclusion on the moderating role of ER strategies and emotional eating.

Introduction

Pediatric overweight and obesity are growing problems, as the prevalence of obesity is tripled since 1975. Worldwide, 18% of the children and adolescents (5–19 years) are diagnosed with overweight and 8% with obesity (World Health Organization [WHO], 2018). Parallel to the global rise in pediatric obesity, metabolic, psychological and social health problems are beginning to appear in childhood; and positive associations between these phenomena are found (Braet et al., 1997; Zipper et al., 2001; Reilly et al., 2003; Erermis et al., 2004; Lobstein et al., 2004; Reilly and Kelly, 2011). Moreover, these diseases are known to track into adulthood (Ferraro and Kelley-Moore, 2003; Reilly et al., 2003; Lifshitz, 2008; Reilly and Kelly, 2011). This progress and the assumed comorbidities emphasize the importance of studying the underlying mechanisms leading to childhood obesity. Psychological and psychophysiological research in this field emphasizes the influence of stress on eating and weight regulation.

Stress Conceptualization

Psychological stress occurs when a person appraises a situation as significant for his or her welfare and when the situation exceeds his or her available coping resources (Lazarus and Folkman, 1986). This definition implicates a subjective component that can be captured only through self-report (Krohne, 2002). Besides, the emotional value of the stressor needs to be taken into account. Lazarus (1993) states that stress is closely related to the conceptualization of emotions because higher levels of stress are consistently associated with higher negative affect but so far, specifically in childhood, this link is less studied in the context of emotional eating (Cohen et al., 1995; Watson, 1998; Lemyre and Tessier, 2003; Mroczek and Almeida, 2004; Minkel et al., 2012; Gustafsson et al., 2013; Bey et al., 2018).

As our stress levels know day-level fluctuations within persons (Beattie and Griffin, 2014), these should be studied as ‘daily hassles,’ which are situations, thoughts or events leading to negative feelings (e.g., annoyance, irritation, worry or frustration) when they occur and inform you on the difficulty or impossibility to achieve your goals and plans (O’Connor et al., 2008).

Stress is associated with the development of different medical and psychological disorders (Salim, 2014). Pertinent to the current study, research in children and adolescents proved that the experience of negative affect and stress is associated with weight gain, a higher waist circumference, a higher BMI and in the long term obesity (Goodman and Whitaker, 2002; Koch et al., 2008; De Vriendt et al., 2009; Midei and Matthews, 2009; Rofey et al., 2009; Van Jaarsveld et al., 2009; Aparicio et al., 2016).

Stress and Eating Behavior

Psychological research shows that stress is associated with overweight and obesity through changes in weight-related health behavior, as stress activates emotional brain networks and elevates the secretion of glucocorticoids and insulin. Both the emotional brain networks and the hormones influence different aspects of our eating behavior such as our food intake, food choice and eating motives (hunger and desire eating) (O’Connor et al., 2008; Dallman, 2010; Jastreboff et al., 2013).

First, regarding food intake, stress may result in under- or overeating depending on the stress source and stress intensity (Willenbring et al., 1986; Steere and Cooper, 1993; O’Connor et al., 2008; Ansari and Berg-Beckhoff, 2015). Concerning undereating, adults and children are often found to eat less after heavy stressful events and, specifically, family stress is associated with underweight (BMI) (Popper et al., 1989; Stone and Brownell, 1994; Oliver and Wardle, 1999; Stenhammar et al., 2010). However, evidence for overeating after experiencing stress is also found. Next to the detected underweight after family stress, Stenhammar et al. (2010) also report possible overweight after family stress, suggesting a link with respectively under- and overeating. Besides, Evers et al. (2010) and Vandewalle et al. (2017b) show in youngsters how induced negative affect leads to an increased food intake, specifically comfort foods.

Second, concerning food choice, stressor intensity is an important factor. In adults, chronic life stress is associated with the intake of more energy-dense food (Steptoe et al., 1998; Torres and Nowson, 2007; O’Connor et al., 2008) a lower consumption of main meals and vegetables (O’Connor et al., 2008). In a systematic review and meta-analysis on children, Hill et al. (2018) report that stress, measured via self-reports and via cortisol measures, is associated with the intake of more unhealthy and less healthy food items (8–18 years).

Third, concerning eating motives, De Vriendt et al. (2009) conclude in their review that in adolescents, stress is specifically associated with increased appetite, which can be seen as a desire for food. Therefore, a distinction should be made between ‘hunger eating motives’ (=eating out of hunger) and ‘desire to eat motives’ (=eating out of a desire to eat; eating out of a craving for food), with the latter defined as rather unhealthy eating (Reichenberger et al., 2016, 2018). Here, a recent EMA study in adults finds that time pressure is associated with more hunger eating (Reichenberger et al., 2016). Goldschmidt et al. (2018) shows in the only available ecologically momentary assessment study (EMA = looking into the current real time behaviors) in obese children in their natural environment that specifically ‘desire to eat motives’ were of importance regarding overeating.

These ecological momentary assessment results (O’Connor et al., 2008; Reichenberger et al., 2016) stress the importance of looking into the daily fluctuations in within-person stress levels to better understand the precise role of different daily stressors in eating behavior, on top of the existing measurements of stress. Besides, as eating behaviors are a daily occupation, and appear in different contexts, a diary study can further help to capture the relationship between daily hassles and different indicators of eating behavior.

However, this research in children and adolescents is lacking, but highly needed and relevant as (1) adolescence is an important developmental stage (Giedd et al., 2009); (2) Goldschmidt et al. (2018) showed the importance of daily desire to eat motives in obese youngsters and (3) daily stress is positively associated with daily fluctuations in scores on emotional eating (Vandewalle et al., 2017a). It is hypothesized that stress- induced eating or emotional eating, defined as “eating your negative emotions away,” can be seen as a maladaptive emotion regulation (ER) strategy in children also, but this remains to be further explored (Braet and Van Strien, 1997; Thayer, 2001; Evers et al., 2010).

Role of Emotion Regulation Processes

Emotion Regulation Strategies

Emotion regulation refers to the actions by which persons try to influence their emotions, more specifically which emotions they experience, when and how they experience the emotions and how they show their emotions to others (Gross, 1998). Emotion regulation strategies are studied most often in contexts in which persons upregulate their (stress related) negative emotions. Emotion regulation strategies influence eating behavior and health behavior (Whiteside et al., 2007; Evers et al., 2010; Koenders and Van Strien, 2011), and weight gain and obesity (De Vriendt et al., 2009). This association is stronger when maladaptive emotion regulation strategies are used to deal with the stressor in comparison with the use of adaptive emotion regulation strategies (Aparicio et al., 2016). In a longitudinal perspective, toddlers with lower levels of emotion regulation skills (i.e., more negative emotion reactions and less adequate emotion regulation) have more overweight at the age of 10 (Graziano et al., 2014), suggesting that difficulties in emotion regulation skills in 2 until 5 years old is related to the development of pediatric obesity later in life (Graziano et al., 2010). Using maladaptive emotion regulation strategies is found to mediate the association between the stressor of maternal rejection and emotional eating (measured as a trait, with the DEBQ-questionnaire) in obese youngsters (Vandewalle et al., 2014). On the other hand, adaptive emotion regulation strategies are related with positive health behaviors such as a higher intake of fruits and vegetables and more physical activity (Isasi et al., 2013). Remarkably, whether people with high/low maladaptive emotion regulation strategies cope differently with stress is less studied.

Emotional Eating as Emotion Regulation Strategy

Emotional eating is highly prevalent in community-wide youth samples (Michels et al., 2012) and in youth with obesity (Braet and Beyers, 2009). Besides, emotional eating is treatment resistant, as it often persists after obesity treatment (Vandewalle et al., 2018). Emotional eating can occur in the presence of a lot of different emotions (Faith et al., 1997) and again, is most studied regarding stress-related emotions. In children, high scores on emotional eating have been associated with both negative feelings on physical competencies (Braet and Van Strien, 1997) and experienced stress (Nguyen-Rodriguez et al., 2009; Michels et al., 2012). Emotional eating has a short term reinforcing effect, by reducing stress-related arousal and negative affect (Popless-Vawter et al., 1998; Macht, 2008). However, in the long-term using the strategy of ‘emotional eating’ in particular in obese persons, often instills feelings of guilt and self-anger (Popless-Vawter et al., 1998), thereby intensifying the existing negative emotions (Popless-Vawter et al., 1998; Gibson, 2006). Even more important, emotional eating will not solve the stress origins: as long as people only cope with their stress by means of emotional eating, the stressor will remain and cause psychological discomfort (Stice et al., 2002; Gibson, 2006). This way, emotional eating can be seen as a proxy of maladaptive emotion regulation (Michopoulos et al., 2015).

Emotional eating is associated with more calorie-intake (Braet and Van Strien, 1997). This is of concern as emotional eating seems to evolve toward a stable trait component later in life and is of greater importance compared with other life style behaviors for explaining longitudinal weight gain (Koenders and Van Strien, 2011). Persons with a high emotional eating style who are confronted with a stressor eat more high-fat food and more energy-dense meals in comparison with persons who have a low emotional eating style (Oliver et al., 2000). O’Connor et al. (2008) concludes in an EMA-study that the confrontation with daily hassles is associated with more snacking, and this is stronger for individuals who have a high emotional eating style. Interesting, the presence of negative emotions decreases the desire eating in individuals with a low emotional eating style; while the negative emotions do not affect the desire eating in individuals with a high emotional eating style (Reichenberger et al., 2016). This suggests that emotional eating evolves toward a stable trait that might be a moderator in the stress-eating relation.

Research Questions and Daily Diary-Design

The current study is innovative in different ways: (1) by including children and adolescents, (2) by using a daily diary design during seven consecutive days and (3) by including three indicators of eating behavior; daily hunger eating motives, desire to eat motives and snacking; and their moderating factors, emotion regulation and emotional eating. However, as using a cellphone during school time is often forbidden, the eating motives and snacking during the day will be reported after school time, which is a well-known critical phase with both school related and peer related stress (Sotardi, 2017). The research question is shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Research question.

First, we hypothesize that emotional eating will be a proxy of maladaptive emotion regulation (based on Michopoulos et al., 2015). Second, we state that daily stress will be significantly associated with (1) the trajectories in desire to eat (2) the trajectories of hunger-eating and (3) the trajectories of snacking (Based on De Vriendt et al., 2009; Reichenberger et al., 2016; Vandewalle et al., 2017a). We also hypothesize that these relations will be stronger for youth high in trait maladaptive emotion regulation and/or reporting a more pronounced emotional eating style (O’Connor et al., 2008; Reichenberger et al., 2016).

Methodology

Participants

Dutch- speaking children and adolescents were recruited in Belgium, Aalter and Deinze. These youngsters already participated in two previous studies: Generation 2020 study (Van Beveren et al., 2016, 2017) and the Reward- study (De Decker et al., 2017a,b). These are both longitudinal studies with different data collection waves over time on the emotional wellbeing of children and adolescents. During a follow-up data collection in both cohorts, participants were recruited for the current study. A total of 109 participants aged 10–17 years were recruited [Mage = 13.49; SD = 1.64; 51 boys (46.8%) and 57 girls (52.3%)]; one participant did not report their age and gender.

Study Design

Before the diary study, the participants were asked to fill out questionnaires on eating behavior and emotion regulation. Following this, the participants were required to complete the stress/eating behavior diary over seven consecutive days. EMA measures were similar to the study of Reichenberger et al. (2016), but extended by adding the variable of snacking and the role of emotion regulation (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Measurements.

The diary was filled out before breakfast, after school time, when the participants came home from school and before bedtime. In the current study, only the data time point after school was used, which was in most cases filled in between 5 PM and 6 PM. At 5 PM the participants received a reminder message to fill out the diary. Participants were asked to report on their snacking behavior since breakfast.

Trait Variables by Questionnaires

FEEL-KJ

The Fragebogen Zur Erhebung der Emotionsregulation Bei Kindern und Jugendlichen (FEEL-KJ, Grob and Smolenski, 2005; A Questionnaire on Emotion Regulation Strategies in Children and Adolescents) is translated from German to Dutch and can be used in children and adolescents between 8 and 18 years old (Braet et al., 2013). The questionnaire measures ER strategies for three emotions: anger, anxiety and sadness. The emotion regulation strategies for every emotion are captured by 30 items, thus the total amount of items is 90. The 15 emotion regulation strategies are divided in three categories of strategies: adaptive, maladaptive and external regulation. The adaptive emotion regulation strategies are distraction, recall a positive emotion, forget, acceptance, problem solving, re-evaluation of the situation, and cognitive reappraisal. The maladaptive strategies are giving up, withdrawing, self- devaluation, rumination and aggression. Looking for social support, controlling your emotions and expressing your emotions are the external strategies. For the Dutch and Flemish population, representative norms are available (Braet et al., 2013). The psychometric qualities of the FEEL-KJ are drawn from a large sample of Dutch speaking Belgian children and adolescents between 8 and 18 years old. A good reliability and validity was found by Cracco et al. (2015). In the current study, a good reliability was also found for both subscales adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation, respectively α = 0.957 and α = 0.831.

Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire

The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ, van Strien et al., 1986; Braet et al., 2008) assesses three eating styles: restrained eating, external eating and emotional eating. In this study only the subscale ‘emotional eating’ will be taken into account. The DEBQ contains 33 items, and is a self-report questionnaire. The items question specific eating behaviors, and should be rated on a five-point- Likert scale from 1 = never to 5 = very often. The DEBQ has been shown useful in research with children and adolescents between 7 and 17 years (Braet et al., 2003). Also, good psychometric qualities are reported, such as a good reliability and external validity (Ricciardelli and McCabe, 2001; Braet et al., 2008). The emotional eating subscale contains 13 items and has a good internal validity in overweight and normal-weight children (respectively α = 0.91 and α = 0.93) (Braet et al., 2008). In the current study, a good reliability for emotional eating was found (α = 0.946).

Diary-Measures

Stress intensity

To measure the amount of stress during a particular school day, the participants were asked to rate ‘how stressed did you feel since your breakfast?’ on a visual analog scale from 0 (not at all stressed) to 100 (extremely stressed). By asking about the period since breakfast until after school, we wanted to capture stress related to school performances and peer-related stress (social stress).

Snacking and motives of eating behavior

The after school diary questions always start with the question ‘How many snacks did you eat since breakfast?’. A snack was defined as ‘healthy and unhealthy food and/or drinks, which were not part of your main meals, e.g., lunch’. Participants had the option of choosing between ‘0 or 1,’ ‘2,’ ‘3,’ and ‘4.’ For every snack, they needed to fill out what they ate as a snack and how many, e.g., 1 apple or 1 large snicker. Using the Nobelguide, two researchers scored and discussed until agreement on the amount of Kcal, and this was double checked by a dietitian, in the case of non-accordance. For every reported snacks, the hunger eating motive and the desire to eat motive was measured using the following questions: ‘rate the extent to which you agree with the following statement of “I am hungry”’ and ‘rate the extent to which you agree with the following statement of “I desire to eat”.’ Both statements were rated on a 5-point Likert scale with 1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree. In the analysis, a calculated mean score on the amount of snacks (kcal) and a calculated mean score on hunger and desire eating motives was used.

Control variables

As control variables, age and gender were taken into account.

Reactivity Measures

It is important to know if the act of completing the daily diary impacted the behavior of the participant (reactivity, e.g., if the participants knew they had to fill out all the questions related to every snack, they might have eaten fewer snacks than normal). Therefore, at the end of the current study, reactivity was measured with a single question regarding the influence the diary had on their eating behavior. The responses were rated on a 7-point Likert scale with 1 = I ate much less, 2 = Sometimes I ate less, 3 = I ate a little bit less, 4 = filling in the diary had no effect on my eating behavior, 5 = I ate a bit more, 6 = sometimes I ate more and 7 = I ate much more.

Procedure

Children and adolescents between 10 and 17 years old were recruited as part of a larger research project (Emotion Regulation in the prevention and treatment of obesity in young adolescents).

When participants and their parents agreed on participating in the diary-study, a home visit was planned. During the first home visit the informed consents were signed by the youngsters and their parents, the trait questionnaires were explained and a smartphone was given to the participants to fill out the diary. Together with them, an example of 1 diary assessment point was filled out. Participants had to fill out the diary for 7 days, with the assessment points after breakfast, after school and before bedtime. In the current study, only the measurement point after school time is used to answer the research questions. Collecting data during this limited window of time, seven consecutive days, was based on other recent studies in youth (Hruska et al., 2017; Vandewalle et al., 2017a; Mabbe et al., 2018).

During the diary- data collection, the participants received at every assessment point an e-mail with the link to the diary questions as a reminder for filling in the diary. To log in on the website, every participant received a unique code, which guaranteed the anonymity of the participants. The diary questions were also given in a written format; in case the online version would give problems. When the participant did not fill in the diary three consecutive times, e-mails with the links were not sent any more to this person. At the fourth day of the diary-study, the participants received a phone call to ask how the study went, if there were difficulties and to encourage them to carry on until day 7 including an integrity check whereby the snack registration of that day was asked.

After the study, a second home visit was planned to collect the trait questionnaires and smartphones. During the second home visit, the reactivity measure was asked to the participants; and they received a film ticket as reward.

This study was approved by the Ethical Committee of Ghent University.

Data-Analysis

Data were analyzed within a multilevel framework using MLwiN (Rasbash et al., 2009) because the seven consecutive days (Level 1) were nested within the individuals (Level 2). In following Nezlek (2011) recommendations, variables at Level 1 reflecting within-youth predictors were centered around the individual’s mean (group-mean centered; Enders and Tofighi, 2007; Nezlek, 2011) in order to eliminate the influence of Level 2 differences in the predictors for the analyses. Predictors at Level 2 reflecting between-youth predictors were centered around the grand mean (grand-mean centered) to improve the interpretation of the intercept values (Nezlek, 2011).

As proposed by Singer and Willet (2003), we fitted two initial models, namely the random intercept-only model and the unconditional growth model for daily desire to eat, hunger eating, and snacking prior to our main analyses. First, random-intercept-only models (Null Models) were used to estimate the variance partitioning coefficient (VPC), which reflects the proportion of variance in the dependent variable situated between youth (Level 2). In a second step, unconditional growth models (Singer and Willet, 2003) were built stepwise from the Null Models by adding Time as a Level 1 predictor, reflecting whether on average there was change over time. Furthermore, we tested whether the growth rate varied between individuals by modeling the coefficient of Time random at Level 2 (i.e., random slope; unconditional growth models). The likelihood ratio test (LRT) was used to compare the goodness of fit of nested models. To evaluate the significance of the fixed and random effects the Wald test was used. Gender, age, and adjusted BMI were included as covariates of no interest in all undermentioned analyses. To test whether daily stress and trait maladaptive emotion regulation interact in explaining trajectories of desire to eat, hunger eating, and snacking respectively we extended the baseline growth models for these variables by adding daily stress, trait maladaptive emotion regulation, and their interaction to the unconditional growth models.

Results

Throughout the week, a decreasing compliance for filling out the online diary was observed, with respectively 96, 83, and 77 youngsters filling out the diary at day 1, day 4, and day 7.

Means, standard deviations, and correlations between all variables are shown in Table 1. Mean scores of all variables in the current sample are comparable to findings of previous studies in youth samples (Braet and Beyers, 2009; Braet et al., 2013). When comparing the mean scores for emotional eating and emotion regulation with the gender- and age specific norms of the trait questionnaires (DEBQ and FEEL-KJ), all mean scores are not significantly different from the norm scores; except for the use of maladaptive emotion regulation strategies in boys between 13 and 17 years old [t(32) = 3.09; p < 0.01] (Table 2; Braet and Beyers, 2009; Braet et al., 2013). Comparing the daily mean score for kcal-intake of snacking with previous research in girls and boys between 9 and 14 years (Field et al., 2004), the mean score was significantly lower from the mean-score in their research.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Comparison of means with gender- and age specific norm scores.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Variable correlations and descriptives.

Maladaptive emotion regulation was positively correlated with emotional eating, both measured as trait variables (r = 0.304, p < 0.01) confirming the presumption that emotional eating functions as a proxy of maladaptive emotion regulation. Additionally, all daily measurements were significantly and positively correlated, except for daily stress with daily snacking. Higher levels of daily stress were significantly associated with higher levels of daily desire to eat and hunger eating (r = 0.267 and r = 0.274, p < 0.01, respectively) and all three indicators of eating behavior were significantly correlated (Table 1). Concerning the hunger eating motives and desire to eat motives, the multicollinearity was checked by the variance inflation factor (VIF). With VIF = 1.00, it was concluded that both constructs did not overlap substantially, and thus multicollinearity was not a problem.

Reactivity Measure

Concerning the reactivity measures, 98 participants filled out this question at the second home visit. Unfortunately, in 11 participants the reactivity measure was missing, due to the fact that the participants were not at home at the time of the second home visit. On the 7-point Likert scale the minimum score indicated by the participants was 2 (“Sometimes I ate less due to filling out the diary”) and the maximum score 5 (“I ate a little bit more due to filling out the diary”), respectively by 4 (3.7%) and 2 (1.8%) participants. The major group of participants stated that “filling out the diary had no influence on their snacking behavior” (Likert-score 4) (n = 82; 75.2%), and 9.2% (n = 10) stated that “they ate a little bit less due to filling out the diary” (Likert-score 3).

Between and Within Youth Variance of Stress and Self-Reported Eating Behavior

We first fitted random intercept-only models (i.e., Null Models) for core study variables as a baseline for more complex models. Variance estimates and standard errors (SE) are depicted in Table 3 (desire to eat), Table 4 (hunger eating), and Table 5 (snacking). For all variables, the between-youth and within-youth variance components were significant. The VPC indicated that 85.08% of the variance in daily desire to eat, 84.86% of the variance in daily hunger eating and 80.87% of the variance in daily snacking was situated between youth. Before running the main analyses, we fitted unconditional growth models for core study variables. These models revealed that levels of desire to eat, hunger eating, as well as snacking generally decreased as the week progressed.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Daily desire to eat as a function of stress, maladaptive emotion regulation, and trait emotional eating.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Daily hunger eating as a function of stress, maladaptive emotion regulation, and trait emotional eating.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Daily snacking as a function of stress, maladaptive emotion regulation, and trait emotional eating.

In addition, we fitted a random intercept-only model for the independent variable stress. Again, the between-youth [σu02 = 217.33, SE = 41.44, χ2(1) = 27.51, p < 0.001] and within-youth [σe02 = 328.37, SE = 21.99, χ2(1) = 223.06, p < 0.001] variance components were significant. The VPC indicated that 39.83% of the variance in daily stress was situated between youth. Furthermore, the unconditional growth model for stress revealed that stress-levels generally decreased as the week progressed.

Do Stress and Maladaptive Emotion Regulation Interact in Explaining Trajectories of Daily Desire to Eat?

To test our hypothesis, we extended the unconditional growth model for daily desire to eat (Model 1; Table 3) by adding stress and emotion regulation, as well as their interaction, to the model. Interestingly, this model (Model 2; Table 3) resulted in a significant improvement in model fit, χ2(6) = 18.49, p = 0.005. Inspection of the individual Wald test showed that daily stress significantly predict trajectories of desire to eat χ2(1) = 11.74, p < 0.001, revealing that with higher levels of stress, desire to eat showed a less steep decrease throughout the week whereas maladaptive emotion regulation χ2(1) = 0.95, p = 0.329, as well as the interaction χ2(1) = 0.19, p = 0.663 were not significantly predictive for these trajectories.

Do Stress and Maladaptive Emotion Regulation Interact in Explaining Trajectories of Hunger Eating?

We extended the unconditional growth model for daily hunger eating (Model 1; Table 4) by adding stress and emotion regulation, as well as their interaction, to the model. Interestingly, this model (Model 2; Table 4) resulted in a significant improvement in model fit, χ2(6) = 15.25, p = 0.018. Inspection of the individual Wald test showed that daily stress significantly predicted trajectories of hunger eating χ2(1) = 13.02, p < 0.001, revealing that with higher levels of stress, hunger eating behavior showed a less steep decrease throughout the week whereas maladaptive emotion regulation χ2(1) = 0.33, p = 0.563, as well as the interaction χ2(1) = 0.18, p = 0.669 were not significantly predictive for these trajectories.

Do Stress and Emotion Regulation Interact in Explaining Trajectories of Daily Snacking?

We extended the unconditional growth model for daily snacking (Model 1; Table 5) by adding stress and emotion regulation, as well as their interaction, to the model. Interestingly, this model (Model 2; Table 5) resulted in a significant improvement in model fit, χ2(6) = 15.23, p = 0.019. Inspection of the individual Wald test showed that daily stress χ2(1) = 0.87, p = 0.351, maladaptive emotion regulation χ2(1) = 1.46, p = 0.226, as well as the interaction χ2(1) = 0.37, p = 0.542 were not significantly predictive for these trajectories.

Do Stress and Trait Emotional Eating Interact in Explaining Trajectories of Desire to Eat?

We extended the unconditional growth model for daily desire to eat (Model 1; Table 3) by adding stress and emotional eating, as well as their interaction, to the model. Interestingly, this model (Model 3; Table 3) resulted in a significant improvement in model fit, χ2(6) = 20.52, p = 0.002. Inspection of the individual Wald test showed that the effect of emotional eating on trajectories of desire to eat was marginally significant χ2(1) = 2.97, p = 0.085, revealing that with higher levels of emotional eating, desire to eat showed a less steep decrease throughout the week. The interaction between daily stress and emotional eating was not significant χ2(1) = 0.33, p = 0.566.

Do Stress and Trait Emotional Eating Interact in Explaining Trajectories of Hunger Eating?

We extended the unconditional growth model for daily hunger eating (Model 1; Table 4) by adding stress and emotional eating, as well as their interaction, to the model. Interestingly, this model (Model 3; Table 4) resulted in a significant improvement in model fit, χ2(6) = 14.94, p = 0.021. Inspection of the individual Wald test showed that despite the significant effect of daily stress, emotional eating χ2(1) = 0.14, p = 0.707, as well as the interaction between stress and emotional eating, χ2(1) = 0.03, p = 0.862, was not significantly predictive for these trajectories.

Do Stress and Trait Emotional Eating Interact in Explaining Trajectories of Daily Snacking?

We extended the unconditional growth model for daily snacking (Model 1; Table 5) by adding stress and EE, as well as their interaction, to the model. Interestingly, this model (Model 3; Table 5) resulted in a significant improvement in model fit, χ2(6) = 139.69, p < 0.001. Inspection of the individual Wald test showed that despite the significant effect of daily stress, emotional eating χ2(1) = 0.72, p = 0.396, was not significantly predictive for these trajectories. The interaction term between stress and emotional eating was only marginally significant χ2(1) = 2.66, p = 0.102.

Discussion

The current study aimed to investigate the relationship between daily stress and eating behavior in children and adolescents (10–17 years old) using a daily diary-design. Combining a daily diary-design with the use of self-report trait questionnaires allowed us to look into the moderating role of emotion regulation and emotional eating.

First, we found that maladaptive emotion regulation was positively correlated with emotional eating, meaning emotional eating can be seen as a proxy of maladaptive emotion regulation, which is in line with Evers et al. (2010) and Michopoulos et al. (2015). Besides, daily stress was positively correlated with daily desire to eat and hunger eating; but not with daily snacking; and these three indicators of daily eating behavior were all significantly positively correlated. The positive correlation between daily stress on the one hand and desire to eat motives and hunger eating motives on the other hand was expected, as (1) the item on ‘desire to eat motives’ is seen as a desire or craving for food after experiencing stress and thus may be a way of regulating the experienced stress and (2) the item on ‘hunger to eat motives’ is seen as eating out of hunger after experiencing stress; and previous research showed that stress, operationalized as a hyperactive cortisol axis, contributes to higher levels of hunger feelings (Gluck et al., 2004; Goldschmidt et al., 2018). The correlations are in line with the reported positive associations between levels of stress and levels of desire to eat- and hunger eating motives by De Vriendt et al. (2009) in children and Reichenberger et al. (2016) in adults. In contrary to previous findings (O’Connor et al., 2008; Reichenberger et al., 2016), we could not find an association between daily stress and snacking; maybe due to specific characteristics of the study population: children instead of adults. Children and youngsters might not have the autonomy to decide what they will eat, especially snacks. Gevers et al. (2015) found that most parents both communicate about food and at the same time restrict their children on snacking behavior.

All three indicators of eating behavior showed bilateral significant positive correlations, which is in line with our expectations. Although the motivation differs in desire and hunger eating, a positive correlation is not unexpected since in both hedonic and homeostatic eating the ghrelin secretion (hunger hormone) is elevated, and the cholecystokinin-33 secretion (satiety hormone) is decreased (Monteleone et al., 2013). Concluding, variables measured on trait questionnaires (FEEL-KJ and DEBQ) correlated bilateral; and the variables measured on a daily basis (daily stress and the indicators of eating behavior) correlated with each other, but no correlations between the trait variables and daily measurements were found. For this, a potential explanation could be found in that the used questionnaires were not sensitive enough for capturing these momentary daily fluctuations.

Second, we found evidence for the hypothesis that daily stress was significantly associated with the trajectories of desire to eat and hunger eating motives, which is in line with De Vriendt et al. (2009) and Reichenberger et al. (2016). With higher levels of daily stress, desire to eat and hunger eating motives showed a less steep decrease throughout the week. As the trajectories of the eating behavior indicators decreased significantly throughout the week for every participant, the steepness of these decreases were of interest. The decreases of the hunger eating motives trajectories and of the desire to eat motives trajectories were significantly less steep in persons reporting higher levels of stress than in persons reporting lower levels of stress. This effect was not found for snacking.

Third, for both eating motives and snacking, no effect of maladaptive emotion regulation nor an interaction between daily stress and maladaptive emotion regulation was found. These findings are in contradiction with Evers et al. (2010), Vandewalle et al. (2014), and Aparicio et al. (2016). A possible explanation is the methodology of the study, as above mentioned studies are longitudinal, cross-sectional (questionnaires) or experimental studies while the current research is a diary study. Still, the moderating factor, maladaptive emotion regulation, was measured as a trait variable and therefore might not be sensitive enough to capture momentary daily effects. Next, the participants only filled out the diary during 7 days, three times a day, but only the measurement point after school was taking into account in the analysis. We could question if using one data time point a day during 7 days is enough to capture the momentary daily fluctuations. To approach these methodological shortcomings, it is recommended to include a daily measurement of emotion regulation and to include signal- or event-contingent sampling.

Fourth, we found marginally significant evidence for the hypothesis that daily stress in interaction with trait emotional eating is associated with the trajectories of desire to eat and snacking; but not for hunger eating. These results mean that in youngsters with a high emotional eating style, when experiencing high stress, a less steep decrease in desire to eat and snacking occurs in comparison with youngsters with a lower emotional eating style. These results are in line with O’Connor et al. (2008) and Reichenberger et al. (2016).

Fifth, analyses of the daily diary data also revealed that daily stress and daily eating behavior decreased as the week progressed. This was not expected and has to be explored in the future. One of the explanations can be the possibility of the contributing factor of the stress-levels fitting the day of the week. These stress-levels may play a role in the findings in this study (Areni et al., 2011; van Roekel et al., 2015). In future research, it is recommended to start the diary study on different days, to control for this order effect.

This study has several notable strengths. First, we used a daily diary design, enabling a more momentary inspection of the relation between stress and eating behavior in a naturalistic environment. Having a good reliability, validity and generalizability, diary studies are able to determine experiences, mood, behavior and contextual factors more detailed (Moskowitz and Young, 2006; Suveg et al., 2010; Engel et al., 2016; Burke et al., 2017; Goldschmidt et al., 2018). Second, we recruited a fairly large sample of youngsters between 10 and 17 years of age given that research in this age group is lacking. Yet, researching the effects of stress on wellbeing in this critical developmental period is of utmost importance given the heightened risk for developing overweight and eating pathology in particular (Giedd et al., 2009). Third, because of the highly demanding design of the study for the youngsters (diary for 7 days), a lot of efforts by the researchers were made to increase the persevered motivation of the youngsters. During a home visit the rationale was explained, a reward at the end of the study was promised and the youngsters received a personalized smartphone as incentive during the study. During the study reminders were sent for all assessing points and youngsters received a motivating phone call in between.

Despite these strengths, several limitations of the current study warrant discussion. The first limitation of the present study design is the limited number of data points each day, more specifically at breakfast, after school and before bedtime. Using signal- or event-contingent data sampling would be better to capture the daily relationship between stress and eating behavior. However, a diary-study for 7 consecutive days might be a long period for children and adolescents between 10 and 17 years old and a great burden. This was confirmed by the missing values in the diary reports. As the ability and willingness of participants are determining factors for the success of a diary study, only three diary data points were used (Moskowitz and Young, 2006; Engel et al., 2016). Besides, practical barriers prevented us from using signal- or event contingent sampling as schools did not give an authorization to fill out the diary during school time. To answer the research questions, only one measurement point a day was included in the analysis, more specifically the data point after school. First of all, two important stressors for youngsters, school and peer related stress (Sotardi, 2017) could be captured at that data point. Second, previous research showed the importance of the food availability preceding the snacking, which is higher at home (or the way to home) in contrast with a school environment. Due to the age of the participants, 10–17 years, and the possibility of having a greater food availability at home (or the way to home), the choice was made to include only this data time point here (Chopra et al., 2002; Johnson et al., 2011; Pearson et al., 2014; De Decker et al., 2017a,b). Whether emotional eating might be even more prevalent later in the evening, remains to be studied.

A second limitation of the study concerns the use of self-report measurements of eating behavior. In future research, it is recommended to use more objective methods, such as taking pictures of the snacks participants ate. This requires the use of event-contingent data sampling.

A third limitation, in addition to the one-time point, is the retrospective character of these daily measures. As mentioned above, including a signal-contingent design of the daily measures would be more suitable in order to investigate the causal daily relationship between stress and eating behavior. A fourth limitation concerns the little variance in the emotion regulation and emotional eating scores. All mean scores are in accordance with the norm scores of the questionnaires, except for maladaptive emotion regulation in boys between 12 and 18 years (Braet and Beyers, 2009; Braet et al., 2013). In future research, it might be interesting to include youngsters with overweight or obesity, as emotional eating is more prevalent among overweight and obese youngsters (Braet and Beyers, 2009). As mentioned above, a fifth limitation concerns the same start day for all participants of the diary- measures, namely on Monday. To control for week-effects, it might be better to randomize the start day for filling in the diary over the participants (Areni et al., 2011; van Roekel et al., 2015). Sixth, in this study, only intensity of stress was included as independent variable, while also the source of stress is of importance (O’Connor et al., 2008). In future research, it is recommended to include sources of daily hassles to have more specified information. Different types of stress are of interest in youngsters, e.g., Family problems; Problems with Friends; Parental Rejection; School Related Stress; Time Pressure; Concerns about Body; Romantic concerns; Sports Pressure and General self-worth (Treffers et al., 2002; O’Connor et al., 2008; Reichenberger et al., 2016). Seventh, in the current study, stress intensity was measured subjectively. As the variance within persons is rather small, the question could raise if youngsters have difficulties reporting on their stress level. In the future, we will analyze with more stringent research whether the level of the stress experience of children is related with more objective measurements, like heart rate variability. Eighth, all daily variables (e.g., stress intensity, hunger- and desire to eat motives and snacking) were measured with only one item. Although, this phenomenon is seen in different daily diary studies (O’Connor et al., 2008; Reichenberger et al., 2018), future research should use more questions to operationalize the variables.

To conclude, we found that daily stress is significantly associated with the trajectories of desire to eat and hunger eating motives. But no moderation effects of maladaptive emotion regulation were found; while emotional eating has marginally significant effects on desire to eat and snacking. In the research on stress and eating behavior, there are still a lot of gaps. The current research is a first step in reducing these gaps by investigating the underlying mechanism in the relationship between daily stress and eating behavior as a predictor of weight gain by including the moderators of emotion regulation, and more specifically emotional eating.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of Ghent University with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Ethical Committee of Ghent University.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

This work was supported by a BOF Grant (BOF.24J.2016.0007.02).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ansari, W. E., and Berg-Beckhoff, G. (2015). Nutritional correlates of perceived stress among university students in Egypt. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 12, 14164–14176. doi: 10.3390/ijerph121114164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aparicio, E., Canals, J., Arija, V., De Henauw, S., and Michels, N. (2016). The role of emotion regulation in childhood obesity: implications for prevention and treatment. Nutr. Res. Rev. 29, 17–29. doi: 10.1017/S0954422415000153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Areni, C. S., Burger, M., and Zlatevska, N. (2011). Factors affecting the extent of Monday blues: evidence from a meta-analysis. Psychol. Rep. 109, 723–733. doi: 10.2466/13.20.PR0.109.6.723-733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beattie, L., and Griffin, B. (2014). Day-level fluctuations in stress and engagement in response to workplace incivility: a diary study. J. Work Stress 28, 124–142. doi: 10.1080/02678373.2014.898712

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bey, G. S., Waring, M. E., Jesdale, B. M., and Person, S. D. (2018). Gendered race modification of the association between chronic stress and depression among black and white US. Adults. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 88, 151–160. doi: 10.1037/ort0000301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braet, C., and Beyers, W. (2009). Subtyping children and adolescents who are overweight: different symptomatology and treatment outcomes. J. Consul. Clin. Psychol. 77, 814–824. doi: 10.1037/a0016304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braet, C., Claus, L., Goossens, L., Moens, E., Van Vlierberghe, L., and Soetens, B. (2008). Differences in eating style between overweight and normal weight youngsters. J. Health Psychol. 13, 733–743. doi: 10.1177/1359105308093850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braet, C., Cracco, E., Theuwis, L., Grob, A., and Smolenski, C. (2013). Feel-KJ Vragenlijst Over Emotieregulatie bij Kinderen en Jongeren, Handleiding. Available at: Vub.ac.be/PE/Documenten/ATOM/FEEL.pdf

Google Scholar

Braet, C., Mervielde, I., and Vandereycken, W. (1997). Psychological aspects of childhood obesity: a controlled study in a clinical and nonclinical sample. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 22, 59–71. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/22.1.59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braet, C., Tanghe, A., De Bode, P., Franckx, H., and Van Winckel, M. (2003). Inpatient treatment of obese children: a multicomponent programme without stringent calorie restriction. Eur. J. Pediatr. 162, 391–396. doi: 10.1007/s00431-003-1155-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braet, C., and Van Strien, T. (1997). Assessment of emotional, externally induced and restrained eating behaviour in nine to twelve-year-old obese and non-obese children. Behav. Res. Ther. 35, 863–873. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00045-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, L. E., Shiffman, S., Music, E., Styn, M., Kriska, A., Smailagic, A., et al. (2017). Ecological momentary assessment in behavioral research: addressing technological and human participant challenges. J. Med. Internet Res. 19:e77. doi: 10.2196/jmir.7138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chopra, M., Galbraith, S., and Darnton-Hill, I. (2002). A global response to a global problem: the epidemic of overnutrition. Bull. World Health Organ. 80, 952–958. doi: 10.1590/S0042-96862002001200009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., Kessler, C., and Gordon, L. U. (1995). “Strategies for measuring stress in studies of psychiatric and physical disorders,” in Measuring Stress: A Guide for Health and Social Scientists, eds S. Cohen, R. C. Kessler, and L. G. Underwood (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 3–28.

Google Scholar

Cracco, E., Van Durme, K., and Braet, C. (2015). Validation of the FEEL-KJ: an instrument to measure emotion regulation strategies in children and adolescents. PLoS One 10:e0137080. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dallman, M. F. (2010). Stress-induced obesity and the emotional nervous system. Trends Endocrinol. Metab. 21, 159–165. doi: 10.1016/j.tem.2009.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Decker, A., Verbeken, S., Sioen, I., Moens, E., Braet, C., and De Henauw, S. (2017a). Fat tissue accretion in children and adolescents: interplay between food responsiveness, gender, and the home availability of snacks. Front. Psychol. 7:2041. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Decker, A., Verbeken, S., Sioen, I., Van Lippelvelde, W., Braet, C., Eiben, G., et al. (2017b). Palatable food consumption in children: interplay between (food) reward motivation and the home food environment. Eur. J. Pediatr. 176, 465–474. doi: 10.1007/s00431-017-2857-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Vriendt, T., Moreno, L. A., and De Henauw, S. (2009). Chronic stress and obesity in adolescents: scientific evidence and methodological issues for epidemiological research. Nutr. Metab. Cardiovasc. Dis. 19, 511–519. doi: 10.1016/j.numecd.2009.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enders, C. K., and Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: a new look at an old issue. Psychol. Methods 12, 121–138. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, S. G., Crosby, R. D., Thomas, G., Bond, D., Lavender, J. M., Mason, T., et al. (2016). Ecological momentary assessment in eating disorder and obesity research: a review of the recent literature. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 18:37. doi: 10.1007/s11920-016-0672-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erermis, S., Cetin, N., Tamar, M., Bukusoglu, N., Akdeniz, F., and Goksen, D. (2004). Is obesity a risk factor for psychopathology among adolescents? J. Pediatr. Int. 46, 296–301. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-200x.2004.01882.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evers, C., Stok, F. M., and De Ridder, T. D. (2010). Feeding your feelings: emotion regulation strategies and emotional eating. Pers. Soc. Pychol. Bull. 36, 792–804. doi: 10.1177/0146167210371383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faith, M. S., Allison, D. B., and Geliebter, A. (1997). “Emotional eating and obesity: theoretical considerations and practical recommendations,” in Overweight and Weight Management: The Health Professional’s Guide to Understanding and Practice, ed. S. Dalton (Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen Publishers), 439–465.

Google Scholar

Ferraro, K. F., and Kelley-Moore, J. A. (2003). Cumulative disadvantage and health: long-term consequences of obesity? J. Am. Sociol. Rev. 68, 707–729. doi: 10.2307/1519759

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, A. E., Austin, S. B., Gillman, M. W., Rosner, B., Rockett, H. R., and Colditz, G. A. (2004). Snack food intake does not predict weight change among children and adolescents. Int. J. Obes. 28, 1210–1216. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gevers, D. W. M., Kremers, S. P. J., De Vries, N. K., and Van Assema, P. (2015). Patterns of food parenting practices and children’s intake of energy-dense snack foods. Nutrients 7, 4093–4106. doi: 10.3390/nu7064093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, E. L. (2006). Emotional influences on food choice: sensory, physiological and psychological pathways. J. Physiol. Behav. 89, 53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giedd, J. N., Keshavan, M., and Paus, T. (2009). Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 947–957. doi: 10.1038/nrn2513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gluck, M. D., Geliebter, A., Hung, J., and Yahav, E. (2004). Cortisol, hunger, and desire to binge eat following a cold stress test in obese women with binge eating disorder. J. Psychosom. Med. 66, 876–881. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000143637.63508.47

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldschmidt, A. B., Smith, K. E., Crosby, R. D., Boyd, H. K., Dougherty, E., Engel, S. G., et al. (2018). Ecological momentary assessment of maladaptive eating in children and adolescents with overweight or obesity. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 51, 549–557. doi: 10.1002/eat.22864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, E., and Whitaker, R. C. (2002). A prospective study of the role of depression in the development and persistence of adolescent obesity. J. Pediatr. 110, 497–504. doi: 10.1542/peds.110.3.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graziano, P. A., Calkins, S. D., and Keane, S. P. (2010). Toddler self-regulation skills predict risk for pediatric obesity. Int. J. Obes. 34, 633–641. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2009.288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graziano, P. A., Kelleher, R., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., and Brien, M. O. (2014). Predicting weight outcomes in preadolescence: the role of toddlers’ self-regulation skills and the temperament dimension of pleasure. Int. J. Obes. 37, 937–942. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2012.165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grob, A., and Smolenski, C. (2005). FEEL-KJ: Manual und Fragebogen zur Erhebung der Emotionsregulation bei Kindern und Jugendlichen. Bern. Available at: http://edoc.unibas.ch/dok/A6223168

Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gustafsson, H., Skoog, T., Podlog, L., Lundqvist, C., and Wagnsson, S. (2013). Hope and athlete burnout: stress and affect as mediators. J. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 14, 640–649. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.03.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, D. C., Moss, R. H., Sykes-Muskett, B., Conner, M., and O’Connor, D. B. (2018). Stress and eating behaviors in children and adolescents: systematic review and meta-analysis. Appetite 123, 14–22. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.11.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hruska, L. C., Zelic, K. J., Dickson, K. S., and Ciesla, J. A. (2017). Adolescents’ Co-rumination and stress predict affective changes in a daily-diary paradigm. Int. J. Psychol. 52, 372–380. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isasi, C. R., Ostrovsky, N. W., and Wills, T. (2013). The association of emotion regulation with lifestyle behaviors in inner-city adolescents. J. Eat. Behav. 14, 518–521. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jastreboff, A. M., Sinha, R., Lacadie, C., Small, D. M., Sherwin, R. S., and Potenza, M. N. (2013). Neural correlates of stress-and food cue-induced food craving in obesity. J. Diabetes Care 36, 394–402. doi: 10.2337/dc12-1112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, L., van Jaarsveld, C. H., and Wardle, J. (2011). Individual and family environment correlates differ for consumption of core and non-core foods in children. Br. J. Nutr. 105, 950–959. doi: 10.1017/S0007114510004484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koch, F., Sepa, A., and Ludvigsson, J. (2008). Psychological stress and obesity. J. Pediatr. 153, 839–844. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenders, P. G., and Van Strien, T. (2011). Emotional eating, rather than lifestyle behavior, drives weight gain in a prospective study in 1562 employees. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 53, 1287–1293. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0b013e31823078a2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krohne, H. W. (2002). Stress and Coping Theories. Available at: http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/schuez/folien/Krohne_Stress.pdf

Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S. (1993). From psychological stress to the emotions: a history of changing outlooks. J. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 44, 1–22. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.000245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1986). “Cognitive theories of stress and the issue of circularity,” in Dynamics of Stress. Physiological, Psychological, and Social Perspectives, eds M. H. Appley and R. Trumbull (New York, NY: Plenum), 63–80.

Google Scholar

Lemyre, L., and Tessier, R. (2003). Measuring psychological stress. Can. Fam. Physician 48, 1159–1160.

Google Scholar

Lifshitz, F. (2008). Obesity in children. J. Clin. Res. Pediatr. Endocrinol. 1, 53–60. doi: 10.4008/jcrpe.v1i2.35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lobstein, T., Baur, L., and Uauy, R. (2004). Obesity in children and young people: a crisis in public health. J. Obes. Rev. 5, 4–85. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2004.00133.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mabbe, E., Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., van der Kaap-Deeder, J., and Mouratidis, A. (2018). Day-to-day variation in autonomy-supportive and psychologically controlling parenting: the role of parents’ daily experiences of need satisfaction and need frustration. Parenting 18, 86–109. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2018.1444131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macht, M. (2008). How emotions affect eating: a five- way model. Appetite 50, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michels, N., Sioen, I., Braet, C., Eiben, G., Hebestreit, A., Huybrechts, I., et al. (2012). Stress, emotional eating behaviour and dietary patterns in children. Appetite 59, 762–769. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michopoulos, V., Powers, A., Moore, C., Villarreal, S., Ressler, K. J., and Bradley, B. (2015). The mediating role of emotion dysregulation and depression on the relationship between childhood trauma exposure and emotional eating. Appetite 91, 129–136. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.03.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Midei, A. J., and Matthews, K. A. (2009). Social relationships and negative emotional traits are associated with central adiposity and arterial stiffness in healthy adolescents. J. Health Psychol. 28, 347–353. doi: 10.1037/a0014214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minkel, J. D., Banks, S., Htaik, O., Moreta, M. C., Jones, C. W., McGlinchey, E. L., et al. (2012). Sleep deprivation and stressors: evidence for elevated negative affect in response to mild stressors when sleep deprived. J. Emot. 12, 1015–1020. doi: 10.1037/a0026871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monteleone, P., Piscitelli, F., Scognamigli, P., Monteleone, A. M., Perillo, D., Cantressli, B., et al. (2013). Gastroenteric hormone responses to hedonic eating in healthy humans. J. Psychoneuroendocrinol. 38, 1435–1441. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moskowitz, D. S., and Young, S. N. (2006). Ecological momentary assessment: what it is and why it is a method of the future in clinical psychopharmacology. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 31, 13–20.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mroczek, D. K., and Almeida, D. M. (2004). The effect of daily stress, personality and age on daily negative affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72, 355–378. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00265.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nezlek, J. B. (2011). Multilevel Modeling for Social and Personality Psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications Ltd. doi: 10.4135/9781446287996

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen-Rodriguez, S. T., Unger, J. B., and Sprijtz-Metz, D. (2009). Psychological determinants of emotional eating in adolescence. J. Eat. Disord. 17, 211–224. doi: 10.1080/10640260902848543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, D. B., Jones, F., Connor, M., McMillan, B., and Ferguson, E. (2008). Effects of daily hassles and eating style on eating behavior. J. Health Psychol. 27, 20–31.

Google Scholar

Oliver, G., and Wardle, J. (1999). Perceived effects of stress on food choice. J. Physiol. Behav. 66, 511–515. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00322-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliver, G., Wardle, J., and Gibson, E. L. (2000). Stress and food choice: a laboratory study. J. Psychosom. Med. 62, 853–865. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200011000-00016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearson, N., Biddle, S. J., Williams, L., Worsley, A., Crawford, D., and Ball, K. (2014). Adolescent television viewing and unhealthy snack food consumption: the mediating role of home availability of unhealthy snack foods. J. Public Health Nutr. 17, 317–323. doi: 10.1017/S1368980012005204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Popless-Vawter, S., Brandau, C., and Straub, J. (1998). Triggers of overeating and related intervention strategies for women who weight cycle. Appl. Nurs. Res. 11, 69–76. doi: 10.1016/S0897-1897(98)80207-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Popper, R., Smits, G., Meiselman, H. L., and Hirsch, E. (1989). Eating in combat: a survey of U.S. marines. J. Mil. Med. 154, 619–623. doi: 10.1093/milmed/154.12.619

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasbash, J., Charlton, C., Browne, W. J., Healy, M., and Cameron, B. (2009). MlwiN Version 2.1. Centre for Multilevel Modelling. Bristol: University of Bristol, 1.

Google Scholar

Reichenberger, J., Kuppens, P., Liedlgruber, M., Wilhelm, F. H., Tiefengrabner, M., Ginzinger, S., et al. (2016). No haste, more taste: an EMA-study on the effects of stress, negative and positive emotions on eating behavior. Biol. Psychol. 131, 54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reichenberger, J., Richard, A., Smyth, J. M., Fischer, D., Pollatos, O., and Blechert, J. (2018). It’s craving time: time of day effects on momentary hunger and food craving in daily life. Nutrition 55–56, 15–20. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2018.03.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reilly, J., and Kelly, J. (2011). Long-term impact of overweight and obesity in childhood and adolescence on morbidity and premature mortality in adulthood: systematic review. Int. J. Obes. 35, 891–898. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2010.222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reilly, J., Methven, E., McDowell, Z., Hacking, B., Alexander, D., Stewart, L., et al. (2003). Health consequences of obesity. Arch. Dis. Child. 88, 748–752. doi: 10.1136/adc.88.9.748

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ricciardelli, L. A., and McCabe, M. P. (2001). Children’s body image concerns and eating disturbance: a review of the literature. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 21, 325–344. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(99)00051-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rofey, D., Kolko, R., Iosif, A., Silk, J. S., Bost, J. E., Feng, W., et al. (2009). A longitudinal study of childhood depression and anxiety in relation to weight gain. J. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 40, 517–526. doi: 10.1007/s10578-009-0141-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salim, S. (2014). Oxidative stress and psychological disorders. J. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 12, 140–147. doi: 10.2174/1570159X11666131120230309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, J. D., and Willet, J. B. (eds). (2003). “A framework for investigating change over time,” in Applied Longitudinal Data analysis: Modeling Change and Event Occurencence, (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 3–15. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195152968.003.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sotardi, V. A. (2017). Exploring school stress in middle childhood: interpretations, experiences, and coping. J. Pastor. Care Educ. 35, 13–27. doi: 10.1080/02643944.2016.1269360

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steere, J., and Cooper, P. J. (1993). The effects on eating of dietary restraints, anxiety and hunger. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 13, 211–219. doi: 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2011.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stenhammar, C., Olsson, G. M., Bahmanyar, S., Hulting, A.-L., Wettergren, B., Edlund, B., et al. (2010). Family stress and BMI in young children. J. Acta Paediatr. 99, 1205–1212. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2010.01776.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A., Lipsey, Z., and Wardle, J. (1998). Stress, hassles and variation in alcohol consumption, food choice and physical exercise: a diary study. Br. J. Health Psychol. 3, 51–63. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8287.1998.tb00555.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., Presnell, K., and Spangler, D. (2002). Risk factors for binge eating onset in adolescent girls: a 2-year prospective investigation. J. Health Psychol. 21, 131–138. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.21.2.131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stone, A. A., and Brownell, K. (1994). The stress-eating paradox: multiple daily measurements in adult males and females. J. Psychol. Health 9, 425–436. doi: 10.1080/08870449408407469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suveg, C., Payne, M., Thomassin, K., and Jacob, M. L. (2010). Electronic diaries: a feasible method of assessing emotional experiences in youth? J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 32, 57–67. doi: 10.1007/s10862-009-9162-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, R. (2001). How People Regulate Mood with Food and Exercise. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Torres, S. J., and Nowson, C. A. (2007). Relationship between stress, eating behavior, and obesity. J. Nutr. 23, 887–894. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2007.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treffers, P. D. A., Goedhart, A. W., Veerman, J. W., Van den Bergh, B. R. H., Anckaert, L., and De Rycke, L. (2002). Competentiebelevingsschaal voor adolescenten. Tijdschr. Psychiatr. 46, 468–469.

Google Scholar

Van Beveren, M. L., Harding, K., and Braet, C. (2017). Don’t worry, be happy: the role of positive emotionality and adaptive emotion regulation strategies for youth depressive symptoms. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 57, 18–41. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Beveren, M. L., Mezulis, A., Wante, L., and Braet, C. (2016). Joint contributions of negative emotionality, positive emotionality, and effortful control on depressive symptoms in youth. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2016.1233499 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Jaarsveld, C. H. M., Fidler, J. A., Steptoe, A., Boniface, D., and Wardle, J. (2009). Perceived stress and weight gain in adolescence: a longitudinal analysis. J. Obes. 17, 2155–2161. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Roekel, E., Ha, T., Verhagen, M., Kuntsche, E., Scholte, R. H., and Engels, R. C. (2015). Social stress in early adolescents’ daily lives: associations with affect and loneliness. J. Adolesc. 45, 274–283. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Strien, T., Frijters, J. E. R., Bergers, G. P. A., and Defares, P. B. (1986). The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 5, 295–315. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(198602)5:2<295::AID-EAT2260050209>3.0.CO;2-T

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandewalle, J., Mabbe, E., Debeuf, T., Braet, C., and Moens, E. (2017a). The daily relation between parental rejection and emotional eating in youngsters: a diary study. Front. Psychol. 8:691. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandewalle, J., Moens, E., Bosmans, G., and Braet, C. (2017b). The effect of parental rejection on the emotional eating behaviour of youngsters: a laboratory-based study. Appetite 108, 219–225. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandewalle, J., Moens, E., and Braet, C. (2014). Comprehending emotional eating in obese youngsters: the role of parental rejection and emotion regulation. Int. J. Obes. 38, 525–530. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandewalle, J., Moens, E., Debeuf, T., and Braet, C. (2018). Parental rejection as a predictor of emotional eating after inpatient weight loss treatment for youngsters. J. Behav. Change 35, 217–227.

Google Scholar

Watson, D. (1998). Intraindividual and interindividual analyses of positive and negative affect: their relation to health complaints, perceived stress and daily activities. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1020–1030. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whiteside, U., Chen, E., Neighbors, C., Hunter, D., Lo, T., and Larimer, M. (2007). Difficulties regulation emotions: do binge eaters have fewer strategies to modulate and tolerate negative affect? J. Eat. Behav. 8, 162–169. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willenbring, M. L., Levine, A. S., and Morley, J. E. (1986). Stress induced eating and food preference in humans: a pilot study. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 5, 855–864. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(198607)5:5<855::AID-EAT2260050507>3.0.CO;2-O

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization [WHO] (2018). Obesity and Overweight. Available at: http://www.who.int/en/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight

Zipper, E., Vila, G., Dabbas, M., Bertrand, C., Mouren-Siméoni, M., Robert, J., et al. (2001). Obesity in children and adolescents, mental disorders and familial psychopathology. Presse Méd. 30, 1489–1495.

Google Scholar

Keywords: daily diary, stress, eating behavior, emotion regulation, emotional eating

Citation: Debeuf T, Verbeken S, Van Beveren M-L, Michels N and Braet C (2018) Stress and Eating Behavior: A Daily Diary Study in Youngsters. Front. Psychol. 9:2657. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02657

Received: 13 July 2018; Accepted: 10 December 2018;
Published: 21 December 2018.

Edited by:

Kelly Costello Allison, University of Pennsylvania, United States

Reviewed by:

Julia Reichenberger, University of Salzburg, Austria
Annette R. Gallant, Laval University, Canada

Copyright © 2018 Debeuf, Verbeken, Van Beveren, Michels and Braet. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Taaike Debeuf, Taaike.debeuf@Ugent.Be

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.