Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 12 October 2018
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

More Realistic Forecasting of Future Life Events After Psilocybin for Treatment-Resistant Depression

  • Psychedelic Research Group, Department of Medicine, Imperial College London, London, United Kingdom

Background: Evidence suggests that classical psychedelics can promote enduring changes in personality, attitudes and optimism, as well as improvements in mental health outcomes.

Aim: To investigate the effects of a composite intervention, involving psilocybin, on pessimism biases in patients with treatment-resistant depression (TRD).

Methods: Patients with TRD (n = 15) and matched, untreated non-depressed controls (n = 15) performed the Prediction Of Future Life Events (POFLE) task. The POFLE task requires participants to predict the likelihood of certain life events occurring within a 30-day period, after which the actual rate of event occurrence is reported; this gives an index of potential pessimism versus optimism bias. Psilocybin was administered in two oral dosing sessions (10 and 25 mg) one week apart. Main outcome measures were collected at baseline and one week after the second dosing session.

Results: Patients showed a significant pessimism bias at baseline [t(14) = -3.260, p = 0.006; 95% CI (-0.16, -0.03), g = 1.1] which was related to the severity of their depressive symptoms (rs = -0.55, p = 0.017). One week after psilocybin treatment, this bias was significantly decreased [t(14) = -2.714, p = 0.017; 95% CI (-0.21, -0.02), g = 0.7] and depressive symptoms were greatly improved [t(14) = 7.900, p < 0.001; 95% CI (16.17, 28.23), g = 1.9]; moreover, the magnitude of change in both variables was significantly correlated (r = -0.57, p = 0.014). Importantly, post treatment, patients became significantly more accurate at predicting the occurrence of future life events [t(14) = 1.857, p = 0.042; 95% CI (-0.01, 0.12), g = 0.6] whereas no such change was observed in the control subjects.

Conclusion: These findings suggest that psilocybin with psychological support might correct pessimism biases in TRD, enabling a more positive and accurate outlook.

Introduction

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is one of the foremost contributors to the overall global burden of disease (Marcus et al., 2012). Approximately 30% of MDD patients suffer from treatment-resistant depression (TRD) (Gaynes, 2009; Al-Harbi, 2012) and much of the burden associated with MDD can be accounted for by treatment resistance (Greden, 2001). Cognitive therapy is the most widely studied and practiced psychotherapeutic intervention for MDD (Butler et al., 2006). The cognitive-bias model of depression states that patients have an unrealistic negative perspective of themselves and the world more generally (Beck, 1967, 1976; Beck and Rush, 1979). Somewhat in contrast to this, the depressive realism hypothesis states that depressed patients actually see themselves and the world in a more realistic way than the general population – who, the hypothesis maintains, are somewhat inaccurate and unrealistic in their optimism (Alloy and Abramson, 1979). The cognitive triad—which comprises a negative attitude towards oneself, the environment and the future (Beck, 1967)—partly forms the theory upon which cognitive therapy for MDD is based. A key cognitive feature in MDD is that patients expect the future to be bleak and anticipate unfavorable outcomes when facing life events of unknown emotional impact. Early research has shown that MDD patients have more dysfunctional attitudes, feelings of hopelessness, negative thoughts and pessimism than healthy individuals (Peterson and Seligman, 1984; Hollon et al., 1986; Beck et al., 1988; Hill et al., 1989). More recent findings have since supported the cognitive-bias model of depression over the depressive realism hypothesis, demonstrating, for example, that MDD patients exhibit pessimistic biases when anticipating future life events (Strunk et al., 2006; Strunk and Adler, 2009).

As MDD is associated with excessive pessimism, it is interesting to note that psilocybin—a naturally occurring psychedelic compound derived from Psilocybe mushrooms—has been shown to increase optimism, psychological wellbeing, trait openness, and life satisfaction in an enduring way following just a single dose in healthy populations (Griffiths et al., 2008; MacLean et al., 2011). Increased optimism and wellbeing have also been found for weeks to months after a controlled administration of LSD to healthy volunteers (Carhart-Harris et al., 2016b; Liechti, 2017). Functional brain imaging of the psychedelic state has implicated brain regions linked to MDD (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012), including the subgenual prefrontal cortex (sgPFC) and default mode network (DMN) (Hamilton et al., 2015). Moreover, the key receptor associated with the action of psychedelics, the serotonin 2A receptor [5-HT2AR (Glennon et al., 1984), has its highest expression in DMN regions (Beliveau et al., 2017) – and 5-HT2AR binding has been reliably linked with trait pessimism (Meyer et al., 2003; Bhagwagar et al., 2006) and neuroticism (Frokjaer et al., 2008, 2010)]. It has been hypothesized that deficient signaling at the 5-HT2AR may confer an inflexible and pessimistic mind-set, and that increased 5-HT2AR signaling may serve to “lubricate the mind,” relaxing prior beliefs to aid new learning (Carhart-Harris and Nutt, 2017); see Matias et al. (2017) for more empirical support.

Recent years have witnessed a resurgence of interest in the ability of classical psychedelic compounds to treat a range of disorders, including MDD (Dos Santos et al., 2016; Carhart-Harris and Goodwin, 2017). Randomized controlled trials (RCTs) have demonstrated that psilocybin can rapidly alleviate depression and anxiety in patients with life-threatening cancer, with sustained effects for 6 months (Griffiths et al., 2016; Ross et al., 2016). A recent feasibility study of ours found that psilocybin with psychological support was well tolerated, and associated with rapid and marked reductions in depressive symptoms in TRD patients (Carhart-Harris et al., 2016a, 2017a), thus endorsing it’s potential as a safe and effective treatment for such clinical populations, albeit with important caveats (Carhart-Harris et al., 2018).

The present study sought to investigate the presence of pessimism in TRD patients, in line with the depressive-bias hypothesis, and the ability of psilocybin to alleviate this. It was hypothesized that the TRD patients would indeed show a significant pessimism bias that could be effectively treated with psilocybin therapy. Relevant measures of pessimism were given at baseline and one-week post-dosing. To demonstrate robustness to order confounds, TRD patients’ data were compared with those from non-treated healthy control subjects matched in terms of age, gender and education tested over an equivalent time period.

Materials and Methods

Ethical Approvals

This study received a favorable opinion from NRES London-West London, was sponsored by Imperial College London, and was carried out in accordance with Good Clinical Practice Guidelines. The National Institute for Health Research/Wellcome Trust ICRF provided site-specific approval and the Medicines and Healthcare products Regulatory Agency reviewed and approved this research. All patients provided written informed consent.

Study Design and Participants

This open-label pilot study with a mixed-model design investigated the effects of psilocybin on measures of depressive symptoms and cognitive biases in TRD patients versus healthy non-treated control subjects matched in terms of age, gender and education tested over an equivalent time period. The patients and research team were not masked to treatment assignment. All TRD patients (n = 15) were administered psilocybin in two dosing sessions: an initial safety dose (10 mg) and a subsequent treatment dose (25 mg) 1 week later. Please refer to Carhart-Harris et al. (2016a) for a full description of the procedures and dosing sessions. Physically and mentally healthy control subjects (n = 15) were recruited via word of mouth and were not administered psilocybin.

Drug

Psilocybin was obtained from THC Pharm GmbH (Frankfurt, Germany) and formulated (5 mg psilocybin per size 0 capsule) by Guy’s and St Thomas’ Hospitals’ Pharmacy Manufacturing Unit (London, United Kingdom). Home Office approvals for storing and dispensing Schedule One drugs were obtained.

Outcome Measures

Depressive symptoms were measured in all study participants using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)—a self-report rating inventory with 21 items that measures characteristic attitudes and symptoms of depression (Beck et al., 1961). Cognitive biases were measured using the Prediction Of Future Life Events task (POFLE; Strunk et al. (2006)). The POFLE involves 40 different life events, 20 desirable and 20 undesirable, and patients must predict the probability of each occurring within the next 30 days [see Strunk et al. (2006) for the scoring procedures]. The POFLE was split into two versions, A and B, each containing 20 items. All patients received both versions in balanced order; half of the patients received POFLE version A at screening and POFLE version B at the follow-up, and the other half vice versa. Patients were contacted 30 days after completing each version to determine which of the events actually occurred. All of the above outcome measures were assessed at screening and then again at the follow-up (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Schematic diagram of the study design.

Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS version 23.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, United States). Within-group comparisons were performed using two-tailed paired t-tests for parametric data and Wilcoxon signed ranks tests for non-parametric data. Between-group comparisons were performed using two-tailed independent t-tests for parametric data and Mann-Whitney U tests for non-parametric data. Depressive symptoms over time were analyzed using the non-parametric Friedman Test with Dunn’s correction for multiple post hoc comparisons. Correlations were analyzed using Pearson’s correlation coefficient for parametric data and Spearman’s rho for non-parametric data. We provide 95% CIs around the mean differences. Effect sizes were calculated using the Hedges’ g formula.

Results

Demographics

Of the total 30 participants who took part in this study, the majority were Caucasian (93.33%) and men (73.33%) with a post-secondary level of education (80%). Self-reported depressive symptoms were collected using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) for all study participants. Of the 15 clinically assessed patients with TRD, the Hamilton Depression Scale (HAM-D) scores ranged from 18 to 29 (M = 23.73, SD = 4.70) and the Montgomery–Åsberg Depression Rating Scale scores from 24 to 42 (M = 32.33, SD = 5.14); thus, all patients recruited were diagnosed with TRD of at least moderate severity, with most (n = 10) meeting criteria for severe depression (HAM-D > 24; BDI > 30). Control subjects were not clinically assessed because they were deemed physically and mentally healthy. A breakdown of the participant demographic details and depressive scores at baseline can be found in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Study participant demographics and baseline depression scores.

Depressive Symptoms

At baseline, patients (M = 34.33, SD = 7.44) scored significantly higher than controls (M = 3.67, SD = 3.83) on the BDI (U = 0.0E0, p < 0.001, g = 5.0). The patients showed a significant decrease in BDI scores one week after the psilocybin sessions (M = 12.13, SD = 9.80) compared to baseline [M = 34.33, SD = 7.44; t(14) = 7.900, p < 0.001; 95% CI (16.17, 28.23), g = 1.9]. There was no significant difference found in the controls’ baseline (M = 3.67, SD = 3.83) versus follow-up BDI scores (M = 2.73, SD = 3.41; Z = -1.071, p = 0.284). These results confirm that depressive symptoms were significantly reduced for the patients 1 week after psilocybin treatment, but did not change over an equivalent period for the non-treated control subjects.

Despite the significant improvement, patients’ post-treatment BDI scores (M = 12.13, SD = 9.80) continued to significantly differ from the controls’ follow-up values (M = 4.29, SD = 4.35; U = 34.50, p = 0.001, g = 1.1). These results suggest that the patients’ depressive symptoms were largely reduced but did not quite reach levels comparable with healthy controls one week after psilocybin treatment (Figure 2A).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. (A) Depressive symptoms. Patients had significantly higher BDI scores than controls at baseline (U = 0.0E0, p < 0.001; g = 5.0). The patients’ BDI scores were significantly reduced 1 week after psilocybin treatment [t(14) = 7.900, p < 0.001; 95% CI (16.17, 28.23), g = 1.9], but remained significantly greater than the controls (U = 34.50, p = 0.001; g = 1.1). (B) Depressive symptoms over time: Patients. There was a significant reduction in the patients’ BDI scores over time [X2(3, N = 15) = 32.35, p < 0.0001]. Baseline BDI scores did not significantly differ from scores at the beginning of dosing day 1 (p = 0.944), confirming that the depressive symptoms had not naturally declined prior to psilocybin treatment. BDI scores were significantly reduced on dosing day 2 when compared with baseline (p < 0.001) and dosing day 1 (p = 0.033) scores. One-week after both dosing sessions, BDI scores were significantly reduced when compared to baseline (p < 0.001), dosing day 1 (p < 0.001) and dosing day 2 (p = 0.009) scores. Data expressed as mean ± SEM (p < 0.05; p < 0.01∗∗; p < 0.001∗∗∗). Note: Time (days) between: (i) baseline and dosing day 1 (M = 35.0, SD = 23.3); (ii) dosing day 1 and dosing day 2 (M = 7.9, SD = 2.6) (iii) dosing day 2 and one-week post follow-up (M = 7.8, SD = 2.5; (iv) baseline and one-week post follow-up (M = 50.8; SD = 25.3).

The patients’ depressive symptoms were also measured prior to the administration of psilocybin at the beginning of dosing days 1 and 2. There was a significant reduction in the patients’ BDI scores over time [X2(3, N = 15) = 32.35, p < 0.0001]. Post hoc comparisons with Dunn’s correction showed no significant difference in baseline (M = 34.33, SD = 7.44) versus dosing day 1 (M = 30.53, SD = 12.36; p = 0.944) BDI scores. This confirms that depressive symptoms did not naturally decline from baseline prior to psilocybin treatment. However, BDI scores were significantly reduced on dosing day 2 (M = 21.13, SD = 12.93) when compared with baseline (M = 34.33, SD = 7.44; p < 0.001) and dosing day 1 (M = 30.53, SD = 12.36; p = 0.033) scores. This demonstrates a reduction in the patients’ depressive symptoms 1 week after receiving the first dose of psilocybin. One-week after both dosing sessions (M = 12.67, SD = 10.40), BDI scores were significantly reduced when compared to baseline (M = 34.33, SD = 7.44; p < 0.001), dosing day 1 (M = 30.53, SD = 12.36; p < 0.001) and dosing day 2 (M = 21.13, SD = 12.93; p = 0.009) scores. This confirms that the patients’ depressive symptoms were reduced even further following each dose of psilocybin (Figure 2B).

Predicting Future Life-Events Before Psilocybin Treatment

Before proceeding to testing the specific hypotheses of this study, some description of the prediction and event occurrence data that is required for bias computation is warranted (see Supplementary Material). As the POFLE contains both desirable and undesirable life events, it is most informative to consider the data split by the desirability of these life events. When predicting life events at baseline, the patients gave similar probability estimates for desirable (M = 0.29, SD = 0.15) and undesirable life events [M = 0.23, SD = 0.15, t(14) = 1.037, p = 0.317; 95% CI (-0.07, 0.19)]. Contrary to their predictions however, patients reported experiencing more desirable (M = 4.60, SD = 1.76) than undesirable events [M = 1.40, SD = 1.35, t(14) = 5.967, p < 0.001; 95% CI (2.05, 4.35), g = 1.5] within the ensuing 30-day period after their baseline predictions. This demonstrates that the patients predicted a negative future that was not borne out by their experience.

Control subjects gave higher probability estimates for desirable (M = 0.57, SD = 0.09) than undesirable life events [M = 0.22, SD = 0.16; t(14) = 8.492, p < 0.001; 95% CI (0.26, 0.43), g = 2.0] at baseline and in line with their predictions, reported the occurrence of more desirable (M = 5.60, SD = 1.45) than undesirable [M = 2.33, SD = 1.84; t(14) = 4.859, p < 0.001; 95% CI (1.8, 4.7), g = 1.2] events within the ensuing 30 days. This suggests that the controls could realistically forecast the occurrence of future life events – most of which were desirable.

Between-group comparisons revealed that patients predicted significantly less desirable events than control subjects at baseline [M = 0.29, SD = 0.15 vs M = 0.57, SD = 0.09; t(28) = 5.820, p < 0.001; 95% CI (0.17, 0.36), g = 2.2], but there was no significant difference found between patients (M = 0.23, SD = 0.15) and controls (M = 0.22, SD = 0.16) in their predictions of undesirable events (U = 108.000, p = 0.852). Therefore, despite the occurrence of relatively more desirable than undesirable life events occurring within the ensuing 30-day period, the patients (wrongly) expected an equal number of desirable and undesirable life events, and predicted significantly less desirable life events than the control subjects. Both results are consistent with an unrealistic pessimism bias in the patients prior to receipt of psilocybin, in which they were especially prone to underestimate the (true) likelihood of desirable future life events.

There were no significant between-groups differences found in the rates at which desirable (patients: M = 4.60, SD = 1.76; controls: M = 5.60, SD = 1.45; U = 75.000, p = 0.113) and undesirable (patients: M = 1.40, SD = 1.35 controls: M = 2.33, SD = 1.84; U = 78.500, p = 0.148) events occurred within the 30-day period from baseline. These findings reflect that both groups were experiencing similar life circumstances, despite patients predicting a more negative reality.

Predicting Future Life-Events After Psilocybin-Treatment (Second 30-Day Period)

In contrast to their pre-treatment pessimism, post-treatment, patients gave significantly higher probability estimates for desirable (M = 0.44, SD = 0.20) than undesirable life events [M = 0.25, SD = 0.22, t(14) = 2.322, p = 0.036; 95% CI (0.01, 0.35), g = 0.6]. Justifying this difference, the patients also reported a higher percentage of desirable events (M = 3.80, SD = 2.57) actually occurring within this (second and post-treatment) 30-day period – relative to undesirable events [M = 1.40, SD = 1.18; t(14) = 3.485, p = 0.004; 95% CI (0.92, 3.88), g = 1.3]; suggestive of an improved accuracy in predicting future life events (see below for more specific accuracy measures).

As they did at baseline, control subjects gave higher probability estimates for desirable (M = 0.57, SD = 0.14) than undesirable (M = 0.20, SD = 0.12) life events at the follow-up [t(14) = 8.399, p < 0.001; 95% CI (0.28, 0.46), g = 2.0], and did not differ from baseline in their second predictions of desirable [t(14) = -0.0001, p = 1.000; 95% CI (-0.07, 0.07)] or undesirable events [t(14) = 0.686 p = 0.504; 95% CI (-0.06, 0.12)]. Consistent with these predictions, control subjects reported the occurrence of more desirable (M = 5.53, SD = 1.19) than undesirable (M = 1.80, SD = 1.78) events within the ensuing 30 days from follow-up [t(14) = 8.071, p < 0.001; 95% CI (2.74, 4.73), g = 2.0].

Contrary to what was seen at baseline, there were no significant between-groups differences in the predictions of both desirable [t(28) = 1.972, p = 0.059; 95% CI (-0.01, 0.25)] and undesirable events (U = 95.000, p = 0.468) for the second block of predictions. Further, there were no significant between-groups differences found in the rates at which desirable (U = 70.000, p = 0.072) and undesirable (U = 105.000, p = 0.741) events actually occurred within the second 30-day period.

In summary, after treatment with psilocybin, consistent with what was actually reported to have happened during the ensuing 30-day period, TRD patients (rightly) expected more desirable than undesirable life events to occur – and thus began to make more accurate predictions of future life events – in line with the behavior of healthy control subjects.

Accuracy of Forecasts

Next, we examined whether there were any changes in the accuracy of participants’ forecasts—defined as the magnitude of discrepancy between predictions and actual event occurrence—between baseline and the post-treatment repeat phase. Closely paralleling the above results, patients were significantly more accurate in their predictions post-treatment (M = 0.76, SD = 0.07) than at pre-treatment baseline [M = 0.71, SD = 0.12; t(14) = 1.857, p = 0.042; 95% CI (-0.01, 0.12), g = 0.6] (Figure 3A).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. (A) Accuracy. Patients were significantly more accurate in their predictions at the follow-up relative to baseline [t(14) = 1.857, p = 0.042; 95% CI (–0.01, 0.12), g = 0.6]. There were no significant changes in accuracy for the control subjects [t(14) = 0.603, p = 0.556; 95% CI (–0.06, 0.09)]. No between-groups differences in accuracy were found at baseline [t(28) = –0.196, p = 0.423] or the follow-up [t(28) = 0.676, p = 0.252; 95% CI (–0.04, 0.08)]. (B) Cognitive bias. The greater the digression from zero on the y-axis, the greater the bias. Patients had a significant pessimism bias at baseline [t(14) = –3.260, p = 0.006; 95% CI (–0.16, –0.03), g = 1.1]. No significant biases were found for the controls [t(14) = 1.823, p = 0.090; 95% CI (–0.09, 0.01)]. Baseline bias scores were significantly lower (i.e., greater pessimism) for the patients than the controls [t(28) = 3.704, p < 0.001; 95% CI (0.06, 0.22), g = 1.3]. Patients’ pessimism bias was significantly reduced [t(14) = –2.714, p = 0.017] and they no longer displayed any biases following psilocybin treatment [t(14) = 0.768, p = 0.455; 95% CI (–0.06, 0.02)]. No significant biases were found for the controls [t(14) = 0.460, p = 0.653; 95% CI (–0.05, 0.03)] and there was no significant between-groups difference [t(28) = –0.265, p = 0.793; 95% CI (–0.07, 0.05)] at the follow-up. Data expressed as mean ± SEM (p < 0.05; p < 0.01∗∗; p < 0.001∗∗∗). Only the TRD patients received psilocybin treatment.

As predicted, there were no significant changes in accuracy observed in the non-treated healthy control subjects from baseline (M = 0.72, SD = 0.12) to the repeat phase [M = 0.74, SD = 0.09; t(14) = 0.603, p = 0.556; 95% CI (-0.06, 0.09)]. There were also no significant between-groups differences found at baseline [t(28) = -0.196, p = 0.423; 95% CI (-0.09, 0.08)] or the follow-up [t(28) = 0.676, p = 0.252; 95% CI (-0.04, 0.08)] (Figure 3A).

Assessing the Presence of a Pessimism Bias in the TRD Patients

Before proceeding to testing specific hypotheses, some description of the computation of bias is warranted. In order to compute cognitive bias, the life events that actually occurred need to be taken into account. Therefore, different base rates for desirable versus undesirable events do not impede the analysis of bias. Bias scores can range from -1 to 1, with a score of 0 indicating that there is neither an optimism nor pessimism bias. The greater the deviation from 0, the greater the pessimism (negative scores) or optimism (positive scores) bias.

The mean amount of bias shown by the patients at baseline (M = -0.10, SD = 0.12) was significantly lower than zero; thus confirming a suspected pessimism bias [t(14) = -3.260, p = 0.006; 95% CI (-0.16, -0.03), g = 1.1]. This should be interpreted as consistent with what one would suspect in a study of people with mostly severe depression. Following psilocybin treatment, this pessimism bias was significantly reduced [M = 0.02, SD = 0.08; t(14) = -2.714, p = 0.017; 95% CI (-0.21, -0.02), g = 0.7] to such an extent that the mean score was no longer significantly different from zero [t(14) = 0.768, p = 0.455; 95% CI (-0.06, 0.02)] – indicating an effective absence of bias. These results demonstrate that the patients were indeed “unjustifiably” or erroneously pessimistic at baseline and that this cognitive bias was remediated following psilocybin treatment (Figure 3B).

The mean amount of bias for the control subjects was negligible and did not significantly differ from zero at baseline [M = 0.04, SD = 0.09, t(14) = 1.823, p = 0.090; 95% CI (-0.09, 0.01)] or the repeat phase [M = 0.01, SD = 0.07, t(14) = 0.460, p = 0.653; 95% CI (-0.05, 0.03)]. This demonstrates an effective absence of bias (i.e., no appreciable optimism or pessimism bias) in the control subjects at both baseline and the follow-up (Figure 3B).

Not surprisingly, bias scores were significantly lower (i.e., greater pessimism) for patients (M = -0.10, SD = 0.12) than controls at baseline [M = 0.04, SD = 0.09; t(28) = 3.704, p < 0.001; 95% CI (0.06, 0.22), g = 1.3], but there was no significant difference for the repeat phase [t(28) = -0.265, p = 0.793; 95% CI (-0.07, 0.05)]. These results demonstrate that the patients were more pessimistic than the controls at baseline, but this bias was alleviated to control levels following psilocybin treatment (Figure 3B).

Relationship Between Cognitive Biases and Depressive Symptoms

Next, we investigated any potential relationships between baseline pessimism and depressive symptoms. Before psilocybin, the patients’ bias scores were significantly related to their BDI scores (rs = -0.55, p = 0.017). This finding demonstrates that greater pessimism (i.e., lower bias scores) was associated with more severe depressive symptoms in the patients. No significant relationships were found between BDI and bias scores for the control subjects (rs = -0.22, p = 0.219) (Figure 4A).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. (A) Relationship between depressive symptoms and bias. At baseline, the patients’ bias scores were moderately and significantly related to their BDI scores (rs = -0.55, p = 0.017). No significant relationships were found between BDI and bias scores for the control subjects (rs = -0.22, p = 0.219). (B) Relationship between the change in depressive symptoms and bias. Bias and BDI scores changed significantly more for the patients than the control subjects’ bias [t(28) = -3.167, p = 0.004; 95% CI (-0.24, -0.05), g = 1.2] and BDI scores [t(28) = 7.228, p < 0.001; 95% CI (15.09, 27.04), g = 2.6]. The decrease in BDI score was significantly related to the decrease in pessimism for the patients (r = -0.57, p = 0.014). There was no significant relationship found between the change in BDI and bias scores (rs = -0.007, p = 0.980) for the control subjects.

When analyzing the magnitude of change in scores between baseline and the follow-up, it was found that the bias (M = 0.11, SD = 0.16) and BDI scores (M = -22.20, SD = 10.88) changed significantly more for the patients than the control subjects [bias: M = -0.03, SD = 0.73, t(28) = -3.167, p = 0.004, 95% CI (-0.24, -0.05), g = 1.2; BDI: M = -1.13, SD = 2.99, t(28) = 7.228, p < 0.001, 95% CI (15.09, 27.04), g = 2.6]. The decrease in BDI score was significantly related to the decrease in pessimism for the patients (r = -0.57, p = 0.014; Figure 4B). There was no significant relationship found between the change in BDI and bias scores (rs = -0.007, p = 0.980) for the control subjects.

Discussion

The present study sought to investigate the effects of psilocybin on pessimism bias in patients with TRD. Before treatment with psilocybin, patients were excessively and unrealistically pessimistic when predicting the occurrence of future life events – and this pessimism was significantly correlated with the severity of their depressive symptoms. One week after treatment, the patients’ pessimism was alleviated and their depressive symptoms greatly improved; moreover, the magnitude of change in both variables was related – such that as their depression improved, so did their ability to accurately forecast their future. No such bias nor change in forecasting was seen in a matched control group assessed over an equivalent time period. Taken together, these findings indicate that the psychologically supportive administration of psilocybin remediates negative cognitive biases characteristic of severe depression – enabling individuals to forecast their futures more accurately, unfettered by unrealistic pessimism.

Depressive Symptoms, Biases and Accuracy of Predictions

The alleviation of depressive symptoms one week after psilocybin treatment found here is consistent with previous research demonstrating the therapeutic potential of psychedelic compounds (Carhart-Harris and Goodwin, 2017). The ability of psychedelics to disrupt reinforced patterns of negative thought and behavior by disintegrating the patterns of brain activity upon which they rest may account for their positive therapeutic effects (Carhart-Harris et al., 2014; Watts et al., 2017). A single dose of the natural psychedelic brew prepared from Amazonian plants, ayahuasca, had rapid, and sustained antidepressant effects in patients with recurrent MDD (Osório Fde et al., 2015). Moreover, a single dose of psilocybin has also been shown to induce long-lasting reductions in anxiety and depression as well as increases in quality of life, life meaning, and optimism in patients suffering from depression and anxiety reactive to advanced-stage cancer (Grob et al., 2011; Griffiths et al., 2016; Ross et al., 2016). Specifically regarding TRD, we previously showed significant reductions in depressive symptoms 1-week after psilocybin treatment, with maximal reductions seen at 5 weeks post-treatment. For many of the patients, these reductions persisted 6 months later. Enduring improvements in psychological wellbeing (Griffiths et al., 2008), trait openness and optimism (MacLean et al., 2011; Carhart-Harris et al., 2016b) have also been observed in healthy volunteers following a single dose of a psychedelic drug.

As predicted by the cognitive-bias model of depression (Beck, 1967, 1976), here we found a significant pessimism bias in the TRD patients when predicting the likelihood of future events prior to psilocybin treatment. Consistent with the depressive bias hypothesis—i.e. the theory that as depressive symptoms increase in severity, judgments become more negatively biased (Beck, 1976)—we found greater pessimism among patients with more severe depressive symptoms. This is in line with previous studies demonstrating a relationship between depression and pessimism (Peterson and Seligman, 1984; Hollon et al., 1986; Beck et al., 1988; Hill et al., 1989). It also opens up the possibility of using pessimism in healthy samples as a proxy of depression in clinically depressed samples. The finding here that patients were unrealistically pessimistic about the likelihood of positive life events befalling them prior to psilocybin treatment appeared not to be entirely justified by poor life circumstances, as the patients’ predictions (e.g., of the likelihood of desirable life events) underestimated their actual rate of occurrence. Moreover, the rates at which desirable and undesirable events actually occurred did not differ from the control subjects, who lacked such biases. This is consistent with classic depression research demonstrating that depressed patients present with excessively negative perspectives that are disproportionate to their true life circumstances (Beck, 1967; Strunk et al., 2006; Strunk and Adler, 2009).

One week after psilocybin treatment, we found that the patients’ pessimism was alleviated and they no longer showed any cognitive biases. In line with this finding, previous research has demonstrated sustained increases in optimism following psilocybin treatment in patients suffering from depression and anxiety reactive to advanced-stage cancer (Grob et al., 2011; Griffiths et al., 2016; Ross et al., 2016). Long-lasting improvements in optimism have also been observed in healthy volunteers following a single dose of a psychedelic (MacLean et al., 2011; Carhart-Harris et al., 2016b) and there is also evidence that recreational psychedelic users are more optimistic (or less pessimistic) than non-users (Grob et al., 1996). Psychedelic compounds have previously been shown to increase positive mood (Kraehenmann et al., 2015; Schmid et al., 2015) and often induce lasting changes in attitudes and behavior associated with a more positive outlook (Savage, 1962; McGlothlin and Arnold, 1971; Griffiths et al., 2006, 2008; Studerus et al., 2011). Here, we found that patients predicted significantly more desirable events post-treatment; making more accurate predictions that better reflected their actual life situation. No cognitive biases were found among the control subjects.

The lack of cognitive bias in the controls found here is in contrast to previous research suggesting that healthy individuals have a tendency towards an optimism bias (Taylor and Brown, 1988; Weinstein, 1989; Sharot et al., 2007). Evidence suggests that healthy people are worse at incorporating information about a more negative than expected future, and instead are more likely to update their beliefs in response to better-than-expected information, resulting in an unrealistic optimism (Izuma and Adolphs, 2011; Sharot et al., 2011; Garrett et al., 2014; Korn et al., 2014; Garrett and Sharot, 2017). It has been shown that this unrealistic optimism is maintained due to a selective update failure and the diminished neural coding of undesirable information (Sharot et al., 2011). Dismissing and/or denying undesirable information may serve an adaptive function by enhancing exploratory behavior and reducing stress and anxiety (Scheier et al., 1989; Taylor et al., 2000; Varki, 2009). This is in line with research showing a relationship between mild depression and accuracy in predictions, with more severe depression being associated with clear pessimism (Strunk et al., 2006). However, an unrealistic optimism has drawbacks as well, e.g., in terms of underestimating risks and so failing to adopt preventative and/or protective measures (Shepperd et al., 2013).

This idea that optimism is conducive to positive mental health has been critiqued recently however, e.g., with arguments made that improved methodologies are required to elucidate such biases and an accurate perception of reality is most important for mental health (Colvin and Block, 1994; Shah et al., 2016) – see also Letheby (2016) in specific relation to the psychedelic experience. Entertaining worst-case scenarios when anticipating future events may help one’s ability to cope with negative outcomes, through contingency plans and preparedness. In this sense, there are benefits of having a pessimistic attitude and expecting an undesirable outcome when facing events of unknown emotional impact (Nesse, 2000), such as not engaging in risky behavior (Gibson and Sanbonmatsu, 2004) and avoiding disappointment by setting low expectations (Norem and Cantor, 1986; Shepperd and McNulty, 2002). However, an unrealistic pessimism about the future also represents a key cognitive feature in MDD (Pyszczynski et al., 1987; Lavender and Watkins, 2004), as expressed in the cognitive triad (Beck, 1967). It follows that a balanced, reality-focused perspective is optimal for mental health – as unrealistic optimism may encourage negligent and reckless behavior that could result in catastrophe, and unrealistic pessimism may lead to avoidant behavior, passivity, low mood, vulnerability to MDD and perhaps related disorders (Hecht, 2013).

The neurobiology of optimism versus pessimism is poorly understood. It is well known however, that serotonergic mechanisms are involved in mood states and cognitive styles (Meneses, 1999; Young and Leyton, 2002; Cowen and Sherwood, 2013); for example, the 5-HT2A receptor, which is the key receptor through which psychedelics elicit their signature effects (Glennon et al., 1984; Nichols, 2017), is upregulated during states of low synaptic 5-HT (Cahir et al., 2007) as well as in unmedicated depressed patients (Bhagwagar et al., 2006) and individuals scoring high on neuroticism (Frokjaer et al., 2008, 2010) and dysfunctional attitudes (Meyer et al., 2003) – an analog of trait pessimism. We recently proposed a mechanistic model by which psychedelic-induced 5-HT2AR signaling rapidly induces an acute state of plasticity in which an enriched context may lead to the revision of cognitive biases (Carhart-Harris and Goodwin, 2017; Carhart-Harris and Nutt, 2017; Carhart-Harris et al., 2018). MDD is associated with structural brain changes that clinically reflect the presence of cognitive and emotional biases (Serafini et al., 2014). Psychedelics have been shown to temporarily deconstruct the default-mode network (DMN) (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012; Palhano-Fontes et al., 2015; Speth et al., 2016), a network in the brain associated with ruminative thought (Berman et al., 2011), self-reflection (Johnson et al., 2002) and introspection more generally (Fleming et al., 2010; Qin and Northoff, 2011) – although the DMN appears to recover its integrity as the effects of a psychedelic wear off, with a potential increase in integration post treatment with psilocybin for TRD (Carhart-Harris et al., 2017b; Watts et al., 2017). To account for the post-treatment brain effects of psilocybin, we recently proposed a “reset” mechanism by which acute modular disintegration may enable a subsequent reintegration and resumption of normal functioning, accompanied by improvements in mood (Carhart-Harris et al., 2017b). We hope to explore the relationship between DMN properties before, during and after psilocybin in future studies and assess its relationship to optimism/pessimism. A functional imaging paradigm that incorporates the actual forecasting of future life events (prospection) might be interesting to explore in this context.

Methodological Strengths

The results reported here substantially extend findings of previous research demonstrating increased optimism following psychedelic drug use (Barbosa et al., 2009; Griffiths et al., 2006, 2011; Carhart-Harris et al., 2016b). Previous studies of psychedelics-induced optimism have used subjective measures, whereas the present study utilized a behavioral instrument to more objectively measure optimism/pessimism. By analyzing the effects of psilocybin on the prediction of one’s personal future, this study addresses an important facet of the cognitive triad, i.e., a negative attitude towards oneself, the environment and the future. To our knowledge, this is the first study to analyze the effects of a psychedelic compound on cognitive biases in patients with TRD and also the first to address psychedelic-induced changes in biases that are not purely subjective and self-referential, but instead feature a behavioral aspect, requiring the predictions of future life events – the accuracy of which can later be assessed based on what is reported to have actually occurred in the individual’s life.

Limitations

It is necessary to address some limitations of this study. It formed part of an open-label clinical trial (Carhart-Harris et al., 2016a) with a small sample size, and although a control group was recruited to examine test re-test reliability on these measures, the controls were healthy subjects not exposed to the same treatment procedures. The cross-sectional design of this study and the inability to confirm the present data using further prospective follow-ups must also be considered. It is possible that a natural decline of depressive symptoms contributed to our findings. Additionally, since treatment with psilocybin involved more than just drug administration (e.g., psychological support before and after the psilocybin dosing sessions), it is possible that some drug-unrelated factor(s) contributed to the changes in cognitive bias observed here. It should be emphasized, however, that such non-drug components are considered an integral part of the therapeutic model and cannot easily be extricated from it without collapsing the model itself (e.g., see Carhart-Harris et al., 2018). The specificity of our main results also requires careful consideration. It is unclear whether the reported changes in cognitive bias found here following psilocybin treatment were selective for these outcomes or rather an epiphenomenon of the treatment’s core effects on depressive symptoms. It might be that cognitive biases are an inextricable part of depression and so an impact on one of these factors will necessarily affect the other. The question of causality is of central relevance here, and only further research can elucidate this. In this context, we would like to propose that there is a common mediating factor at play, driving both the improvements in mental health and changes in biases seen here – as well as elsewhere with psilocybin and other psychedelics (Krebs and Johansen, 2013; Hendricks et al., 2015; Carhart-Harris et al., 2016b; Watts et al., 2017).

Conclusion

In conclusion, this study sought to investigate the effects of psilocybin with psychological support using the Prediction of Future Life Events task in patients with TRD. To our knowledge, this is the first study using a behavioral measure to objectively address cognitive biases integral to depression and how these change post-treatment with a psychedelic. The present findings suggest that the psilocybin with psychological support treatment model may alleviate pessimism bias in depression, giving patients a clearer, more accurate outlook on their future. Further controlled studies are warranted to better determine the causality, reliability, specificity and durability of these findings.

Author Contributions

RC-H conceptualized the study design. TL and RC-H collected the data and TL plotted, analyzed and interpreted the data. TL wrote the manuscript and RC-H provided edits.

Funding

This study was funded by an MRC clinical development scheme grant (UK MRC: MR/J00460X/1). TL was supported by an MRC-DTP Studentship and RC-H was supported by the Alex Mosley Charitable Trust.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This article/paper/report presents independent research funded by the UK MRC and supported by the NIHR CRF and BRC at Imperial College Healthcare NHS Trust. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the MRC, the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01721/full#supplementary-material

References

Al-Harbi, K. S. (2012). Treatment-resistant depression: therapeutic trends, challenges, and future directions. Patient Prefer. Adherence 6, 369–388. doi: 10.2147/PPA.S29716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alloy, L. B., and Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: sadder but wiser? J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 108, 441–485. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.108.4.441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbosa, P. C., Cazorla, I. M., Giglio, J. S., and Strassman, R. (2009). A six-month prospective evaluation of personality traits, psychiatric symptoms and quality of life in ayahuasca-naive subjects. J. Psychoactive Drugs 41, 205–212. doi: 10.1080/02791072.2009.10400530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects. New York, NY: Hoeber Medical Division.

Google Scholar

Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders. New York, NY: International Universities Press.

Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Riskind, J. H., Brown, G., and Steer, R. A. (1988). Levels of hopelessness in DSM-III disorders: a partial test of content specificity in depression. Cogn. Ther. Res. 12, 459–469. doi: 10.1007/BF01173413

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., and Rush, A. J. (1979). Cognitive Therapy of Depression. New York, NY: Guilford.

Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., and Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4, 561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beliveau, V., Ganz, M., Feng, L., Ozenne, B., Højgaard, L., Fisher, P. M., et al. (2017). A high-resolution in vivo atlas of the human brain’s serotonin system. J. Neurosci. 37, 120–128. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2830-16.2016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berman, M. G., Peltier, S., Nee, D. E., Kross, E., Deldin, P. J., and Jonides, J. (2011). Depression, rumination and the default network. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 548–555. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhagwagar, Z., Hinz, R., Taylor, M., Fancy, S., Cowen, P., and Grasby, P. (2006). Increased 5-HT(2A) receptor binding in euthymic, medication-free patients recovered from depression: a positron emission study with [(11)C]MDL 100,907. Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 1580–1587. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.9.1580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman, E. M., and Beck, A. T. (2006). The empirical status of cognitive-behavioral therapy: a review of meta-analyses. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 26, 17–31. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahir, M., Ardis, T., Reynolds, G. P., and Cooper, S. J. (2007). Acute and chronic tryptophan depletion differentially regulate central 5-HT1A and 5-HT 2A receptor binding in the rat. Psychopharmacology 190, 497–506. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0635-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Bolstridge, M., Day, C. M. J., Rucker, J., Watts, R., Erritzoe, D. E., et al. (2017a). Psilocybin with psychological support for treatment-resistant depression: six-month follow-up. Psychopharmacology 235, 399–408. doi: 10.1007/s00213-017-4771-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Roseman, L., Bolstridge, M., Demetriou, L., Pannekoek, J. N., Wall, M. B., et al. (2017b). Psilocybin for treatment-resistant depression: fMRI-measured brain mechanisms. Sci. Rep. 7:13187. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-13282-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Bolstridge, M., Rucker, J., Day, C. M., Erritzoe, D., Kaelen, M., et al. (2016a). Psilocybin with psychological support for treatment-resistant depression: an open-label feasibility study. Lancet Psychiatry 3, 619–627. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30065-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Kaelen, M., Bolstridge, M., Williams, T. M., Williams, L. T., Underwood, R., et al. (2016b). The paradoxical psychological effects of lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD). Psychol. Med. 46, 1379–1390. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715002901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Erritzoe, D., Williams, T., Stone, J. M., Reed, L. J., Colasanti, A., et al. (2012). Neural correlates of the psychedelic state as determined by fMRI studies with psilocybin. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 2138–2143. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1119598109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., and Goodwin, G. M. (2017). The therapeutic potential of psychedelic drugs: past, present and future. Neuropsychopharmacology 42, 2105–2113. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.84

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Leech, R., Hellyer, P. J., Shanahan, M., Feilding, A., Tagliazucchi, E., et al. (2014). The entropic brain: a theory of conscious states informed by neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:20. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., and Nutt, D. J. (2017). Serotonin and brain function: a tale of two receptors. J. Psychopharmacol. 31, 1091–1120. doi: 10.1177/0269881117725915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Roseman, L., Haijen, E., Erritzoe, D., Watts, R., Branchi, I., et al. (2018). Psychedelics and the essential importance of context. J. Psychopharmacol. 32, 725–731. doi: 10.1177/0269881118754710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colvin, C. R., and Block, J. (1994). Do positive illusions foster mental health? An examination of the Taylor and Brown formulation. Psychol. Bull. 116, 3–20. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowen, P., and Sherwood, A. C. (2013). The role of serotonin in cognitive function: evidence from recent studies and implications for understanding depression. J. Psychopharmacol. 27, 575–583. doi: 10.1177/0269881113482531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dos Santos, R. G., Osório, F. L., Crippa, J. A., Riba, J., Zuardi, A. W., and Hallak, J. E. (2016). Antidepressive, anxiolytic, and antiaddictive effects of ayahuasca, psilocybin and lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD): a systematic review of clinical trials published in the last 25 years. Ther. Adv. Psychopharmacol. 6, 193–213. doi: 10.1177/2045125316638008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleming, S. M., Weil, R. S., Nagy, Z., Dolan, R. J., and Rees, G. (2010). Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure. Science 329, 1541–1543. doi: 10.1126/science.1191883

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frokjaer, V. G., Mortensen, E. L., Nielsen, F. A., Haugbol, S., Pinborg, L. H., Adams, K. H., et al. (2008). Frontolimbic serotonin 2A receptor binding in healthy subjects is associated with personality risk factors for affective disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 569–576. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frokjaer, V. G., Vinberg, M., Erritzoe, D., Baaré, W., Holst, K. K., Mortensen, E. L., et al. (2010). Familial risk for mood disorder and the personality risk factor, neuroticism, interact in their association with frontolimbic serotonin 2A receptor binding. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 1129–1137. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, N., and Sharot, T. (2017). Optimistic update bias holds firm: three tests of robustness following Shah et al. Conscious. Cogn. 50, 12–22. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, N., Sharot, T., Faulkner, P., Korn, C. W., Roiser, J. P., and Dolan, R. J. (2014). Losing the rose tinted glasses: neural substrates of unbiased belief updating in depression. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:639. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaynes, B. N. (2009). Identifying difficult-to-treat depression: differential diagnosis, subtypes, and comorbidities. J. Clin. Psychiatry 70(Suppl. 6), 10–15. doi: 10.4088/JCP.8133su1c.02

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, B., and Sanbonmatsu, D. M. (2004). Optimism, pessimism, and gambling: the downside of optimism. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30, 149–160. doi: 10.1177/0146167203259929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glennon, R. A., Titeler, M., and McKenney, J. D. (1984). Evidence for 5-HT2 involvement in the mechanism of action of hallucinogenic agents. Life Sci. 35, 2505–2511. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(84)90436-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greden, J. F. (2001). The burden of disease for treatment-resistant depression. J. Clin. Psychiatry 62(Suppl. 16), 26–31.

Google Scholar

Griffiths, R., Richards, W., Johnson, M., McCann, U., and Jesse, R. (2008). Mystical-type experiences occasioned by psilocybin mediate the attribution of personal meaning and spiritual significance 14 months later. J. Psychopharmacol. 22, 621–632. doi: 10.1177/0269881108094300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, R. R., Johnson, M. W., Carducci, M. A., Umbricht, A., Richards, W. A., Richards, B. D., et al. (2016). Psilocybin produces substantial and sustained decreases in depression and anxiety in patients with life-threatening cancer: a randomized double-blind trial. J. Psychopharmacol. 30, 1181–1197. doi: 10.1177/0269881116675513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, R. R., Johnson, M. W., Richards, W. A., Richards, B. D., McCann, U., and Jesse, R. (2011). Psilocybin occasioned mystical-type experiences: immediate and persisting dose-related effects. Psychopharmacology 218, 649–665. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2358-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, R. R., Richards, W. A., McCann, U., and Jesse, R. (2006). Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance. Psychopharmacology 187, 268–283; discussion 284–292. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0457-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grob, C. S., Danforth, A. L., Chopra, G. S., Hagerty, M., McKay, C. R., Halberstadt, A. L., et al. (2011). Pilot study of psilocybin treatment for anxiety in patients with advanced-stage cancer. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68, 71–78. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grob, C. S., McKenna, D. J., Callaway, J. C., Brito, G. S., Neves, E. S., Oberlaender, G., et al. (1996). Human psychopharmacology of hoasca, a plant hallucinogen used in ritual context in Brazil. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 184, 86–94. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199602000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, J. P., Farmer, M., Fogelman, P., and Gotlib, I. H. (2015). Depressive rumination, the default-mode network, and the dark matter of clinical neuroscience. Biol. Psychiatry 78, 224–230. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hecht, D. (2013). The neural basis of optimism and pessimism. Exp. Neurobiol. 22, 173–199. doi: 10.5607/en.2013.22.3.173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hendricks, P. S., Thorne, C. B., Clark, C. B., Coombs, D. W., and Johnson, M. W. (2015). Classic psychedelic use is associated with reduced psychological distress and suicidality in the United States adult population. J. Psychopharmacol. 29, 280–288. doi: 10.1177/0269881114565653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, C., Oei, T., and Hill, M. (1989). An empirical investigation of the specificity and sensitivity of the automatic thoughts questionnaire and dysfunctional attitudes scale. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 11, 291–311. doi: 10.1007/BF00961529

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollon, S. D., Kendall, P. C., and Lumry, A. (1986). Specificity of depressotypic cognitions in clinical depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 95, 52–59. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.95.1.52

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izuma, K., and Adolphs, R. (2011). The brain’s rose-colored glasses. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1355–1356. doi: 10.1038/nn.2960

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, S. C., Baxter, L. C., Wilder, L. S., Pipe, J. G., Heiserman, J. E., and Prigatano, G. P. (2002). Neural correlates of self-reflection. Brain 125, 1808–1814. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korn, C. W., Sharot, T., Walter, H., Heekeren, H. R., and Dolan, R. J. (2014). Depression is related to an absence of optimistically biased belief updating about future life events. Psychol. Med. 44, 579–592. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraehenmann, R., Preller, K. H., Scheidegger, M., Pokorny, T., Bosch, O. G., Seifritz, E., et al. (2015). Psilocybin-induced decrease in amygdala reactivity correlates with enhanced positive mood in healthy volunteers. Biol. Psychiatry 78, 572–581. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krebs, T. S., and Johansen, P. O. (2013). Psychedelics and mental health: a population study. PLoS One 8:e63972. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavender, A., and Watkins, E. (2004). Rumination and future thinking in depression. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 43, 129–142. doi: 10.1348/014466504323088015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Letheby, C. (2016). The epistemic innocence of psychedelic states. Conscious. Cogn. 39, 28–37. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liechti, M. E. (2017). Modern clinical research on LSD. Neuropsychopharmacology 42, 2114–2127. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.86

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLean, K. A., Johnson, M. W., and Griffiths, R. R. (2011). Mystical experiences occasioned by the hallucinogen psilocybin lead to increases in the personality domain of openness. J. Psychopharmacol. 25, 1453–1461. doi: 10.1177/0269881111420188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcus, M., Yasamy, T., van Ommeren, M., et al. (2012). “Depression: a global public health concern,” in Proceedings of the 65th World Health Assembly, (Geneva: World Health Organization).

Google Scholar

Matias, S., Lottem, E., Dugué, G. P., and Mainen, Z. F. (2017). Activity patterns of serotonin neurons underlying cognitive flexibility. eLife 6:e20552. doi: 10.7554/eLife.20552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGlothlin, W. H., and Arnold, D. O. (1971). LSD revisited. A ten-year follow-up of medical LSD use. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 24, 35–49. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1971.01750070037005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meneses, A. (1999). 5-HT system and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 1111–1125. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00067-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, J. H., McMain, S., Kennedy, S. H., Korman, L., Brown, G. M., DaSilva, J. N., et al. (2003). Dysfunctional attitudes and 5-HT2 receptors during depression and self-harm. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 90–99. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.90

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nesse, R. M. (2000). Is depression an adaptation? Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, 14–20. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nichols, D. E. (2017). Chemistry and structure-activity relationships of psychedelics. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 36, 1–43. doi: 10.1007/7854_2017_475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norem, J. K., and Cantor, N. (1986). Defensive pessimism: harnessing anxiety as motivation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1208–1217. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osório Fde, L., Sanches, R. F., Macedo, L. R., Santos, R. G., Maia-de-Oliveira, J. P., Wichert-Ana, L., et al. (2015). Antidepressant effects of a single dose of ayahuasca in patients with recurrent depression: a preliminary report. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. 37, 13–20. doi: 10.1590/1516-4446-2014-1496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palhano-Fontes, F., Andrade, K. C., Tofoli, L. F., Santos, A. C., Crippa, J. A., Hallak, J. E., et al. (2015). The psychedelic state induced by ayahuasca modulates the activity and connectivity of the default mode network. PLoS One 10:e0118143. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, C., and Seligman, M. E. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: theory and evidence. Psychol. Rev. 91, 347–374. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.91.3.347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pyszczynski, T., Holt, K., and Greenberg, J. (1987). Depression, self-focused attention, and expectancies for positive and negative future life events for self and others. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52, 994–1001. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.5.994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qin, P., and Northoff, G. (2011). How is our self related to midline regions and the default-mode network? Neuroimage 57, 1221–1233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, S., Bossis, A., Guss, J., Agin-Liebes, G., Malone, T., Cohen, B., et al. (2016). Rapid and sustained symptom reduction following psilocybin treatment for anxiety and depression in patients with life-threatening cancer: a randomized controlled trial. J. Psychopharmacol. 30, 1165–1180. doi: 10.1177/0269881116675512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savage, C. (1962). LSD, alcoholism and transcendence. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 135, 429–435.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Scheier, M. F., Matthews, K. A., Owens, J. F., Magovern, G. J. Sr., Lefebvre, R. C., Abbott, R. A., et al. (1989). Dispositional optimism and recovery from coronary artery bypass surgery: the beneficial effects on physical and psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1024–1040. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmid, Y., Enzler, F., Gasser, P., Grouzmann, E., Preller, K. H., Vollenweider, F. X., et al. (2015). Acute effects of lysergic acid diethylamide in healthy subjects. Biol. Psychiatry 78, 544–553. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serafini, G., Hayley, S., Pompili, M., Dwivedi, Y., Brahmachari, G., Girardi, P., et al. (2014). Hippocampal neurogenesis, neurotrophic factors and depression: possible therapeutic targets? CNS Neurol. Disord. Drug Targets 13, 1708–1721. doi: 10.2174/1871527313666141130223723

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shah, P., Harris, A. J., Bird, G., Catmur, C., and Hahn, U. (2016). A pessimistic view of optimistic belief updating. Cogn. Psychol. 90, 71–127. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharot, T., Korn, C. W., and Dolan, R. J. (2011). How unrealistic optimism is maintained in the face of reality. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1475–1479. doi: 10.1038/nn.2949

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharot, T., Riccardi, A. M., Raio, C. M., and Phelps, E. A. (2007). Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias. Nature 450, 102–105. doi: 10.1038/nature06280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shepperd, J. A., Klein, W. M., Waters, E. A., and Weinstein, N. D. (2013). Taking stock of unrealistic optimism. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 395–411. doi: 10.1177/1745691613485247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shepperd, J. A., and McNulty, J. K. (2002). The affective consequences of expected and unexpected outcomes. Psychol. Sci. 13, 85–88. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speth, J., Speth, C., Kaelen, M., Schloerscheidt, A. M., Feilding, A., Nutt, D. J., et al. (2016). Decreased mental time travel to the past correlates with default-mode network disintegration under lysergic acid diethylamide. J. Psychopharmacol. 30, 344–353. doi: 10.1177/0269881116628430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strunk, D. R., and Adler, A. D. (2009). Cognitive biases in three prediction tasks: a test of the cognitive model of depression. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 34–40. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strunk, D. R., Lopez, H., and DeRubeis, R. J. (2006). Depressive symptoms are associated with unrealistic negative predictions of future life events. Behav. Res. Ther. 44, 861–882. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Studerus, E., Kometer, M., Hasler, F., and Vollenweider, F. X. (2011). Acute, subacute and long-term subjective effects of psilocybin in healthy humans: a pooled analysis of experimental studies. J. Psychopharmacol. 25, 1434–1452. doi: 10.1177/0269881110382466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. E., and Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychol. Bull. 103, 193–210. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Reed, G. M., Bower, J. E., and Gruenewald, T. L. (2000). Psychological resources, positive illusions, and health. Am. Psychol. 55, 99–109. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.99

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varki, A. (2009). Human uniqueness and the denial of death. Nature 460:684. doi: 10.1038/460684c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watts, R., Day, C., Krzanowski, J., Nutt, D., and Carhart-Harris, R. (2017). Patients’ accounts of increased “Connectedness” and “Acceptance” after psilocybin for treatment-resistant depression. J. Humanist. Psychol. 57, 520–564. doi: 10.1177/0022167817709585

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinstein, N. D. (1989). Optimistic biases about personal risks. Science 246, 1232–1233. doi: 10.1126/science.2686031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, S. N., and Leyton, M. (2002). The role of serotonin in human mood and social interaction. Insight from altered tryptophan levels. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 71, 857–865. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(01)00670-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: treatment-resistant depression, psilocybin, cognitive bias, pessimism, forecasting

Citation: Lyons T and Carhart-Harris R (2018) More Realistic Forecasting of Future Life Events After Psilocybin for Treatment-Resistant Depression. Front. Psychol. 9:1721. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01721

Received: 23 May 2018; Accepted: 24 August 2018;
Published: 12 October 2018.

Edited by:

Nuno Barbosa Rocha, Escola Superior de Saúde do Porto, Politécnico do Porto, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Gianluca Serafini, Ospedale San Martino (IRCCS), Italy
Felix Müller, Universitäre Psychiatrische Kliniken Basel, Switzerland

Copyright © 2018 Lyons and Carhart-Harris. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Taylor Lyons, t.lyons15@imperial.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.