Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 08 June 2018
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Methods for Evaluating Emotions Evoked by Food Experiences: A Literature Review

  • 1Kikkoman Europe R&D Laboratory B.V., Wageningen, Netherlands
  • 2Microbiology and Systems Biology, TNO, Zeist, Netherlands
  • 3Perceptual and Cognitive Systems, TNO, Soesterberg, Netherlands
  • 4Human Media Interaction, University of Twente, Enschede, Netherlands

Besides sensory characteristics of food, food-evoked emotion is a crucial factor in predicting consumer's food preference and therefore in developing new products. Many measures have been developed to assess food-evoked emotions. The aim of this literature review is (i) to give an exhaustive overview of measures used in current research and (ii) to categorize these methods along measurement level (physiological, behavioral, and cognitive) and emotional processing level (unconscious sensory, perceptual/early cognitive, and conscious/decision making) level. This 3 × 3 categorization may help researchers to compile a set of complementary measures (“toolbox”) for their studies. We included 101 peer-reviewed articles that evaluate consumer's emotions and were published between 1997 and 2016, providing us with 59 different measures. More than 60% of these measures are based on self-reported, subjective ratings and questionnaires (cognitive measurement level) and assess the conscious/decision-making level of emotional processing. This multitude of measures and their overrepresentation in a single category hinders the comparison of results across studies and building a complete multi-faceted picture of food-evoked emotions. We recommend (1) to use widely applied, validated measures only, (2) to refrain from using (highly correlated) measures from the same category but use measures from different categories instead, preferably covering all three emotional processing levels, and (3) to acquire and share simultaneously collected physiological, behavioral, and cognitive datasets to improve the predictive power of food choice and other models.

Introduction

People experience and appreciate many types of food and beverages (referred to as “foods” in this study) during their life. Specific emotions have been considered as determinants of affective responses to foods (Ferber and Cabanac, 1987; Willner and Healy, 1994) and food-related behavior including food choice (e.g., Oliver and Wardle, 1999; Russell, 2003; Gibson, 2006; Thomson et al., 2010; Ng et al., 2013; Dalenberg et al., 2014; Gutjar et al., 2015a; Kenney and Adhikari, 2016). Liking ratings do not predict food choice behavior accurately (Zandstra and El-Deredy, 2011; Griffioen-Roose et al., 2013). Gutjar et al. (2015b) suggested that food-evoked emotions can predict individual's food choice more accurate than liking scores. Dalenberg et al. (2014) mention that consumers' emotions add predictive power to a food choice (predicting) model based on hedonic scales. These studies suggest that assessing emotional responses to foods may reveal previously unknown product attributes which can be a valuable source of information for product development and marketing that goes beyond traditional sensory and acceptability measurements (Thomson et al., 2010). Therefore, it is important to obtain valid and reliable (combinations of) measurements of food-evoked emotion.

Despite its importance, different authors use different definitions of emotion. For instance, King and Meiselman (2010) define emotions as brief, intense physiological and mental reactions, Gibson (2006) defines emotions as short-term affective responses to the appraisal of particular stimuli, Bagozzi et al. (1999) define emotion as a mental state of readiness that arises from cognitive appraisals of events or thoughts, and Cabanac (2002) proposed that an emotion is any mental experience with high intensity and high hedonic content (pleasure/displeasure). These different definitions contain elements of both internal and external, bottom-up and top-down, as well as physiological and cognitive elements. All these facets are considered relevant and illustrate that there is not a single measure that would be able to capture the full range of relevant aspects. To organize the complex response patterns, we introduce a conceptual framework in section Assessment Methods and Their Classification, including the methodologies to assess the different response patterns as there is a wide variety of instruments available.

Verbal self-reporting questionnaires are the most commonly used techniques to measure emotional responses, due to their ease of application, cost-effectiveness and discriminative power (Churchill and Behan, 2010; Dorado et al., 2016). However, they have specific shortcomings, including: (1) emotions are difficult to verbalize (Köster and Mojet, 2015), (2) the “emotional” lexicon varies across cultures and languages, particularly when it comes to foods (Gutjar et al., 2015b), (3) verbalizing emotions can interfere with the food experience itself, and (4) self-reports only capture conscious, declared opinions (Winkielman et al., 2011; Venkatraman et al., 2015). Wilson et al. (1993) asked their participants to answer whether they liked or disliked five different posters with or without providing the reason why. Subsequently, they could take one of the posters with them. Participants who provided the reasons were less satisfied with their choice 3 weeks later (Wilson et al., 1993) showing that questioning individuals about affective experience can affect the affective experience itself. Regarding EsSense Profile, one of the most widely used self-report questionnaires for evaluating an individual's emotional responses (King and Meiselman, 2010), Jaeger et al. (2013) stated that this technique might not capture the full range of emotions individuals may experience in response to food and therefore may not properly measure food-evoked emotions. Thus, it seems worth the effort to include other types of measurements as well like behavioral and physiological measurements. Köster (2009) proposed that research groups should develop implicit measurement techniques and use these where possible and combine them with explicit measures if feasible in order to compare and eventually cross-validate results Examples include facial expression recognition (happy, sad, angry, surprised, scared, and disgust: Kostyra et al., 2016) and physiological variables reflecting activity of the autonomic nervous system (ANS: de Wijk et al., 2012). However, there is no “golden standard” to assess food-evoked emotions at this moment yet.

We aim to provide an exhaustive list of tools that have been used to measure food-evoked emotions over the last 20 years. We also categorize them using a general model describing the relevant aspects of emotion processing and the range of methodologies to assess the relevant facets (see section Assessment Methods and Their Classification). This categorization helps to identify gaps in the currently prevailing set of instruments and enables researchers to choose (a combination of) measures in a balanced way. Our categorization indicates to what extent different methods are redundant or complementary and helps researchers in this area to compile a set of complementary methods that provides the maximal amount of information. In addition, it may serve to guide further development of new methods to assess food evoked emotions that predict future consumer behavior.

Literature Search

We used the databases of PsycINFO to select relevant articles that were published between January 1997 until the end of December 2016.

Inclusion Criteria

We used the following inclusion criteria:

1. The article should report empirical studies in peer-reviewed journals and be written in English.

2. The article should include original data from healthy human populations.

3. The study should investigate consumers' emotions evoked by directly experiencing foods. “Direct experience” could be tasting foods, viewing images of food, or sniffing food odorants. The following were considered to be indirect (and therefore excluded from this review): viewing packages, viewing printed names of brands and thinking about food or beverages (e.g., by asking “How do you feel when you think of “apple”?”).

4. In this study the term “emotions” includes hedonic liking, pleasantness, preference, and moods.

Gibson (2006) describes moods as more long-lasting psychological arousal states than emotions with interacting dimensions related to energy, tension and pleasure that may appear and persist in the absence of obvious stimuli and may be more covert to observers. However, mood and emotion both reflect emotional states and are often used interchangeably in common language (Köster and Mojet, 2015). Also, as Gibson (2006) mentions, the relationships between mood, emotions and physiological arousal may be complex. Therefore, we included “mood” in our criteria. While the sensory characteristics of food (e.g., appearance, aroma, taste and texture) are important drivers of emotional experience, we here focus on methods to measure food-evoked emotion, and not on methods to assess the perception of sensory characteristics (e.g., this tastes sweeter and feels softer than others). Thus, studies on the appraisal of the qualitative characteristics of foods (e.g., intensity of sweetness, sourness, saltiness, spiciness, and bitterness) are not included here.

Search Procedure

Three reviewers (DK, AT, and AB) constructed the inclusion criteria, searched and evaluated the relevant literature. To obtain relevant articles from the PsycINFO database, the following combination of keywords was used: (food OR foods OR beverage OR beverages) AND [(“explode” emotions OR emotional responses OR emotional states OR physiological arousal) OR (pleasantness OR hedonic OR liking OR preference)]. We used the “explode” function in the PsycINFO search tool. For instance, exploding “emotions” provides all articles related to emotions. In addition to exploding emotion-related keywords, we further searched relevant articles using keywords such as pleasantness, hedonic, liking, and preference. As a result, we obtained an initial pool of 9,873 articles. Then, by limiting our search to articles in English reporting empirical studies in peer-reviewed journals, 8,156 articles were selected. Among them, we excluded articles targeting animals and disordered populations and kept 3,031 articles. We finally obtained 2,355 articles published in a 20 year period ranging from 1997 to 2016 (Table S1). Based on reviewing the title and abstract of those articles, we excluded articles with a lack of relevance (i.e., they did not meet our inclusion criteria as described above) and ended up with 65 articles. Then, full-text screening resulted in 57 relevant articles. For most of the relevant papers, measuring food-evoked emotions was not the main topic but part of the methodology to answer a different question of interest. This is why it proved difficult to capture all relevant articles using keywords like the ones listed above. Another 44 relevant articles were extracted based on cited references in the set of 57 articles and based on searches for more work by the first author, resulting in a final set of 101 papers. The eligibility of these additional 44 articles was independently assessed and confirmed by all three reviewers via an in-depth critical full-text review. A schematic representation of the search procedure is shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic representation of literature search and selection procedure.

Overview of Selected Articles

Table S2 gives a summary of the final set of 101 articles about the stimuli, the methods to measure food-evoked emotions, and the key findings provided by those methods. More than half of these articles were published in the last 4 years, and about ten times more articles were published in the last 4 years than during the first 4 years (Figure 2). This suggests that there is a growing interest in understanding emotions evoked by foods. Figure 3 gives an overview of the stimuli that have been used to evaluate food-related emotions. Actual foods were used by far most often as stimuli. Representative foods stimuli were sweet products, such as chocolates and cakes, while savory foods were less frequently selected as stimuli. Most studies evaluated an individual's emotions for a sole product, not for a full meal. The vast majority of measures were conducted just before, during and right after experiencing foods, although some studies asked participants to report their emotions a certain amount of time after experiencing the sample stimuli.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Total number of publications over successive 4-year intervals from 1997 to 2016.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The frequency of stimuli used within 101 studies (RF, Regular solid foods; RD, Regular drinks; SS, Simple solutions; M, Meal; FP, Food Pictures; O, Odors).

From the 101 selected articles, we identified a total of 59 different measures for the assessment of food-evoked emotions. Table 1 presents a brief description of each of these 59 different measures and a reference to a more elaborate description. The total number of times each measure was used within the selected group of 101 relevant studies is depicted in Figure 4. In this figure, we also grouped the measures according to general type of methodology (physiological, behavioral and cognitive). More than 80% of the papers used hedonic scaling measures to evaluate food-evoked emotions, indicating that this measure is most often used in addition to other measures. Following hedonic scales are several versions of emotional lexicon questionnaires such as CD-CATA, EsSense Profile, and unique instruments created by researchers themselves. Recording facial expression, usually by analyzing picture or movie data or electromyography (EMG), is a popular behavioral/implicit method. It should also be noted that more than 50% of measures were only used once among the 59 measures extracted from the 101 selected articles, indicating that there is still no representative measure or combination of measures developed for the evaluation of food-evoked emotions.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Brief description and abbreviation of measures used for the assessment of food elicited emotions.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Total number of studies (from the selected set of 101) in which each of the measures is used. The black bars, the white bars, and the shaded bars represent the cognitive, behavioral, and physiological measures, respectively. (All abbreviations are described in Table 1).

Assessment methods and their classification

To structure the measures described in the set of 101 papers we use a 3 × 3 framework consisting of three levels of emotional processing and three levels of measurement level as described in the next section.

As the first dimension of our 3 × 3 framework (processing level), we use three levels of processing: the lower level referring to unconscious and basic sensory processing, the intermediate level, referring to perception and early cognitive processing, and the higher level, referring to the conscious processing stage after cognition including decision-making and food-related behavior. This first dimension is similar to processing levels as used in SOR (Stimulus-Organism-Response) paradigm introduced by Mehrabian and Russell (1974) and adjusted by Bitner (1992), Lin (2004), and Schreuder et al. (2016). The second dimension of our 3 × 3 framework is the measurement level: physiological, behavioral, and cognitive. Physiological measures (like heart rate and skin conductivity) reflect the (largely unconscious) activity of the autonomic nervous system and bodily functions. Behavioral measures (like face and body movement and choice reaction time) reflect the unconscious and conscious responses of the body. Finally, the cognitive measures (like rating scales and questionnaires) reflect conscious opinions, choices, and decisions. This 3 × 3 framework provides a concise description of the different processing levels involved in the experience of multisensory environmental stimuli and their link to perceptual, emotional, and cognitive and behavioral outcomes. This framework is, therefore, well suited for our purposes.

Below, we discuss each of the nine combinations of processing and measurement levels in regard to assessing food-evoked emotions and used these nine categories to classify the 59 different measurement instruments reported.

Lower Processing Level (Sensory Processing)

When presented with food stimuli, individuals perceive and integrate information from all senses: vision, audition, taste, olfaction, and touch through unconscious, neurophysiological processes. Measures used to evaluate the emotional aspects of these processes were grouped in this category.

Physiological Measures

Major peripheral physiological measures like heart rate (HR), electrodermal activity (EDA), skin temperature (ST), and blood pressure (BP) fall into this measurement level. HR is a cardiovascular measure and the most frequently used measure to evaluate emotional states as e.g., induced by viewing emotional pictures or film clips (Kreibig, 2010). However, HR has not been used much to evaluate emotions evoked by experiencing foods. One of the exceptions is a study conducted by de Wijk et al. (2012), who demonstrated that HR can indeed be used to assess food-related emotions resulting from the sight, smell, and taste of liked and disliked foods. Similar to HR, EDA has often been used to investigate how people react to viewing emotional pictures and film clips. The study by de Wijk et al. (2012) included EDA as well. Skin Temperature (ST, also referred to as finger temperature or FT) is a measure reflecting autonomic nervous system activity. Rimm-Kaufman and Kagan (1996) suggested that researchers interested in emotion might consider using ST as an informative variable recorded with infrared tele thermography. Similar to HR, Blood Pressure (BP) is also used to examine an individuals' unconscious emotional states (Kreibig, 2010). Marczinski et al. (2014) used BP and found that the consumption of energy drinks elevated BP, while it is still unclear which part of this rise was caused by the intake of nutrients and which part was caused by emotion.

In addition to these peripheral physiological measures, measures reflecting brain activity such as electroencephalography (EEG), magnetoencephalography (MEG), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and positron emission tomography (PET) can be also used to evaluate an individual's unconscious response to food stimuli at the physiological level (Bercea, 2013; Agarwal and Xavier, 2015). Event-related potentials (ERPs) are specific positive or negative peaks in the EEG following the presentation of a stimulus. An example is the P300, the size of which relates to the amount of attention given to the stimulus (Hoffman and Polich, 1998; Patel and Azzam, 2005). In addition to the P300, the late positive potential (LPP) is an ERP component that is related to stimulus control and the use of attentional resources and regulatory factors in the brain (Hajcak et al., 2009). MEG is a functional neuroimaging technique that maps electrophysiological activities inside the brain through very sensitive magnetometers (Yoshikawa et al., 2014; Tsourides et al., 2016). For instance, MEG identified a robust neural correlate of the food and non-food distinction (Tsourides et al., 2016). fMRI is another technique to evaluate more detailed activities and responses inside the brain by detecting changes in blood oxygenation and flow that occur in response to neural activity. For instance, Grabenhorst et al. (2008) showed that perceived pleasantness correlated with activity in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and the pregenual cingulate cortex. More recently, Hoogeveen et al. (2015) demonstrated that older people reported higher liking ratings for sweet and salty, lower ratings for sour, and similar ratings for bitter compared to young people. Their findings indicated that these differences between younger and older adults may be associated with the reduction of right amygdala activity in older persons. PET is a functional imaging technique that is used to observe metabolic processes. Small et al. (2001) combined PET and MRI and demonstrated that different neural substrates mediate positive or appetitive and negative or aversive stimuli.

It is important to note that for every physiological sensor, several categories of physiological variables can be extracted (e.g., heart rate variability (HRV) and HR for ECG, and ERPs and power spectra for EEG). Within these categories there are usually further differences as to how the variable is defined. For instance, from subsequent RR intervals, HRV can be defined as the Root Mean Square of the Successive Differences (RMSSD), or as the power in frequency bands of interest (Veltman and Gaillard, 1998). In addition, extracting these indices relies heavily on choices with respect to time intervals across which data is examined and advanced data-processing techniques to filter, clean and classify the, often noisy, data from the physiological sensors. When using advanced analyses such as deep learning, it is not uncommon to try and compare different algorithms or parameter settings (e.g., Saeed et al., 2017). Finally, we want to point out the fact that physiological variables can be affected by body movements or time related factors such that failing to properly control or correct for those could lead to incorrect conclusions (Brouwer et al., 2015). These aspects need to be kept in mind when reviewing and comparing studies using physiological measures. A detailed description and discussion of the different signal processing techniques as used in the studies using physiological measures referred to in this review is outside the scope, but literatures exist on good practice [e.g., for machine learning in the context of EEG (Lotte et al., 2007); for heart rate variability (Camm et al., 1996)].

Behavioral Measures

A representative measure here is startle response (SR), also known as the alarm reaction or the startle reflex. The SR is a completely natural, involuntary reaction to a stimulus such as a flash of light, a sudden threatening movement or loud noise, and is associated with negative affect. Walla et al. (2010) provided evidence that SR modulation (eye blinks in their study) can be used reliably to quantify human motivational states related to the intake of different kinds of food (Walla et al., 2010). The postauricular reflex (PAR) is a vestigial muscle response in humans that acts to pull the ear backward and can be also grouped into this category. A study by Hebert et al. (2015) suggests that both PAR and SR are modulated by emotional states with valence scores of appetitive, neutral, and disgusting food images affecting SR in a direction opposite to PAR. In particular, pleasant stimuli enhanced the PAR (Gable and Harmon-Jones, 2009). Sandt et al. (2009) suggest that the PAR might be useful to measure appetitive responding in human. Another unconscious behavioral index categorized in this group is the postural sway (PS): a covert horizontal movement in response to a stimulus. There is strong evidence for bidirectional links between approach and avoidance (i.e., motivational state) and overt and covert indices of motor behavior (Elliot and Covington, 2001), including PS. Brunyé et al. (2013), using 100 food images, found evidence that individual preferences modulated anterior–posterior postural sway, with pleasant stimuli eliciting anterior-going sway and unpleasant stimuli elicit posterior-going sway.

Cognitive Measures

The lower emotional processing level is defined as the stage in which stimuli are automatically and unconsciously processed through our senses and the brain's sensory cortices without conscious intervention or interpretation. Cognitive measures rely on for instance individuals rating their food-evoked emotions and are thus not applicable at this level of processing.

Intermediate Processing Level (Perception and Early Cognitive Processing)

Following the integrated multisensory perception of food stimuli, individuals relate their percepts to previous experiences and information stored in memory. This can occur through both conscious and unconscious processes. This intermediate processing level concerns a short-term emotional state that is directly related to the object of focus. This state can be observed consciously (feeling aroused, pleasant, etc.) or can be experienced unconsciously, and drives the allocation of processing resources and priorities for the consecutive processing level (cognition, behavior, and decision).

Physiological Measures

Frontal alpha asymmetry measured using EEG fits in this category. Some studies, using pictures of desserts, showed that alpha asymmetry is an unconscious response that depended on whether the subject would like to approach or avoid that dessert (Gable and Harmon-Jones, 2008; Harmon-Jones and Gable, 2009). Such a response can only occur after the perception of the dessert has been integrated with information from memory. Other neuroimaging techniques (fMRI, MEG, and PET) were also considered as measures to assess the intermediate processing level.

Behavioral Measures

The autobiographical congruency test (ACT) and the emotive projection test (EPT) measure the reaction time needed to think of a happy or a sad life event and are employed to indirectly measure food-related emotions (Mojet et al., 2015). Mojet et al. (2015) showed that the ACT didn't differentiate between products, and that the EPT was the most promising measure since it had no significant correlation with either liking and differentiated between products. The temporal duration judgment (TDJ) evaluates how long someone is looking at food images. Gil et al. (2009) provided evidence that the time looked at disliked food images was longer and at liked food shorter than the time looked at neutral food images. The pick-up latency (PL) method is another indirect measure for liking based on the principles of approach-avoidance motivations: PL is smaller for positive valence and larger for negative valence (Krieglmeyer et al., 2010). Davies et al. (2012) showed that PL was reduced for positively conditioned flavors and increased for negatively conditioned flavors. Visual selective attention (VSA) is a related behavioral measure: a transitory decline in the pleasantness of the taste modulates covert VSA (di Pellegrino et al., 2011). Finally, the facial expression response (FER) resulting from the integrated stimulation evoked by food experience over a short time period is a behavioral measure to assess the intermediate processing level. Several recent studies provide evidence that the FER correlates with valence and arousal ratings (de Wijk et al., 2012, 2014; Garcia-Burgos and Zamora, 2013; Danner et al., 2014; He et al., 2016).

Cognitive Measures

This category contains instruments that do not rely on directly asking questions about the subject's emotions (as these would tap into the higher emotional processing level) but on implicit cognitions. The experimental auction (ExpAuc) technique is such an instrument, using a real product and real money (Poole et al., 2007). They showed that the ExpAuc implicitly measures an individual's willingness to pay for a certain product. A second instrument in this category is the implicit association test (IAT: Greenwald et al., 1998): a tool to measure implicit attitudes toward stimuli. However, no study using an IAT on food stimuli was present in our final set of articles.

Higher Processing Level (Conscious Reflection and Decision Making)

The higher processing level could be considered as the final stage in which individuals consciously recognize what foods are, which emotions they evoke, how these are associated with their social relationships, how food stimuli are related with their expectations, etc. The measures that evaluate these conscious emotions were categorized in this group.

Physiological Measures

All of the physiological responses to food stimuli are unconscious and automatically occur in the human body and brain. Because of the implicit assumption that individuals are not able to intentionally control their physiological response to food stimuli, none of the papers selected for this review employs a measure at the physiological level to assess the higher level of emotional processing.

Behavioral Measures

While the behavioral measures at the intermediate processing level reflect unconscious, short-term (immediate) emotional state, the instruments at the higher level relate to more deliberate approach or avoidance behaviors, influenced by more cognitive and long-term emotion. In this category, the measures of Amount Consumed (AC: Zandstra et al., 1999) and Take Away Behavior (TAB: Weiss et al., 2010; Wichchukit and O'mahony, 2010) can be included.

Cognitive Measures

There are many instruments that fall within this category (more than in all eight other categories combined). Therefore, we use five subcategories to provide further structure: (1) hedonic scaling, and questionnaires with (2) preferable foods, (3) emotional lexicons, (4) emotional pictures, and (5) mood-related lexicons.

Hedonic Scaling

In Hedonic Scaling using a n-point scale (HSn), product evaluation (liking) is typically scored on 5-point (Brunyé et al., 2013), 7-point (Caporale et al., 2009; Awazu, 2013) or 9-point (Ares et al., 2010; Chung et al., 2012; Bhumiratana et al., 2014) liking scale. Adjectives are sometimes used to label the points in order to aid the interpretation. In most studies consumers are asked to rate several samples sequentially without reference to other elements in the set (a serial monadic approach). Some modified methods from HSn are Labeled Affective Magnitude (LAM) scale and Hybrid Hedonic Scale (HHS). Schutz and Cardello (2001) developed the LAM scale, which is 9-point hedonic scale with magnitude-scaled semantic labels. HHS is also a linear scale with marked equidistant points and verbal affective labels serving as anchors in the middle and extreme regions of the scale (Villanueva et al., 2005). Finally, Preference Mapping (PM) is a technique to describe the relationship between hedonic ratings of a randomized population of subjects and the sensory scores of the products rated by trained panels (Clark, 1998).

Questionnaires choosing preferable foods

Questionnaires asking participants to select one or more products are also based on their higher processing level of food stimuli. Instruments in this subcategory are the Product Choice (PC) test, where assessors are presented with different products and asked to select the one they prefer for consumption (Lévy and Köster, 1999), the Free Choice Profiling (FCP) test, where assessors describe products in their own words and rate the perceived intensity of those terms (Kim et al., 2013), and the Positional Relative Rating (PRR) test. Kim and O'mahony (1998) used the latter measure, in which assessors are given all products at once and order them along a line in order of liking. The Hard Laddering (HL) method also belongs in this category. In a laddering task, assessors are asked to compare products or their attributes and elicit their reasons for choosing a certain product for purchase or consumption. In HL, a structured questionnaire is applied, while in soft laddering the participant is interviewed by a trained experimenter (Russell et al., 2004). There was no study using the soft laddering technique selected by our inclusion criteria. In addition to using HL and studying their Buying Behavior (BUYBeh), Rosas-Nexticapa et al. (2005) also asked participants to rate their Buying Preference (BUYPref) and Buying Ranking (BUYRank). They demonstrated that these ratings might predict purchase frequency of products over a 1 year period of experiments. However, this type of study is too costly and time-consuming to be practical, and these rating scales do not reflect a person's actual buying behavior in all circumstances as shown by Lange et al. (2002). The last measures in this subcategory are the Best-Worst Scaling (BWS: Jaeger et al., 2008) and the Best-Worst Scaling of Lexicon Terms (BWSLT: Thomson et al., 2010).

Questionnaires with emotional lexicons

King and Meiselman (2010) compiled a list of 39 emotional terms that consumers associate with products, known as the EsSense Profile (ESP). Each of these terms is rated on a 5-point scale. When applied to evaluate food products, the ESP provides additional information that is not explained by overall product liking (King et al., 2010). A shortened version of ESP, named EsSense 25 (compiling 25 emotional terms) was developed later (Nestrud et al., 2013). Also, Spinelli et al. (2014) developed a product-specific questionnaire, called EmoSemio, based on one-on-one interviews conducted with a modified version of the Repertory Grid Method (RGM: Kelly, 2003). They provided evidence that EmoSemio discriminated product specific emotions better than ESP with chocolate and hazelnut spreads as samples (Spinelli et al., 2014). The Scent Move (SM: Porcherot et al., 2010) is also a tool using an emotional lexicon (a simplified version of the Geneva Emotion and Odor Scale or GEOS: Porcherot et al., 2010) as is the Check-All-That-Apply (CATA) technique, in which assessors are presented with a list of sensory emotional terms or phrases and are asked to select all those terms or phrases they consider applicable to describe the focal sample (Adams et al., 2007). The modified CATA measure to evaluate food-evoked emotions is the Consumer-Defined Check-All-That-Apply (CD-CATA), and was developed and demonstrated by Ng et al. (2013). The Rate-All-That-Apply (RATA) is a rating-based variant of CATA (Ares et al., 2014). In the Word Association (WA) technique, assessors are asked about concepts, images and thoughts that come into their mind for each product, yielding thoughts and associations about the products, after conscious evaluation. The Temporal Dominance of Emotions (TDE) tool is based on the Temporal Dominance of Sensations (TDS), which evaluates the sequence of dominant sensations of a product during a certain time period (Pineau et al., 2003), but with emotional instead of sensory attributes (Ares et al., 2008). They showed that temporal emotional attitude was related to the sensory profiles obtained with TDS. Other self-reported emotion questionnaires were also included in this category, such as EmoSensory Profile (EMP) rating (a combination of 14–17 emotional terms and 13 sensory terms: Schouteten et al., 2015), Affect Self Report (ASR) scale (rating 18 affective terms on a 7-point scale: Christie and Friedman, 2004), Empathic Food Test (EFT; rating 12 empathic terms: Geier et al., 2016), Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; rating 10 positive and 10 negative emotion terms: Watson et al., 1988), Implicit Positive, Negative Affect Test (IPANAT; rating 3-positive and 3-negative emotions: Quirin et al., 2009), and International Positive and Negative Affect Schedule Short Form (I-PANAS-SF: Thompson, 2007), a shortened version of the PANAS (Watson et al., 1988; Thompson, 2007).

Questionnaires with emotional pictures

The Self-Assessment Mannikin (SAM) developed by Bradley and Lang (1994) is a measure to evaluate emotions (valence, arousal, and dominance) that uses pictures instead of text (as do the emotional lexicons described above). Similar to the SAM, the Affect Grid (AG: Russell et al., 1989) and PrEmo (PrEmo-2; Desmet et al., 2000; Laurans and Desmet, 2012) were also developed for participants to more easily describe their emotions with pictures. Visually expressed emotions are hypothesized to more closely resemble intuitively experienced emotions (Dalenberg et al., 2014). Evidence for this hypothesis stems from EEG-experiments showing that emotion processing is faster for facial expressions than for emotional words (Schacht and Sommer, 2009; Frühholz et al., 2011; Rellecke et al., 2011).

Questionnaires with mood-related lexicons

The final subcategory consists of self-report techniques that evaluate mood, such as the Multi Dimensional Mood Questionnaire (MDMQ) that employs 24 (long form) or 12 (short form) items to cover three bipolar dimensions of mood (Geier et al., 2016), the Profile Of Mood States (POMS) that uses ratings of 6 mood states along with 6 different dimensions (McNair et al., 1971), the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) that uses ratings of 40 self-report items pertaining to anxiety affect (Spielberger, 1983), and the Visual Analog Mood Scales (VAMS) that employs ratings of 8 different general mood states (Bond and Lader, 1974).

Discussion

Emotions are considered to be important drivers of food-related cognitions and behavior like food choice and eating behavior. Indeed, Dalenberg et al. (2014) provided the results indicating that the predicting power of individual's food choice got better by adding the evaluation of food-evoked emotions with liking rating scores. As Köster and Mojet (2015) state, there is no doubt that unconscious emotions can play a role in eating and drinking behavior in a way that is independent of hedonic pleasure as measured by liking. In addition, also mentioned in the introduction, some studies provided the results that liking scaling do not predict individual's actual food choice (Zandstra and El-Deredy, 2011; Griffioen-Roose et al., 2013). Valid, reliable and sensitive instruments that assess food-evoked emotions are therefore valuable for fundamental and applied research and for instance in developing new food products and advocating a healthy lifestyle. A complicating factor in this field is that human emotion is a multifaceted construct linked to physiological, behavioral, and cognitive processes, and we may not assume to find a single measure that covers the full range, while there is a general conviction that all facets are relevant.

We listed, organized, and reviewed the prevailing instruments based on a literature review consisting of 101 peer-reviewed articles published between 1997 and 2016. Our main observations are: (1) There is an overabundance of different measures (about 59 in our set of 101 papers); (2) The majority of these measures assess the cognitive level of emotional processing using subjective ratings or questionnaires (i.e., self-reports are over-represented); (3) Articles that report two or more measures generally use measures that all tap into the same level of emotional processing, while it may be expected that redundant measures have limited added value.

The Overabundance of Measures

As also mentioned in the introduction, the fact that about 59 different measures are employed in 101 papers makes it evident that there is not a “golden standard” to assess food-evoked emotions. This has consequences regarding the validity of the measures, the generalizability of reported effects, and the integration (or even meta-analysis) over studies. From a methodological point of view, the use of uncommon measures of which the validity is unknown is not desirable. Some measures are developed and exclusively used by few research groups. For instance, previous studies developed questionnaires with emotional lexicons for a range of product categories (chocolate in Thomson et al., 2010; blackcurrant squashes in Ng et al., 2013; chocolate and hazelnut spreads in Spinelli et al., 2014; coffee in Bhumiratana et al., 2014). Those questionnaires with lexicons are specific for each product and cannot be applied for universal food products. Few other research groups use this technique with the same emotional lexicons. We recommend to choose widely applied, validated measures whenever possible. One should not construct one's own instrument before having verified that there is not an existing tool that may serve one's goals. Both our Tables 1, 2 may be of assistance here.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The toolbox table: a categorization of all 59 emotional measures extracted from our set of 101 articles.

The Over-Representation of Self-Report Measures

Of the 59 measures reported, eight are at the physiological, 11 at the behavioral, and 40 at the cognitive measurement level. More than 60% of the reported measures are based on self-reports assessing cognitive emotional processing. Among them, more than 80% studies used a HSn measure as one of the measures. There are two important comments to be made here. First, as also discussed in the introduction, self-reports (although successful) have inherent shortcomings: emotions are difficult to verbalize, the “emotional” lexicon varies across cultures and languages, answers may be biased, and verbalizing emotions can interfere with food experience itself. Second, self-reports assess almost per definition the higher levels of emotional processing and cannot assess unconscious emotional processing, while this is deemed important to improve both our understanding of food-evoked emotions as well as our algorithms to predict future cognition and behavior. As indicated from the toolbox table with 3 × 3 framework provided here (Table 2), most of the papers selected by our inclusion criteria are of a mono-disciplinary nature and contribute more to the perpetuation of the narrow tunnel view of the cognitive measurement level.

The Limited Added Value of Redundant Measures

Although authors often report the data of two or more measures, these are almost always of the same category, again reflecting the over-representation of self-report measures: the majority of those studies used a combination of HSn (including liking and pleasantness scale) ratings and questionnaires with emotional lexicons. These redundant tests with comparable results can be useful to prove the robustness of a specific effect such as hedonic asymmetry. Hedonic asymmetry refers to the finding that people overwhelmingly use positive rather than negative words, whether describing recalled food experiences or describing reactions to food samples. Hedonic asymmetry was described by Desmet and Schifferstein (2008) and has been replicated for different types of commercial products by using ESP and LAM (Cardello et al., 2012), ESP and HSn (King and Meiselman, 2010), CD-CATA and HSn (Ng et al., 2013), and with the original emotional lexicons and HSn (Desmet and Schifferstein, 2008; Manzocco et al., 2013). High correlation between tests was also found by Cordonnier and Delwiche (2008) who reported that PRR yielded similar results to HSn with lemonades as stimuli. Similarly it has been reported that LAM has equal reliability and sensitivity to HSn and a somewhat greater discrimination ability among highly liked foods than HSn (Schutz and Cardello, 2001). These results indicate that, although redundant instruments showing the same results may improve the robustness of the findings, the added value of their repeated use may be limited and the scope of the conclusions is necessarily restricted to that of the chosen measurement category. This limited added value may not outweigh the extra costs of the test and the burden subjected to the participants. In case the redundant instruments show contradictory effects, this could be a useful indication for the lack of robustness and replicability of the effect, but one should carefully consider the quality of the data and the validity of one or both tests. We recommend to refrain from using instruments from the same category. We do recommend using multiple tests but to choose tests from different categories (see below under toolbox for guidance on how to choose your combination of tests, that should preferably be along the diagonal of Table 2). Although these different measures sometimes provided complementary information, there were also many cases in which they yielded redundant and sometimes even contradictory results.

A Toolbox Table to Support the Selection of a Combination of Instruments

To structure the 59 measures, we used nine categories based on a 3 × 3 framework with the dimensions: measurement level (physiological, behavioral, and cognitive) and emotional processing level (low—unconscious sensory processing, intermediate - perception and early cognitive processing, and high—conscious decision-making and behavior). Furthermore, the category “cognitive high processing level” was divided into five different subcategories. The resulting classification (see Table 2) indicates that current physiological methods are used to evaluate the low and intermediate processing levels, behavioral methods to evaluate all three processing levels, and cognitive methods to evaluate the intermediate and high processing levels. The resulting classification or “toolbox table” (see Table 2) can be used to select a minimal set of methods that provides maximal (complementary) information on the aspects of affective food experience that are of interest, for instance by choosing methods along the table's diagonal from top-left to bottom-right. Current widespread practice is to use one or more measures from the bottom-right category only. Although such measures can certainly provide much information about the consciously experienced effects of food, they may miss essential nuances in feelings and emotions that may influence later behavior. Non-verbal, implicit measurements may complement verbal self-report questionnaires to better understand individual's food-evoked emotions. Although the number of tools in some of the framework's cells is still limited, new instruments have recently been developed and tested. Examples include facial movements, such as smacks of mouth and lips and tongue protrusion for hedonic reactions and gape, eye quench, and nose wrinkle for aversive reactions (Steiner et al., 2001), facial expressions (Kostyra et al., 2016), ANS responses such as HR and EDA as signals for negative emotions like disgust and anger that also provided detailed information on food preference (de Wijk et al., 2012). Using machinelearning techniques, Brouwer et al. (2017) recently found that a combination of different physiological measures showed whether a participant was cooking and tasting a dish with conventional ingredients (a chicken stirfry) or a dish with high arousal, low valence ingredients (a mealworm stirfry).

In addition to these successful demonstrations, some studies conducted a correlation analysis between self-reports questionnaires and physiological or behavioral measurements (Garcia-Burgos and Zamora, 2013; Mojet et al., 2015). Garcia-Burgos and Zamora (2013) conducted linear regression analysis between self-reported hedonic value and the intensity of disgust facial expression analysis on bitter-tasting foods and found a quite low correlation value. The fact that the different categories indeed seem to measure different facets of emotional processing implies that combining them may increase our understanding (for example of the discrepancies between hedonic ratings and consumer choice) and ultimately improve our predictions of food-related cognitions and behavior taking into account that food-evoked emotions is only part of a range of factors that influence future liking, choosing or buying behavior. It was also proposed that the implicit nature of food-related behavior requires the development of more appropriate/adequate research methods that measure the motives of the consumer and her reactions to food in a more implicit way. We recommend acquiring more accumulative and simultaneously collected physiological, behavioral, and self-report datasets.

Implications for Related Research

Viewing images of food triggers the desire for the real thing: just looking at pictures of food causes salivation (Spence, 2011) and an uptick in ghrelin, a hormone that causes hunger (Schüssler et al., 2012). These effects increase when images represent food in a more vivid way (Spence, 2011; Moore and Konrath, 2015). Vividness (Steuer, 1992), also referred to as media richness (Daft and Lengel, 1986) refers to the sensory breadth (the number of sensory dimensions) and sensory depth (the information quality and resolution) of stimuli. Vivid stimuli allow observers to fill in more missing sensory information and thereby diminish the user's perception of mediation (i.e., the indirect perception of a product through technical means or devices). This enables users to activate a fuller, more concrete or vivid mental model of a mediated product, which in turn affects their product appraisal (Choi and Taylor, 2014) and intensifies the imagined product experience (Roggeveen et al., 2015). It has for instance been found that vivid (full color) images of pizza elicited higher levels of food craving, a stronger salivation response, and stronger eating intentions, than similar pallid (black and white) images (Moore and Konrath, 2015). Also, vivid food cinemagraphs evoke stronger appetitive responses than similar stills (Toet et al., in press). Virtual reality (VR: Gorini et al., 2010; Nordbo et al., 2015; Ung et al., 2018) and augmented reality (AR: Narumi et al., 2012; Pallavicini et al., 2016) appear to be promising tools to study the impact of environmental cues on human nutritional behavior since they typically provide vivid imagery. In the field of Human-Computer Interaction (HCI), novel multisensory (taste, smell, tactile) interfaces are being developed and used to support studies on food-related emotions and behavior, personal health and wellbeing (Comber et al., 2014; Obrist et al., 2016), or simply to enhance or augment the experience of food (Narumi et al., 2011; Schöning et al., 2012; Spence and Piqueras-Fiszman, 2014; Velasco et al., 2018). HCI can promote healthy food practices and social dining experiences (Comber et al., 2013). The addition of tactile and olfactory channels to VR and AR systems will further enhance the vividness of the mediated perception, and may for instance afford shared distributed virtual multisensory dining experiences (Braun et al., 2016). Multisensory technologies also allow researchers to control the various inputs that accompany a given food experience (Velasco et al., 2018). In all the aforementioned applications it is essential to monitor the emotional and behavioral responses to the perceived virtual or mediated food, and to assess how these responses compare to those evoked by real food (Pallavicini et al., 2016). Reliable emotion assessment techniques are therefore required to further develop and optimize multisensory interactive experiences and to assess the ecological validity of mediated food presentations (Obrist et al., 2016, 2017).

Conclusions and recommendations

Common practice in assessing food-evoked emotions relies on many different instruments of which the majority only assess a limited aspect of emotional processing. While this common practice resulted in robust and relevant findings, the plethora of different instruments hampers the validation of infrequently used instruments and the comparison of results over studies. The restriction to assess only the cognitive emotional processing level makes it difficult to obtain a complete picture and impoverishes our predictive models. We recommend the following:

(1) use widely applied, validated measures and refrain from constructing a new tool unless absolutely necessary

(2) refrain from using (highly correlated) instruments from the same category

(3) use multiple measures from different categories, preferably covering all three emotional processing levels, for instance by selecting tests along the diagonal of Table 2.

(4) acquire and share simultaneously collected physiological, behavioral, and cognitive datasets to improve the predictive power of food choice and other models.

It must be noted that there are many more factors to fill in the gap between individual's food-evoked emotions and food choice and buying behavior in the supermarket, such as food culture, habitats, incomes, and family structure in addition to food-evoked emotions. However, finding even more accurate measures or proper combination of measures to better interpret individual's food-evoked emotions is a definite step to better predict individual's actual food choice and buying behavior.

Author Contributions

DK, AT, and A-MB constructed the inclusion criteria, searched the relevant literature, put forward the review idea, read the literature, and organized the article structure. DK wrote the draft manuscript. AT, A-MB, VK, and JvE critically revised the manuscript on article structure, logical organization and language.

Funding

This review was sponsored by Kikkoman Europe R&D Laboratory B.V.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dymphie van der Heyden for her assistance with the search for relevant papers in the PsycINFO database.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00911/full#supplementary-material

References

Adams, J., Williams, A., Lancaster, B., and Foley, M. (2007). “Advantages and uses of Check-All-That-Apply response compared to traditional scaling of attributes for salty snacks,” in 7th Pangborn Sensory Science Symposium (Minneapolis, MN).

Agarwal, S., and Xavier, M. J. (2015). “Innovations in consumer science: applications of neuro-scientific research tools,” in Adoption of Innovation: Balancing Internal and External Stakeholders in the Marketing of Innovation, eds A. Brem and É. Viardot (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 25–42.

Google Scholar

Ares, G., Barreiro, C., Deliza, R., Giménez, A., and Gambaro, A. (2010). Application of a check-all-that-apply question to the development of chocolate milk desserts. J. Sens. Stud. 25(Suppl 1), 67–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-459X.2010.00290.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ares, G., Bruzzone, F., Vidal, L., Cadena, R. S., Giménez, A., Pineau, B., et al. (2014). Evaluation of a rating-based variant of check-all-that-apply questions: rate-all-that-apply (RATA). Food Qual. Prefer. 36, 87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ares, G., Giménez, A., and Gambaro, A. (2008). Understanding consumers' perception of conventional and functional yogurts using word association and hard laddering. Food Qual. Prefer. 19, 636–643. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2008.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Awazu, S. (2013). Affective information influences affective evaluation but not affective expectation. Psychologia 56, 237–245. doi: 10.2117/psysoc.2013.237

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagozzi, R. P., Gopinath, M., and Nyer, P. U. (1999). The role of emotions in marketing. J. Acad. Market. Sci. 27, 184–206. doi: 10.1177/0092070399272005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barylko-Pikielna, N., Matuszewska, I., Jeruszka, M., Kozlowska, K., Brzozowska, A., and Roszkowski, W. (2004). Discriminability and appropriateness of category scaling versus ranking methods to study sensory preferences in elderly. Food Qual. Prefer. 15, 167–175. doi: 10.1016/S0950-3293(03)00055-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benson, S., and Scholey, A. (2014). Effects of alcohol and energy drink on mood and subjective intoxication: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover study. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 29, 360–369. doi: 10.1002/hup.2414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bercea, M. D. (2013). “Anatomy of methodologies for measuring consumer behavior in neuromarketing research,” in Lupcon Center for Business Research (LCBR) European Marketing Conference 2012 (Ebermannstadt: Lupcon Center for Business Research).

Google Scholar

Bhumiratana, N., Adhikari, K., and Chambers, E. (2014). The development of an emotion lexicon for the coffee drinking experience. Food Res. Int. 61, 83–92. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2014.03.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bitner, M. J. (1992). Servicescapes: the impact of physical surroundings on customers and employees. J. Market. 56, 57–71. doi: 10.2307/1252042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bond, A., and Lader, M. (1974). The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. Bri. J. Med. Psychol. 47, 211–218. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1974.tb02285.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M., and Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 25, 49–59. doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braun, M. H., Pradana, G. A., Buchanan, G., Cheok, A. D., Velasco, C., Spence, C., et al. (2016). Emotional priming of digital images through mobile telesmell and virtual food. Int. J. Food Design 1, 29–45. doi: 10.1386/ijfd.1.1.29_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brouwer, A.-M., Hogervorst, M., Grootjen, M., van Erp, J., and Zandstra, E. (2017). Neurophysiological responses during cooking food associated with different emotions. Food Qual. Prefer. 62, 307–316. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brouwer, A.-M., Zander, T. O., Van Erp, J. B., Korteling, J. E., and Bronkhorst, A. W. (2015). Using neurophysiological signals that reflect cognitive or affective state: six recommendations to avoid common pitfalls. Front. Neurosci. 9:136. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunyé, T. T., Hayes, J. F., Mahoney, C. R., Gardony, A. L., Taylor, H. A., and Kanarek, R. B. (2013). Get in my belly: food preferences trigger approach and avoidant postural asymmetries. PLoS ONE 8:e72432. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0072432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabanac, M. (2002). What is emotion? Behav. Process. 60, 69–83. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(02)00078-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camm, A. J., Malik, M., Bigger, J., Breithardt, G., Cerutti, S., Cohen, R., et al. (1996). Heart rate variability: standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Circulation 93, 1043–1065. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.93.5.1043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caporale, G., Policastro, S., Tuorila, H., and Monteleone, E. (2009). Hedonic ratings and consumption of school lunch among preschool children. Food Qual. Prefer. 20, 482–489. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.04.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardello, A. V., Meiselman, H. L., Schutz, H. G., Craig, C., Given, Z., Lesher, L. L., et al. (2012). Measuring emotional responses to foods and food names using questionnaires. Food Qual. Prefer. 24, 243–250. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2011.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaya, C., Eaton, C., Hewson, L., Vázquez, R. F., Fernández-Ruiz, V., Smart, K. A., et al. (2015). Developing a reduced consumer-led lexicon to measure emotional response to beer. Food Qual. Prefer. 45, 100–112. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, Y. K., and Taylor, C. R. (2014). How do 3-dimensional images promote products on the Internet? J. Bus. Res. 67, 2164–2170. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.04.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chrea, C., Grandjean, D., Delplanque, S., Cayeux, I., Le Calv,é, B., Aymard, L., et al. (2009). Mapping the semantic space for the subjective experience of emotional responses to odors. Chem. Senses 34, 49–62. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjn052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christie, I. C., and Friedman, B. H. (2004). Autonomic specificity of discrete emotion and dimensions of affective space: a multivariate approach. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 51, 143–153. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, L., Chung, S.-J., Kim, J.-Y., Kim, K.-O., O'mahony, M., Vickers, Z., et al. (2012). Comparing the liking for Korean style salad dressings and beverages between US and Korean consumers: effects of sensory and non-sensory factors. Food Qual. Prefer. 26, 105–118. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.03.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Churchill, A., and Behan, J. (2010). Comparison of methods used to study consumer emotions associated with fragrance. Food Qual. Prefer. 21, 1108–1113. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2010.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, J. E. (1998). Taste and flavour: their importance in food choice and acceptance. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 57, 639–643. doi: 10.1079/PNS19980093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collinsworth, L. A., Lammert, A. M., Martinez, K. P., Leidheiser, M., Garza, J., Keener, M., et al. (2014). Development of a novel sensory method: image Measurement of Emotion and Texture (IMET). Food Qual. Prefer. 38, 115–125. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.04.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Comber, R., Choi, J. H.-J., Hoonhout, J., and O'hara, K. (2014). Designing for human–food interaction: an introduction to the special issue on ‘food and interaction design'. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 72, 181–184. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2013.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Comber, R., Hoonhout, J., Halteren, A. V., Moynihan, P., and Olivier, P. (2013). “Food practices as situated action: exploring and designing for everyday food practices with households,” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (Paris: ACM).

Google Scholar

Cordonnier, S., and Delwiche, J. (2008). An alternative method for assessing liking: positional relative rating versus the 9-point hedonic scale. J. Sens. Stud. 23, 284–292. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-459X.2008.00155.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daft, R. L., and Lengel, R. H. (1986). Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Manage. Sci. 32, 554–571. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.32.5.554

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalenberg, J. R., Gutjar, S., Ter Horst, G. J., de Graaf, K., Renken, R. J., and Jager, G. (2014). Evoked emotions predict food choice. PLoS ONE 9:e115388. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0115388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danner, L., Sidorkina, L., Joechl, M., and Duerrschmid, K. (2014). Make a face! Implicit and explicit measurement of facial expressions elicited by orange juices using face reading technology. Food Qual. Prefer. 32, 167–172. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, S. R., El-Deredy, W., Zandstra, E. H., and Blanchette, I. (2012). Evidence for the role of cognitive resources in flavour–flavour evaluative conditioning. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 65, 2297–2308. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.701311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Den Uijl, L. C., Jager, G., De Graaf, C., Meiselman, H. L., and Kremer, S. (2016a). Emotion, olfaction, and age: a comparison of self-reported food-evoked emotion profiles of younger adults, older normosmic adults, and older hyposmic adults. Food Qual. Prefer. 48, 199–209. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.09.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Den Uijl, L. C., Jager, G., Zandstra, E. H., De Graaf, C., and Kremer, S. (2016b). Self-reported food-evoked emotions of younger adults, older normosmic adults, and older hyposmic adults as measured using the PrEmo2 tool and the Affect Grid. Food Qual. Prefer. 51, 109–117. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desmet, P. M. A., Hekkert, P., and Jacobs, J. J. (2000). “When a car makes you smile; development and application of n instrument to measure product emotions,” in Advances in Consumer Research, eds S. J. Hoch and R. J. Meyer, 111–117.

Desmet, P. M., and Schifferstein, H. N. J. (2008). Sources of positive and negative emotions in food experience. Appetite 50, 290–301. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Wijk, R. A., He, W., Mensink, M. G. J., Verhoeven, R. H. G., and De Graaf, C. (2014). ANS responses and facial expressions differentiate between the taste of commercial breakfast drinks. PLoS ONE 9:e93823. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0093823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Wijk, R. A., Kooijman, V., Verhoeven, R. H. G., Holthuysen, N. T. E., and De Graaf, C. (2012). Autonomic nervous system responses on and facial expressions to the sight, smell, and taste of liked and disliked foods. Food Qual. Prefer. 26, 196–203. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.04.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

di Pellegrino, G., Magarelli, S., and Mengarelli, F. (2011). Food pleasantness affects visual selective attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (Hove). 64, 560–571. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2010.504031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dorado, R., Chaya, C., Tarrega, A., and Hort, J. (2016). The impact of using a written scenario when measuring emotional response to beer. Food Qual. Prefer. 50, 38–47. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duka, T., Tasker, R., and Stephens, D. N. (1998). Alcohol choice and outcome expectancies in social drinkers. Behav. Pharmacol. 9, 643–653. doi: 10.1097/00008877-199811000-00019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Einöther, S. J. L., Baas, M., Rowson, M., and Giesbrecht, T. (2015). Investigating the effects of tea, water and a positive affect induction on mood and creativity. Food Qual. Prefer. 39, 56–61. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.06.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Einöther, S. J., Rowson, M., Ramaekers, J. G., and Giesbrecht, T. (2016). Infusing pleasure: mood effects of the consumption of a single cup of tea. Appetite 103, 302–308. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliot, A. J., and Covington, M. V. (2001). Approach and avoidance motivation. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 13, 73–92. doi: 10.1023/A:1009009018235

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esteves, F., Arriaga, P., Carneiro, P., and Flykt, A. (2010). Emotional responses (verbal and psychophysiological) to pictures of food stimuli. Psicologia 24, 89–111. doi: 10.17575/rpsicol.v24i2.308

CrossRef Full Text

Fairbairn, C. E., and Sayette, M. A. (2013). The effect of alcohol on emotional inertia: a test of alcohol myopia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 122, 770. doi: 10.1037/a0032980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferber, C., and Cabanac, M. (1987). Influence of noise on gustatory affective ratings and preference for sweet or salt. Appetite 8, 229–235. doi: 10.1016/0195-6663(87)90022-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frühholz, S., Jellinghaus, A., and Herrmann, M. (2011). Time course of implicit processing and explicit processing of emotional faces and emotional words. Biol. Psychol. 87, 265–274. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gable, P. A., and Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Postauricular reflex responses to pictures varying in valence and arousal. Psychophysiology 46, 487–490. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00794.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gable, P., and Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Relative left frontal activation to appetitive stimuli: considering the role of individual differences. Psychophysiology 45, 275–278. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00627.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia-Burgos, D., and Zamora, M. C. (2013). Facial affective reactions to bitter-tasting foods and body mass index in adults. Appetite 71, 178–186. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geier, U., Büssing, A., Kruse, P., Greiner, R., and Buchecker, K. (2016). Development and application of a test for food-induced emotions. PLoS ONE 11:e0165991. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, E. L. (2006). Emotional influences on food choice: sensory, physiological and psychological pathways. Physiol. Behav. 89, 53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gil, S., Rousset, S., and Droit-Volet, S. (2009). How liked and disliked foods affect time perception. Emotion 9, 457–463. doi: 10.1037/a0015751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorini, A., Griez, E., Petrova, A., and Riva, G. (2010). Assessment of the emotional responses produced by exposure to real food, virtual food and photographs of food in patients affected by eating disorders. Ann. Gen. Psychiatry 9:30. doi: 10.1186/1744-859X-9-30

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grabenhorst, F., Rolls, E. T., and Bilderbeck, A. (2008). How cognition modulates affective responses to taste and flavor: top-down influences on the orbitofrontal and pregenual cingulate cortices. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1549–1559. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., and Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1464. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffioen-Roose, S., Smeets, P. A., Weijzen, P. L., Van Rijn, I., Van Den Bosch, I., and De Graaf, C. (2013). Effect of replacing sugar with non-caloric sweeteners in beverages on the reward value after repeated exposure. PLoS ONE 8:e81924. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutjar, S., Dalenberg, J. R., De Graaf, C., De Wijk, R., Palascha, A., Renken, R. J., et al. (2015a). What reported food-evoked emotions may add: a model to predict consumer food choice. Food Qual. Prefer. 45, 140–148. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.06.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutjar, S., De Graaf, C., Kooijman, V., De Wijk, R. A., Nys, A., Ter Horst, G. J., et al. (2015b). The role of emotions in food choice and liking. Food Res. Int. 76(Pt 2), 216–223. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2014.12.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hajcak, G., Dunning, J. P., and Foti, D. (2009). Motivated and controlled attention to emotion: time-course of the late positive potential. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 505–510. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.11.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmon-Jones, E., and Gable, P. A. (2009). Neural activity underlying the effect of approach-motivated positive affect on narrowed attention. Psychol. Sci. 20, 406–409. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02302.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, W., Boesveldt, S., De Graaf, C., and De Wijk, R. A. (2016). The relation between continuous and discrete emotional responses to food odors with facial expressions and non-verbal reports. Food Qual. Prefer. 48(Pt A), 130–137. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hebert, K. R., Valle-Inclán, F., and Hackley, S. A. (2015). Modulation of eyeblink and postauricular reflexes during the anticipation and viewing of food images. Psychophysiology 52, 509–517. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hein, K. A., Jaeger, S. R., Tom Carr, B., and Delahunty, C. M. (2008). Comparison of five common acceptance and preference methods. Food Qual. Prefer. 19, 651–661. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2008.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, L. D., and Polich, J. (1998). EEG, ERPs and food consumption. Biol. Psychol. 48, 139–151. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(98)00010-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoogeveen, H. R., Dalenberg, J. R., Renken, R. J., ter Horst, G. J., and Lorist, M. M. (2015). Neural processing of basic tastes in healthy young and older adults - an fMRI study. Neuroimage 119, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horska, E., Bercik, J., Krasnodebski, A., Matysik-Pejas, R., and Bakayova, H. (2016). Innovative approaches to examining consumer preferences when choosing wines. Agric. Econ. 62, 124–133. doi: 10.17221/290/2015-AGRICECON

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeger, S. R., Cardello, A. V., and Schutz, H. G. (2013). Emotion questionnaires: a consumer-centric perspective. Food Qual. Prefer. 30, 229–241. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.05.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeger, S. R., and Hedderley, D. I. (2013). Impact of individual differences in emotional intensity and private body consciousness on EsSense Profile responses. Food Qual. Prefer. 27, 54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeger, S. R., Jørgensen, A. S., Aaslyng, M. D., and Bredie, W. L. (2008). Best–worst scaling: an introduction and initial comparison with monadic rating for preference elicitation with food products. Food Qual. Prefer. 19, 579–588. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2008.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jager, G., Schlich, P., Tijssen, I., Yao, J., Visalli, M., De Graaf, C., et al. (2014). Temporal dominance of emotions: measuring dynamics of food-related emotions during consumption. Food Qual. Prefer. 37, 87–99. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.04.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, G. (2003). The Psychology of Personal Constructs: Volume Two: Clinical Diagnosis and Psychotherapy. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Kenney, E., and Adhikari, K. (2016). Recent developments in identifying and quantifying emotions during food consumption. J. Sci. Food Agric. 96, 3627–3630. doi: 10.1002/jsfa.7717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, I.-A., Kim, M.-A., Van De Velden, M., and Lee, H.-S. (2013). Psychological positioning of bottled tea products: a comparison between two Kansei profiling techniques. Food Sci. Biotechnol. 22, 257–268. doi: 10.1007/s10068-013-0035-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, K. O., and O'mahony, M. (1998). A new approach to category scales of intensity I: traditional versus rank-rating. J. Sens. Stud. 13, 241–249. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-459X.1998.tb00086.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, S. C., and Meiselman, H. L. (2010). Development of a method to measure consumer emotions associated with foods. Food Qual. Prefer. 21, 168–177. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, S. C., Meiselman, H. L., and Carr, B. T. (2010). Measuring emotions associated with foods in consumer testing. Food Qual. Prefer. 21, 1114–1116. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2010.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, S. C., Meiselman, H. L., and Thomas Carr, B. (2013). Measuring emotions associated with foods: important elements of questionnaire and test design. Food Qual. Prefer. 28, 8–16. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koller, M., and Walla, P. (2015). Towards alternative ways to measure attitudes related to consumption: introducing startle reflex modulation. J. Agric. Food Indust. Organ. 13, 83. doi: 10.1515/jafio-2015-0023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Köster, E. P. (2009). Diversity in the determinants of food choice: a psychological perspective. Food Qual. Prefer. 20, 70–82. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2007.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Köster, E. P., and Mojet, J. (2015). From mood to food and from food to mood: a psychological perspective on the measurement of food-related emotions in consumer research. Food Res. Int. 76, 180–191. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2015.04.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kostyra, E., Rambuszek, M., Waszkiewicz-Robak, B., Laskowski, W., Blicharski, T., and Poławska, E. (2016). Consumer facial expression in relation to smoked ham with the use of face reading technology. The methodological aspects and informative value of research results. Meat Sci. 119, 22–31. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2016.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kozak, M., and Cliff, M. A. (2013). Systematic comparison of hedonic ranking and rating methods demonstrates few practical differences. J. Food Sci. 78, S1257–S1263. doi: 10.1111/1750-3841.12173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: a review. Biol. Psychol. 84, 394–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krieglmeyer, R., Deutsch, R., De Houwer, J., and De Raedt, R. (2010). Being moved: valence activates approach-avoidance behavior independently of evaluation and approach-avoidance intentions. Psychol. Sci. 21, 607–613. doi: 10.1177/0956797610365131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kringelbach, M. L., O'doherty, J., Rolls, E. T., and Andrews, C. (2003). Activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its subjective pleasantness. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1064–1071. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.10.1064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuenzel, J., Blanchette, I., Lion, R., Zandstra, E. H., Thomas, A., and El-Deredy, W. (2011). Conditioning specific positive states to unfamiliar flavours influences flavour liking. Food Qual. Prefer. 22, 397–403. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2010.10.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuenzel, J., Zandstra, E. H., Lion, R., Blanchette, I., Thomas, A., and El-Deredy, W. (2010). Conditioning unfamiliar and familiar flavours to specific positive emotions. Food Qual. Prefer. 21, 1105–1107. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2010.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuesten, C., Chopra, P., Bi, J., and Meiselman, H. L. (2014). A global study using PANAS (PA and NA) scales to measure consumer emotions associated with aromas of phytonutrient supplements. Food Qual. Prefer. 33, 86–97. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.11.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwak, H. S., Ahn, B. H., Lee, Y., Kreger, J., and Lee, S.-Y. (2013). Correlation of liking and disliking measurements in consumer acceptance tests. Food Qual. Prefer. 30, 86–92. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Labbe, D., Ferrage, A., Rytz, A., Pace, J., and Martin, N. (2015). Pleasantness, emotions and perceptions induced by coffee beverage experience depend on the consumption motivation (hedonic or utilitarian). Food Qual. Prefer. 44, 56–61. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.03.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange, C., Martin, C., Chabanet, C., Combris, P., and Issanchou, S. (2002). Impact of the information provided to consumers on their willingness to pay for Champagne: comparison with hedonic scores. Food Qual. Prefer. 13, 597–608. doi: 10.1016/S0950-3293(02)00059-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laurans, G. F. G., and Desmet, P. M. A. (2012). “Introducing PrEmo2: new directions for the non-verbal measurement of emotion in design,” in Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Design and Emotion (London).

Google Scholar

Leitch, K. A., Duncan, S. E., O'keefe, S., Rudd, R., and Gallagher, D. L. (2015). Characterizing consumer emotional response to sweeteners using an emotion terminology questionnaire and facial expression analysis. Food Res. Int. 76(Pt 2), 283–292. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2015.04.039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lévy, C. M., and Köster, E. P. (1999). The relevance of initial hedonic judgements in the prediction of subtle food choices. Food Qual. Prefer. 10, 185–200. doi: 10.1016/S0950-3293(99)00016-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, J. (2011). Hedonic scaling: a review of methods and theory. Food Qual. Prefer. 22, 733–747. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2011.05.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, J., and Padmanabhan, A. (2013). Retronasal olfaction in vegetable liking and disliking. Chem. Senses 38, 45–55. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjs080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, I. Y. (2004). Evaluating a servicescape: the effect of cognition and emotion. Int. J. Hosp. Manage. 23, 163–178. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhm.2003.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lotte, F., Congedo, M., Lécuyer, A., Lamarche, F., and Arnaldi, B. (2007). A review of classification algorithms for EEG-based brain–computer interfaces. J. Neural Eng. 4:R1. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/4/2/R01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macht, M., Gerer, J., and Ellgring, H. (2003). Emotions in overweight and normal-weight women immediately after eating foods differing in energy. Physiol. Behav. 80, 367–374. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2003.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manzocco, L., Rumignani, A., and Lagazio, C. (2013). Emotional response to fruit salads with different visual quality. Food Qual. Prefer. 28, 17–22. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.08.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marczinski, C. A., Stamates, A. L., Ossege, J., Maloney, S. F., Bardgett, M. E., and Brown, C. J. (2014). Subjective state, blood pressure, and behavioral control changes produced by an “Energy Shot”. J. Caffeine Res. 4, 57–63. doi: 10.1089/jcr.2014.0005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClure, S. M., Li, J., Tomlin, D., Cypert, K. S., Montague, L. M., and Montague, P. R. (2004). Neural correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar drinks. Neuron 44, 379–387. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNair, D., Lorr, M., and Droppleman, L. (1971). Mannual for the Profile of Mood States (POMS). San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.

Mehrabian, A., and Russell, J. A. (1974). An Approach to Environmental Psychology. Boston, MA: The MIT Press.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mielby, L. H., Nørgaard, M. K., Edelenbos, M., and Thybo, A. K. (2012). Affective response of adolescents toward fruit- and vegetable-based snacks and the role of neophobia, gender and age. J. Sens. Stud. 27, 425–438. doi: 10.1111/joss.12007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mojet, J., Dürrschmid, K., Danner, L., Jöchl, M., Heini,ö, R. L., Holthuysen, N., et al. (2015). Are implicit emotion measurements evoked by food unrelated to liking? Food Res. Int. 76(Pt 2), 224–232. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2015.06.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, D. J., and Konrath, S. (2015). “I can almost taste it:” why people with strong positive emotions experience higher levels of food craving, salivation and eating intentions. J. Consum. Psychol. 25, 42–59. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2014.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Narumi, T., Ban, Y., Kajinami, T., Tanikawa, T., and Hirose, M. (2012). “Augmented perception of satiety: controlling food consumption by changing apparent size of food with augmented reality,” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (Austin, TX: ACM), 109–118.

Google Scholar

Narumi, T., Nishizaka, S., Kajinami, T., Tanikawa, T., and Hirose, M. (2011). “Augmented reality flavors: gustatory display based on edible marker and cross-modal interaction,” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (Vancouver, BC: ACM), 93–102.

Google Scholar

Nestrud, M. A., Meiselman, H. L., King, S. C., Lesher, L. L., and Cardello, A. V. (2013). “A method for development of EsSense25–A shorter version of the EsSense (R) profile,” in 10th Pangborn Sensory Science Symposium 2013: Rio de Janeiro, Brazil (Rio de Janeiro).

Nestrud, M. A., Meiselman, H. L., King, S. C., Lesher, L. L., and Cardello, A. V. (2016). Development of EsSense25, a shorter version of the EsSense Profile. Food Qual. Prefer. 48, 107–117. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, M., Chaya, C., and Hort, J. (2013). Beyond liking: comparing the measurement of emotional response using EsSense Profile and consumer defined check-all-that-apply methodologies. Food Qual. Prefer. 28, 193–205. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.08.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nordbo, K., Milne, D., Calvo, R. A., and Allman-Farinelli, M. (2015). “Virtual food court: a VR environment to assess people's food choices,” in Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Australian Special Interest Group for Computer Human Interaction, 2015 (New York, NY: ACM), 69–72.

Google Scholar

Obrist, M., Gatti, E., Maggioni, E., Vi, C. T., and Velasco, C. (2017). Multisensory experiences in HCI. IEEE MultiMedia 24, 9–13. doi: 10.1109/MMUL.2017.33

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obrist, M., Velasco, C., Vi, C., Ranasinghe, N., Israr, A., Cheok, A., et al. (2016). Sensing the future of HCI: touch, taste, and smell user interfaces. Interactions 23, 40–49. doi: 10.1145/2973568

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'doherty, J., Rolls, E. T., Francis, S., Bowtell, R., and McGlone, F. (2001). Representation of pleasant and aversive taste in the human brain. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 1315–1321. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.3.1315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliver, G., and Wardle, J. (1999). Perceived effects of stress on food choice. Physiol. Behav. 66, 511–515. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00322-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oreskovich, D. C., Klein, B. P., and Sutherland, J. W. (1991). “Procrustes analysis and its applications to free-choice and other sensory profiling,” in Sensory Science Theory and Applications in Foods, eds H. T. Lawless, & B. P. Klein (New York, NY: Marcel Dekker), 353–393.

Pallavicini, F., Serino, S., Cipresso, P., Pedroli, E., Chicchi Giglioli, I. A., Chirico, A., et al. (2016). Testing augmented reality for cue exposure in obese patients: an exploratory study. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 19, 107–114. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2015.0235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parma, V., Castiello, U., Köster, E. P., and Mojet, J. (2014). Selecting food. The contribution of memory, liking, and action. Appetite 76, 186–196. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, S. H., and Azzam, P. N. (2005). Characterization of N200 and P300: selected studies of the event-related potential. Int. J. Med. Sci. 2:147. doi: 10.7150/ijms.2.147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pineau, N., Cordelle, S., and Schlich, P. (2003). “Temporal dominance of sensations: a new technique to record several sensory attributes simultaneously over time,” in 5th Pangborn Symposium (Boston, MA), 121.

Piqueras-Fiszman, B., Kraus, A. A., and Spence, C. (2014). “Yummy” versus “Yucky”! Explicit and implicit approach–avoidance motivations towards appealing and disgusting foods. Appetite 78, 193–202. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.03.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plassmann, H., O'doherty, J., Shiv, B., and Rangel, A. (2008). Marketing actions can modulate neural representations of experienced pleasantness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 1050–1054. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706929105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poole, N. D., Mart,i, L., and Giménez, F. V. (2007). Quality perceptions under evolving information conditions: implications for diet, health and consumer satisfaction. Food Policy 32, 175–188. doi: 10.1016/j.foodpol.2006.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porcherot, C., Delplanque, S., Raviot-Derrien, S., Le Calvé, B., Chrea, C., Gaudreau, N., et al. (2010). How do you feel when you smell this? Optimization of a verbal measurement of odor-elicited emotions. Food Qual. Prefer. 21, 938–947. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2010.03.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porcherot, C., Petit, E., Giboreau, A., Gaudreau, N., and Cayeux, I. (2015). Measurement of self-reported affective feelings when an aperitif is consumed in an ecological setting. Food Qual. Prefer. 39, 277–284. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.07.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quirin, M., Kazén, M., Rohrmann, S., and Kuhl, J. (2009). Implicit but not explicit affectivity predicts circadian and reactive cortisol: using the implicit positive and negative affect test. J. Pers. 77, 401–426. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00552.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rellecke, J., Palazova, M., Sommer, W., and Schacht, A. (2011). On the automaticity of emotion processing in words and faces: event-related brain potentials evidence from a superficial task. Brain Cogn. 77, 23–32. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Resano, H., Sanjuán, A. I., and Albisu, L. M. (2009). Consumers' acceptability and actual choice. An exploratory research on cured ham in Spain. Food Qual. Prefer. 20, 391–398. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., and Kagan, J. (1996). The psychological significance of changes in skin temperature. Motiv. Emot. 20, 63–78. doi: 10.1007/BF02251007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roggeveen, A. L., Grewal, D., Townsend, C., and Krishnan, R. (2015). The impact of dynamic presentation format on consumer preferences for hedonic products and services. J. Mark. 79, 34–49. doi: 10.1509/jm.13.0521

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosas-Nexticapa, M., Angulo, O., and O'mahony, M. (2005). How well does the 9-point hedonic scale predict purchase frequency? J. Sens. Stud. 20, 313–331. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-459X.2005.00027.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, C. G., Busson, A., Flight, I., Bryan, J., Van Lawick Van Pabst, J. A., and Cox, D. N. (2004). A comparison of three laddering techniques applied to an example of a complex food choice. Food Qual. Prefer. 15, 569–583. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2003.11.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychol. Rev. 110:145. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, J. A., Weiss, A., and Mendelson, G. A. (1989). Affect grid: a single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 493–502. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.3.493

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saeed, A., Trajanovski, S., Van Keulen, M., and Van Erp, J. (2017). “Deep physiological arousal detection in a driving simulator using wearable sensors,” in IEEE International Conference on Data Miningworkshop: Data Mining in Biomedical Informatics and Healthcare (DMBIH) (New Orleans, LA).

Google Scholar

Sandt, A. R., Sloan, D. M., and Johnson, K. J. (2009). Measuring appetitive responding with the postauricular reflex. Psychophysiology 46, 491–497. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00797.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schacht, A., and Sommer, W. (2009). Emotions in word and face processing: early and late cortical responses. Brain Cogn. 69, 538–550. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schifferstein, H. N. J., Fenko, A., Desmet, P. M. A., Labbe, D., and Martin, N. (2013). Influence of package design on the dynamics of multisensory and emotional food experience. Food Qual. Prefer. 27, 18–25. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitt, N. (1998). Quantifying word association responses: what is native-like? System 26, 389–401. doi: 10.1016/S0346-251X(98)00019-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schöning, J., Rogers, Y., and Krüger, A. (2012). Digitally enhanced food. IEEE Pervasive Comput. 11, 4–6. doi: 10.1109/MPRV.2012.40

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schouteten, J. J., De Steur, H., De Pelsmaeker, S., Lagast, S., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., and Gellynck, X. (2015). An integrated method for the emotional conceptualization and sensory characterization of food products: the EmoSensory-≪ Wheel. Food Res. Int. 78, 96–107. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2015.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schouteten, J. J., De Steur, H., De Pelsmaeker, S., Lagast, S., Juvinal, J. G., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., et al. (2016). Emotional and sensory profiling of insect-, plant- and meat-based burgers under blind, expected and informed conditions. Food Qual. Prefer. 52, 27–31. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.03.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schreuder, E., van Erp, J., Toet, A., and Kallen, V. L. (2016). Emotional responses to multisensory environmental stimuli: a conceptual framework and literature review. SAGE Open 6, 1–19. doi: 10.1177/2158244016630591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schüssler, P., Kluge, M., Yassouridis, A., Dresler, M., Uhr, M., and Steiger, A. (2012). Ghrelin levels increase after pictures showing food. Obesity 20, 1212–1217. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutz, H. G., and Cardello, A. V. (2001). A labeled affective magnitude (LAM) scale for assessing food liking/disliking. J. Sens. Stud. 16, 117–159. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-459X.2001.tb00293.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silva, A. P., Jager, G., Van Bommel, R., Van Zyl, H., Voss, H.-P., Hogg, T., et al. (2016). Functional or emotional? How Dutch and Portuguese conceptualise beer, wine and non-alcoholic beer consumption. Food Qual. Prefer. 49, 54–65. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.11.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Small, D. M., Zatorre, R. J., Dagher, A., Evans, A. C., and Jones-Gotman, M. (2001). Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolateFrom pleasure to aversion. Brain 124, 1720–1733. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.9.1720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, C. (2011). Mouth-watering: the influence of environmental and cognitive factors on salivation and gustatory/flavor perception. J. Texture Stud. 42, 157–171. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-4603.2011.00299.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, C., and Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (2014). The Perfect Meal: The Multisensory Science of Food and Dining. John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D. (1983). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Adults, Mountain View. Mountain View, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.

Spinelli, S., Masi, C., Dinnella, C., Zoboli, G. P., and Monteleone, E. (2014). How does it make you feel? A new approach to measuring emotions in food product experience. Food Qual. Prefer. 37, 109–122. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spinelli, S., Masi, C., Zoboli, G. P., Prescott, J., and Monteleone, E. (2015). Emotional responses to branded and unbranded foods. Food Qual. Prefer. 42, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.12.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiner, J. E., Glaser, D., Hawilo, M. E., and Berridge, K. C. (2001). Comparative expression of hedonic impact: affective reactions to taste by human infants and other primates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 53–74. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00051-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steuer, J. (1992). Defining virtual reality: dimensions determining telepresence. J. Commun. 42, 73–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.1992.tb00812.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sudre, J., Pineau, N., Loret, C., and Martin, N. (2012). Comparison of methods to monitor liking of food during consumption. Food Qual. Prefer. 24, 179–189. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2011.10.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sulmont-Ross,é, C., Chabanet, C., Issanchou, S., and Köster, E. P. (2008). Impact of the arousal potential of uncommon drinks on the repeated exposure effect. Food Qual. Prefer. 19, 412–420. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2007.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swan, A. B., Cohen, A., Evans, S. R., and Drescher, B. A. (2013). Influence of taste quality on affective state. Psi Chi J. Psychol. Res. 18, 61–66. doi: 10.24839/2164-8204.JN18.2.61

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Symoneaux, R., Galmarini, M. V., and Mehinagic, E. (2012). Comment analysis of consumer's likes and dislikes as an alternative tool to preference mapping. A case study on apples. Food Qual. Prefer. 24, 59–66. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2011.08.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 38, 227–242. doi: 10.1177/0022022106297301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomson, D. M. H., Crocker, C., and Marketo, C. G. (2010). Linking sensory characteristics to emotions: an example using dark chocolate. Food Qual. Prefer. 21, 1117–1125. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2010.04.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toet, A., Van Schaik, C. S., Kaneko, D., and Van Erp, J. B. F. (2019). Do food cinemagraphs evoke stronger appetitive responses than stills? Int. J. Food Design 4.

Tsourides, K., Shariat, S., Nejati, H., Gandhi, T. K., Cardinaux, A., Simons, C. T., et al. (2016). Neural correlates of the food/non-food visual distinction. Biol. Psychol. 115, 35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuorila, H., Peränen, K., Uutela, A., and Jallinoja, P. (2015). Expectations and experiences of institutional foods among Finnish male conscripts. Food Qual. Prefer. 43, 141–144. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.02.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ung, C.-Y., Menozzi, M., Hartmann, C., and Siegrist, M. (2018). Innovations in consumer research: the virtual food buffet. Food Qual. Prefer. 63, 12–17. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.07.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varela, P., Beltrán, J., and Fiszman, S. (2014). An alternative way to uncover drivers of coffee liking: preference mapping based on consumers' preference ranking and open comments. Food Qual. Prefer. 32, 152–159. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Velasco, C., Obrist, M., Petit, O., and Spence, C. (2018). Multisensory technology for flavor augmentation: a mini review. Front. Psychol. 9:26. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veltman, J., and Gaillard, A. (1998). Physiological workload reactions to increasing levels of task difficulty. Ergonomics 41, 656–669. doi: 10.1080/001401398186829

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venkatraman, V., Dimoka, A., Pavlou, P. A., Vo, K., Hampton, W., Bollinger, B., et al. (2015). Predicting advertising success beyond traditional measures: new insights from neurophysiological methods and market response modeling. J. Market.Res. 52, 436–452. doi: 10.1509/jmr.13.0593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villanueva, N. D. M., Petenate, A. J., and Da Silva, M. A. A. P. (2000). Performance of three affective methods and diagnosis of the ANOVA model. Food Qual. Prefer. 11, 363–370. doi: 10.1016/S0950-3293(00)00006-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villanueva, N. D. M., Petenate, A. J., and Da Silva, M. A. A. P. (2005). Performance of the hybrid hedonic scale as compared to the traditional hedonic, self-adjusting and ranking scales. Food Qual. Prefer. 16, 691–703. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2005.03.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walla, P., Richter, M., Färber, S., Leodolter, U., and Bauer, H. (2010). Food-evoked changes in humans. J. Psychophysiol. 24, 25–32. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, B., O'mahony, M., and Wichchukit, S. (2010). Various paired preference tests: experimenter effect on take home choice. J. Sens. Stud. 25, 778–790. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-459X.2010.00309.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wichchukit, S., and O'mahony, M. (2010). Paired preference tests:‘Liking',‘Buying'and ‘Take Away'preferences. Food Qual. Prefer. 21, 925–929. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2010.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willner, P., and Healy, S. (1994). Decreased hedonic responsiveness during a brief depressive mood swing. J. Affect. Disord. 32, 13–20. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(94)90056-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, T. D., Lisle, D. J., Schooler, J. W., Hodges, S. D., Klaaren, K. J., and Lafleur, S. J. (1993). Introspecting about reasons can reduce post-choice satisfaction. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 19, 331–339. doi: 10.1177/0146167293193010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winkielman, P., Berridge, K. C., and Sher, S. (2011). “Emotion, consciousness, and social behavior,” in The Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience, eds J. Decety and J. T. Cacioppo (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 195–211. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195342161.013.0012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeomans, M. R., and Symes, T. (1999). Individual differences in the use of pleasantness and palatability ratings. Appetite 32, 383–394. doi: 10.1006/appe.1999.0224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshikawa, T., Tanaka, M., Ishii, A., and Watanabe, Y. (2014). Suppressive responses by visual food cues in postprandial activities of insular cortex as revealed by magnetoencephalography. Brain Res. 1568, 31–41. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.04.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshimura, H., Honjo, M., Sugai, T., Kawabe, M., Kaneyama, K., Segami, N., et al. (2011). Influences of audio-visual environments on feelings of deliciousness during having sweet foods: an electroencephalogram frequency analysis study. Nutr. Neurosci. 14, 210–215. doi: 10.1179/1476830511Y.0000000014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zandstra, E. H., De Graaf, C., Mela, D. J., and Van Staveren, W. A. (2000). Short- and long-term effects of changes in pleasantness on food intake. Appetite 34, 253–260. doi: 10.1006/appe.1999.0304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zandstra, E. H., De Graaf, C., Van Trijp, H. C. M., and Van Staveren, W. A. (1999). Laboratory hedonic ratings as predictors of consumption. Food Qual. Prefer. 10, 411–418. doi: 10.1016/S0950-3293(98)00050-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zandstra, E. H., and El-Deredy, W. (2011). Effects of energy conditioning on food preferences and choice. Appetite 57, 45–49. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeinstra, G. G., Koelen, M. A., Colindres, D., Kok, F. J., and De Graaf, C. (2009). Facial expressions in school-aged children are a good indicator of ‘dislikes', but not of ‘likes'. Food Qual. Prefer. 20, 620–624. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: food-evoked emotion, physiological, behavioral, cognitive, toolbox, emotional processing level

Citation: Kaneko D, Toet A, Brouwer A-M, Kallen V and van Erp JBF (2018) Methods for Evaluating Emotions Evoked by Food Experiences: A Literature Review. Front. Psychol. 9:911. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00911

Received: 06 October 2017; Accepted: 18 May 2018;
Published: 08 June 2018.

Edited by:

Sergio Machado, Salgado de Oliveira University, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Mariano Luis Alcañiz Raya, Universitat Politècnica de València, Spain
Marianna Obrist, University of Sussex, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2018 Kaneko, Toet, Brouwer, Kallen and van Erp. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Daisuke Kaneko, ZGthbmVrb0BtYWlsLmtpa2tvbWFuLmNvLmpw

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.