- School of Psychology, National University of Ireland, Galway, Ireland
Music listening may serve many adaptive functions in everyday life. However, studies examining the relationship between the functions of music listening (FML) and wellbeing outcomes have produced mixed findings. The purpose of this study is to develop a new measure to assess music listening functions that is psychometrically robust, and suitable for outcomes-based research on music listening and wellbeing. Scale items were developed based on a literature review and a prior qualitative enquiry. The items were reviewed by four content experts in music psychology and scale development. Scale structure was investigated by EFA and CFA in two large samples of participants (N = 1,191, 17–66 years, M = 22.04, SD = 6.23, 326 males). Tests of dimensionality revealed a 46-item scale with 11 factors for the Adaptive Functions of Music Listening (AFML) scale. Namely, Stress Regulation, Anxiety Regulation, Anger Regulation, Loneliness Regulation, Rumination, Reminiscence, Strong Emotional Experiences, Awe and Appreciation, Cognitive Regulation, Identity, and Sleep FML. The scale and its subscales possess good internal consistency and construct validity. In line with theory and research on gender differences in FML, scores on factors representing affect regulation FML were significantly higher among female respondents. Supporting the concurrent validity of the AFML scale, factors were positively correlated with an existing measure of the FML—the Music USE questionnaire. Further evidence of construct validity derives from positive associations between affect regulation factor scores and level of reappraisal, and lack of association with suppression, as measured by the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. Consistent with the view that adaptive FML are positively related to wellbeing, a number of factors, affect regulation factors in particular, were significantly positively correlated with subjective, psychological, and social wellbeing measures across two cross-sectional studies.
Introduction
Listening to music is a common behavior engaged in by most people from childhood through to adulthood and into advanced old age (Laukka, 2007; Juslin et al., 2008). Music listening may serve many adaptive functions for individuals across their lifespan. Questionnaire and survey studies have been generative in providing lists of common functions of music listening (FML). A number of these studies have also uncovered a latent factor structure of the FML using cluster or factor analysis—but further scale development and validation efforts to create instruments to measure the FML have been slower to emerge. Previous investigations have found that the FML are broadly affective, cognitive, and social (Hargreaves and North, 1999). In a review of twenty eight empirical studies, Schäfer et al. (2013) identified 129 FML which they synthesized into three high-level functions: arousal and mood regulation, self-awareness, and social relatedness. This synthesis was based on the item-level analysis of existing FML scales and taxonomies. Qualitative studies have highlighted a wider range of other potentially adaptive FML including goal-attainment, cognitive control, transcendence, absorption, and flow, amongst others (Hays and Minichiello, 2005; Lamont, 2011; Herbert, 2012; Schäfer et al., 2014; Groarke and Hogan, 2016). While a number of FML scales have been developed to measure specific functions of music listening, such as mood regulation (Saarikallio, 2008), or specific musical experiences like absorption (Sandstrom and Russo, 2013), the current study sought to validate a general measure of the FML. Table 1 provides a summary of the scale structure and psychometric properties of five existing global measures of the FML identified in the literature. A review of Table 1 suggests there is a need for a psychometric measure of the functions of music listening that incorporates a larger set of FML in a single multi-factorial scale.
A survey of the literature suggests that music listening may produce effects that relate to a number of adaptive outcomes, such as enhanced wellbeing. For instance, music may enhance wellbeing by inducing positive affective experiences (Juslin et al., 2008), regulating negative affective experiences (Knight and Rickard, 2001; Sandstrom and Russo, 2010; Radstaak et al., 2014), enhancing social functioning (Hays and Minichiello, 2005; North and Hargreaves, 2006; Miranda and Claes, 2009), and giving rise to experiences such as transcendence, peak experience, and flow (Gabrielsson, 2010; Lamont, 2011; Harrison and Loui, 2014).
At the same time, there have been relatively few direct investigations of the relationship between self-reported functions of music listening and specific wellbeing outcomes (Laukka, 2007; Thoma et al., 2012; Chin and Rickard, 2014a,b; Papinczak et al., 2015; Saarikallio et al., 2015; Randall and Rickard, 2017). The limited number of studies is perhaps due to the lack of suitable measures. Some of these studies have used well-developed psychometric measures of FML (Chin and Rickard, 2014a,b; Saarikallio et al., 2015), whereas other studies have used a limited number of researcher-selected lists of FML without carrying out further scale development or validation. This suggests that a new integrative measure of music listening functions that is psychometrically sound may be a useful addition to the emerging field of music, health, and wellbeing. Contrary to expectations, some studies examining relationships between FML and wellbeing highlight some negative effects of music listening on measures of wellbeing. For example, among university students, listening to music “to reduce loneliness” predicted lower quality of life (Thoma et al., 2012). Similarly, Randall and Rickard (2017) found that more frequent listening to music for emotional reasons (e.g., “to cope,” “to forget”) was related to higher depression and anxiety.
Integrating research on the functions of music listening is difficult due to the varied conceptual definitions of constructs used across studies (i.e., outcomes, effects, responses, goals, reasons, rewards, reactions, experiences, uses, motives, and functions of music listening). For example, there is an important distinction to be made between the outcome or effect a listener is pursuing (i.e., the goal, motive, reason) and the outcome they actually experience listening to music (i.e., the outcome, effect, response, reaction). Relationships with various outcomes, including wellbeing, would be expected to differ based on this distinction. In the current project, in an effort to support greater synthesis, a long-standing theory of learning and behavior, Social Cognitive Theory (Bandura, 1989, 2001) underpins the development of items for the The Adaptive Functions of Music Listening Scale (AFML scale).
Social Cognitive Theory (SCT) would predict that music listening behavior is guided by listeners' beliefs about the effects of music (i.e., self-efficacy) and their expectations of achieving particular outcomes by listening to music (i.e., outcome expectations). The listener's beliefs and expectations about the effects and outcomes of music are acquired through experience and vicarious learning. With time, these effects and outcomes provide incentives that become goals guiding the behavior of music listening. When such goal-directed behavior is effective, outcomes of musical experiences become functions that music listening serves in an individual's life.
The application of SCT as an approach to scale item development undertaken in the current study is also in line with recent theoretical developments in music psychology, where goals and effects are increasingly being seen as important facets of the music listening experience (van Goethem and Sloboda, 2011; Baltazar and Saarikallio, 2016; Schäfer, 2016). The AFML scale asks participants to rate their level of agreement with statements about the efficacy of achieving a range of potential adaptive outcomes when listening to music. This allows researchers to test the hypothesis that endorsing an adaptive function of music listening is indeed associated with an adaptive outcome, such as the enhancement or maintenance of wellbeing.
This approach relies on the retrospective self-reports of listeners, which can be limited by recall bias and influenced by commonly held positive beliefs about the efficacy of music for different functions. An alternative approach would be to use Experience Sampling Methodologies (ESM) to measure an individual's goal or function of music listening prior to an episode of music listening, and then measure the efficacy of music listening for that function in that individual episode, as well as over the course of all music listening episodes sampled. Clearly this is a powerful methodological approach. Yet, there are some drawbacks that limit the application of ESM in all research contexts. For instance, the pagers and palmtop computers that have been used to randomly sample episodes of everyday life are prohibitively expensive and ESM studies of music using them have had relatively small sample sizes (Sloboda et al., 2001; Juslin et al., 2008). Other studies have used text messages to prompt participants to complete questionnaires at random intervals throughout the day (North et al., 2004; Greasley and Lamont, 2011). While this approach is cost-effective, the potential for response forms to be back-filled or forward-filled reduces the validity of the data. A new tool for conducting ESM studies on music listening is now available: The MuPsych application (Randall and Rickard, 2013). The MuPsych app collects data in real-time using pop-up questionnaires presented on the participant's smartphone allowing for ecologically valid data collection in naturalistic settings. In general, participant burden is high in ESM studies as participants are requested to carry new devices or questionnaires at all times for the duration of the study. There is also some concern that participant burden would be higher for older people who may struggle to adopt new devices and technologies (i.e., pagers, palmtops, smartphones and portable music players) (Zhou et al., 2014). Experience sampling is certainly a robust method of studying adaptive FML in context and over time. However, it may be less suitable for collecting data with older cohorts, or for studies requiring large sample sizes, such as scale development work.
The current study followed guidelines for scale development advocated by DeVellis (2012). This involves following a series of steps in the scale development process, specifically, conducting a thorough review of the literature surrounding the constructs to be measured, generating an exhaustive item pool, selecting an appropriate response format, item evaluation by experts, application of both exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis in separate scale development samples, alpha reliability assessment, and optimizing scale length.
Qualitative work has been identified as the ideal starting point for scale construction, particularly for item development and identifying valid domains to include in item pools (O'Brien, 1993; Rowan and Wulff, 2007; DeVellis, 2012). The conceptual framework informing scale development in the current study arose out of focus group style sessions with music listeners (Groarke and Hogan, 2016) and a review of existing FML research, with a particular focus on research linking music listening and wellbeing. The initial conceptual model informing the development of scale items is presented in Figure 2.
A summary overview of the adaptive functions of music listening identified by literature review and qualitative enquiry, and their relevance for wellbeing outcomes and construct validation are described below. These include affective, social, eudaimonic, and cognitive functions of music listening.
Affective functions, such as mood and emotion regulation, are the most commonly reported reasons for music listening (Tarrant et al., 2000; Schäfer et al., 2013). Wellbeing research and theory highlights important distinctions between positive and negative affective experiences, between affective experience and affective regulation, and distinguishes between subjective, psychological, and social dimensions of wellbeing. Scale items on existing FML measures have not been developed in line with these distinctions limiting our ability to make theoretically-derived hypotheses regarding potential relationships between these discrete FML and wellbeing outcomes.
Affective experiences can be distinguished by valence (positive and negative) and arousal (high arousal and low arousal) dimensions (Russell, 1980; Watson and Tellegen, 1985), and many studies have demonstrated that music listening can induce a range of affective experiences that vary along these dimensions (Juslin et al., 2008). However, existing FML measures have not always distinguished affective experiences in music listening along these dimensions. For example, The Barcelona Music Reward Questionnaire; (Mas-Herrero et al., 2013) includes an affective factor measuring emotional evocation, which includes general items such as “I get emotional listening to certain pieces of music.” The Uses of Music Inventory (Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2007) includes a factor to measure emotional use of music—participants rate their agreement with statements such as “Listening to music really affects my mood.” Such items become problematic when trying to predict relationships between FML factor scores and wellbeing, as they do not distinguish the valence of the emotion referred to, and research shows that positive and negative affective experiences can independently predict wellbeing outcomes (Diener et al., 2004). Therefore, it is important to develop a measure of the FML that distinguishes a range of affective experiences that can be stimulated by music. As shown in Figure 2, these include positive and negative affect, strong and mixed emotional experiences and reminiscence (Sloboda, 1999; Gabrielsson, 2010; Schäfer et al., 2014). It is expected that scores on affective experience factors will associate positively with both higher positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA).
Affect regulation strategies influence the frequency and intensity of ongoing affective experience (Folkman and Lazarus, 1988). As noted above, positive and negative affective experiences induced by music listening may relate differentially to wellbeing, whereas, the successful regulation of NA in music listening should relate to increased wellbeing (Larsen, 2009). However, with the exception of The Barcelona Music Reward Questionnaire (Mas-Herrero et al., 2013) FML scales have not distinguished between affective experience and affective regulation FML.
Every FML measure reviewed in Table 1 has extracted at least one factor measuring affect regulation. However, a drawback of these global FML scales is that affect regulation factors do not always enable simultaneous measurement of the strategies by which music can regulate negative affective experiences. For example, items measuring cognitive and emotional regulation on the MUSE questionnaire include “I often listen to music when I'm feeling down” and “Specific types of music makes me feel better” (Chin and Rickard, 2012). At the same time, music psychology research has pointed to a substantial range of different strategies listeners use to regulate affective experience, such as relaxation, distraction/diversion, venting/discharge, mental work/rational thinking, solace/comfort, and rumination (Saarikallio and Erkkila, 2007; Saarikallio, 2011; van Goethem and Sloboda, 2011). Some of these strategies correspond with the general affect regulation literature, which, in the context of mainstream health psychology points to various behavioral and cognitive, emotion-focused and problem-focused regulation strategies (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984; Carver et al., 1989; Gross, 1998; Larsen, 2000). Item development for the AFML scale builds upon this theoretical and empirical research and proposes a diverse and comprehensive range of regulation strategies by which listening to music may regulate NA. Namely, distraction, venting, reappraisal, emotional support, emotional approach, generating positive emotions, escape, and rumination (see Figure 2). The wider literature also suggests regulation strategies may vary in terms of their relationship with wellbeing outcomes (Gross and John, 2003; Aldao et al., 2010). Therefore, including diverse affect regulation FML in scales will be useful for research questions regarding the efficacy of different musical affect regulation strategies for wellbeing enhancement.
Given the importance of affect regulation for wellbeing, it is expected that affect regulation FML will be associated with higher subjective wellbeing (SWB), specifically, lower negative affect, higher positive affect and higher reported life satisfaction.
Most existing measures of the FML have extracted a factor relating to the social functions of music (see Table 1). Factors measuring increased connection and bonding between listeners over shared musical tastes and group listening experiences have been determined (Chin and Rickard, 2012; Mas-Herrero et al., 2013). Other measures have focused on the value of music in social situations, such as increased atmosphere and celebration (Kuntsche et al., 2016). Therefore, music listening may have an important function in the development and maintenance of positive relationships with others. At the same time, using existing measures, the relationship between the social functions of music and wellbeing outcomes have not been firmly established. For instance, in one study, the factor measuring social connection on the MUSE questionnaire was not associated with enhanced subjective, psychological or social wellbeing; rather it was significantly associated with increased use of the emotion regulation strategy of suppression, which predicted lower levels of wellbeing (Chin and Rickard, 2014a). A survey study of younger adults by Papinczak et al. (2015) found that higher social wellbeing was predicted by the total effect of four FML (i.e., relationship building, immersing in emotions, modifying emotions, modifying cognitions). Theoretically, social FML should be related to greater psychological wellbeing (Ryff and Keyes, 1995) and social wellbeing (Keyes, 1998), however, empirical support is lacking.
In relation to the social function of identity, although further scale development work was not undertaken, identity FML have been extracted using Principal Components Analysis (PCA) in survey studies with adolescent (Lonsdale and North, 2011) and older samples (Laukka, 2007). In line with theory and research it is predicted that if identity FML are uncovered in factor analysis in the current study, they may relate to greater SWB through increased positive affect (Kahn et al., 1985), and higher psychological wellbeing through increased self-acceptance, psychological growth, and meaning (Ryff, 1989; Pennebaker and Seagal, 1999; Haslam et al., 2009).
Previous investigations have uncovered a number of FML that could be described as eudaimonic functions. These include music-induced peak experiences (Maslow, 1999; Gabrielsson, 2010), transcendence (Hays and Minichiello, 2008; Schäfer et al., 2014), and engagement or flow (Lamont, 2011). Participants engaged in focus groups voted that a number of these FML (i.e., transcendence and meaning) were beneficial for wellbeing enhancement (Groarke and Hogan, 2016). Outside of musical contexts, such eudaimonic experiences, particularly transcendence, have been associated with increased happiness and life satisfaction, and greater meaning in life (Gillham et al., 2011). Empirical studies of FML have tended not to include items to measure these eudaimonic functions, thus it remains to be seen whether eudaimonic experiences in music listening also relate positively to subjective, psychological, and social wellbeing outcomes.
Factors relating to listening to music for its cognitive effects are a focus of existing measures of the FML (Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2007; Chin and Rickard, 2012). Cognitive functions include music analysis (Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2007), and it is possible that the pleasure derived from the analysis of music may relate to enhanced SWB. A more reflective style of music listening may also provide a sense of awe and appreciation, stimulating self-reflection and insight (Cupchik, 1995; Groarke and Hogan, 2016), which may theoretically relate to higher psychological wellbeing (Ryff and Singer, 2008).
The use of music to regulate cognitive states like curiosity and creativity, as well as focus, attention, and motivation have been noted in surveys (North et al., 2000; Tarrant et al., 2000) and measures of the FML (Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2007; Chin and Rickard, 2012). These effects of music may support listeners in the achievement of everyday goals that depend on cognitive engagement and proficiency (DeNora, 2000; Groarke and Hogan, 2016). Goal-attainment and achievement are central to models of wellbeing (Ryan and Deci, 2000; Seligman, 2012), and have been related to emotional wellbeing in empirical research (Schultheiss et al., 2008). Therefore the pursuit of such cognitive goals by music listening may also relate to increased wellbeing. This view is consistent with some available research. For example, the cognitive and emotion regulation factor on the MUSE questionnaire was associated with higher subjective, psychological and social wellbeing (Chin and Rickard, 2014a,b). These effects were fully mediated by increased use of the affect regulation strategy reappraisal. Qualitative research has also proposed that cognitive regulation in music may support affect regulation goals (DeNora, 1999; Papinczak et al., 2015). Thus, it is hypothesized that cognitive FML may be associated with higher self-reported wellbeing in the current study.
The construct validity of a measure is assessed by forming theoretically-based hypotheses regarding potential relationships with other measures (Carmines and Zeller, 1979). In this study construct validity will be assessed using convergent validity, that is, how closely scores on the measure under development converge with scores on other measures of related constructs (Furr and Bacharach, 2008). In line with theory and research summarized above, significant associations should be observed between FML constructs as measured by the AFML scale and measures of affect, affect regulation, and wellbeing. A measure's construct validity can also be established using concurrent validity, that is, by demonstrating even greater convergence with scores on other measures of the same construct. In the current study the well-validated measure of affective, social, and cognitive FML—The Music USE Questionnaire (MUSE) (Chin and Rickard, 2012) will be employed to test the concurrent validity of the AFML scale.
Materials and Methods
Design
The current scale development study involved two phases: Firstly, the initial scale development phase involved item generation, assessing the dimensionality of the measure using EFA, reducing the initial pool of questionnaire items, and examining the reliability and construct validity of the AFML scale. The second phase involved confirming the factor structure derived from EFA in a separate sample of participants (recruited 9 months later). In the first phase construct validity was assessed by testing hypothesized relationships between AFML factors and subjective wellbeing outcomes. In the second confirmatory phase additional relationships between FML factors and psychological and social wellbeing outcomes were examined, in addition to relationships with a general measure of emotion regulation, and an existing general measure of music listening functions.
Initial Scale Development
Item Generation
Two-hundred and forty items were generated on the basis of an extensive literature review and four focus group sessions, with two younger adult groups (N = 25, M = 22.49 years, SD = 2.25) and two older adult groups (N = 19, M = 65.86, SD = 4.46). At least five items and one reverse scored item were generated for 38 hypothesized functions of music listening derived from these sessions.
Content Validity
Four content experts on scale development (3 in music psychology, 1 in psychometrics) responding to an online questionnaire rated these 240 items for their clarity, relevance, and comprehensiveness. The experts made a number of suggestions to increase the clarity and meaningfulness of items, removal of redundant items, and restructuring of the affect regulation subscales to allow for more differentiated responding. Overall, 164 items were rated by experts as relevant and were retained for EFA.
Pilot Testing
The 164-item AFML measure was administered to 9 lay experts, or potential participants (4 male, 18–30 years, M = 21.55, SD = 4.80) for pilot testing. All 164 items were rated as very or quite clear. Although participants rated the items highly, they also reported that the questionnaire was long and repetitive in certain respects. However, all items were retained for factor analysis in order to identify the items of highest psychometric quality for inclusion in the final AFML scale. DeVellis (2012) emphasizes the need for a large pool of items, and multiple indicators for each hypothesized construct at the development stage. Therefore, multiple indicators (including 1 reverse scored item) representing each of the remaining 33 hypothesized FML were administered to a development sample for item reduction and EFA.
Further Scale Development
Procedure
Potential participants were invited to take part in an online survey of why they listen to music via online advertisements, university email campaigns, and national media. All participants provided informed consent, and completed the questionnaire packet online using Survey Gizmo.
Participants
Development sample
In the development phase, 1,396 participants initially consented to take part. 673 participants (452 Females) completed all items in the online questionnaire packet (48% completion rate).
Confirmatory sample
Of 1,267 prospective participants who consented to take part in the online questionnaire at the confirmatory stage, 47% completed it (N = 597 participants, 413 Females).
Seventy three percent of the development sample and 55% of the confirmatory sample were undergraduate psychology students receiving research participation credit. One instructed item was included to identify insufficient effort responding (i.e., please select the “neutral” response option). Thirty seven participants were removed from the EFA analysis for failing to select the correct response to the instructed item. The remaining 637 respondents included in EFA analyses were mostly female (68%) university students (98%). In the confirmatory factor analysis, thirty two participants were removed for insufficient effort responding, and the remaining 554 participants were mostly female (69%) university students (87%). The final sample included in analyses in both phases of the study includes only those participants who completed all items in the online questionnaire, and who selected the correct response to the instructed item.
Materials
Development sample
In addition to demographic questions (i.e., age, gender, educational, and occupational status), participants completed the following measures:
The adaptive functions of music listening scale—(AFML scale). Participants rate their level of agreement with 164 items representing outcome expectations of a range of music listening functions using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The AFML scale has a Flesch-Kincaid Grade level of 5 and a Flesch Reading Ease score of 83.8 placing it in the “easy” range (Flesch, 1948; Kincaid et al., 1975).
Music engagement intensity subscale of the Music USE questionnaire (MUSE) (Chin and Rickard, 2012). This 8-item measure provides 3 indices of music engagement. Scores range from 1 to 25 on the Index of Music Listening (IML), with higher scores indicating more intense music listening. The Index of Music Training (IMT) assesses an individual's music education, higher scores indicate more musical training. The Index of Music Instrument Playing (IMIP) provides a total score based on respondents' years of instrument playing, hours of practice per day and regularity of practice. Higher scores represent greater engagement with instrument playing.
Subjective wellbeing
Positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) (Watson et al., 1988). The PANAS consists of 20 adjectives: 10 describe positive emotions and 10 describe negative emotions. Participants indicate the extent to which they have experienced these emotions in the previous week, using a Likert scale ranging from “very slightly or not at all” (1) to “extremely” (5). Two sub-scale scores are derived, with higher scores indicating greater positive and negative affect, respectively.
The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) (Diener et al., 1985). Participants indicate their level of agreement with 5 life satisfaction statements, using a 7-point scale that ranges from 7 (strongly agree) to 1 (strongly disagree).
Confirmatory sample. In addition to the measures completed by participants in the development phase, the participants in the confirmatory sample completed a number of additional measures to further assess construct validity:
Adaptive functions of music listening scale. A shortened version of the AFML scale with 48 items was administered (based on EFA results). Responses to statements of potential outcomes of music listening were made on a 5-point Likert-scale ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree). Subscale scores are calculated by averaging scores across items.
Mental health continuum- short form (MHC-SF) (Lamers et al., 2010). The MHC-SF is a 14-item measure of positive psychological functioning. It assesses three dimensions of wellbeing: Emotional, Psychological and Social wellbeing. Respondents rate the frequency of various feelings of wellbeing on a 6-point Likert scale (never - everyday) and scores are then summed. The emotional wellbeing dimension is an alternative 3-item measure of SWB, thus only Psychological and Social wellbeing scores on the MHC-SF will be presented.
The Music USE Questionnaire (MUSE) (Chin and Rickard, 2012). The Music Engagement Styles (MES) subscales measure five styles of music listening. These styles are (i) cognitive and emotional regulation (7items), (ii) engaged production (9 items), (iii) social connection (3 items), (iv) physical exercise (3 items), and (v) dance (2 items). Respondents rate their agreement with a series of statements (e.g., “I often listen to music when I'm feeling down”) on a 6-point Likert scale from “0” (not at all/not applicable to me) to “5” (strongly agree). Scores are summed.
Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) (Gross and John, 2003). The ERQ is a 10 item measure composed of two subscales: Reappraisal and Suppression. Reappraisal is the cognitive restructuring of thoughts to increase positive and reduce negative emotions, whereas suppression involves inhibiting the expression of emotion. Scores, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), are summed for each subscale.
Descriptive statistics and Cronbach's alpha for all of the measures included in this study with both samples of participants are presented in Table 2.
Table 2. Participant characteristics and descriptive statistics for all measures included in two phases of data collection with the development sample and a separate confirmatory sample.
Results
Dimensionality
Exploratory Factor Analysis—Development Sample
An Exploratory Factory Analysis (Principal Axis Factoring) was carried out using SPSS version 22. Factors representing music listening functions were expected to correlate therefore oblique rotation using Direct Oblimin with Kaiser Normalization was deemed appropriate. The Kaiser Meyer-Olkin measure (KMO = 0.95), and a significant Bartlett's test of sphericity [ = 17,454.06, p < 0.001] indicated suitability of the dataset for factor analysis.
Factor retention decisions were made on the basis of Horn parallel analysis 1965, the Kaiser criterion (Eigenvalues greater than 1) (Kaiser, 1960), visual inspection of the Scree plot (Cattell, 1966), the proportion of variance explained (Beavers et al., 2013), as well as conceptual considerations.
Consistent with standard practice, items were retained if they had loadings in excess of 0.40, no cross-loadings above 0.32, and item communalities over 0.40 (Worthington and Whittaker, 2006). Each factor was assessed for the presence of redundant items, and within factors inter-item correlations were between 0.30 and 0.90. Item-total correlations were required to be above 0.30 to allow averaging of factor scores without applying item weights (Field, 2009).
The related but distinct processes of factor analysis and item deletion should be carried out iteratively (Worthington and Whittaker, 2006). This process involves removing items from the analysis, one at a time, repeating the EFA and comparing the solutions using multiple criterion methods (i.e., parallel analysis, the Kaiser rule, percentage of variance explained, and the scree plot) (Costello and Osborne, 2005; Schönrock-Adema et al., 2009).
Applying Principal Axis Factoring (PAF) to the 164-item dataset, 32 factors were extracted accounting for 60.40% of the variance. One hundred and sixteen items were deleted iteratively for failing to meet item retention criteria. Using syntax provided by O'Connor (2000) parallel analysis of this 48-item dataset recommended a 13-factor solution (63.88% variance explained), and the Kaiser criterion suggested 8 factors be retained accounting for 56.72% of the variance. Forcing 8 factors led to a solution that was not sufficiently conceptually differentiated. The 13-factor solution suggested by parallel analysis contained 4 factors that, on theoretical grounds and by reference to the existing literature, were better represented as 2 factors. Ultimately, an 11-factor solution was deemed most parsimonious and comprehensive and accounted for 61.78% of the variance. This decision was guided primarily by parallel analysis in conjunction with theoretical considerations, while aiming to maximize the proportion of variance explained. In addition, all 48 items and 11 factors possessed good psychometric properties set forth by item and factor retention criteria above, and are reported in Table 3.
Confirmatory Factor Analysis—Confirmatory Sample
In a separate confirmatory sample of participants, CFA was conducted using Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) in Amos version 23 (Arbuckle, 2014). Model fit was assessed using a number of indices. Firstly, a non-significant chi square test is indicative of a well-fitting model. The normed chi-square (Q) is the chi square index divided by the degrees of freedom: acceptable criteria vary from under 2 (Ullman, 2001) to less than 5 (Schumacker and Lomax, 2004). The comparative fit index (CFI) was also used: values at or greater than 0.90 and 0.95 reflect acceptable and excellent fit to the data, respectively (Kenny and McCoach, 2003). Finally, we used the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), with values between 0.05 and 0.09 indicating adequate model fit and values below 0.05 indicating a very good fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999). Modification indices available in CFA can be used to identify misspecification in the model. Decisions regarding modifications were based on theoretical and psychometric considerations of item and scale content. We would not allow error residuals to covary, however we would eliminate items if they had low factor loadings (i.e., standardized regression coefficients) (<0.60), or if modification indices suggested they had significant loadings (>0.30) with unintended latent factors (Byrne, 2010).
The 11-factor solution identified using EFA was tested with CFA with a separate sample of participants. The initial model specified was the 48 scale items loading onto their respective factors. This initial measurement model was an acceptable fit: = 2,178.92, p < 0.001, Q = 2.13, CFI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.045 (90% CI, 0.042–0.048). One item was removed from the Rumination factor, and one from the Identity factor for failing to load above 0.60. These two items were also deemed less conceptually related to the other items. This led to improvement in model fit, and this final model represented a very good fit of the data: = 1,879.33, p < 0.001, Q = 1.99, CFI = 0.94, RMSEA = 0.042 (90% CI, 0.039–0.045). The final 46 items of the AFML scale and their beta weights (β), that is their factor loadings, as well as, the proportion of variance in the latent construct explained by that item (r2) in the confirmatory sample are reported in Table 4.
Table 4. The adaptive functions of music listening scale, internal consistency and descriptive statistics of 11 subscales, and psychometric properties of 46 final scale items.
Intercorrelations between the 11 subscales (see Table 5) suggest they measure related, yet distinct, constructs. Inspection of factor scale items led to the following interpretations. Factor 1, Stress Reduction reflects the use of music for distraction, escape and comfort when stressed. Factor 2, Strong Emotional Experiences taps into intense and blended emotional experiences afforded by music listening. Factor 3, Rumination measures dwelling and focusing on sadness and anxiety in music listening. Factor 4 focuses on music as an aid to Sleep. Factor 5 represents Reminiscence as an expected outcome of music listening. Factors 6 and 7 measure listeners' beliefs that music provides positive reappraisal, positive emotions, distraction and comfort for Anger Regulation and Anxiety Regulation, respectively. Factor 8, measures a sense of Awe and Appreciation during music listening. Factor 9, Loneliness Regulation captures listeners' expectations that listening to music reduces feelings of loneliness. Factor 10, Cognitive Regulation indicates beliefs of increased concentration and focus when listening to music. Factor 11, Identity refers to listening to music to develop and express the self.
Table 5. Bivariate correlations between factors of the AFML scale in the development sample (below the main diagonal), and bivariate correlations between factors of the AFML scale in the confirmatory sample (above the main diagonal).
Reliability
Cronbach alpha 1951 was used to assess scale score reliability, with values of at least 0.70 indicating acceptable internal consistency (Nunnally, 1978).
Internal Consistency
Results support the reliability of the AFML scale in both samples. Cronbach's alpha coefficients for all subscales were high suggesting good internal consistency of the measure and its subscales (see Tables 3, 4).
Validity
Construct Validity
Gender differences in FML have been noted with females using music for affect regulation more than males (North et al., 2000; Sloboda et al., 2001). Saarikallio (2008) found that in a sample of young people (aged 10–20 years) scores on her Music in Mood Regulation scale were significantly higher for females than males indicating greater endorsement of the affect regulation effects of music. Independent t-tests are expected to find, as in previous research, that scores on the affect regulation FML are significantly higher among females.
Gender differences were observed on a number of AFML factors across two samples of participants. Specifically, in the development sample females scored significantly higher than males on the following factors1: Stress Regulation [males M = 3.95, SD = 0.77, females M = 4.15, SD = 0.67; t(635) = −3.36, p = 0.001], Anger Regulation [males M = 3.53, SD = 0.86, females M = 3.70, SD = 0.84; t(635) = −2.36, p = 0.02], Anxiety Regulation [males M = 3.80, SD = 0.77, females M = 3.98, SD = 0.69, t(635) = −2.96, p = 0.005], Loneliness Regulation [males M = 3.73, SD = 0.79, females M = 3.95, SD = 0.75, t(635) = −3.39, p = 0.001] and Sleep [males M = 2.93, SD = 1.16, females M = 3.15, SD = 1.24; t(635) = −2.22, p = 0.03]. Females in the confirmatory sample had significantly higher scores than males on the factors of Anger Regulation [males M = 3.52, SD = 0.67, females M = 3.75, SD = 0.68, t(551) = −3.70, p < 0.001], Anxiety Regulation [males M = 3.69, SD = 0.58, females M = 3.87, SD = 0.64, t(551) = −3.04, p = 0.003] and Reminiscence [males M = 3.95, SD = 0.79, females M = 4.13, SD = 0.69, t = −2.71, p = 0.01]. Confirming hypotheses and providing evidence of construct validity, scores on affect regulation factors were significantly higher for female relative to male respondents in both samples of participants.
Convergent Validity
To evaluate the convergent validity of the constructs being measured, proposed relationships between subscales of the AFML scale and wellbeing outcomes were assessed by Pearson's correlations. Hypotheses were confirmed and results are presented in Table 6.
The AFML scale includes a number of affect regulation subscales: Stress Regulation, Anger Regulation, and Anxiety Regulation. To validate these subscales convergence with the emotion regulation questionnaire (ERQ; Gross and John, 2003) was examined. The ERQ measures two regulation strategies: Reappraisal and Suppression. The first strategy, reappraisal, is considered the most effective strategy in regulating NA (Augustine and Hemenover, 2009) and higher reappraisal scores as measured by the ERQ have been associated with higher SWB (Gross and John, 2003). Items measuring positive reappraisal were developed for each AFML affect regulation subscale (see Figure 1). Positive reappraisal items loaded on the factors of Anger Regulation and Anxiety Regulation. It is expected therefore that scores on these factors will converge (i.e., correlate positively) with reappraisal as measured by the ERQ. The second strategy, suppression, involves the inhibition of positive and negative emotional expression. Suppression was unrelated to music listening functions in validation tests of other FML measures, specifically, the MUSE (Chin and Rickard, 2012) and the MMR (Saarikallio, 2008). There are no items measuring the strategy of suppression in music listening on the AFML scale. Additionally, in the regulation of NA it is a largely ineffective strategy (Larsen and Prizmic, 2004). Thus suppression is not expected to correlate with affect regulation FML, demonstrating divergent validity. Further evidence of divergent validity will be provided by the lack of significant associations between scores on the emotion regulation questionnaire and scores on AFML scale factors not measuring emotion regulation (i.e., Strong Emotional Experiences, Sleep, Reminiscence, Awe and Admiration, Cognitive Regulation, and Identity). Results are presented in Table 7. Hypotheses were confirmed with two exceptions—scores on the Rumination factor of the AFML scale were positively associated with Suppression, and Sleep was associated with higher Reappraisal scores as measured by the ERQ.
Figure 1. Adaptive effects of music listening identified in the literature linked with functions of music listening via Social Cognitive Theory (Bandura, 1989, 2001).
Table 7. Bivariate correlations between the AFML subscales and the music engagement subscales of the MUSE and ERQ.
Concurrent Validity
Bivariate correlations (Pearson's r) between the AFML subscales and another general measure of music listening functions (the MUSE, Chin and Rickard, 2012) examined the concurrent validity of the AFML scale. Results are presented in Table 7. It was not expected that the factor of cognitive and emotional regulation on the MUSE would correlate with all 11 subscales of the AFML scale. Reassuringly, the largest correlations were with the affect regulation subscales, however, the positive correlation with the Identity subscale was slightly larger. It was interesting that there was consistently positive, albeit modest, correlations between all subscales of the AFML scale and scores on the MUSE social connection factor, as only one social FML was retained in the current study (Identity). Again, the relationship between these two factors was the strongest. Such widespread associations across the factors were not predicted, however the remainder of the correlations were low and positive. This is indicative of convergence between the MUSE and the AFML scale, but also suggests there are distinctions between the constructs measured by each scale. Demonstrating concurrent validity of the AFML scale, correlations were considerably stronger between subscales of the AFML scale and the MUSE (i.e., two measures of the same construct), than between scores on the AFML scale and scores on measures of wellbeing (i.e., two measures of related constructs).
Discussion
The AFML scale is a 46-item measure composed of 11 factors. Namely, Stress Regulation, Anxiety Regulation, Anger Regulation, Loneliness Regulation, Rumination, Reminiscence, Strong Emotional Experiences, Awe and Appreciation, Cognitive Regulation, Identity, and Sleep. In line with past research, factors relating to affective, social and cognitive functions of music listening were extracted. Contrary to the literature and our previous qualitative work, eudaimonic FML did not emerge as significant factors. The scale and its subscales possess good internal consistency and construct validity.
Informed by established guidelines in scale development (DeVellis, 2012) and in psychometric statistical analysis (Costello and Osborne, 2005; Worthington and Whittaker, 2006; Byrne, 2010), the rigorous scale development process undertaken in this large-sample study has contributed to the creation of a high quality measure of FML. In developing this measure listeners were consulted directly in a qualitative inquiry of the FML (Groarke and Hogan, 2016). These proposed constructs were expanded upon by review of the music psychology literature, and by importing theories from general and positive psychology. Padgett (1998) advocates for a mixed-methods approach to scale construction—with qualitative study to explore concepts preceding quantitative work. When initially grounded in qualitative work the psychological concepts developed in quantitative research are said to have greater validity because they are derived from real life experiences and observations (Rowan and Wulff, 2007).
Following best practice, structural equation modeling (SEM) using data from an independent sample of participants was used in the current study to confirm the factor structure identified using exploratory factor analysis (EFA) in the development sample. SEM performs best when the model being evaluated is grounded in theory (Byrne, 2010). The measurement model evaluated in the current study provided a very good fit of the data. As is always the case with SEM, there may be alternative models which fit the data equally well. In the case of scale development work alternative models could arise if alternative criteria were adopted during the EFA process. That being said, confidence in the dimensionality of the AFML measure is enhanced in that not only was the factor structure replicated in an independent sample using CFA, the measurement model evaluated was initially grounded in qualitative enquiry and further refined through a synthesis with existing theory and research, expert review, and well-established and conservative criteria for the extraction and identification of factors in EFA. The last step in particular, the exploratory factor analysis, is essential to ensure an unbiased approach to the identification of statistically reliable factors that have some chance of being confirmed in subsequent research.
Providing support for the concurrent validity of the measure, the AFML scale was moderately and positively correlated with another measure of music listening functions (i.e., the MUSE; Chin and Rickard, 2012). At the same time the broad range of factors extracted and confirmed in this study builds upon existing measures and allows for a broader investigation of the adaptive functions of music listening. Further, the AFML scale was highly correlated with the MUSE Index of Music Listening, and reassuringly, was not related to its Index of Musical Training in either sample. It was modestly correlated with an Index of Musical Instrument Playing. Subscales measuring affect regulation were also positively correlated with a standardized measure of emotion regulation from the mainstream psychology literature (i.e., the ERQ; Gross and John, 2003), in line with expectations. Positive relationships between the affect regulation factors (Stress, Anger, Anxiety, and Loneliness regulation) and the reappraisal subscale of the ERQ, as well as the lack of relationships between these same AFML factors and the suppression subscale of the ERQ was in line with predictions and provides support for the convergent and divergent validity of these AFML scale constructs. Providing further evidence of divergent validity, the affective experience factors (Strong Emotional Experiences and Reminiscence), cognitive factors (Cognitive Regulation, Awe and Appreciation), everyday listening factors (Sleep), and social factors (Identity) were not significantly associated with either of the emotion regulation subscales on the ERQ. These findings highlight AFML factors as discrete and meaningfully distinct functions of music listening.
Construct validity was also assessed by examining correlates between FML factors and wellbeing, specifically subjective, psychological, and social wellbeing measures. These constructs and their relationship to wellbeing outcomes are discussed below.
Affective Functions
Affective Experience
The literature on music listening functions and the results of our qualitative work suggested people use music to generate affective experiences, such as positive and negative affect, intense emotional experiences, and also to experience reminiscence or nostalgia (Sloboda, 1999; Sloboda et al., 2001; Gabrielsson, 2002; Juslin et al., 2008; Groarke and Hogan, 2016). In the current study factors related to Strong Emotional Experiences and Reminiscence in music listening were extracted. It was predicted that higher scores on the affective experience factors would be associated with greater positive and negative affect. Notably, Reminiscence was associated with higher negative affect (NA) only, but Strong Emotional Experiences was associated with both PA and NA across samples as predicted. Affective functions including Strong Emotional Experiences and Reminiscence were among participants' highest ranked FML for enhancing wellbeing (Groarke and Hogan, 2016), but were not associated with higher psychological, and social wellbeing outcomes in the current study.
Affective Regulation
Supporting Juslin and Sloboda's (2010) assertion that mood regulation is the most important function of music, the majority of FML factors in the current study were affect regulatory functions. Factors emerged relating to the use of music for Anger Regulation, Anxiety Regulation and Stress Regulation. A diverse set of affect regulation strategies were derived from music psychology (Saarikallio and Erkkila, 2007) and the general psychology literature on mood regulation (Larsen, 2000) and coping (Folkman and Lazarus, 1985; Carver et al., 1989). A number of these proposed strategies (see Figure 2) were retained in factor analysis and endorsed as musical affect regulation strategies by two large samples of respondents in the current study (i.e., distraction, reappraisal, emotional support, positive emotions, escape, and rumination). As predicted, higher scores on affect regulation factors (Anger Regulation and Anxiety regulation) were significantly associated with some core indicators of enhanced SWB (i.e., higher PA and life satisfaction, but not lower NA). In the confirmatory sample, in addition to higher SWB, higher scores on these regulation factors were also associated with greater psychological and social wellbeing. In line with past research (North et al., 2000; Miranda and Claes, 2009), the use of music for affect regulation was significantly greater among females. Although Stress Regulation scores were also significantly higher in female respondents, the Stress Regulation factor was not associated with SWB as expected, with the exception of one small positive correlation with PA in the confirmatory sample. Consistent with research on music listening by Knobloch and Zillmann (2002), Saarikallio (2011), and Saarikallio et al. (2015), a factor measuring the mood regulation strategy of Rumination in music listening was also extracted. Items reflect music-induced rumination on sadness and anxiety. Rumination is often considered a maladaptive regulation strategy, due to its role in the maintenance of negative affective states (Aldao et al., 2010). This is also true of musical rumination (Miranda and Gaudreau, 2010). In the current study, Rumination was associated with lower subjective, psychological, and social wellbeing. According to theory women are more likely to use rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991), and this was also true of Rumination FML in the current study.
Figure 2. Conceptual structure informing development of the adaptive functions of music listening scale.
Social Functions
Listening to music for functions of identity development and expression are documented in qualitative and quantitative research with participants of all ages (Hays and Minichiello, 2005; Laukka, 2007; Lonsdale and North, 2011). The formation of identity is a cornerstone in theories of psychosocial development (Erikson, 1959). Further, development and maintenance of self-concept is an important motive driving social interaction, and is of particular significance in youth development (Carstensen, 1995). Identity has been previously linked to enhanced eudaimonic wellbeing (Bauer et al., 2006), and in the current study higher scores on the Identity factor was associated with higher scores on a measure of psychological wellbeing that included constructs such as self-acceptance and personal growth.
Previous research has proposed a number of social FML, such as the facilitation of social relationships (Huron, 2001; Panksepp and Bernatsky, 2002). However, few of these proposed social factors emerged in the current study. Although conceptualized as a social FML, the Loneliness Regulation factor was strongly related to the affect regulation subscales, and was not positively associated with social wellbeing as expected. Similar to Stress Regulation, however, it was modestly associated with higher PA in the confirmatory sample.
Cognitive Functions
Items pertaining to the use of music for cognitive reasons, such as regulation, music analysis, awe and appreciation, curiosity, and creativity were administered based on previous investigations and existing measures (North et al., 2000; Tarrant et al., 2000; Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2007; Chin and Rickard, 2012). A factor relating to the use of music for Awe and Appreciation was identified through the process of factor analysis. Awe and appreciation in music listening was not associated with greater psychological wellbeing as hypothesized. Unexpectedly, it was associated with greater NA in the development sample. The use of music for increased focus and concentration, Cognitive Regulation, also emerged as a factor in the current study, but it was not significantly associated with wellbeing as predicted, except for one small positive correlation with PA in the development sample.
A complex multi-factorial model of music listening functions was created from qualitative enquiry, and an extensive literature review. However a great many of these varied FML did not emerge as distinct factors in the context of EFA. In particular no Eudaimonic Functions were extracted. It may be that Eudaimonic FML are less distinctly identifiable among items measuring potentially more dominant Affective FML. It is also possible that Eudaimonic FML are less widely distributed in the general population, or less common in a sample of university students. Developmental differences in FML have been observed in cross-sectional surveys (Lonsdale and North, 2011), and in our qualitative work we found that Eudaimonic FML were more pronounced among older adults (Groarke and Hogan, 2016). The aim of the current study was to develop a general measure of music listening functions, and determine the latent factor structure underlying a large set of items measuring a great many hypothesized constructs. The conservative approach taken to develop a scale of high psychometric quality may have come at the expense of a more comprehensive factor structure. However, other researchers may wish to develop unidimensional scales around the wider set of constructs uncovered and can expand further upon the scale items developed and administered for EFA. To that end the full set of items are provided as Supplementary Material.
Limitations
The large sample of participants across both studies was for the most part drawn from a convenience sample of university students. Item and page randomization of the online questionnaire was employed to minimize item-order bias (Siminski, 2008) and maximize the possibility that data lost due to attrition could be substituted later. However, despite taking these steps the high rate of attrition led to a large proportion of missing data. Although data was found to not be Missing Completely At Random (MCAR) (Little and Rubin, 1987), a complete cases analysis approach was adopted as data imputation techniques included in the SPSS software package (e.g., Expectation Maximization) are less appropriate when the proportion of missing data is greater than 5–10% (Enders, 2001; Scheffer, 2002). However, this limits the generalizability of the findings to more diverse populations.
Retention criteria recommended the removal of all but two of the negatively worded (i.e., reverse scored) items. Although it is recommended to include such items (DeVellis, 2012), some argue that negatively worded items are difficult to understand, do not reduce response bias, and should be avoided (van Sonderen et al., 2013). While potentially important from a psychometric perspective, other well-developed measures of FML have also found the inclusion of such items problematic (Saarikallio, 2008; Chin and Rickard, 2012). The factors representing Sleep and Cognitive Regulation functions of music are made up of 2 indicators each. Although factor loadings and inter-item correlations for these items were high, three to five indicators with significant loadings are recommended for factors to be sufficiently identified (Costello and Osborne, 2005). Therefore, researchers are cautioned against using these scales as unidimensional measures of Sleep and Cognitive Regulation FML.
It has been recommended that studies of music consider features of the listener, the context, and the music (Juslin and Västfjäll, 2008). The AFML scale focuses on the listener's FML, but does not fully consider the FML in context, or music preferences and choices (e.g., specific music for specific functions). At the same time, an individual's efficacy beliefs regarding the functions of music listening differ conceptually, and may in fact precede, the music selection strategies that listeners engage to fulfill these FML (North and Hargreaves, 1996, 2000; Van den Tol and Edwards, 2013). As such, the AFML scale may be used to shed light on individual differences in the listener's approach to music listening that influence how music is experienced in context. Future studies are needed to examine this issue.
The primary aim of correlation analyses in the current study was to examine the construct validity of The AFML measure. While the results may provide some insight into the nature of the relationships between adaptive FML and wellbeing outcomes, the cross-sectional nature of the research precludes causal claims being made regarding the direction of these relationships. Experimental designs provide better evidence of causation, yet the laboratory is a fairly unnatural environment for music listening. Experience and mobile sampling technologies provide considerable potential in this regard. A limitation of existing ESM studies (Sloboda et al., 2001; North et al., 2004; Juslin et al., 2008; Greasley and Lamont, 2011; Randall and Rickard, 2017) is that they have generally not included established measures of study variables; in particular none have used a psychometric measure of the FML, instead using a limited number of researcher-selected lists to track different functions, goals and reasons for listening to music in episodes of daily life. Building upon correlational studies, future research should incorporate superior predictive modeling analyses using longitudinal and experience sampling techniques in combination with high quality psychometric tools, such as The AFML Scale, to better address questions as to the relationship between adaptive FML, music listening behaviors, and wellbeing over longer periods of time in everyday contexts. At the same time, easy to administer surveys remain a valuable tool in contexts where these more complex and demanding experience sampling designs are less appropriate, for example screening in applied settings, or for use in more traditional research where longitudinal effects, or ecological validity are not the primary concern.
Conclusion
The Adaptive Functions of Music Listening Scale is a measure suited for outcomes-based research on music listening functions. The AFML measure and its 11 subscales possess good internal consistency and validity. Further psychometric investigation is needed to establish the predictive, discriminant and known-groups validity of all subscales of the AFML measure. However, the affect regulation subscales in particular demonstrate good validity and reliability. Furthermore, factor scores on affective, social, and cognitive FML associated positively with indicators of wellbeing supporting the adaptivity of music listening functions.
Ethics Statement
This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of National University of Ireland, Galway Research Ethics Committee with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the National University of Ireland, Galway Research Ethics Committee.
Author Contributions
JG devised the project. JG and MH designed the study. JG collected and analyzed the data. JG and MH wrote the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by the Irish Research Council (grant number GOIP/2013/876).
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors sincerely thank content experts Dr. S. Saarikallio, Dr. F. Russo, Dr. G. Dingle, and Dr. T. Morrisson for reviewing the initial scale items developed.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00516/full#supplementary-material
Footnotes
1. ^Given multiple comparisons a more conservative alpha level of 0.005 (p = 0.05/11) would be more acceptable for interpreting mean differences in light of the Bonferroni approach.
References
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 217–237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004
Augustine, A. A., and Hemenover, S. H. (2009). On the relative effectiveness of affect regulation strategies: a meta-analysis. Cogn. Emot. 23, 1181–1220. doi: 10.1080/02699930802396556
Baltazar, M., and Saarikallio, S. (2016). Toward a better understanding and conceptualization of affect self-regulation through music: a critical, integrative literature review. Psychol. Music 44, 1500–1521. doi: 10.1177/0305735616663313
Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. Am. Psychol. 44, 1–10. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.9.1175
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: an agentic perspective. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 1–26. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1
Bauer, J. J., McAdams, D. P., and Pals, J. L. (2006). Narrative identity and eudaimonic well-being. J. Happiness Stud. 9, 81–104. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9021-6
Beavers, A. S., Lounsbury, J. W., Richards, J. K., Huck, S. W., Skolits, G. J., Esquivel, S. L., et al. (2013). Practical considerations for using exploratory factor analysis in educational research. Prac. Assess. Res. Eval. 18, 1–13.
Byrne, B. M. (2010). Structural Equation Modeling with AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Routledge.
Carmines, E. G., and Zeller, R. A. (1979). Reliability and Validity Assessment. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Carstensen, L. L. (1995). Evidence for a life-span theory of socioemotional selectivity. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 4, 151–156. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512261
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., and Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: a theoretically based approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 56:267.
Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate Behav. Res. 1, 245–276.
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., and Furnham, A. (2007). Personality and music: can traits explain how people use music in everyday life? Br. J. Psychol. 98, 175–185. doi: 10.1348/000712606X111177
Chin, T., and Rickard, N. S. (2012). The Music USE (MUSE) Questionnaire: an instrument to measure engagement in music. Music Percept. 29, 429–446. doi: 10.1525/mp.2012.29.4.429
Chin, T., and Rickard, N. S. (2014a). Emotion regulation strategy mediates both positive and negative relationships between music uses and well-being. Psychol. Music 42, 692–713. doi: 10.1177/0305735613489916
Chin, T., and Rickard, N. S. (2014b). Beyond positive and negative trait affect: Flourishing through music engagement. Psychol. Well-Being Theory Res. Prac. 4:25. doi: 10.1186/s13612-014-0025-4
Costello, A. B., and Osborne, J. W. (2005). Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Prac. Assess. Res. Eval. 10, 1–9.
Cox, W. M., and Klinger, E. (1988). A motivational model of alcohol use. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 97, 168–180.
Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika 16, 297–334. doi: 10.1007/BF02310555
DeNora, T. (1999). Music as a technology of the self. Poetics 27, 31–56. doi: 10.1016/S0304-422X(99)00017-0
DeVellis, R. F. (2012). Scale Development: Theory and Applications, 3rd Edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71–75.
Diener, E., Larsen, R. J., Levine, S., and Emmons, R. A. (2004). Intensity and frequency: dimensions underlying positive and negative affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 48, 1253–1265. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.48.5.1253
Enders, C. K. (2001). A primer on maximum likelihood algorithms available for use with missing data. Struct. Equ. Model. 8, 128–141. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0801_7
Folkman, S., and Lazarus, R. S. (1985). If it changes it must be a process: study of emotion and coping during three stages of a college examination. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 48, 150–170. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.48.1.150
Folkman, S., and Lazarus, R. S. (1988). Coping as a mediator of emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 466–475. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.3.466
Gabrielsson, A. (2002). Emotion perceived and emotion felt: same or different? Musicae Scientiae 2001–2002, 123–147. doi: 10.1177/10298649020050S105
Gabrielsson, A. (2010). “Strong experiences with music,” in Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, Applications, eds P. N. Juslin and J. A. Sloboda (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 547–574.
Gillham, J., Adams-Deutsch, Z., Werner, J., Reivich, K., Coulter-Heindl, V., Linkins, M., et al. (2011). Character strengths predict subjective well-being during adolescence. J. Posit. Psychol. 6, 31–44. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2010.536773
Greasley, A. E., and Lamont, A. (2011). Exploring engagement with music in everyday life using experience sampling methodology. Music. Sci. 15, 45–71. doi: 10.1177/1029864910393417
Groarke, J. M., and Hogan, M. J. (2016). Enhancing wellbeing: an emerging model of the adaptive functions of music listening. Psychol. Music 44, 769–791. doi: 10.1177/0305735615591844
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
Gross, J. J., and John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
Hargreaves, D. J., and North, A. C. (1999). The functions of music in everyday life: redefining the social in music psychology. Psychol. Music 27, 71–83. doi: 10.1177/0305735699271007
Harrison, L., and Loui, P. (2014). Thrills, chills, frissons, and skin orgasms: toward an integrative model of transcendent psychophysiological experiences in music. Front. Psychol. 5:790. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00790
Haslam, S. A., Jetten, J., Postmes, T., and Haslam, C. (2009). Social identity, health and well-being: an emerging agenda for applied psychology. Appl. Psychol. 58, 1–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00379.x
Hays, T., and Minichiello, V. (2005). The meaning of music in the lives of older people: a qualitative study. Psychol. Music 33, 437–451. doi: 10.1177/0305735605056160
Hays, T., and Minichiello, V. (2008). Older people's experience of spirituality through music. J. Relig. Spiritual. Aging 18, 83–96. doi: 10.1300/J496v18n01_06
Herbert, R. (2012). Musical and non-musical involvement in daily life: the case of absorption. Music. Sci. 16, 41–66. doi: 10.1177/1029864911423161
Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika 30, 179–185. doi: 10.1007/BF02289447
Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equat. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118
Huron, D. (2001). Is music an evolutionary adaptation? Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 930, 43–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05724.x
Juslin, P. N., Liljeström, S., Västfjäll, D., Barradas, G., and Silva, A. (2008). An experience sampling study of emotional reactions to music: listener, music, and situation. Emotion 8, 668–683. doi: 10.1037/a0013505
Juslin, P. N., and Sloboda, J. A. (eds.) (2010). Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, Applications. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Juslin, P. N., and Västfjäll, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: the need to consider underlying mechanisms. Behav. Brain Sci. 31, 559–621. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X08005293
Kahn, S., Zimmerman, G., Csikszentmihalyi, M., and Getzels, J. W. (1985). Relations between identity in young adulthood and intimacy at midlife. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 49, 1316–1322. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.49.5.1316
Kaiser, H. F. (1960). The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 20, 141–151. doi: 10.1177/001316446002000116
Kenny, D. A., and McCoach, D. B. (2003). Effect of the number of variables on measures of fit in structural equation modeling. Struct. Equat. Model. 10, 333–351. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM1003_1
Kincaid, J. P., Fishburne, R. P., Rogers, R. L., and Chissom, B. S. (1975). Derivation of New Readability Formulas (Automated Readability Index, Fog Count, and Flesch Reading Ease Formula) for Navy Enlisted Personnel. Research Branch Report 8–75. Chief of Naval Technical Training; Naval Air Station Memphis.
Knight, W., and Rickard, N. S. (2001). Relaxing music prevents stress-induced increases in subjective anxiety, systolic blood pressure, and heart rate in healthy males and females. J. Music Ther. 38, 254–272. doi: 10.1093/jmt/38.4.254
Knobloch, S., and Zillmann, D. (2002). Mood management via the digital jukebox. J. Commun. 52, 351–366. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2002.tb02549.x
Kuntsche, E., Le Mevel, L., and Berson, I. (2016). Development of the four-dimensional Motives for Listening to Music Questionnaire (MLMQ) and associations with health and social issues among adolescents. Psychol. Music 44, 219–233. doi: 10.1177/0305735614562635
Lamers, S. M. A., Westerhof, G. J., Bohlmeijer, E. T., Klooster ten, P. M., and Keyes, C. L. M. (2010). Evaluating the psychometric properties of the mental health Continuum-Short Form (MHC-SF). J. Clin. Psychol. 67, 99–110. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20741
Lamont, A. (2011). University students' strong experiences of music: pleasure, engagement, and meaning. Music. Sci. 15, 229–249. doi: 10.1177/102986491101500206
Larsen, R. J. (2000). Toward a science of mood regulation. Psychol. Inq. 11, 129–141. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1103_01
Larsen, R. J. (2009). The contributions of positive and negative affect to emotional well-being. Psychol. Top. 18, 247–266.
Larsen, R. J., and Prizmic, Z. (2004). “Affect regulation,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation, eds R. F. Baumeister and K. D. Vohs (New York, NY: Guilford), 40–61.
Laukka, P. (2007). Uses of music and psychological well-being among the elderly. J. Happiness Stud. 8, 215–241. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9024-3
Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1984). “Coping and adaptation,” in The Handbook of Behavioral Medicine, ed W. D. Gentry (New York, NY: Guildford), 282–325.
Little, R. J. A., and Rubin, D. B. (1987). Statistical Analysis with Missing Data. New York, NY: John Wiley.
Lonsdale, A. J., and North, A. C. (2011). Why do we listen to music? A uses and gratifications analysis. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 108–134. doi: 10.1348/000712610X506831
Mas-Herrero, E., Marco-Pallares, J., Lorenzo-Seva, U., Zatorre, R. J., and Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2013). Individual differences in music reward experiences. Music Percept. 31, 118–138. doi: 10.1525/mp.2013.31.2.118
Miranda, D., and Claes, M. (2009). Music listening, coping, peer affiliation and depression in adolescence. Psychol. Music 37, 215–233. doi: 10.1177/0305735608097245
Miranda, D., and Gaudreau, P. (2010). Music listening and emotional well-being in adolescence: a person- and variable-oriented study. Eur. Rev. Appl. Psychol. 61, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.erap.2010.10.002
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 100:569.
North, A. C., and Hargreaves, D. J. (1996). Responses to music in aerobic exercise and yogic relaxation classes. Br. J. Psychol. 87, 535–547. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1996.tb02607.x
North, A. C., and Hargreaves, D. J. (2000). Musical preferences during and after relaxation and exercise. Am. J. Psychol. 113, 43–67. doi: 10.2307/1423460
North, A. C., and Hargreaves, D. J. (2006). Music and adolescent identity. Music Educ. Res. 1, 75–92. doi: 10.1080/1461380990010107
North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., and Hargreaves, J. J. (2004). Uses of music in everyday life. Music Percept. 22, 41–77. doi: 10.1525/mp.2004.22.1.41
North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., and O'Neill, S. A. (2000). The importance of music to adolescents. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 70, 255–272. doi: 10.1348/000709900158083
O'Brien, K. (1993). Using focus groups to develop health surveys: an example from research on social relationships and AIDS-Preventive behaviour. Health Educ. Q. 20, 361–372. doi: 10.1177/109019819302000307
O'Connor, B. P. (2000). SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of components using parallel analysis and Velicer's MAP test. Behav. Res. Methods Instrument. Comput. 32, 396–402. doi: 10.3758/BF03200807
Padgett, D. K. (1998). Qualitative Methods in Social Work Research: Challenges and Rewards. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Panksepp, J., and Bernatsky, G. (2002). Emotional sounds and the brain: the neuro-affective foundations of musical appreciation. Behav. Processes 60, 133–155. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(02)00080-3
Papinczak, Z. E., Dingle, G. A., Stoyanov, S. R., Hides, L., and Zelenko, O. (2015). Young people's uses of music for well-being. J. Youth Stud. 18, 1119–1134 doi: 10.1080/13676261.2015.1020935
Pennebaker, J. W., and Seagal, J. D. (1999). Forming a story: the health benefits of narrative. J. Clin. Psychol. 55, 1243–1254. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(199910)55:10<1243::AID-JCLP6>3.0.CO;2-N
Radstaak, M., Geurts, S. A. E., Brosschot, J. F., and Kompier, M. A. J. (2014). Music and psychophysiological recovery from stress. Psychosom. Med. 76, 529–537. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000094
Randall, W. M., and Rickard, N. S. (2013). Development and trial of a mobile experience sampling method (m-ESM) for personal music listening. Music Percept. 31, 157–170 doi: 10.1525/mp.2013.31.2.157
Randall, W. M., and Rickard, N. S. (2017). Reasons for personal music listening: a mobile experience sampling study of emotional outcomes. Psychol. Music 45, 479–495. doi: 10.1177/0305735616666939
Rowan, N., and Wulff, D. (2007). Using qualitative methods to inform scale development. Qual. Rep. 12, 450–466. Available online at: https://nsuworks.nova.edu/tqr/vol12/iss3/7
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39, 1161–1178. doi: 10.1037/h0077714
Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am. Psychol. 55, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1069–1081. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069
Ryff, C. D., and Keyes, L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 719–727. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.4.719
Ryff, C. D., and Singer, B. H. (2008). Know thyself and become what you are: a eudaimonic approach to psychological well-being. J. Happiness Stud. 9, 13–39. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9019-0
Saarikallio, S. (2008). Music in mood regulation: initial scale development. Music. Sci. 12, 291–309. doi: 10.1177/102986490801200206
Saarikallio, S. (2011). Music as emotional self-regulation throughout adulthood. Psychol. Music 39, 307–327. doi: 10.1177/0305735610374894
Saarikallio, S., and Erkkila, J. (2007). The role of music in adolescents' mood regulation. Psychol. Music 35, 88–109. doi: 10.1177/0305735607068889
Saarikallio, S., Gold, C., and McFerran, K. (2015). Development and validation of the Healthy-Unhealthy Music Scale. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 20, 210–217. doi: 10.1111/camh.12109
Sandstrom, G. M., and Russo, F. A. (2010). Music hath charms: the effects of valence and arousal on recovery following an acute stressor. Music Med. 2, 137–143. doi: 10.1177/1943862110371486
Sandstrom, G. M., and Russo, F. A. (2013). Absorption in music: development of a scale to identify individuals with strong emotional responses to music. Psychol. Music 41, 216–228. doi: 10.1177/0305735611422508
Schäfer, T. (2016). The goals and effects of music listening and their relationship to the strength of music preference. PLoS ONE 11:e0151634. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0151634
Schäfer, T., Sedlmeier, P., Stadtler, C., and Huron, D. (2013). The psychological functions of music listening. Front. Psychol. 4:511. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00511
Schäfer, T., Smukalla, M., and Oelker, S. A. (2014). How music changes our lives: a qualitative study of the long-term effects of intense musical experiences. Psychol. Music 42, 525–544. doi: 10.1177/0305735613482024
Scheffer, J. (2002). Dealing with missing data. Res. Lett. Inform. Math. Sci. 3, 153–160. Available online at: http://muir.massey.ac.nz/bitstream/handle/10179/4355/Dealing_with_Missing_Data.pdf?sequence=1
Schönrock-Adema, J., Heijne-Penninga, M., van Hell, E. A., and Cohen-Schotanus, J. (2009). Necessary steps in factor analysis: enhancing validation studies of educational instruments. The PHEEM applied to clerks as an example. Med. Teach. 31, 226–232. doi: 10.1080/01421590802516756
Schultheiss, O. C., Jones, N. M., Davis, A. Q., and Kley, C. (2008). The role of implicit motivation in hot and cold goal pursuit: effects on goal progress, goal rumination, and emotional well-being. J. Res. Pers. 42, 971–987. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.12.009
Schumacker, R. E., and Lomax, R. G. (2004). A Beginner's Guide to Structural Equation Modelling, Vol. 2. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Seligman, M. E. (2012). Flourish: A Visionary New Understanding of Happiness and Well-being. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.
Siminski, P. (2008). Order effects in batteries of questions. Qual. Quant. 42, 477–490. doi: 10.1007/s11135-006-9054-2
Sloboda, J. (1999). “Everyday uses of music listening: a preliminary study,” in Music, Mind and Science, ed S. W. Yi (Seoul: Western Music Institute), 354–369.
Sloboda, J. A., O'Neill, S. A., and Ivaldi, A. (2001). Functions of music in everyday life: an exploratory study using the experience sampling method. Music. Sci.5, 9–32. doi: 10.1177/102986490100500102
Tarrant, M., North, A. C., and Hargreaves, D. J. (2000). English and American adolescents' reasons for listening to music. Psychol. Music 28, 166–173. doi: 10.1177/0305735600282005
Thoma, M. V., Scholz, U., Ehlert, U., and Nater, U. M. (2012). Listening to music and physiological and psychological functioning: the mediating role of emotion regulation and stress reactivity. Psychol. Health 27, 227–241. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2011.575225
Ullman, J. B. (2001). “Structural Equation Modeling,” in Using Multivariate Statistics, 5th Edn., eds B.G Tabachnick and L. S Fidell (Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon), 676–680.
Van den Tol, A. J. M., and Edwards, J. (2013). Exploring a rationale for choosing to listen to sad music when feeling sad. Psychol. Music 41, 440–465. doi: 10.1177/0305735611430433
van Goethem, A., and Sloboda, J. (2011). The functions of music for affect regulation. Music. Sci. 15, 208–228. doi: 10.1177/1029864911401174
van Sonderen, E., Sanderman, R., and Coyne, J. C. (2013). Ineffectiveness of reverse wording of questionnaire items: Let's learn from cows in the rain. PLoS ONE 8:e68967. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068967
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
Watson, D., and Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychol. Bull. 98, 219–235. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.219
Werner, P. D., Swope, A. J., and Heide, F. J. (2006). The music experience questionnaire: development and correlates. J. Psychol. 140, 329–345. doi: 10.3200/JRLP.140.4.329-345
Worthington, R. L., and Whittaker, T. A. (2006). Scale development research: a content analysis and recommendations for best practices. Couns. Psychol. 34, 806–838. doi: 10.1177/0011000006288127
Keywords: music, functions of music listening, wellbeing, affect, regulation, psychometrics, scale development
Citation: Groarke JM and Hogan MJ (2018) Development and Psychometric Evaluation of the Adaptive Functions of Music Listening Scale. Front. Psychol. 9:516. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00516
Received: 20 October 2017; Accepted: 27 March 2018;
Published: 12 April 2018.
Edited by:
Jin Eun Yoo, Korea National University of Education, South KoreaReviewed by:
Claudio Barbaranelli, Sapienza Università di Roma, ItalyWolfgang Rauch, Universität Heidelberg, Germany
Copyright © 2018 Groarke and Hogan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Jenny M. Groarke, amVubnkuZ3JvYXJrZUBudWlnYWx3YXkuaWU=