Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 29 March 2018
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement
This article is part of the Research Topic Clinical Psychometrics: Old Issues and New Perspectives View all 20 articles

Assessment of Affect Lability: Psychometric Properties of the ALS-18

  • 1Department of Human Sciences, European University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 2Dipartimento di Tecnologie, Comunicazione e Società, Università degli Studi Guglielmo Marconi, Rome, Italy
  • 3Dipartimento di Scienze Psicologiche, della Salute e del Territorio, Università degli Studi “G. d’Annunzio” Chieti-Pescara, Chieti, Italy

Affect lability, an important aspect of emotion dysregulation, characterizes several psychiatric conditions. The short Affective Lability Scales (ALS-18) measures three aspects of changeability between euthymia and affect states (Anxiety/Depression, AD; Depression/Elation, DE; and Anger, Ang). The aim of our study was to investigate the psychometric characteristics of an Italian version of the ALS-18 in a sample of adults recruited from the general population. The sample was composed of 494 adults (343 women and 151 men) aged 18 and higher (mean age = 31.73 years, SD = 12.6). All participants were administered a checklist assessing socio-demographic variables, the ALS-18 and measures of depression and difficulties in emotion regulation. Confirmatory factor analyses indicated adequate fit of the three-factor model (RMSEA = 0.061, 95% CI = 0.054/0.069; CFI = 0.99; SRMR = 0.055), and the presence of a higher-order general factor. Internal consistency was satisfactory for all the lower-order dimensions and the general factor (ordinal α > 0.70). The ALS-18 was significantly associated with concurrent measures of depression and difficulties in emotion regulation. These findings indicate that the ALS-18 is a valid and reliable instrument for measuring affect lability, although discriminant validity of subdimensions scores could be problematic.

Introduction

Past studies suggested that emotion dysregulation could be associated with the development and maintenance of various psychiatric disorders and maladaptive behaviors (Amstadter, 2008; Aldao et al., 2010; Svaldi et al., 2012; American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Contardi et al., 2013). An important aspect of emotion dysregulation is affect lability intended as abnormally frequent, intense, and wide ranging changes in affective states (Thompson et al., 2011). Affect lability is present in several psychiatric conditions and is characteristic of the bipolar disorder and the borderline personality disorder (Henry et al., 2008; Aminoff et al., 2012; Reich et al., 2012). For example, affect lability has recently been found to mediate the relationship between childhood trauma and suicide attempts in bipolar patients (Aas et al., 2017).

The construct of affect lability has strong relationships with other personological constructs such as neuroticism (McCrae and Costa, 1987) and cyclothymia (Akiskal, 2001). Neuroticism (also known as emotional stability-instability or negative emotionality) is part of major models of normal personality structure (i.e., Eysenck’s Three Factor model, and the Big Five model) and it is an ubiquitous element of many personality measures (McCrae and Costa, 1987; Zuckerman et al., 1993). Negative emotionality is a central component in neuroticism, along with cognitive and behavioral facets (McCrae and Costa, 1987). For example, in the NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-3) neuroticism is composed of six facets (i.e., Anxiety, Angry Hostility, Depression, Self-Consciousness, Impulsiveness, and Vulnerability) (McCrae et al., 2005). Questionnaires assessing neuroticism generally measure the frequency of negative emotional states and how easily they are experienced by the individual (e.g., “Get stressed out easily.”, “Often feel blue.”, “Lose my temper.”) (Maples et al., 2014). Conversely, measures of affect lability assess how frequently emotionality change between two specific polarized emotions (e.g., “At times I feel just as realized as everyone else and then within minutes I become so nervous that I feel light-headed and dizzy.”, “I switch back and forth between being extremely energetic and having so little energy that it’s a huge effort just to get where I’m going.”). Thus, although affect lability could be considered a facet of neuroticism, it has a close relationship with the psychiatric construct of cyclothymia. Kraepelin described the cyclothymic disposition as one of the constitutional substrates of the manic-depressive illness (Akiskal, 2001). According to Akiskal (2001) cyclothymic individuals report short cycles of mood swings, characterized mainly by depression and hypomania but also by labile-angry-irritable moods. Mood swings are essentially biphasic, with lethargy alternating with eutonia, or unexplained tearfulness alternating with excessive punning and jocularity (Akiskal and Mallya, 1987).

To measure affect lability, Harvey et al. (1989) developed the Affective Lability Scales (ALS), a 58-item questionnaire measuring changeability among euthymia and four affect states (i.e., depression, elation, anger, and anxiety). The four studies presented in their research indicated satisfactory reliability (internal consistency and stability), discriminant validity with a measure of affect intensity, and concurrent validity with measures of depression (Harvey et al., 1989). Nevertheless, Oliver and Simons (2004) considered the ALS too lengthy and developed a 18-item short form (ALS-18) consisting of at least two items from each dimensions of the ALS. In a non-clinical sample of university students a confirmatory factor analysis supported the adequacy of both a three-factor structure (Anxiety/Depression, AD; Depression/Elation, DE, Anger, Ang) (Bentler–Bonnett Non-normed Fit Index [NNFI] = 0.90; Comparative Fit Index [CFI] = 0.92; Root Mean Square Error of Approximation [RMSEA] = 0.06), and a six-factor model reflecting the structure of the original 58-item version (NNFI = 0.94; CFI = 0.96; RMSEA = 0.05) (Oliver and Simons, 2004). However, the six-factor model included two dimensions composed of only 2 items (Elation and Hypomania), and internal consistency was found to be lower than for the three-factor model. Further studies investigated successfully the adequacy of the three-factor model in different clinical populations (e.g., personality disorders, bipolar disorder patients and relatives, and ADHD) (Look et al., 2010; Aas et al., 2015; Weibel et al., 2017). For example, Look et al. (2010) investigated factor structure and psychometric properties of the ALS-18 in patients with personality disorders and individuals without any psychiatric conditions, and reported satisfactory reliability and good discriminant validity (i.e., people with DSM-IV Cluster B personality disorders reported higher scores than individuals with Cluster A and Cluster C disorders, and people without any psychiatric condition) (Look et al., 2010). Discriminant validity was also supported when differentiating ADHD patients from healthy controls (Weibel et al., 2017), or bipolar patients from relatives and healthy controls (Aas et al., 2015).

Based on the results presented above, and given that the psychometric characteristics of an Italian version of the ALS-18 (as well as the original 54-item version) have not already been investigated, the aim of our study was to investigate factor structure, validity and reliability of the Italian version of the ALS-18 in a non-clinical sample of adults from the general population, as a first step for a cross-cultural validation of the questionnaire. In line with previous studies (Look et al., 2010; Aas et al., 2015; Weibel et al., 2017), we tested the fit of a three-factor model and its superiority over a one-factor model. Considering that previous studies indicated that dimensions of the ALS-18 could be strongly correlated (r ≥ 0.64) (Amstadter, 2008; Look et al., 2010), we also tested whether a hierarchical factor model, with three specific factors (AD, DE, and Ang) loading on a higher-order general factor, or a bi-factor model, with each items loading upon both a group factor (AD, DE, and Ang) and a general factor (AL) could represent well the factor structure of the ALS-18 (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Structural models (A) Three factor model, (B) Hierarchical three factor model, (C) Bifactor model, and (D) One-factor model.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedure

The sample was composed of 494 adults (343 women and 151 men). Mean age of the participants was 31.73 years (SD = 12.61). Inclusion criteria were ages of 18 and higher. Exclusion criteria were the presence of any condition affecting the ability to take the assessment, including illiteracy or denial of informed consent. The sample was recruited through advertisements (flyers, newspaper and online ads) posted for established community groups, and directly from university communities (n = 194) of the authors of the present research. Individuals were approached by psychologists who informed them about the aim of the study and explained how to fill-in the questionnaire. They participated in the study voluntarily and received no payment. Each participant provided written, informed consent prior to data collection. The study protocol received ethics approval from the local research ethics review board. Sociodemographic characteristics of the sample are reported in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Characteristics of the sample (n = 494).

Measures

At entry into the study, all participants were administered a checklist assessing socio-demographic variables (sex, age, marital status, job, and school attainment), and the Italian version of the ALS-18, the Teate Depression Inventory (TDI; Balsamo et al., 2014; Balsamo and Saggino, 2014), and the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) (Gratz and Roemer, 2004).

The original version of the ALS-18 is an 18-item self-report measure used to assess the affect lability. Items are rated on a 4-point Likert type scale (from 0 = very uncharacteristic of me to 3 = very characteristic of me). In the present study, we used an Italian adaptation of this scale. Two bilingual researchers adapted the present version of the questionnaire from the original English version using the back-translation procedure.

The TDI is a 21-item self-report instrument designed to assess major depressive disorder as specified by the latest editions of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM; American Psychiatric Association, 2000, 2013), in order to overcome psychometric weaknesses of existing measures of depression (Balsamo and Saggino, 2007). Each item is rated on a five-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 0 (always) to 4 (never). The TDI demonstrated good psychometric properties (Balsamo et al., 2013, 2014, 2015a,b,c; Innamorati et al., 2013; Saggino et al., 2017). In the present sample, Cronbach’s α was 0.93.

The DERS is a 36-item multidimensional self-report measure assessing the individual’s characteristic patterns of emotion regulation. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale (from 1 = almost never to 5 = almost always) indicating the degree to which each statement describes the respondent’s behavior. It contains the following six subscales: (1) Non-acceptance of emotional responses; (2) Difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior when experiencing negative emotions (Goals); (3) Impulse control difficulties when experiencing negative emotions (Impulse); (4) Lack of emotional awareness; (5) Limited access to emotion regulation strategies that are perceived as effective; and (6) Lack of emotional clarity. In the current sample internal consistency ranged between 0.76 for Awareness and 0.88 for Acceptance.

Statistical Analysis

All the analyses were performed with the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) 19.0 for Windows, and Lisrel 8.80 (Jöreskog and Sörbom, 2006).

Confirmatory factor analysis was performed using a Robust Diagonally Weighted Least Squares estimator (DWLSE) with a polychoric correlation matrix. Model fit was assessed using the following indices: (1) the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), with values between 0.05 and 0.08 indicative of adequacy of the model, and values below 0.05 indicating evidence of good fit (Browne and Cudek, 1993; Hu and Bentler, 1999); (2) the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), with values greater than 0.95/0.96 indicating good fit of the model; (3) the Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR), with values of less than 0.08 indicating good fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999); and (4) the Satorra-Bentler scaled chi-square (χ2) test and the normed χ2 (χ2/degrees of freedom). P-values for the χ2 test greater than 0.05 and a normed χ2 less than 3 (Schreiber et al., 2006) indicate that the model is an adequate fit to the data, although the χ2 test over-reject true models for large samples. The Expected Cross-validation Index (ECVI) was used to compare competing models (Browne and Cudeck, 1989).

Although the aim of the present study was to compare the four competing factor models and select the one with the best fit, the proposed three-factor model and the hierarchical three-factor model are equivalent (i.e., each factor directly or indirectly is related to all the other latent variables) and yield the same fit to the data (MacCallum et al., 1993; Leone, 2009). Thus, the comparison of fit indices is inconclusive in demonstrating which of the two models is better.

As measures of reliability, we reported ordinal Cronbach’s alpha (α) (Zumbo et al., 2007). Associations with sociodemographic variables and other measures were evaluated by means of a series of t-tests and Pearson’s r indices of correlations.

Results

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

The bi-factor model did not converge, and the statistical software issued a warning message indicating that Phi (i.e., the variance/covariance matrix between latent variables) was not positive definite. The other competing models all had significant χ2 (p < 0.001), indicating potential misfit of the models (see Table 2). On the contrary other fit indices indicated the adequacy of both the one-factor model (RMSEA = 0.072, 95% CI = 0.064/0.079; CFI = 0.98; SRMR = 0.066), and the three factor model (RMSEA = 0.061, 95% CI = 0.054/0.069; CFI = 0.99; SRMR = 0.055). Nevertheless, the ECVI suggested the superiority of the three-factor model (0.93 vs. 1.41).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Fit indices for the competing factor models.

The latent dimensions of the three-factor model were highly correlated (r between 0.83 for AD/DE and 0.93 for AD/Ang), and when modeling a hierarchical factor model, factor loadings on the higher-order general factor were all significant (0.93 for AD, 0.89 for DE and 0.99 for Ang). Each item of the ALS-18 loaded significantly on its hypothesized dimension (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Standardized factor loadings for the ALS-18 items (n = 494).

Psychometric Properties of the ALS-18

Considering that fit indices suggested that the three-factor model could represent better the latent structure of the ALS-18, the following analyses will be based on this factor model. Internal consistency of the ALS-18 was satisfactory for all the lower-order dimensions (Table 4), and for the general factor (ordinal alpha = 0.95). Scores on the ALS-18 were not associated with sex (p > 0.05 for t-tests), or with age (r between -0.09 for DE and -0.14 for AD). The ALS-18 dimensions and the general factor were all significantly associated with concurrent measures of depression and difficulties in emotion regulation (Table 4). Correlations with the TDI were all significant and moderate (r ≥ 0.4), ranging from 0.47 for Ang to 0.59 for AD.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Correlations between measures and descriptive statistics (n = 494).

Discussion

In our sample, the three-factor model fitted the data well. This is in line with previous studies which evaluated the structure of other versions of the ALS-18 (Look et al., 2010; Aas et al., 2015; Weibel et al., 2017). Our results could also support the presence of a higher-order general factor suggesting the possibility to compute a total score as generally reported in the literature (Look et al., 2010; Aas et al., 2015; Weibel et al., 2017). Nevertheless, when comparing the three-factor and the hierarchical models the design of our study was not conclusive in demonstrating the superiority of one model over the other (MacCallum et al., 1993; Leone, 2009). Furthermore, the bi-factor model was empirical underidentified denoting that our study did not permit a test of the hypothesized bi-factor model (Green and Yang, 2017). However, as far as we know, this was the first temptative study which assessed directly the fit of a hierarchical or bifactor model for the ALS-18.

Inter-correlations among the three dimensions of the ALS-18 were high (r ≥ 0.83), and despite also other studies reported strong intercorrelations among latent factors (Amstadter, 2008; Look et al., 2010), our figures are higher than those reported in those studies. These data could indicate non-satisfactory discriminant validity of subdimensions scores (Look et al., 2010). The three dimensions and the general factor all had adequate internal consistency.

ALS-18 subdimensions and total score were not associated with sex or age. In the past only Harvey et al. (1989) have investigated this topic for the 54-item ALS and reported sex differences for the depression scale only, suggesting a possible tendency for men to experience depression as a more transient and changeable phenomenon than do women. Conversely, the ALS-18 subdimensions and the general factor were all significantly associated with concurrent measures of depression and difficulties in emotion regulation. Our results are partially discordant from findings of previous studies. For example, Oliver and Simons (2004), in a non-clinical sample of university students, reported significant but negative correlations between the ALS-18 and the Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression Scale (r between -0.33 for Ang and -0.47 for AD). Conversely in our sample, the correlations were all significant and positive. Unfortunately, Oliver and Simons (2004) did not comment this result in their article. Nevertheless, Weibel et al. (2017), who administered a short version of the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), reported an r of 0.34 (p < 0.05) between the ALS-18 total score and the BDI. The positive association between depression and affect lability possibly indicates that people with higher lability could experience phases of depression, despite the two constructs are only moderately correlated and both depression and affect lability should be assessed independently (Weibel et al., 2017). The results could also be seen as supportive of the concept of “soft bipolars spectrum” (Akiskal and Mallya, 1987). In fact, several patients who receive a diagnosis of major depression (MDD) have subthreshold symptoms of bipolarity, which includes biphasic mood swings and cyclothymic traits. These patients differ from pure MDD patients and patients with bipolar disorder for their temperamental profile and for clinical variables (Innamorati et al., 2015). Innamorati et al. (2015), investigating the role of cyclothymic temperament in characterizing mood disorder patients, evidenced that around 39% of inpatients with unipolar depression could be included in the soft bipolar spectrum according to their affective temperament. These patients seem to differ from patients with pure major mood disorders for levels of hopelessness and suicide risk.

Correlations with the DERS were also generally moderate (r ≥ 0.4) with the exclusion of the dimension Awareness of the DERS whose correlations with the ALS-18 were weak (r between 0.10 and 0.16). This means that also the relationship between affect lability and difficulties in emotion regulation could be complex with a partial independence of these two constructs.

Our results must be considered in light of some issues referred to the design of the study. First, in our sample there was a disproportion of female participants compared to males probably associated with the recruitment of university students from the authors’ university communities. This bias also prevented us from assessing structural invariance of the questionnaire between sex. Second, our results are based on a general community sample of adults, composed mostly of young adults, which limits the generalizability of these findings to clinical conditions or older adults. Third, we administered only self-report measures potentially affected by social desirability. In conclusion, this may considered only a first necessary step in the process of the cross-cultural validation of the ALS-18.

Conclusion

Our results indicate that the Italian version of the ALS-18 can produce valid and reliable assessments of affect lability. Additional studies are needed from clinical samples or samples of older adults with further psychometric assessments of reliability and measurement errors to draw clear recommendations for clinical practice use and research.

Ethics Statement

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Author Contributions

AC was involved in the study conception and design, acquisition, analysis and interpretation of data, and drafting of the manuscript. CI was involved in the study conception and design, interpretation of data, and the critical revision of the manuscript, provided final approval of the version to be published. IA was involved in the acquisition of data, interpretation of data, and the critical revision of the manuscript. MB was involved in the study conception and design and the analysis and interpretation of data. MI was involved in the study conception and design, acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of data, and drafting of the manuscript, and provided final approval of the version to be published.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer EP and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation.

References

Aas, M., Henry, C., Bellivier, F., Lajnef, M., Gard, S., Kahn, J., et al. (2017). Affective lability mediates the association between childhood trauma and suicide attempts, mixed episodes and co-morbid anxiety disorders in bipolar disorders. Psychol. Med. 47, 902–912. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716003081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aas, M., Pedersen, G., Henry, C., Bjella, T., Bellivier, F., Leboyer, M., et al. (2015). Psychometric properties of the Affective Lability Scale (54 and 18-item version) in patients with bipolar disorder, first-degree relatives, and healthy controls. J. Affect. Disord. 172, 375–380. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.10.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akiskal, H. S. (2001). Dysthymia and cyclothymia in psychiatric practice a century after Kraepelin. J. Affect. Disord. 62, 17–31. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00347-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akiskal, H. S., and Mallya, G. (1987). Criteria for the “soft” bipolar spectrum: treatment implications. Psychopharmacol. Bull. 23, 68–73.

Google Scholar

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 217–237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Aminoff, S. R., Jensen, J., Lagerberg, T. V., Hellvin, T., Sundet, K., Andreassen, O. A., et al. (2012). An association between affective lability and executive functioning in bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res. 198, 58–61. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amstadter, A. (2008). Emotion regulation and anxiety disorders. J. Anxiety Disord. 22, 211–221. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balsamo, M., Carlucci, L., Sergi, M. R., Klein Murdock, K., and Saggino, A. (2015a). The mediating role of early maladaptive schemas in the relation between co-rumination and depression in young adults. PLoS One 10:e0140177. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balsamo, M., Innamorati, M., Van Dam, N. T., Carlucci, L., and Saggino, A. (2015b). Measuring anxiety in the elderly: psychometric properties of the state trait inventory of cognitive and somatic anxiety (STICSA) in an elderly Italian sample. Int. Psychogeriatr. 27, 999–1008. doi: 10.1017/S1041610214002634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balsamo, M., Macchia, A., Carlucci, L., Picconi, L., Tommasi, M., Gilbert, P., et al. (2015c). Measurement of external shame: an inside view. J. Pers. Assess. 97, 81–89. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2014.947650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balsamo, M., Giampaglia, G., and Saggino, A. (2014). Building a new Rasch-based self-report inventory of depression. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 13, 153–165. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S53425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balsamo, M., Romanelli, R., Innamorati, M., Ciccarese, G., Carlucci, L., and Saggino, A. (2013). The state-trait anxiety inventory: shadows and lights on its construct validity. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 35, 475–486. doi: 10.1007/s10862-013-9354-5

CrossRef Full Text

Balsamo, M., and Saggino, A. (2007). Test per l’assessment della depressione nel contesto italiano: un’analisi critica. [Tests for the assessment of depression in Italian Context: a critical review]. Psicoterapia Cogn. Comportamentale 13, 167–199.

Google Scholar

Balsamo, M., and Saggino, A. (2014). Determining a diagnostic cut-off on the Teate Depression Inventory. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 10, 987–995. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S55706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browne, M. W., and Cudeck, R. (1989). Single sample cross-validation indices for covariance structures. Br. J. Math. Stat. Psychol. 37, 62–83. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr2404_4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browne, M. W., and Cudek, R. (1993). “Alternative ways of assessing model fit,” in Testing Structural Equation Models, ed. J. S. Long (Newbury Park, CA: Sage), 136–162.

Google Scholar

Contardi, A., Farina, B., Fabbricatore, M., Tamburello, S., Scapellato, P., Penzo, I., et al. (2013). [Difficulties in emotion regulation and personal distress in young adults with social anxiety]. Riv. Psichiatr. 48, 155–161. doi: 10.1708/1272.14040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gratz, K. L., and Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 26, 41–54. doi: 10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, S., and Yang, Y. (2017). Empirical underidentification with the bifactor model: a case study. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1–20. doi: 10.1177/0013164417719947

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, P. D., Greenberg, B. R., and Serper, M. R. (1989). The affective lability scales: development, reliability, and validity. J. Clin. Psychol. 45, 786–793. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198909)45:5<786::AID-JCLP2270450515>3.0.CO;2-P

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, C., Van den Bulke, D., Bellivier, F., Roy, I., Swendsen, J., M’Bailara, K., et al. (2008). Affective lability and affect intensity as core dimensions of bipolar disorders during euthymic period. Psychiatry Res. 159, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.11.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Modeling 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Innamorati, M., Rihmer, Z., Akiskal, H., Gonda, X., Erbuto, D., Belvederi Murri, M., et al. (2015). Cyclothymic temperament rather than polarity is associated with hopelessness and suicidality in hospitalized patients with mood disorders. J. Affect. Disord. 170, 161–165. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.08.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Innamorati, M., Tamburello, S., Contardi, A., Imperatori, C., Tamburello, A., Saggino, A., et al. (2013). Psychometric properties of the attitudes toward self-revised in Italian young adults. Depress. Res. Treat. 2013:209216. doi: 10.1155/2013/209216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jöreskog, K. G., and Sörbom, D. (2006). LISREL for Windows [Computer Software]. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc.

Google Scholar

Leone, L. (2009). Testing conceptual distinctions among Carver and White’s (1994) BAS scales: a comment and extension on Smillie, Jackson, and Dalgleish (2006). Pers. Individ. Dif. 46, 54–59. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.004

CrossRef Full Text

Look, A. E., Flory, J. D., Harvey, P. D., and Siever, L. J. (2010). Psychometric properties of a short form of the Affective Lability Scale (ALS-18). Pers. Individ. Dif. 49, 187–191. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacCallum, R. C., Wegener, D. T., Uchino, B. N., and Fabrigar, L. R. (1993). The problem of equivalent models in applications of covariance structure analysis. Psychol. Bull. 114, 185–199. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maples, J. L., Guan, L., Carter, N. T., and Miller, J. D. (2014). A test of the International Personality Item Pool representation of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory and development of a 120-item IPIP-based measure of the five-factor model. Psychol. Assess. 26, 1070–1084. doi: 10.1037/pas0000004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R., and Costa, P. T. Jr. (1987). Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52, 81–90. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.1.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T. Jr., and Martin, T. A. (2005). The NEO-PI-3: a more readable revised NEO personality inventory. J. Pers. Assess. 84, 261–270. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8403_05

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliver, M. N. I., and Simons, J. S. (2004). The Affective Lability Scales: development of a short-form measure. Pers. Individ. Dif. 37, 1279–1288. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2003.12.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reich, D. B., Zanarini, M. C., and Fitzmaurice, G. (2012). Affective lability in bipolar disorder and borderline personality disorder. Compr. Psychiatry 53, 230–237. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saggino, A., Carlucci, L., Sergi, M. R., D’Ambrosio, I., Fairfield, B., Cera, N., et al. (2017). A validation study of the psychometric properties of the Other as Shamer Scale–2. SAGE Open 7, 1–10. doi: 10.1177/2158244017704241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schreiber, J. B., Nora, A., Stage, F. K., Barlow, E. A., and King, J. (2006). Reporting structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis results: a review. J. Educ. Res. 99, 323–337. doi: 10.3200/JOER.99.6.323-338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svaldi, J., Griepenstroh, J., Tuschen-Caffier, B., and Ehring, T. (2012). Emotion regulation deficits in eating disorders: a marker of eating pathology or general psychopathology? Psychiatry Res. 197, 103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, R. J., Berenbaum, H., and Bredemeier, K. (2011). Cross-sectional and longitudinal relations between affective instability and depression. J. Affect. Disord. 130, 53–59. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.09.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weibel, S., Micoulaud-Franchi, J. A., Brandejsky, L., Lopez, R., Prada, P., Nicastro, R., et al. (2017). Psychometric properties and factor structure of the short form of the Affective Lability Scale in adult patients with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. doi: 10.1177/1087054717690808 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., Joireman, J., Teta, P., and Kraft, M. (1993). A comparison of three structural models for personality: the big three, the big five, and the alternative five. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 65, 757–768. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.4.757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zumbo, B. D., Gadermann, A. M., and Zeisser, C. (2007). Ordinal versions of coefficients alpha and theta for likert rating scales. J. Mod. Appl. Stat. Methods 6, 21–29. doi: 10.22237/jmasm/1177992180

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: affect lability, emotion dysregulation, Affective Lability Scales (ALS-18), psychometric properties of ALS-18, validity and reliability of ALS-18

Citation: Contardi A, Imperatori C, Amati I, Balsamo M and Innamorati M (2018) Assessment of Affect Lability: Psychometric Properties of the ALS-18. Front. Psychol. 9:427. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00427

Received: 02 February 2018; Accepted: 14 March 2018;
Published: 29 March 2018.

Edited by:

Pietro Cipresso, Istituto Auxologico Italiano (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Dejan Stevanovic, Clinic for Neurology and Psychiatry for Children and Youth, Serbia
Marco Lauriola, Sapienza Università di Roma, Italy
Elisa Pedroli, Istituto Auxologico Italiano (IRCCS), Italy

Copyright © 2018 Contardi, Imperatori, Amati, Balsamo and Innamorati. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anna Contardi, YW5uYS5jb250YXJkaUB1bmllci5pdA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.